REFLECTION ON THE AGREEMENT AND TENSE OMISSION MODEL OF SPECIFIC LANGUAGE IMPAIRMENT: A CORPUS-BASED STUDY* YI-AN LIN Abstract On the basis of the Leonard corpus in the Child Language Data Exchange System, the present paper tries to evaluate Wexler, Schütze & Rice’s (1998) two-factor account of specific language impairment (SLI), the Agreement and Tense Omission Model (ATOM), and to figure out the nature of syntactic errors made by SLI children. The result shows that the SLI children in the Leonard corpus did experience difficulties in the use of the third-person, singular, present tense –s and the preterite verb forms as predicted by ATOM. However, it is found that these SLI children mark tense better than agreement. ATOM cannot explain such a discrepancy. In addition, it is shown that case marking is unimpaired in these children’s grammars. Therefore, an alternative account which claims that nominative case is assigned by an interpretable mood feature on T is adopted in order to account for the findings here. Moreover, it is found that these SLI children do not have any problem with A-movement. This suggests that their agreement marking errors are not due to the underspecification of the agreement feature on T as proposed by ATOM. They may instead be merely spellout errors. By contrast, it was shown that these SLI children’s problems with auxiliary inversion may be due to the underspecification of the tense feature. Hence, this indicates that their tense marking errors are caused by the underspecification of the tense feature on T. However, the proposal that the interpretable tense feature may sometimes be underspecified requires the functional head T to have another interpretable feature: mood. Such an amendment prevents the derivation of sentences from crashing at semantic interface. In summary, the primary deficit in SLI children is shown to be the specification of the tense feature, and this is arguably due to the fact that tense is a conceptually complex notion. 1. Introduction Specific Language Impairment (SLI), also known as developmental dysphasia, refers to a delayed or deviant language development of children in the absence of neurological trauma, cognitive impairment, psycho-emotional disturbance, or motor-articulatory disorders (Eisenbeiss, Bartke, & Clahsen 2005). Due to the fact that SLI is a heterogeneous disorder, the identification of SLI is usually based on a set of exclusionary criteria. As Leonard (1998) mentioned, there are some primary criteria for the diagnosis of SLI. Namely, language test scores are of -1.25 standard * This paper is based on the third chapter of my 2005 master’s dissertation submitted to University of Essex. Therefore, I am grateful to my supervisor, Prof. Andrew Radford, for his guidance on the analysis presented here. I also thank the audience at the 1st Newcastle Postgraduate Conference in Theoretical and Applied Linguistics and the anonymous reviewer for the valuable comments and suggestions. All the remaining errors are my own responsibility. 137 Lin deviations from the mean, the nonverbal performance IQ is 85 or above, the hearing passes screening at conventional levels, no recent episodes of otitis media are known, no neurological dysfunction is shown, oral motor structure and function are both intact, and the pattern of social interaction is normal. However, language impairment is the defining feature of SLI. Individuals with SLI have been reported to experience difficulties with most or all areas of language, including vocabulary, morphosyntax, phonology, and pragmatics. Nevertheless, these areas are not equally affected. In English, vocabulary and pragmatic skills are less deficient than phonology and morphosyntax (Leonard 2004). For theoretical linguists and psycholinguists, the primary deficit in SLI relates to the acquisition of morphosyntax. As a result, the aim of linguistics-oriented studies of SLI is to characterize the morphosyntax of individuals with SLI and the difference between SLI and typically-developing (TD) children in the framework of current grammatical theories or theories of grammatical development (Eisenbeiss, Bartke, & Clahsen 2005). This paper is especially concerned with the nature of syntactic errors made by SLI children, which involve core syntactic operations such as movement, agreement and case-marking. As a result, a two-factor account of SLI, the Agreement and Tense Omission Model (ATOM), is evaluated on the basis of a corpus of spontaneous speech production data, the Leonard corpus in the Child Language Data Exchange System (CHILDES), which was collected before the formulation of the ATOM. In the next section, the key claims and predictions made by ATOM are presented. 2. Literature Review on the Agreement and Tense Omission Model The Agreement and Tense Omission Model of SLI originates from Wexler’s (1994, 1996) studies on clause structure and inflection in young TD children. The grammar of TD children can be captured in terms of Wexler’s Optional Infinitive (OI) stage. He claims that TD children undergo a protracted stage during which they alternate between producing finite and infinitive forms of verbs in contexts where finite forms are required in adult grammar. Additionally, TD children at the OI stage tend to omit auxiliaries and copula BE1 in finite contexts as Wexler observed. Further, Wexler asserts that optional infinitives arise because of the underspecification or omission of the tense feature in the clause representation. Adopting Wexler’s idea, Rice, Wexler & Cleave (1995) propose that SLI children go through an Extended Optional Infinitives (EOI) stage in which a similar pattern of optional infinitives is found until they are 7 or 8 years of age. Rice and her collaborators propose that the grammar of SLI children is characterized in terms of the tense omission model. They predict that SLI children will display limited proficiency in the use of morphemes marking tense while leaving other inflectional morphemes unaffected. Additionally, when these children do specify the tense feature, they respect all its morphosyntactic properties. That is, when SLI children use tense morphemes or auxiliaries, they use them correctly in the same way as TD children do. Nevertheless, such a tense-deficit hypothesis wrongly predicts that SLI children never use accusative subjects with past tense verbs. 