32 Cognitive and Psychological Assessment and Support 21st International Symposium on ALS/MND SESSION 8B COGNITIVE AND PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT AND SUPPORT C46 SOCIAL COGNITION IN ALS: IMPAIRED COGNITIVE AND AFFECTIVE THEORY OF MIND VAN DER HULST E-J, BAK TH, ABRAHAMS S University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom C47 THE NATURE OF LANGUAGE DEFICITS IN ALS: SEMANTIC IMPAIRMENTS IN ACTION SEQUENCING VAN DER HULST E-J, ABRAHAMS S, BAK TH E-mail address for correspondence: s.abrahams@ed.ac.uk University of Edinburgh, Human Cognitive Neuroscience, Edinburgh, United Kingdom Keywords: social dysfunction E-mail address for correspondence: dina.jozefa@ed.ac.uk cognition, cognitive change, prefrontal Background: A key component of Social Cognition is Theory of Mind (ToM) conceptualized as the ability to ascribe mental states to others. The Eye Gaze Test (EGT) is a classical ToM test and assesses an individual’s ability to judge the preference of another by using the direction of their eye gaze as a cue. This process is fundamental for appropriate social interaction. Deficits on this test have been found in both ALS and FTD. ToM has been fractionated into cognitive and affective components, involving the recognition of the thoughts and feelings of another respectively. Disruption of affective ToM has been related to damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, a region implicated early in FTD but which has not been as yet identified as typically affected in ALS. Objectives: This study examined whether ALS patients display changes in both cognitive and affective ToM. Furthermore, the influence of attentional factors were explored. Methods: Sixteen patients with ALS (non-demented) and 16 healthy participants, matched for age, sex and years of education, completed the cognitive and affective EGT. The EGT comprised of three features: type of judgement (cognitive, affective or physical control), attention (presence or absence of a distracter) and complexity of judgement (first or second order ToM). In addition, each participant was administered two tests of visual processing. Results: The analyses revealed that ALS patients were significantly impaired in both cognitive and affective ToM trials. ALS patients displayed significantly lower scores for first order cognitive (P 0.01) and affective judgements (P 0.005) as well as second order cognitive judgements (P 0.05) with a trend for second order affective judgements (P0.067). The patients were not impaired on physical control judgements or visual processing. Moreover there was no effect of having a distracter present. Discussion and conclusions: Cognitive and affective components of ToM appear to be affected in some non-demented ALS patients who show difficulties in recognizing the thoughts and feelings of another. This deficit indicates involvement of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and suggests that the prefrontal involvement in ALS may be extensive in those who do not show overt dementia. This selective deficit was found in patients with intact visual processing abilities. Moreover there was no effect of distracter present indicating that this was not the result of an attentional dysfunction. Hence this deficit reflects a basic social cognition impairment. These results could account for some of the behavioural abnormalities observed in ALS and implies that some ALS patients may have difficulties in primary interactions with carers. Keywords: neuropsychology, language, embodied cognition Background: A subset of patients with ALS show subtle difficulties in a range of cognitive functions, including language. In particular, recent research has uncovered deficits in action processing in ALS-patients both with and without dementia, although the exact nature of these deficits remains to be elucidated. Findings from these studies have been interpreted as support in favour of the influential theory of embodied cognition, which states that both motor functions and knowledge of actions are dependent on the same underlying neural networks. Objectives: This study investigated the nature of linguistic and conceptual deficits in patients with classical ALS. In particular, it explored the distinction between two types of relationship between actions: parallel (actions of a similar character, eg typing - writing) and sequential (actions, which usually follow each other, eg peeling - cutting). Methods: Twenty-one ALS-patients and 17 healthy controls (HCs), matched for age, education and sex, were compared on a neuropsychological battery of tests. The patients had to choose the correct answer from an array of two different choices. The battery comprised of language tests: semantic association for Objects and Actions (O and A) and Action Sequencing (AS). Furthermore, a