Indices of frequency and temporal resolution as a function of level RobynM. Cox MemphisStateUniversity, andDepartment of Veterans •4ffairsMedicalCenter,Memphis, Tennessee 38105 Genevieve C. Alexander Department of Veterans AffairsMedicalCenter,Memphis.Tennessee 38105 (Received15December1991;acceptedfor publication6 October1992) Severalinvestigators havesuggested that between-subject variationsin hearingaid benefitmay be relatedto between-subject variationsin auditoryresolution.Evaluationof thisrelationship iscomplicated by theneedto assess resolution at severalfrequencies andlevelsin hearingimpairedlisteners whomaynottoleratelengthytestprocedures. Thispaperreportsdatafrom normal-hearing listenersfor abbreviated indicesof frequencyandtemporalresolution.For a particularfrequency-stimulus levelcombination, bothindicesmaybederivedfromthreepuretonethresholds obtainedwith differentmaskingstimuli.Normsare reportedasa functionof maskerlevelfor eachindexat fourtestfrequencies. Changes in theindicesasa functionof maskerlevelwereconsistent withexpectations basedon relatedstudies by otherinvestigators. Normswereobtainedusinga supra-auralearphone.Comparison of resultsfromsupra-aural andinsertearphones suggested that low-frequency soundleakagesubstantially affectedthe normsfor 500Hz. Reliabilityof individualindiceswasgoodto excellent. PACS numbers:43.66.Yw, 43.66.Ts, 43.66.Mk [WDW] INTRODUCTION In viewof the wide variabilityin frequencyandtemporal resolutionobservedin individualswith the samehearing Whenwe measurethe extentto whichamplificationimloss,and theprobablerelationshipbetweenauditoryresoluprovestheabilityof hearing-impaired personsto understand tionandspeechintelligibility,it is reasonable to hypothesize speech,it is commonto observesubstantialbetween-subject that between-subject differencesin hearingaid benefit (dedifferences,even for individualswith similar pure-tone finedasimprovedspeechunderstanding ability) canbeparthresholds,especiallyif speechintelligibilityis measuredin tially explainedby between-subject differences in auditory the presenceof a competingnoise (Cox and Alexander, resolutionvariables.Onefactorthat complicates the evalua1991). Someinvestigatorshavehypothesizedthat thesebetion of anyrelationshipbetweenhearingaid benefitandresotween-subjectdifferencesin benefitmay be relatedto differlutionabilitiesis the effectof stimuluslevel.Bothfrequency encesin the frequencyand/or temporal analysisabilitiesof and temporal resolutionare known to vary with stimulus the individuals' auditory systems (e.g., G!asberg and levelin normal-hearinglistenersbut data describingvariaMoore, 1989). tions with level in hearing-impairedlisteners are more Many studies suggestthat frequency resolution is scarce.If auditory resolutionvarieswith stimuluslevels,the poorerthan normal,but widelyvariable,in individualswith relationshipbetween resolution and hearing aid benefit sensorineural hearingimpairment(seeTyler, 1986,for a rewouldbeinfluenced by thelisteninglevelsfor amplifiedand view). Somestudiesof the relationshipbetweenfrequency unamplifiedspeech.The potentialimportanceof this factor resolutionand speechintelligibility havefoundevidenceof a is supportedby studiesshowingthat speechrecognitionis significantlink betweenthesetwo variables(e.g., Glasberg stronglyrelatedto presentation levelin hearing-impairedlisand Moore, 1989;ter Keurs et al., i 992; Thibodeau and Van tenersand that there is often an optimal presentationlevel Tasell, 1987) althoughothershavefailedto finda link (e.g., for which the highestscoresare obtained (e.g., Gutnick, Dubno and Schaefer, 1992; Lutman and Clark, 1986). Althoughtemporalresolutionper semaynot be affected by cochlearhearing loss (e.g., Moore and Glasberg, 1988), manystudiespresentevidencethat performanceon certain temporalresolutiontasksis,on average,poorerthan normal in listenerswith sensorineural hearingimpairment,and that 1982; Yantis etak, 1966). Beforethe relationshipbetweenauditory resolutionand hearing aid benefitcan be explored, it is necessaryto formu- there are wide individual differences (e.g., Zwicker and lateprocedures formeasurement of frequency andtemporal resolutionin hearingaid wearers.Most laboratorymethods arearduousandmaybetoodemandingfor thetypicalelderly hearingaid wearer,especially if we wishto generatereso- Schorn,1982;Nelson and Freyman, 1987;Glasberget al., 1987). Of the studiesexploring the relationship between temporal resolutionand speechintelligibility, some have found evidenceof a significantlink betweenthe two (e.g., Tyler etal., 1982;Dreschler, 1989), but somehavenot (e.g., Festenand Piomp, 1983). lution data at severalfrequenciesand stimuluslevels.This paper describesresultsfor two indices,one for frequency resolutionand one for temporalresolution,that weredevelopedfor usewith hearingaid wearers.The presentdata encompassresultsobtainedfor normal-hearingsubjectsas a functionof presentationlevel.The goalsof thiswork wereto 1068 J. Acoust.Soc.Am.93 (2), February1993 0001-4966/93/021068-08506.00 ¸ 1993 AcousticalSocietyof America 1068 determine whetherthesetemporalandfrequency resolution 2. Procedure indiceswouldproducepatternsfor normalhearersthatwere The indicesweremeasured usinga Bekesytrackingtest consistent with publisheddata basedon relatedbut more method.Thresholdfor the pulsedtesttonewastrackedin traditionalmeasurement procedures; to generatenormsfor the presence of eachof thethreecontinuousmaskingnoises. each index at severaltest frequencies;and, to evaluatethe reliability of the indices. I. EQUIPMENT I: DERIVATION OF NORMS A. Method 1. Resolution indices The frequencyresolutionindex (FRI) measurement procedurewasbasedon a methodsuggested by Pattersonet al. (1982). The approachis an abbreviationof the notchednoisemaskingmethodusedby theseand manyotherinvestigatorsto determineauditoryfilter bandwidth.The FRI was taken as the differencebetweentwo pure-tonethresholds. The first thresholdwasobtainedin the presenceof a bandpasswhite noisemaskercenteredon the testfrequencyand havinga bandwidthof 0.8foHz ( fo = testfrequency).The secondthresholdwasobtainedin thepresence of a notchednoisemaskerin whicha notch,_+0.3foHz wide,centered on the testfrequency,wasdefinedby two bandsof whitenoise each0.4foHz in width. Notch slopeswere about200 dB/ octaveandminimumnotchdepthwas35 dB. This minimum depthwaschosenbasedon a pilot studywhichdetermined that the FRI wasessentiallyunchangedfor notchdepths rangingfrom 50 to 30 dB when maskerspectrumlevel was heldconstantat 40 dB. In practice,notchdepthsassmallas 35 dB were usedonly for low-levelmaskers.Typical notch depthswere40-50 dB. The temporal resolutionindex (TRI) measurement procedurewasan adaptationof thetestfor temporalresolution factordescribedby Zwieker (1980). Like the FRI, the TRI was taken as the differencebetweentwo pure tone thresholds. The firstthreshold wasobtainedusingthesame 0.8f)-Hz bandwidthwhite noisemaskerasusedfor the FRI. The secondthresholdwasobtainedwith the samemasking noise(0.8fo-Hzbandwidth) 100% amplitudemodulatedby a 14-Hz squarewave. Thus,for onetestfrequency, at onemaskinglevel,both the FRI and theTRI werederivedfrompure-tonethresholds obtained with three different bandlimited white noise The one-octaveband level of the bandpassand modulated maskers wasvariedfrom40 to 90dBSPLat all testfrequencies.For thenotchedmasker,thelowestsound-pressure level usedwasthe lowestlevelat whichit waspossibleto maintain a 35-dB notchdepth (44 dB $PL at the three lower test frequencies and 52 dB SPL at 3000 Hz), 90 dB SPL wasthe upperlimit. A test run beganwith the maskerat the lowestlevel. During a run, the levelof the maskerwasincremented in ldB stepsat therateof 10dB/min. The testtonewaspulsedat a rate of 2.5 pulses/s(rise/fall time = 15 ms) with a 50% dutycycle.The subjectwasinstructedto respondby depressing a button when the tone was audibleand releasingthe button when the tone was not audible. The level of the tone wasvariedat a rate of 3 dB/s. If the buttonwasdepressed, the tone level was decremented. If the button was released, the tone level was incremented. A reversal occurred when the testtonelevelchangedfrom increasingto decreasing or vice versa. The levels of test tone and masker were recorded at each reversal. For all testfrequencies, a whitenoisewith low-passcutoff below the frequencylimits of the primary maskerswas addedat a spectrumlevel25 dB belowthat of the primary maskersto ensurethat switchingtransientsor other distortion productswould be inaudible. In addition, to prevent participationby the untestedear, white noiseat an effective maskinglevel of 40-45 dB SPL was presentedto the contralateral earphone. Data werecollectedat four testfrequencies: 500, 1250, 2000, and 3000 Hz. For eachtestfrequency,two runswere executedfor eachof the threemaskers. This testprocedure requiredabout40 min of testingtimeper frequency. 