Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2010 30: 611–617 Material and lighting hues of object colour Rumi Tokunaga and Alexander D. Logvinenko Department of Vision Sciences, Glasgow Caledonian University, City Campus, Cowcaddens Road, Glasgow, G4 0BA, UK Abstract Observers can easily differentiate between a pigmented stain and the white surface that it lies on. The same applies for a colour shadow cast upon the same surface. Although the difference between these two kinds of colour appearance (referred to as material and lighting hues) is self-evident even for inexperienced observers, it is not one that has been captured by any colour appearance model thus far. We report here on an experiment supplying evidence for the dissociation of these two types of hue in the perceptual space. The stimulus display consisted of two identical sets of Munsell papers illuminated independently by yellow, neutral, and blue lights. Dissimilarities between all the paper/ light pairs were ranked by five trichromatic observers, and then analysed by using non-metric multidimensional scaling (MDS). In the MDS output configuration, the Munsell papers lit by the same light made a closed configuration retaining the same order as in the Munsell book. The paper configurations for the yellow and blue lights were displaced transversally and in parallel to each other, with that of the neutral light located in between. The direction of the shift is interpreted as the yellowblue lighting dimension. We show that the yellow-blue lighting dimension cannot be reduced to that of the reflected light. Keywords: chromatic illumination, dissimilarity judgements, human colour vision, multidimensional scaling, Munsell papers, object colour Introduction The colour of an evenly illuminated object can be described in terms of three dimensions. While the terminology varies, these are usually named as hue, chroma, and lightness (Evans, 1974; Wyszecki and Stiles, 1982; Brainard, 2003; Foster, 2008). However, three dimensions are enough to describe the object colours only when illumination is constant and homogeneous. We refer to these three dimensions as the material dimensions of object colour (Tokunaga and Logvinenko, 2010). When illumination varies across the scene, one needs more dimensions to describe the object colour appearance (Hunt, 1977; Fairchild, 2005). We refer to the dimensions of object colour which mainly Received: 5 September 2009 Revised form: 15 January 2010 Accepted: 24 January 2010 Correspondence and reprint requests to: Rumi Tokunaga. Tel. & Fax: +81 (0)22 217 5469. E-mail address: rumi.tokunaga@riec.tohoku.ac.jp correlate with illumination variation as lighting dimensions of object colour (Tokunaga and Logvinenko, 2010). The existence of lighting dimensions is supported by the fact that observers can readily distinguish a change in a scene produced by a material change from that made by an illumination change (Craven and Foster, 1992; Foster et al., 2001; Kingdom, 2008). Actually, it is a matter of everyday experience that we are capable of distinguishing coloured shadows from pigmented areas (e.g., stains). The question is whether this ability is due to cognitive inferring, or is mediated by additional dimensions of the object colour manifold. Some evidence obtained when studying the colour transformations induced by pseudoscopic depth reversal, testifies in favour of the latter (Logvinenko, 2009a). Logvinenko and Maloney (2006) showed that for achromatic objects an achromatic lighting dimension emerged when the light intensity varied. Specifically, as anticipated by Katz (1935) nearly a century ago, a single lightness dimension was proven not to be enough to describe the colour appearance of achromatic papers illuminated by an achromatic light source of a different intensity. Using multidimensional scaling, achromatic ª 2010 The Authors, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists doi: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2010.00733.x 612 Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2010 30: No. 5 colours were found to constitute a two-dimensional manifold with the dimensions Ôsurface-lightnessÕ and Ôsurface-brightnessÕ: relating to difference in surface albedo and to difference in illumination intensity respectively. Surface-brightness was shown to be independent from brightness of reflected light (Logvinenko, 2005; Logvinenko