relatively difficult test of Audio Visual Information Processing (AVIP) was added as a measure of complex non-semantic information processing. Both reaction times (RTs) and error rates were recorded. Results: Analyses revealed that ALS-patients made significantly more errors than HCs on AS (P 0.021), while both groups displayed an equal amount of errors on O and A (Ps0.067). In terms of the reaction times, taking out the possible influence of motor slowing, basic motor speeds were subtracted in all three conditions, yielding cognitive decision times. Results showed a main effect of group (P 0.016). Although ALS-patients exhibited longer cognitive decision times than HCs overall, patients were not slowed on any of the individual tests in particular (P 0.158). There were no differences in reaction times and errors between patients and controls on AVIP (Ps0.233). Discussions and conclusions: Our study detected a specific deficit in the task requiring the comprehension of action sequencing in a group of non-demented ALS-patients. In contrast, no deficits were observed on action and object association tests as well as in audiovisual information processing. The deficits in action sequencing were confined to the accuracy of responses only and did not appear to influence cognitive decision times. Our findings suggest that action deficits observed in ALS patients might in fact be due to a sequencing deficit. Moreover, they raise the question whether sequencing deficits Platform Communications are specific to language or possibly extend to other neuropsychological domains, constituting one of the basic cognitive deficits in ALS. Cognitive and Psychological Assessment and Support 33 C49 DIAGNOSTIC VALIDITY OF THE ALS COGNITIVE BEHAVIORAL SCREEN RUSH B1, WOOLLEY SC2, BOYLAN K1 1Mayo Clinic, Jacksonville, FL, United States, 2California Pacific Medical Center, San Francisco, CA, United States C48 PREFRONTAL LOBAR DETECTING: A MORE SENSITIVE BATTERY FOR SCREENING COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT IN EARLY-PHASE ALS? E-mail address for correspondence: rush.beth@mayo.edu JI Y1, WEI L2, DENG M1, WANG K2, FAN D1 Keywords: cognitive screen, cognitive impairment, diagnostic validity 1Peking University Third Hospital, Beijing, China, 2 Anhui Medical University First Hospital, Hefei, China E-mail address for correspondence: dsfan@yahoo.cn Keywords: prefrontal lobar dysfunction, prospective memory, early phase Background: ALS is now considered as a multisystem disease with co-occurrence of frontotemporal syndrome. Imaging studies show the prefrontal lobe is the most involved area in ALS patients with behavioral and cognitive impairment. Objective: The aim of this study is to find a screening battery to detect subtle cognitive deficits in ALS patients at early stage. Methods: Eighty consecutive patients diagnosed as ALS according to the El Escorial criteria and 59 normal controls were matched for sex, age and education. Both groups were assessed with a series of neuropsychological tests state as follows: the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI), Frontal Behavior Inventory (FBI), verbal fluency test (VFT), Stroop Color Word Interference Test (CWT), and prospective memory (PM) including event-based prospective memory (EBPM) and time-based prospective memory (TBPM). To further explore the topic a subset of patients underwent further examination involving picture and music emotional perception, theory of mind or decision making (Iowa Gambling Task and risk-taking task). Results: As previous studies reported, our patients also did not differ from normal controls on MMSE, but significantly differed on behavioral inventories including NPI (4.604.78 vs 0.160.59, P0.001) and FBI (3.193.59 vs 0.180.54, P0.001). However, we found our patients did not differ from normal controls in traditional neuropsychological tests on executive function including VFT and CWT. On the other hand, statistically significant differences were found between ALS patients and the normal controls on the prospective memory (EBPM: 5.552.20 vs 6.371.80, P0.043; TBPM: 4.851.51 vs 5.651.10, P0.001), faux pas task (23.1310.09 vs 28.916.20, P0.002) and the arousal of negative pictures (6.381.32 vs 7.181.12, P0.008). More interestingly, we found TBPM is more sensitive than EBPM in early phase patients (possible and probable-laboratory supported ALS). We did not find differences between ALS patients and normal controls on the valence and recognition of pictures, music emotional perception, Iowa Gambling Task and risk-taking task. Conclusions: Prefrontal lobar dysfunction does exist among ALS patients, and may be spread from medial to lateral part. Behavioral test, PM and theory of mind used in this study is more sensitive in detecting behavioral and cognitive impairment in ALS patients at early stage than