3. Subjects Ten subjectsprovideddata for eachindex at each test frequency.Exceptfor onesubjectat 3000 Hz, the sameindividualsservedat all frequencies.Subjects'pure-tonethresh- oldsfor the testfrequencies werelessthan25 dB SPL. Ages rangedfrom 22 to 34 years,with a meanof 26. maskers. Theywill bereferredto asbandpass (unmodulated-unnotched), notched (unmodulated-notched), and modulated (modulated-unnotched). Wide-band modulated B. Results and discussion and unmodulated noiseswereproducedby a functionsyn- For eachsubjectandtestfrequency, maskingfunctions weregeneratedfor eachof the threemaskers.For a given werenotchedand/or bandlimitedusingtwo cascaded twomasker,the functionwascomputedusingthe midpointsof channelbrickwallfilters(Wavetek model852), and a bandtrackingexcursions for bothtestrunscombined. The analypassfilter (Frequencydevicesmodel9002), respectively. sis proceduresprovidedfor discardingany data where thesizer (Hewlett Packard model 8904A). These noises Signals werepresented viaa Telephonics TDH49 earphone maskedthresholds werewithin5 dB of threshold in quiet encased in a Telephonics MX41-AR supra-aural cushion (however,it wasnotnecessary to discardanydataon this andspringheadband.Levelswerecalibratedin a standard6- cm3coupler.The nominallevelof all maskers wasthecorresponding one-octave bandlevelof thebandpass masker.At eachnominalmaskerlevel,the threemaskingnoiseswere presentedat the samespectrumlevel. 1069 d.Acoust. Sec.Am.,VoL93, No.2, February1993 basis).The numberof excursion midpointsusedto produce eachfunctionvariedacrosssubjects because trackingexcursionwidthsdiffered.The rangewas84 to 238,with a meanof 147data pointsper function.To generatea maskingfunction,thesemidpointdataweresubjected to regression analy- R.M. CoxandG. C. Alexander: Frequency andtemporalresolution 1069 sisto establisha least-squares best-fitline. A preliminary analysiswas performed to determine whether data for each masker were more accurately describedby a first-or second-order polynomial.Bothfirstand second-orderfunctionsweregeneratedfor eachsubject-frequency-masker conditionand the standarderror of estimate (S•) wasdeterminedfor eachfunction.The S,• data were subjectedto a three-wayanalysisof variancewith test frequency,masker,and regressionorderasvariables.This analysisproduceda significantinteractionbetweenmaskerand regressionorder (F[2,18] = 111.1, p<0.001). Post hoe analysisrevealedthat, for the notchedmasker,the S• was significantlysmaller ( p < 0.001) when a second-order regression wasusedwhereasregression orderdid not significantly impact the Seefor the two other maskers.Basedon thisoutcome,a first-orderpolynomialwasusedto produce maskingfunctionsfor both the bandpassand modulated maskers.A second-orderpolynomialwas usedto generate functionsfor the notchedmasker.Usingtheseprocedures, the meanSeeWasin the range2.0 to 2.2 dB, and standard deviationsof Seeswere < 0.5 dB, for all maskers. The resultingmaskingfunctionsareillustratedin Fig. 1. This figuredepictsmaskingfunctionsdeterminedfor each subjectfor the 3000-Hz testfrequency.The left paneldepicts the two functions that would be used to derive the FRI and the right panelshowscorresponding data for the TRI. Resultsfor the othertestfrequencies werequalitativelysimilar to thesefor 3000 Hz. Functionsfor the bandpassnoise showeda relativelysmallrangeof variationacrosssubjects. For the modulatednoisemasker,between-subject variability wasconsiderably larger.The notchednoisealsoproduceda fairly widerangeof resultsacrosssubjects andthe functions generallywere substantiallycurved with a slope that increasedwith maskerlevel. Weber (1977) reportedsimilarly curved growth of masking functions for notched-noise maskerswith notchwidthsgreaterthan + 0.075. For eachtestfrequency,thesedata wereusedto generate normsfor eachindex.The procedurewasasfollows:( 1) the maskingfunctionsfor the bandpassnoisewere normalized at a one-octaveband maskerlevel of 70 dB; (2) for each subject,the two other maskingfunctionswere adjustedin levelby the sameamountas the bandpass function;(3) means and unbiased standard deviations for the normalized datawerecomputedfor eachfunctionacrossthe 10subjects at 5-dB maskerincrementsencompassing the testedrange; (4) for eachmaskerincrement,the rangeof levelsestimated to encompass 95% of the populationdataweredetermined. The resultsareshownin Figs.2-5. Thesolidlinesdepict the 95% rangeof the maskingfunctionfor the bandpass masker after normalization at a one-octave band level of 70 dB SPL. The dash-dotlinesdescribethe 95% rangeof the maskingfunction for the correspondingnotchedmasker (leftpanel) andthecorresponding modulated masker( right panel). It is anticipatedthat only 5% of normalhearinglistenerswould producedata outsidethesebounds.Normalization of the maskingfunctionsfor the bandpassmasker minimizedthe effectsof between-subject differencesin detectorefficiency.Nevertheless, evenafter normalization,between-subjectdifferenceswere substantialfor the other two maskers.This is revealedby the relativelywide separation betweenthe upperand lower boundsof the normsfor these maskersin Figs.2-5 (the dash-dotlines). To allow accurate reproductionof thesenormsby otherinvestigators, thefour curvescomprisingeachpanelwereempiricallyfitted with second-order polynomials.All of the derivedcurvesproduceda correlationin excess of 0.999with theoriginalcurve. Coefficients for the resultingequationsare givenin Table I. The slopeof the meanfunetonfor the bandpass masker wascloseto 1.0for all testfrequencies (range0.98-1.01), as 500 Hz go / 70 80 FRI 60 50 7O • cn • r• 'T 30! "r I.