and Maloney, 2006). The existence of the surface-brightness dimension was confirmed in the chromatic domain as well (Tokunaga et al., 2008). Moreover, it was recently shown that an additional chromatic lighting dimension emerged when the illumination chromaticity varied (Tokunaga and Logvinenko, 2010). In fact, this study was an extension of the work of Logvinenko and Maloney (2006) to the chromatic domain. The major difference between the two studies was that Tokunaga and Logvinenko (2010) varied both papers and lights in the yellow-blue chromatic dimension, whereas Logvinenko and Maloney (2006) used the achromatic dimension. The results obtained by Tokunaga and Logvinenko (2010) were similar to those obtained by Logvinenko and Maloney (2006). Specifically, the Munsell papers lit by three different lights were represented as three slightly curved contours approximately parallel to each other in the dissimilarity space. Figure 1. Experimental set-up and a stimulus display. A digital projector (DP) provided independent illumination of the six fields of the stimulus display (SD). Seven Munsell papers were presented against a white background covered by a black random-dot design, inside fields #1 and #2. The fields #3 and #4 were used to present a pair of Munsell papers of standard dissimilarity (100). The fields #5 and #6 were illuminated in such a way that there were always two fields illuminated by each of three lights. A computer (PC) randomly flashed on a pair of light emitting diodes (one in the field #1, and one in the field #2) indicating the pair of Munsell papers to be evaluated. An observer entered the dissimilarity rank for the pair pressing a button on the response box (RB). It must be noted that in both previous studies the chromaticity of the papers was equal to that of the lights. Also in each study only two hues were engaged: achromatic hues in Logvinenko and Maloney (2006), and yellow and blue hues in Tokunaga and Logvinenko (2010). Here we report on an experiment in which, along with yellow and blue, Munsell papers of various other hues were used. The rationale was to make sure that evidence for the existence of the chromatic lighting dimension of object colour could also be obtained whilst using more than two hues. Methods The experimental set-up was similar to that used in our previous experiments (Tokunaga et al., 2008; Tokunaga and Logvinenko, 2010) as shown in Figure 1. The stimulus display was divided into six equal rectangular fields covered by white paper with a random-dot pattern. Illumination of each field was adjusted independently by using a digital projector (MT1050; NEC Display Solutions Ltd, Tokyo, Japan). Two fields (#1 and #2) were used to present stimulus papers: Munsell papers of various hues and maximal (for each hue) chroma (5R4/14, 5YR7/12, 5Y8/12, 5G6/10, 10BG5/8, 5PB5/12 and 10P5/12). The angular size of each paper was approximately 4. Two identical sets of these seven papers were simultaneously displayed in each field. Figure 2. The stimuli presented in the CIE 1976 uniform chromaticity diagram. The symbol shape depicts the Munsell paper: circle = 5R4/14, triangle (up) = 5YR7/12, triangle (right pointing) = 5Y8/12, square = 5G6/10, star = 10BG5/8, triangle (down) = 5PB5/12, diamond = 10P5/12. The colour of the lines indicates the colour of illumination: yellow, neutral (black line), and blue. ª 2010 The Authors, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists Material and lighting hues: R. Tokunaga and A. D. Logvinenko Illumination of each field was equi-illuminant (60 lux), and its chromaticity independently varied from session to session at three levels: neutral, yellow, and blue. Their CIE 1931 x,y-chromaticity coordinates were (0.28, 0.34), (0.38, 0.44), and (0.16, 0.08). Figure 2 shows the CIE 1976 uÕvÕ-coordinates of the light reflected from all the Munsell papers under each of the three illuminations. The observerÕs task was to evaluate dissimilarity between a pair of papers (one from each of two fields). Red light-emitting diodes (3 mm in diameter) set up next to each of the papers indicated which pair was to be assessed. A yellow paper (5Y8/12) under the yellow illumination and a blue paper (5PB5/12) under the blue illumination were mounted in two other fields (#3 and #4) as a standard of dissimilarity (Figure 1). They were present throughout the experiment within the observerÕs field