the classical cognitive measures. Background: Cognitive and behavioral symptoms of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) are increasingly recognized. The ALS Cognitive Behavioral Screen (ALS-CBS) (1–2) is a 20 point screening measure with unclear diagnostic validity. Objectives: To compare diagnostic classifications of cognitive impairment from the ALS-CBS to those from gold standard neuropsychological evaluation. Methods: Retrospective review of 24 cases of ALS referred for evaluation of cognitive and behavioral symptoms between January 1, 2008 and March 31, 2010. Each participant was administered the ALS-CBS and a standard of care neuropsychological evaluation and received two diagnoses; one from the ALS-CBS and one from neuropsychological evaluation. The following diagnoses were considered: ALS, ALS with cognitive impairment/no dementia (ALS-ci), ALS with behavioral impairment/no dementia (ALS-bi), or ALS with frontal-temporal dementia (ALS-FTD). A score of 11 on the cognitive form of the ALS-CBS suggested cognitive impairment and a score 5 suggested ALS-FTD. Diagnostic classifications from the ALS-CBS and the neuropsychological evaluation were compared and diagnostic validity was determined. Results: Ten out of the 24 cases were female (42%), with a median age of 68 years (range: 43 - 83 years), median education of 14 years (range: 9 - 20 years), and median of 18 months (range: 6 - 55 months) duration between motor neuron disease symptom onset to cognitive evaluation. Fifty percent of cases had bulbar-onset disease. Neuropsychological evaluation confirmed cognitive impairment in 16 out of 24 cases (67% base rate; 9 ALS-FTD and 7 ALS-ci). There were 50% bulbar cases in the cognitively impaired and unimpaired groups. Using a cut-off score of 11 on the ALS-CBS, 46% cases (11/24) had cognitive impairment (8 ALS-FTD, 3 ALSci). This cut-off score yielded a sensitivity of 69% and a specificity of 100%. Of the 5 cases for which the ALS-CBS yielded a false negative diagnosis of cognitive impairment, 4 of the cases had a diagnosis of ALS-ci and 1 case had a diagnosis of ALS-FTD on neuropsychological evaluation. Conclusion: The ALS-CBS demonstrates excellent specificity using a cut-off score of 11 for determining cognitive impairment. The ALS-CBS may have limited sensitivity to ALS-ci or cognitive impairment without dementia. Discussion: Data support the validity of the ALS-CBS as a screen for cognitive impairment in ALS. The cut-off score of 11 on the cognitive form of the ALS-CBS is empirically justified particularly when ALS-FTD is present. Alternate cut-off scores or language screening items could improve the sensitivity of the ALS-CBS to ALS-ci when it is present. References: 1. Woolley SC. Third International Research Workshop on Frontotemporal Dementia in ALS. London, Ontario, Canada, 2009. 2. Woolley SC, et al. Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis. In Press. 34 Cognitive and Psychological Assessment and Support C50 LONGITUDINAL COGNITIVE SCREENING USING THE ALS COGNITIVE BEHAVIORAL SCREEN (ALS-CBS™) WOOLLEY SC1, MOORE DH1, VALAN M1, SPITALNY M2, KUSHNER G1, FORSHEW DA1, MILLER R1, KATZ J1 21st International Symposium on ALS/MND C51 COMPUTERIZED FRONTAL ASSESSMENT OF AMYOTROPHIC LATERAL SCLEROSIS POLETTI B, LAFRONZA A, SOLCA F, TICOZZI N, MORELLI C, MESSINA S, SILANI V 1California Pacific Medical Center, San Francisco, CA, United States, 2Research Computing, Mill Valley, CA, United States Department of Neurology and Laboratory of Neuroscience, ‘Dino Ferrari’ Center, University of Milan, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Milan, Italy E-mail address for correspondence: Woolles@sutterhealth.org E-mail address for correspondence: poletti.barbara@gmail.com Keywords: cognition, screening, longitudinal Keywords: frontal functions, computerized tools, neuropsychological assessment Background: The ALS Cognitive Behavioral Screen (ALSCBSTM) has been validated as a clinical measure but its utility in research is unknown. Its ease of administration is ideal for longitudinal assessment of cognition in large cohorts. Objectives: To evaluate cognitive screening data obtained from the WALS multi-center trial of lithium carbonate and to characterize cognitive status of this research cohort over time. Methods: 109 patients with ALS were enrolled. The maximum cognitive score on the ALS-CBSTM is 20; scores at or below 10 are suggestive of frontotemporal dementia (1). The mean score for a clinical cohort (n112) was 14.6 (1). Longitudinal cognitive change was calculated using the difference in scores over 12 months. Age of onset, duration of symptoms, forced vital capacity (FVC), ALSFRS-R scores, and fatigue were measured in the trial. Results: The mean age of subjects was 56 years, mean ALSFRS-R at study initiation was 37.5, and mean FVC was 