-- ,,, ..30 z o 10• I-- L,..I 10 r- z o 40 50 60 70 BO 90 40 50 60 70 80 90 ONE-OCTAVE BANDMASKER LEVEL(dB SPL) d. Acoust.Soc. Am., Vol. 93, No. 2, February1993 60 70 BO 90 40 50 60 70 80 90 FIG.2.l•orms formasking functions comprising thefrequency resolution FIG. 1. Maskingfunctionsfor a 3000-Hz toneas a functionof the oneoctavebandsound-pressure levelof thebandpass musket.The right panel depictsfunctionsfor thebandpass (solidlines)andnotched(dashedlines) maskers. The left panelshowsfunctionsfor thebandpass (solidlines)and modulated(dashedlines}maskers.Funclionsaregivenfor all tensubjects. 1070 ½0 50 ONE-OCTAVE BANDMASKERLEVEL(dB SPL) index(FRI, rightpanel)andthe temporalresolutionindex(TRI, left panel) at 500 Hz. The pair of solidlinesillustratesthe estimatedrangeof the maskingfunctionfor the bandpass maskerfor 95% of normalhearinglisteners,afternormalizationat a maskerlevelof 70 dB SPL. The pair of dashdot linesillustratesthe corresponding rangeof maskingfunctionsfor the notchedmasker(right) and modulatedmasker(left). R.M. Cox and G. C. Alexander:Frequencyand temporalresolution 1070 •, 90 •9øi 1250Hz 5000 Hz 3000 Hz TRI ca 70 o T u'J 60 LO -r 50 3:50 P- p- z 40 P- 30 o /- / i,i / I-- ca 20 /' N 'q z 4-0 p- .30 //// ; b.I ! I'-- ./ ca 20 / b.I N 10 • 10 ;:2 o 0 40 z 50 60 70 80 90 40 50 60 70 80 O Z 90 ONE-OCTAVE 8ANDMASKER LEVEL(dB SPL) 40 50 60 70 80 90 4-0 50 60 70 BO 90 ONE-OCTAVE BANDMASKERLEVEL(dR SPL) FIG. 3. Sameas Fig. 2, for I250 Hz. FIG. 5. Sameas Fig. 2, for 3000 Hz. would be expectedfor simultaneousmaskingwith coincident testand maskerfrequency(e.g., Stelmachowicz et aL, 1987). On the basisof the reports by Zwicker and Schorn (1982, 1990), we wouldpredictthat the slopesof the mean maskingfunctions for the modulated noisewouldapproximate 0.5. The obtained mean functions were close to this prediction, rangingfrom0.53to0.42 (thesefunctions would bisectthe TRI dash-dotlinesin Figs.2-5). However,there wasa systematic decrease in slopewith increasein testfrequency.The fourmeanslopes were0.53,0.50,0.43,and0.42 at testfrequencies 500, 1250,2000,and3000Hz, respectively. Studiesof gapdetectionin bandlimitednoisessimilarto thoseusedin this studyhaveshownthat normal-hearing listenerscan detectshortergapsat higher test frequencies (e.g.,Shailerand Moore, 1985). Basedon thisoutcome,it wasnotsurprising to observe apparentlybetterperformance TABLE i. Coefficients of second-order polynomials describingthe upper and lower boundsof normsshownin Figs. 2-5. For each polynomial, Y =/•o +/•x + .82x •, wherey = testtonethreshold, afternormalization (dB), and x = one-octaveband masker level {dB SPL). BP = bandpass masker,NCH = notchedmasker,MOD = modulatedmasker. Hz 500 Masker BP Bound Upper Lower 500 500 NCH MOD BP •, 0.0022 -- 0.0022 46.872 -- 0.8527 0.0127 16.159 -- 0.5066 0.0111 Upper Upper 26.472 0. i 149 -- 12.423 0.9542 8.9214 0.5092 -- 25.442 1.4697 0.003 -- 0.003 0.0035 -- 0.0035 Upper 58.624 -- 1.6244 0.0209 Lower 27.911 0.0102 Upper i 1.57 - i.2783 90 2000 Hz m 80 ul NCH 1.2973 Lower Lower 1250 0.6788 18.228 Upper Lower 1250 3.8995 -- , 1250 MOD Lower TRI 70 2000 BP Upper Lower 0.3667 5.8259 0.6408 1.4003 0.6972 -- 21.179 1.3283 0.0017 -- 0.0017 0.0023 - 0.12)023 •o 6o 2000 hi ac 'T 50 NCH Upper Lower 105.89 31.339 -- 3.4657 0.0368 -- 1.2504 0.0179 p- La z o 1- 40 2000 Upper Lower bd P- MOD 30 3000 20 BP Upper Lower 14.466 -- 3.2339 3.873 23.209 0.2691 0.5923 0.6026 1.3595 0.0019 -- 0.0019 0.0027 -- 0.0027 b.I N-- 10 3000 NCH Upper Lower O Z 4-0 50 60 70 80 90 40 ,50 60 70 60 90.425 i 19.88 -- 2.5124 0.0267 -- 4.0686 0.0383 90 ONE-OCTAVE BANDMASKER LEVEL(dB SPL) 3000 MOD Upper Lower 21.301 -- 5.4767 0.2453 0.6021 0.0013 -- 0.0013 FIG. 4. Sameas Fig. 2, for 2000 Hz. 1071 J. Acoust.Sec. Am., Vol. 93, No. 2, February 1993 R.M. Cox and G. C. Alexander:Frequencyand temporal resolution 1071 at higherfrequencies in thepresentstudy,sincethetemporal