of view. The remaining two fields (#5 and #6) were void of Munsell papers, and were used to balance the overall illumination in the experimental display. They were illuminated so that there were always two displays illuminated by each of three lights (Figure 1). This measure was taken to reduce chromatic adaptation and to keep the global adaptation state of the observers as constant as possible throughout the experiment. Five normal trichromatic observers took part in the experiment. All of them except one (the co-author RT) were naı̈ve as to the purpose of the experiment. They sat 2 m away from the stimulus display, the size of which was 102 · 93 cm (29 · 26). Viewing was binocular. The experimental room was semi-darkened, so that there must have been cues which indicated that these were real papers lit by real lights. The experiment was divided into six sessions. In each session a fixed pair of lights was used. A session consisted of 49 trials in which all possible 7 · 7 pairs of papers were evaluated. In each trial a pair of papers (one in the field #1, and one in the field #2 in Figure 1) was indicated randomly by the light-emitting diodes. Observers were instructed to estimate the dissimilarity (a) 613 between the papers as compared to the standard pair (fields #3 and #4 in Figure 1) with a number, taking the standard dissimilarity as 100. Each session was repeated six times for each observer. Results The averaged (across subjects and repetitions) dissimilarities were analysed using a non-metric multidimensional scaling algorithm (Cox and Cox, 2001). Figure 3 presents a three dimensional MDS solution for the average dissimilarity matrix, that is, such a configuration in the 3D space that the distances between the points are, in general, in the same order as the dissimilarities between the corresponding stimulus pairs (paper/light). The stress for the output configuration in Figure 3, that is, an index showing the relative proportion of the mismatches between the distances and the dissimilarities (for a formal definition of stress see e.g., Cox and Cox, 2001, p. 64–68) was 0.03. In Figure 3a the yellow line connects the symbols denoting the seven Munsell papers illuminated by the yellow light. Likewise, the blue and black lines correspond to the blue and neutral illuminations. The colour of the symbol indicates the colour of the paper. As one can see, for each of the illuminations, the seven papers form a closed contour (referred to as a hue contour), and the order of papers within each contour is in keeping with the order of papers in the Munsell book. So it is with the orders of the circular configurations in the chromaticity diagram (Figure 2). There is, however, an essential difference between Figures 2 and 3. Whereas the three hue contours in Figure 2 are displaced within the same chromaticity plane (recall that the stimuli were illuminated by equiilluminant lights), the hue contours in Figure 3a are transversally shifted. Figure 4 shows the relationship between the dissimilarity judgements (Figure 3) and the corresponding distances in the CIE 1976 uniform chromaticity diagram. The top row indicates that the (b) Figure 3. Two different views of the output configuration produced by the non-metric MDS algorithm. Each point represents a Munsell paper illuminated by a particular light. Notations are the same as in Figure 2. (a) The markers corresponding to the papers lit by the same light are connected by the lines of the same colour. (b) The markers corresponding to the same paper are connected by lines. See Appendix. ª 2010 The Authors, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists 614 Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2010 30: No. 5 Figure 4. Dissimilarities vs chromaticity differences. In each plate the vertical axis is the dissimilarity between a pair of Munsell papers illuminated separately by one of the three lights: yellow (Y), neutral (N), or blue (B); the horizontal axis is the chromaticity difference between the reflected lights evaluated in terms of the CIE 1976 uniform chromaticity diagram. Dissimilarities (respectively, chromaticity differences) were normalised by the dissimilarity (respectively, the chromaticity difference) of the pair (5Y8/12; 5PB5/12) being used as the standard (see Figure 1), produced under the lighting conditions in question (i.e., B-N, Y-B, etc.) for each observer individually. The