94.8%. Ninety-eight subjects completed the ALS-CBSTM longitudinally. The average cognitive score was 16.68 (2.67) at baseline and the cohort remained stable over 1 year (16.69). Only 7% of subjects revealed significant cognitive declines (5 points) but 4% revealed significant increases. Declines in cognition were not associated with age, symptom duration, low FVC, low ALSFRS-R scores, fatigue, or drug side effects. Discussion: The subjects in this clinical trial did not show significant decline over a 12-month period. This suggests that frontal lobe decline is not prominent among patients who meet inclusion criteria for a clinical trial. The mean cognitive score in the trial was higher than in a clinical cohort suggesting that patients enrolling in trials are less likely to be impaired than clinical patients. These results also differ from previous longitudinal research in a clinical cohort which revealed longitudinal cognitive decline (2). Conclusion: Clinical trials may enroll patients with different cognitive profiles than those seen in routine clinic. This may relate to research patients needing to be motivated, having limited duration of disease due to inclusion criteria, or having higher FVC’s which limits those enrolled with significant bulbar involvement. While the methodology does not allow us to generalize our conclusions, the lack of progression in this cohort also points towards a relative paucity of cognitive impairment in this group. The results add to increasing evidence that a screening tool is a practical way to monitor cognition and behavior longitudinally in large cohorts. References: 1. Woolley SC, York MK, Moore DH, et al. ALS 2010; in press. 2. Phukan J, Gallagher L, Jordan N, et al. ALS 2009;10(1):43. Background: The study of cognition and behaviour as a feature of Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis (ALS) is an evolving field still lacking a full consensus on terminology, diagnostic criteria, and clinical significance of any detected abnormalities. Marked discrepancies remain regarding the incidence of abnormalities and characteristics of the impairments that define ALS, frequently depending on the kind of cognitive tool used. Frontal alterations in ALS have been variously described and assessed in literature. Moreover, different degrees of frontal involvement in ALS starting from minimal frontal changes to overt Fronto-Temporal Dementia (FTD) have been documented, thus revealing the importance of detecting the whole spectrum of frontal involvement characterizing motor neurone disease’s cognitive pattern. The terms ALSci (ALS with cognitive impairment), ALSbi (ALS with behavioural impairment), and ALS-FTD are developing concepts that aim to capture the key differences between the various clinical phenotypes. Computerized neuropsychological tools seem to be ideal in exploring and characterizing frontal cognitive functions. Objectives: The purpose of this study was to analyze frontal cognitive functioning of ALS patients with frontal cognitive computerized measures. Methods: Fifteen patients fullfilling El Escorial Criteria for ALS and fifteen controls underwent an extensive neuropsychological and psychodiagnostic assessment. Patients received the Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis Functional Rating Scale (ALSFRSr). Attentional skills and frontal functioning have been investigated with a computerized neuropsychological battery named TEA (Test of Everyday Attention). This battery, focused on attention and frontal functions, assess the ability of patients to selectively attend, sustain their attention, divide their attention between two tasks, switch attention from one task to another, and withhold (inhibit) verbal or motor responses. Clinical tools for assessing psychological and emotional status included: MOS 36-Item Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36), Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) and State-Trait Anxiety Inventory-Y (STAI-Y). Results: Our data show quantitative differences in cognitive performances between patients and controls, with higher difficulties in the more complex attentional tasks for the former and better scores for the latter. Significant differences of data emerge on different frontal measures, while a general cognitive slowness characterized patient’s performances. Patients displayed lower performances on frontal task of cognitive functioning. Subjects differed significantly for the presence of anxiety symptoms. Neuropsychological and psychological data were correlated with functional and respiratory parameters. Discussion and conclusions: Computerized neuropsychological assessment seems an ideal tool in detecting small ‘frontal’ cognitive changes frequently observed in the cognitive frontal spectrum of ALS. Implications for clinical purposes will be discussed.