resolutiontaskrequireddetectionof a toneduringbriefgaps (36 ms) in the maskingnoises. Figure 6 depictsthe meanTRI functiondeterminedfor eachtestfrequency.Thesefunctionswereobtainedby subtractingthemeannormalizedmaskingfunctionfor themodulatedmaskerfrom that for the bandpassmasker.They describethe extent to which the typical listenerwas able to exploitthe brief intervalsin the maskingnoiseto improve detection of the test tone. Each TRI function rose monotoni- callywith maskerlevel,indicatingthat thismeasureof temporalresolutionproducedimprovedperformanceasmasker level increased. A similar outcome can be seen in the data 25 ,>5.,2o z z o •- 15 o n, 10 >= z b.l La 5 [] _2?00 HZ O 3000 Hz \' [3 generatedby Zwicker (1980) and Zwicker and Schorn I I I I I I I I I I 4-5 50 55 60 65 70 75 80 B5 gO ( 1982, 1990), usingsimilarprocedures.Studiesof temporal gap detectionthresholdshavealsotypicallyproducedimONE-OCTAVE BANDMASKER LEVEL(dR SPL) provedperformance with increasing level(e.g.,Fitzgibbons, FIG. 7. MeanFRI functionfor eachtestfrequency. Eachfunctionwasde1983;ShailerandMoore, 1983), althoughotherapproaches rivedbysubtracting themeannormalized masking functionforthenotched to the quantificationof temporalresolutionhavenot always maskerfrom that for the bandpassmasker. shownthis trend (e.g., Nelson and Freyman, 1987). The curvedmaskingfunctionsfor the notchednoise wereindicativeof a regionof maskerlevelswherefrequency resolution abilitieswereat a maximum:bothhigherandlower maskerlevelsproducedsmallerindices.Theseresultscan beseenin Figs.2-5 but are especially clearin Fig. 7 which depictsthe meanFRI functionfor eachtestfrequency. To producethesefunctions,the mean normalizedmasking function for the notched masker was subtracted from that for thebandpass maskerat eachtestfrequency.Thesemean functionsdescribethe extentto which the typicallistener wasable to benefitfrom the spectralnotchin the masking noiseto improvedetectionof thetesttone.As Fig. 7 shows, theinfluence ofmaskerlevelonFRI wasquitemarkedforall fourfrequencies. The maskerlevelproducing themaximum FRI wassomewhatdifferentacrosstestfrequencies, with octavebandlevelsrangingfrom 60 to 70 dB SPL. The magnitudeof themaximumFRI increased slightlywith testfre- 0 500 Hz 35 quency,rangingfrom 20 dB at 500 Hz to 26 dB at 3000Hz. Spreadof maskingstudiesreportedby ZwickerandJaroszewski (1982) and Lutfi and Patterson (1984) indicate that low levelmaskersproducedownwardspreadof masking effects,whereashigh level maskersproduceupward spreadof maskingeffects.This resultis consistentwith the changingshapeof auditoryfiltersas a functionof level (see Moore and Glasberg,1987;Glasbergand Moore, 1990,for reviews).Theseobservations providea basisforinterpreting the FRI data in the presentstudy.It seemslikely that the effectsof the notched-noise maskerwere dominatedby downward spreadof maskingfrom the high-frequency maskingbandat lowmaskerlevelsandby upwardspreadof maskingfrom the low-frequencymaskingband at high maskerlevels.At each testfrequency,therewas a narrow regionofmaskerlevelsfor whichneitherupwardnordownward spreadof maskingwas substantialand thesemasker levelsproducedthe maximumFRI values. Lutfi and Patterson(1984) reported that the masker spectrumlevelproducinga symmetricmaskingpattern(not dominatedby upwardor downwardspreadof masking)was about 35 dB at 1000 Hz whereas Zwicker and Jaroszewski z 25 o 10 (1982) estimateda levelof 40 dB SPL to producesymmetric effectsat this frequency.Glasbergand Moore (1990) note that the maskerlevelfor whichthe auditoryfilter is roughly symmetriccorresponds to approximately51 dB SPL/ERB. At the testfrequencies usedin this study,the corresponding spectrumlevelswould be approximately32, 29, 27, and 26 dB for 500, 1250, 2000, and 3000 Hz, respectively. In the presentstudy, the masker spectrumlevels yield- 0 I 40 I I 4,5 50 I I I I I 55 60 65 70 75 I I BO B5 I <:JO ONE-OCTAVE BANDMASKERLEVEL(dB SPL) FIG. 6. Mean TRI functionfor eachtestfrequency.Eachfunctionwasderivedbysublractingthemeannormalizedmaskingfunctionfor themodulated maskerfrom lhat for the bandpassmasker. 