correlation between dissimilarity and chromaticity difference is significant (p < 0.05) in each panel, R denoting the correlation coefficient. The bottom row of plots display a selection from the middle row that consist of pairs of papers which were identical. Hence, they show how the dissimilarities, produced only by a difference in illumination, correlate with the corresponding chromaticity differences. distances along each contour in Figure 3 are more or less in line with the chromaticity differences between the corresponding colour stimuli. However, as we can see in the middle row in Figure 4, the distances between the various points of different hue contours in Figure 3a correlate rather poorly with the chromaticity differences between the corresponding colour stimuli. Consider, for example, the yellow paper 5Y8/12 under the blue light and the blue paper 5PB5/12 under the neutral light. They have practically the same chromaticity (Figure 2). Yet, they were judged at 60% of the maximal dissimilarity (which was 104% of the standard, and observed between papers 5PB5/12 and 5R4/14 under the blue and yellow lights respectively). This indicates that observers did not rest their judgments upon the chromaticity of the light reflected from these papers. In other words, the dissimilarity between these papers renders a different dimension. As argued elsewhere (Tokunaga and Logvinenko, 2010), this dimension is unlikely to be brightness. More specifically, although the yellow and blue lights might have differed from the neutral light in brightness, it is highly unlikely that the hue contours are displaced with respect to each other because of the possible brightness difference. Indeed, if this were the case then in our previous experiment, in which the same three illuminants and only yellow, neutral, and blue Munsell papers were used, the obtained (i.e., lighting) shift should have been observed in a direction collinear with that along which the papers were arranged. However, as mentioned above, the lighting shift was transversal to the material hue contours. Hence, we believe that this new dimension cannot be reduced to either the brightness or chromaticity of the reflected light. In line with the terminology put forward previously (Tokunaga and Logvinenko, 2010) it will be referred to as lighting hue. Thus, the material hue varies along the hue contours, the lighting hue in a transverse direction. The Friedman test (a two-way non-parametric ANOVA) performed for a subset of data obtained only for sessions where both fields were equally illuminated, showed a significant effect of illumination between hues (v22 ¼ 85:3; p<0:001). This means that the three hue contours in Figure 3 are significantly different from each other. To look into this difference we calculated the centroid of each hue contour, and the radial distances from each centroid to all the seven points of each contour (Figure 5). The distances for the hue contour obtained under the blue illumination were found to be smaller than for the neutral and yellow illuminations. It follows that the hue contour for the blue illumination is somewhat shrunken. This is similar to the lightness continuum shrinking observed for darker lights by Logvinenko and Maloney (2006). Figure 5. Shrinking of the blue hue contour. The vertical axis represents the distance from each point in Figure 3 to the corresponding centroid. The colour of line represents the colour of light. ª 2010 The Authors, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists Material and lighting hues: R. Tokunaga and A. D. Logvinenko Figure 6. Location of the colour stimuli in the CIELAB space. Notations are the same as in Figure 2. The yellow cross, black plus sign, and blue circle (which overlap) indicate the white surface under yellow, neutral and blue illumination respectively. Discussion Multidimensional scaling of Munsell papers lit by chromatic lights shows that papers of different illumination are dissociated into separate layers in the dissimilarity space. For example, the papers under blue light are significantly dislocated from the papers under yellow and neutral lights. Therefore, contrary to an old but still popular belief that illumination is discounted by the visual system so as to compute object colour (e.g., Kaiser and Boynton, 1996; Whittle, 2003), illumination is represented in the perceptual space. Specifically, illumination