1072 J. Acoust.Sec. Am., Vol. 93, No. 2, February1993 ing maximumFRI data werehypothesised to correspondto regionsof symmetricmaskingeffects.Usingthedataof Fig. 7, the spectrumlevelswere found to be 44.5, 30.5, 28.5, and 31.7 dB at 500, 1250,2000, and 3000 Hz, respectively.Exceptfor the somewhathigh valueat 500 Hz (seeexperiment II), theseresultscorrespondwell to the levelscitedby other investigators asproducingsymmetricmasking,lendingsup- R M. Cox and (3. C. Alexander:Frequencyand temporalresolution 1072 portto ourinterpretation of theFRI datain termsof a progression fromdownward spreadof masking to symmetric masking andthento upwardspreadof masking effects as masker level is increased. Figure 7 revealsthat the mean FRI functionobtained for 500 Hz wasconsiderably lesscurvedthan thosefor the threeothertestfrequencies, suggesting lessupwardspreadof n 90 ...................... ,• •m80 --• TDH49 ER q 70 •- 50 masking at high notched-maskerlevels. Furthermore, the octavebandmaskerlevelproducingthe maximumFRI was higher (70 dB) at this frequencythan at the other testfrequencies.We postulatedthat this differencemight be the result of low-frequencyleakageunder the TDH-49 ear- • 4o phones. Previous studies haveshownthat acoustic leakage Q lo under the TelephonicsMX41-AR cushion/headbandassemblymaybecomesubstantial asfrequencies decrease be- o z N o 40 50 low about 400 Hz (Cox, ! 986). This would havethe effectof 60 70 80 90 40 50 60 70 80 90 ONE-OCTAVE BANDM•KER L•EL (dB SPL) reducingthelevelin thelow-frequency bandof thenotched maskerat 500 Hz by 5-10 dB. This would, in turn, decrease FIG. 8. Norms for maskin[ functionscomprisin[the frequencyresolution upwardspreadof maskingeffectsat highmaskerlevelsdue to thisband.Additionaldatawereobtainedto explorethis index(FRI, rightpanel)andthe(emporalresolution index(TRI, leftpanel) at 5• Hz. Dashedlinesdepictresultsfor TDH49 earphoneand solid linesshowcorresponding datafor ER-3A earphone. Eachpairof linesillus- hypothesis. II. EXPERIMENT tratesthe estimatedran[e of the maskingfunctionfor 95% of normalhearin[ listeners,after normalizationat a one-octavemaskerlevelor70 dB SPE. I1: NORMS FOR INSERT EARPHONE To evaluatethe likely effectsof low-frequencyleakage underthe Telephonicsearphonecushion,additionalnorms (both FRI and TRI) were generatedfor the 500-Hz test frequencyusingan insertearphone(EtymoticER-3A) cou- pledto the earcanalwith a compressible foamearplug.Becausethe foamearplugmakesa tight sealwith theearcanal, low-frequency acousticleakagewasessentially eliminated. A. Method I. Subjects Ten normal-hearing subjects participated,eightof them alsoservedin experimentI to generatenormsat 500 Hz for thesupra-auralearphone.The twonewsubjects weresimilar greaterfor the insertearphoneand the width of the normal rangeof resultswas reducedat highermaskerlevels.This resultisin agreement withourhypothesis thatleakageunder the supra-auralearphonecushionreducedthe level of the low-frequency bandof thenotchedmasker,resultingin reducedupwardspreadof maskingat highermaskerlevels. The outcomesuggests that presentation of stimuliusingan insertearphonereducedlow-frequency soundleakageand also generatedmore consistentearcanallevelsacrosssubjects,thusdecreasing variability. Thedifferentoutcomeforthetwoearphones isseenvery clearlyin Fig. 9, whichillustratesthe meanFRI functionfor to thosein theoriginalgroup. 2. Procedure Instrumentation,measurementprocedures,and methodsfor generatingmaskingfunctionsand index functions were identicalto thoseusedin experimentI. Levels were 30 ,oiFRii1 TDiH4 ..... 25 calibrated in a standard HA-2 2-cm3couplerusingtheprocedurerecommended by the earphonemanufacturer. B. Results 20 and discussion Figure8 depictsthe FRI andTRI normsobtainedusing the insertearphone(solid lines) and comparesthem with thecorresponding normsobtainedwith the supra-auralearphonereproducedfrom Fig. 2 (dashedlines). Examination of thisfigurerevealsthat thenormsfor thebandpass masker forbothearphones (thetwoupperpairsoflinesinbothpanels) were almost identical. In contrast, the norms for the notched masker (lower pairsoflines,leftpanel)hada substantially different pattern fortheinsertearphone (solidlines)andthesupra-aural earphone(dashedlines): The maskingfunctionslopewas 1073 J.Acoust. SOC. Am.,Vol.93,No.2, February 1993 10 I I I I I I 40 45 50 55 60 65 I I I I I 70 75 80 85 90 ONE-OCTAVE BANDk{•KER •EL (dB SPL) FIG. 9. MeanFRI andTRI functions at 500Hz obtained usingTDH49 and ER-3A earphones. R.M.CoxandG.C.Alexander: Frequency andtemporal resolution 1073 each earphone.The function for the insert (ER-3A) ear- phoneis similarto that for the supra-aural(TDH49) earphoneat low maskerlevelsbut divergessystematically as masker level increases. The octave band masker level that producedthe maximum meanFRI wasreducedfrom 70 dB for the supra-auralearphoneto 60 dB for the insert earphone.This valueis moreconsistent with the supra-aural earphoneresultsfor higherfrequencies whereacousticleakagewas not expectedto occur (seeFig. 7). In addition, the 45 50 55 60 6,5 70 75 80 8.5 90 corresponding spectrumlevelof 34.5 dB is in closeraccord ONE-OCTAVE BANDblASKER LEVF_J_ (dB SPL) with levelsdetermined by previousinvestigators to produce symmetrical maskingandto correspond to a symmetrically FIG. 10. Mean lest-retestdifferencesfor frequencyresolutionindices (FRI) and letuporalresolutionindices(TRI) asa functionof maskerlevel. shapedauditory filter. Error barsgiveone standarddeviation. The right panelof Fig. 8 compares the TR! normsobtainedwith thesupra-auralandinsertearphones. The results for the modultednoiseare similarbut at slightlydifferent levelsfor the two earphones: For a givennominalmasker the data suggested that therewereno systematic differences level, tone thresholds in the modulated masker were a few in testreliabilityat differenttestfrequencies. Thusdatawere decibelslower in the insertearphonecondition.This outcombinedacrossfrequenciesfor analysis.Meansand unbicomemay be attributableto the slightlyhigherlevelproasedstandarddeviationswere computedfor the difference ducedin the earcanalby the insertearphone.The resultwas dataat 5-dBmaskerincrements. The resultsaregivenin Fig. a slightlylargermeanTRI for the insertearphoneat all 10. For each index, the mean differenceat each maskerlevel nominalmaskerlevels,asillustratedin Fig. 9. Comparison wasbetween0.0 and 0.6 dB, suggesting that there was no of thesedatawith thosein Fig. 6 revealsthat the inserteartrend for a systematicdifferencebetweenthe two tests.In phonedataat 500Hz arein goodagreement withthesupra- addition,only oneof the standarddeviations(FRI: 90 dB) aural earphonedataat higherfrequencies. waslargerthan 2.5 dB and mostwereconsiderably smaller than this. This outcome indicates that both indices demonIII. EXPERIMENT II1: RELIABILITY stratedgoodrepeatabilitywithinthemaskerlevelrangetest- OF INDIVIDUAL ed. DATA A. Method IV. GENERAL I. Subjects Overall, thesesimplifiedmeasuresof frequencyand temporal resolutionproducedresultsfor normal hearers that wereconsistentwith data reportedin severalprevious investigations that usedsimilar or relatedproceduresand stimuli.At eachtestfrequency,the rangeof resultsfor normal-hearinglistenerswasfairly wide, but the individualindiceswerequitereliable.Further researchis neededto determinethe extentto whichperformanceof hearing-impaired listenerstypicallyfallsoutsideof thesebounds. It shouldbe kept in mind that the norms presentedin Table I applyto measurement of FRI andTRI obtainedwith TelephonicsTDH49 earphones,MX41-AR supra-aural cushions,and springheadband.This transducerassembly waschosenbecauseit is in widespreadusein both research and clinical endeavors.However, the resultsof experiment II suggestthat FRI and TRI data obtainedat 500 Hz with this earphoneare stronglyinfluencedby leakageunder the Nine normalhearingsubjects servedin thispart of the study.Six of themalsoservedin the groupusedin experiment I. The three new subjectsweresimilar to thosein the originalgroup. 2. Procedure Reliabilityof the indiceswasevaluatedby comparing two independentmeasurementsof both FRI and TRI functions. A total of 16 test-retest measurements of both indices were made.Five subjectsprovideddata at two frequencies. Oneprovideddataat threefrequencies. Testfrequencies for the 16 measurements weredistributedas follows:eight at 3000 Hz, six at 500 Hz, and two at 2000 Hz. Instrumenta- tion, measurement procedures, andmethodsfor generating masking functions and index functions were identical to those usedin experimentI. TDH49 earphoneswere used. For 15 measurements, the two sets of data were obtained consecutivelywithout removing the earphones.For one measurement, the two sets of data were collected several daysapart. B. Results and discussion Two FRI functionsweregeneratedfor eachdatasetand differencesbetweenthe two functionswere determinedby subtractingthesecondfunctionfromthefirst.The sameprocedureswere followedfor the TRI functions.Inspectionof 1074 J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 93, No. 2, February 1993 DISCUSSION cushion-headbandassembly.Although this doesnot invalidate the norms at 500 Hz, thesedata might be of limited usefulness.For measurementsat 500 Hz, it would be reason- ableto usetheER-3A earphoneinstead.However,thebandwidth andoutputlimitationsof thisearphonemakeit unsuitablefor testsat high frequenciesor high levels. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This work was supportedby the Department of VeteransAffairs RehabilitationResearchand DevelopmentSer- R.M. Cox and G. C. Alexander: Frequency and temporal resolution 1074 vice. Izel Taylor and April Powell assistedwith pilot data collection.Softwarewas written by Robert Joyceand Lawrence Brainerd. Patterson,R. D., Nimmo-Smith,1., Weber,D. L., and Milroy, R. (1982). Cox,R. M. (1986). "NBS-9Acouplertoeardrumtransformation: TDH-39 andTDH-49 earphones," J. Acoust.Sec.Am. 79, 120-123. Cox, R. M., andAlexander,O. C. (1991). "Hearingaid benefitin everyday withoutcompression andtheroleof temporal resolution," Audiology 28, 49-60. Dubno,J. R., andSchaefer, A. B. (1992). "Comparisonof frequencyselectivity and consonant recognition amonghearing-impaired and masked normal-hearinglisteners,"J. Acoust.Sec. Am. 91, 2110-2121. Festen,J. M., and Plomp,R. (1983). "Relationsbetweenauditoryfunctionsin impairedhearing,"J. Acoust.Sec.Am. 72, 740-752. Fitzgibbons, P.J. (1983). "Temporalgapdetection in noiseasa function of frequency, bandwidthandlevel,"J. Acoust.Sec.Am. 74. 67-72. Olasberg, B. R., andMoore,B.C. { 1989)."Psycheacoustic abilities ofsubjectswithunilateral andbilateralcochlear hearingimpairments andtheir relationship to theabilityto understand speech,"Sound.Audiel.Suppl. 32, 1-25. Glasberg,B. R., and Moore, B.C. (1990). "Derivationof auditoryfilter shapes fromnolched~noise data,"Hear.Res.47, 103-138. Glasberg, B.R., Moore,B.C., andBacon,S.P. (1987). "Gapdetection and maskingin hearing-impaired and normal-hearing subjects," J. Acoust. 1788-1803. Shailer,M. J., and Moore,B.C. J. (1983). "Gap detectionasa funclionof Shailer,M. J., and Moore. B.C. J. (1985). "Detectionof temporalgapsin bandlimitednoise:Effectsof variationsin bandwidthandsignal-to-masker ratio," J. Acousl. Sec. Am. 77, 635-639. Stelmachowicz, P. G., Lewis,D. E., Larson,L. L., andJesteadt, W. (1987). "Growthof masking asa measure of response growthin hearing-impairedlisteners," J. Acoust.Sec.Am. 81, 1881-1887. tcr Keurs,M., Fcsten,J. M., and Pierap,R. (1992). "Effectof spectral envelope smearing onspeech reception. I," J.Acoust. Sec.Am.91,28722880. Tyler,R. S. (1986)."Frequency resolution in hearing-impaired listeners," in Frequency Selectivity in Hearing,editedbyB.C. $. Moore(Academic, London), pp. 309-372. Tyler,R. S.,Summerfield, Q., Wood,E. J.,andFernandes, M. A. (1982). "Psychoacoustic andphonetic temporal processing in normalandhearing-impaired listeners," J. Acoust.Sec.Am.72,753-760. Thibodeau,L. M., and Van Tasell,D. J. (1987). "Tonedetectionandsyn- theticspeech discrimination inband-reject noise byhearing-impaired listener-s,"J. Acoust. Sec. Am. 82, 864-873. Weber,D. L. (1977). "Growth of maskingand the auditoryfilter," J. Sec. Am. 81, 1546-1556. Gutnick, H. N. (1982). "Consonantfeaturetransmission as a functionof presentation levelin hearing-impaired listeners," J. Acoust.Sec.Am. 72. 1124-1130. Lutfi, R. A., and Patterson,R. D. (1984). "On the growth of masking asymmetrywith stimulusintensity,"J. Acoust.Sec.Am. 76, 739-745. Lutman,M. E., andClark,J. (1986). "Speechidentification undersimulat- ed hearingaid frequency response characteristics in relationto sensitivity, frequency resolution andtemporalresolution," J. Acoust.Sec.Am. Acoust. Sec. Am. 62, 424-429. Yantis,P.A., Millin,J.P., andShapiro,1. (1966). "Speech discrimination in sensorineural hearingloss:Twoexperiments ontheroleof intensity," J. SpeechHear. Res.9, 178-193. Zwicker,E. (1980). "A deviceformeasuring thetemporalresolution of the ear," Audiolog.Acoust.19, 94-108. Zwicker, E., and Jaroszewski,A. (1982). "Inversefrequencydependence of simultaneous tone-on-tonemaskingpatternsat low levels,"J. Acoust. Sec. Am. 71, 1508-1512. 80, 1030-1040. Moore,B.C. J.,andGlasberg. B. R. (1987). "Formulaedescribing frequencyselectivity asa functionof frequency andlevel,andtheirusein calculatingexcitationpatterns,"Hear. Res.28, 209-225. Moore,B.C. l., andGlasberg,B. R. (1988). "Gap detectionwith sinusolds and noisein normal, impaired,and electricallystimulatedears," J. d. Acoust.Sec. Am., Vol. 93, No. 2, February1993 "Thedeterioration ofhearingwithage:Frequency selectivity, thecritical ratio,the audiogram, andspeechthreshold," J. Acoust.Sec.Am. 72, frequency, bandwidth andlevel,"J.Acoust.Sec.Am. 74.467-473. environments," Ear Hear. 12, 127-139. Dresehler,W. (1989). "Phonemeperceptionvia hearingaids with and 1075 Acousl. Sec. Am. 83, 1093-1101. Nelson,D. A., andFreyman,R. L. (1987). "Temporalresolution insensorineuraihearingimpairedlisteners," J. Acousl.Sec.Am. 81, 709-720. Zwicker,E., andSchorn,K. (1982). "Temporalresolution in hard-of-hearingpatients," Audiology21,474-492. Zwicker,E., andSchorn,K. (1990). "Frequency selectivity andtemporal resolution in patients withvarious innereardisorders," Audiology 29, 8 20. R.M. Cox and G. C. Alexander:Frequencyand temporalresolution 1075