is represented with what we call lighting dimensions of the object colour manifold (e.g., surface brightness and lighting hue). There is every indication that the lighting dimensions cannot be reduced to the colour dimensions of reflected light. Indeed, as shown in our experiment, differently illuminated papers reflecting nearly metameric lights were judged as very dissimilar. Moreover, the lighting dimensions are unlikely to be deduced from the tristimulus coefficients of the reflected light. For example, CIELAB fails to predict a dissociation of hue contours as regards a lighting hue similar to that observed in Figure 3. The CIELAB prediction is displayed in Figure 6. The three hue contours as predicted by CIELAB are intertwined with each other. There is no systematic shift between the hue contours produced for different illuminations in Figure 6. This is hardly surprising because, first of all, CIELAB (as well as more recent colour appearance models approved by the CIE, such as the CIECAM02) is applicable only for single-illuminant scenes. They are not supposed to be used for scenes with multiple lights, such as that of our experiment. Secondly, while CIECAM02 duplicates some colour dimensions (i.e., brightness vs lightness, colourfulness vs chroma), it retains hue as a single dimension. It follows that within the conceptual framework on which CIELAB and 615 CIECAM02 are based, there is no room for two types of hue. Admittedly, there are two ÔkindsÕ of hue in CIECAM02. However, these are merely two ways of representing what is actually the same entity, rather than actually two sorts of hue, which we are proposing the existence of in the present paper. Although lighting hue is obviously related to the illumination, it is not the hue of the ambient illumination. Assuming that our observers saw the colour of objects and the colour of their illumination separately and independently, (as suggested by some scientists, e.g., Mausfeld, 1998; MacLeod, 2003), the dissimilarity between the pairs of identical papers would be approximately equal. This is because the dissimilarity would be determined only by the difference in illumination. In other words, the symbols in the bottom row graphs in Figure 4 would be arranged horizontally at the same level. However, this is definitely not the case. The Friedman test performed for this subset of data showed the effect of paper to be highly significant (v26 ¼ 131:0; p<0:001). Thus, dissimilarity between paper/light pairs is not separable, that is, it cannot be split into that dissimilarity between papers, and that between lights. Therefore, it would be a mistake to treat lighting hue as the illumination hue. The above discussion leads to the following question: are lighting and material hues different colour dimensions, or are they just two sides of the same coin? In other words, is there a qualitative difference between the hue difference produced by the neutral paper lit by yellow and the same paper lit by blue light, on the one hand; and the hue difference produced by the yellow paper and blue paper under day light? Our data suggests that lighting and material hues are qualitatively different types of experience. Indeed, when the yellow paper lit by the blue light is observed, we experience strong lighting blue and strong material yellow in the same place and at the same time. Therefore, the major dogma of colour vision that yellow and blue cannot be simultaneously experienced (Hering, 1920/1964) should be restricted to hues of the same type: i.e., either material or lighting ones. For example, material yellow and material blue cannot be simultaneously experienced in the same place under normal viewing conditions. The coexistence of lighting blue and material yellow hues can be understood if we assume that each illumination generates its own three-dimensional material colour manifold. Recall that the colour solids induced by different illumination are geometrically different, that is, they take different shapes (Logvinenko, 2009b). Therefore, our assumption is that different colour solids induce different three-dimensional manifolds of object colours. In a multiple light scene, as in our experiment, observers are able to distinguish areas of object colours belonging to different object colour manifolds. ª 2010 The Authors, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists 616 Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2010 30: No. 5 Moreover, they are able to order these areas as they can order self-luminous objects. Thus, in a multiple-light variegated scene there are two colour orders: material and lighting. Therefore, the distinction between the material and lighting colour dimensions is nothing more than the distinction between the material and lighting colour orders. In fact, there is just a single set of hues, but a two-dimensional one. There are two colour orders, acting on this set: the material order and the lighting order. For instance, a circular order along the material hue contours in Figure 3a illustrates one order, whereas the same points in Figure 3b are ordered in the transverse direction that illustrates another order. Although both the material and the lighting dimensions were found to contribute to the dissimilarity judgements, contribution from the lighting dimensions was found to be considerably less effective than that from the material dimensions. Indeed, the separation between the three hue contours in Figure 3a is much smaller than their size. For example, the shift between the yellow and blue contours in the CIE uniform chromaticity diagram (i.e., the distance between the neutral points under the yellow and blue illuminations) is 1.38 times as much as the distance between the most distant papers 5R4/14 and 10P5/12. On the contrary, the shift between the yellow and blue hue contours (i.e., the distance between the corresponding centroids) in Figure 3a is only 31% of the largest dissimilarity between papers under the blue light (papers 5R4/14 and 5PB5/ 12). Therefore, the lighting hue difference is discounted by a factor of 4.4. A similar lighting discounting was found in related studies (Tokunaga and Logvinenko, 2010; Logvinenko and Maloney, 2006). As pointed out elsewhere (Tokunaga and Logvinenko, 2010), the existence of the lighting dimensions alongside the material dimensions of object colours addresses the paradox familiar to everyone who has dealt with objects under chromatic illumination. On the one hand, chromatic light makes objects change their colour appearance. On the other, in a sense, the objectsÕ colour appearance remains relatively constant despite the change of the illumination. Although such objectcolour constancy has been intensively studied (e.g., Katz, 1935; Pokorny et al.,1991; Smithson, 2005; Foster, 2008; Brainard, 2009), it still remains unclear why object-colour constancy has never been registered perfect, and what exactly it is that remains constant when illumination changes. The imperfection of object-colour constancy manifests itself in the impossibility of achieving an asymmetric (i.e., across illuminants) colour match, a well known phenomenon in literature on colour constancy (e.g., Brainard et al., 1997; Foster, 2003; Logvinenko and Maloney, 2006). An asymmetric colour match is indeed impossible, because the difference in illumination results in a difference in lighting dimensions; for instance, in lighting hue – as in our experiment. However, it might happen that despite the difference in the lighting dimensions, the colour of the objects will be the same with respect to the material dimensions. In this case one can say that a sort of objectcolour constancy takes place. However, asymmetric colour matching is not quite the appropriate method of measuring the constancy of the material dimensions of object colour. One needs more elaborate techniques for this purpose. To summarise, the multidimensional analysis of Munsell papers lit by various chromatic lights shows that we experience two types of hues: material hues and lighting hues. This paves the way for a bold idea – that the object colour manifold can be envisaged as a tridimensional bundle of the traditional (material) threedimensional object colour manifolds. Acknowledgement Supported by EPSRC research grant EP/C010353/1 (AL). References Brainard, D. H. (2003) Color appearance and color difference specification. In: The Science of Color, 2nd edn (ed. S. K. Shevell), Optical Society of America, Washington D.C, pp. 191–216. Brainard, D. H. (2009) Color constancy. In: The Sage Encyclopedia of Percept (ed. B. Goldstein), Sage Publications, Los Angeles, pp. 253–257. Brainard, D. H., Brunt, W. A. and Speigle, J. M. (1997) Colour constancy in the nearly natural image. I. Asymmetric matches. J. Opt. Soc. Am.A. 14, 2091–2110. Cox, T. F. and Cox, M. A. A. (2001) Multidimensional scaling, 2nd edn, Chapman & Hall/CRC, Boca Raton, FL. Craven, B.J. and Foster, D.H. (1992) An operational approach to colour constancy. Vision Res. 32, 1359–1366. Evans, R. M. (1974) The Perception of Color. Wiley, New York. Fairchild, M. D. (2005) Color Appearance Models, 2nd edn, Wiley, Chichester, UK. Foster, D.H. (2003) Does colour constancy exist? Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 439–443. Foster, D.H. (2008) Color Appearance. In: The Senses: A Comprehensive Reference, Vol. 2, Vision II (Volume Eds T. Albright and R. H. Masland; Series Eds. A. I. Basbaum, A. Kaneko, G. M. Shephard and G. Westheimer), Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 119–132. Foster, D.H., Nascimento, S. M. C., Amano, K., Arend, L., Linnell, K. J., Nieves, J. L., Plet, S. and Foster, J. S. (2001) Parallel detection of violations of color constancy. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 8151–8156. Hering, E. (1920/1964) Outlines of a Theory of the Light Sense (translated by Hurvich, L. M. and D. Jameson), Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. ª 2010 The Authors, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists Material and lighting hues: R. Tokunaga and A. D. Logvinenko Hunt, R. W. G. (1977) The specifications of colour appearance. 1. Concepts and Terms. Color Res. Appl. 2, 55–68. Kaiser, P. K. and Boynton, R. M. (1996) Human Color Vision, Optical Society of America, Washington, DC. Katz, D. (1935) The world of Colour, Kegan Paul Trench, Trubner & Co., London. Kingdom, F. (2008) Perceiving light versus material. Vision Res. 48, 2090–2105. Logvinenko, A. D. (2005) Does luminance contrast determine lightness? Spat. Vis. 18(3), 337–346. Logvinenko, A. D. (2009a) Pseudoscopic colour illusions. In: Proceedings of ‘‘11th Congress of the International Colour Association (AIC). 27 Sep – 2 October, Sydney, Australia.’’ (in press). Logvinenko, A. D. (2009b) An object-colour space. J. Vis. 9, 1–23. Logvinenko, A. D. and Maloney, L. T. (2006) The proximity structure of achromatic colors and the impossibility of asymmetric lightness matching. Percept. Psychophys. 68, 76– 83. MacLeod, D. I. (2003) New dimensions in color perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 97–99. Mausfeld, R. (1998) Color perception: from Grassmann codes to a dual code for object and illumination colors. In: Color Vision (eds W. Backhaus, R. Kliegl and J. Werner), De Gruyter, Berlin, pp. 219–250. Pokorny, J., Shevell, S. K. and Smith, V. C. (1991) Colour appearance and colour constancy. In: The Perception of Colour, Vol 6, Vision & Visual Dysfunction (ed. P. Gouras), Macmillan, Basingstoke, England, pp. 43–61. Smithson, H. E. (2005) Sensory, computational and cognitive components of human colour constancy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360, 1329–1346. Tokunaga, R. and Logvinenko, A. D. (2010) Material and lighting dimensions of object colour. Vision Res. 50, 1740– 1747. Tokunaga, R., Logvinenko, A. D. and Maloney, L. T. (2008) Dissimilarity of yellow-blue surfaces under neutral light sources differing in intensity: Separate contributions of light intensity and chroma. Vis. Neurosci. 25, 395–398. 617 Whittle, P. (2003) Contrast colours. In: Colour Perception: Mind and the Physical World (eds R. Mausfeld and D. Heyer), Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 115–138. Wyszecki, G. and Stiles, W. S. (1982) Color Science. Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae, 2nd Edn, John Wiley & Sons, New York. Appendix Table A1. The coordinates of the MDS output configurations plotted in Figure 3. Dimension Illumination Munsell paper 1 2 3 Yellow 5R4/14 5YR7/12 5Y8/12 5G6/10 10BG5/8 5PB5/12 10P5/12 5R4/14 5YR7/12 5Y8/12 5G6/10 10BG5/8 5PB5/12 10P5/12 5R4/14 5YR7/12 5Y8/12 5G6/10 10BG5/8 5PB5/12 10P5/12 )16.523 )11.443 )8.252 8.427 7.448 7.738 )4.549 )15.826 )9.483 )8.306 8.594 8.337 9.111 )1.311 )7.941 )5.200 )4.545 9.373 11.608 13.997 8.746 )8.426 5.331 14.498 6.010 0.973 )4.684 )7.843 )9.291 4.735 15.570 5.984 1.097 )4.657 )7.350 )13.734 )0.659 10.201 2.457 0.266 )3.253 )7.225 4.954 1.962 )2.993 4.231 5.130 1.535 0.265 1.790 )0.913 )0.340 1.297 2.364 )3.933 )4.788 )1.984 )2.379 )2.949 0.285 )1.322 0.210 )2.423 Neutral Blue ª 2010 The Authors, Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics ª 2010 The College of Optometrists