1 In this paper, the capitalized word is the label for various forms of that word. 138 Lin In later work, Schütze & Wexler (1996) claim that optional infinitives can result from the underspecification or omission of either the tense or agreement features (or both) in a child’s structural representation of a sentence. As a result, Wexler, Schütze and Rice (1998) revise their account of SLI with a two-factor theory. They assume that SLI involves a syntactic feature deficit leading to the optional specification of tense [TNS]2 and agreement [AGR] features in obligatory contexts. They refer to their model as ATOM. Further, they argue that the case-marking of subjects as nominative (Nom) or non-Nom by SLI children directly correlates with whether or not the agreement is marked on verbs. In other words, the case-marking difficulties of SLI are a secondary effect of problems with verb agreement. From their study, Wexler and his collaborators report that English SLI children (ESLI children) are more likely to use non-Nom subjects and to produce non-Nom subjects at a much later age than TD children. Additionally, they find that both TD and the SLI children virtually always correctly mark the case of object. Moreover, their data show that the optional occurrence of Nom and non-Nom subjects is correlated with the optional occurrence of finite and non-finite verb forms. More specifically, they point out that non-Nom subjects almost never appear with agreeing auxiliary/main verbs that are inflected for both tense and agreement. Therefore, Wexler and his colleagues take the subject case errors as a reflection of the OI stage. In summary, Wexler and his collaborators draw the following main conclusions from their study: (i) SLI children make more frequent case-marking errors with subjects than MLU-matched TD children (ii) SLI children use an accusative (Acc) form of subjects in contexts where adults require a Nom form (iii) the case-marking of subjects is determined by whether clauses contain an auxiliary/main verb3 which agrees in person and number with the subject or not (iv) SLI children may leave verbs underspecified for [TNS] and/or [AGR] features in finite contexts, resulting in the production of OI structures with Acc subjects (v) ATOM makes predictions about the relative frequency of case and tense/agreement errors made by children in different types of clause structure (e.g. with subject case-marking errors predicted to be most frequent with uninflected verbs, less frequent with ambiguous verbs, and least frequent with agreeing verbs). However, ATOM makes no specific claims about whether the [TNS] feature is omitted more frequently than the [AGR] feature – or conversely. 3. Results and Discussion Tense Marking and Agreement Marking Since ATOM argues that SLI involves an impairment of tense marking, it predicts that SLI children may sometimes leave verbs underspecified for the [TNS] feature in finite contexts. It is found that all eleven children in the Leonard corpus 2 3 By convention, features are enclosed in square brackets and often abbreviated. More precisely, an INFL/Agr constituent 139 Lin indeed commit tense marking errors. Their mean correctness score for tense marking on main lexical verbs is 53.87%. In more detail, five of these children display limited proficiency in the specification of the [TNS] feature on main verbs. Each of the five children’s percentage specification of the [TNS] feature is at chance level. Furthermore, it is found that there is a significant positive correlation between the MLU and the child’s performance on past-tense marking (r=.73, p=.005, one-tailed 4 ). The children with a higher MLU tend to have higher percentage specification of the [TNS] feature. The existence of such a positive correlation may indicate that the SLI children in the Leonard corpus have a delayed acquisition of tense marking. Since ATOM also claims that SLI involves an optional specification of the [AGR] feature in obligatory contexts, it predicts that SLI children will experience difficulty in the use of the third-person, singular, present tense (3SgPres) –s and the use of inflected forms of auxiliaries. It is found that all eleven children in the Leonard corpus frequently use bare verbs in contexts requiring the 3SgPres –s, and nine of these children show use of the 3SgPres –s at chance level. Additionally, all eleven children commit agreement marking errors on auxiliaries, and six of them mark the [AGR] feature on auxiliaries at chance level. Furthermore, the mean percentage group scores for specification of the [AGR] feature on main verbs and auxiliaries are 30.76% (SD=21.35) and 38.15% (SD=27.79), both below chance level. However, unlike the specification of the [TNS] feature, there is no significant correlation between the MLU and the child’s performance with agreement marking on either main verbs or auxiliaries. The non-existence of such a significant correlation may suggest that the SLI children in the Leonard corpus have a deficit in the acquisition of agreement marking. Moreover, there is a statistically significant difference between these children’s performance with the specification of [TNS] and [AGR] features on main verbs (t(10)=3.26, p=.009, two-tailed5). The SLI children in the Leonard corpus underspecified the [AGR] feature much more frequently than the [TNS] feature. However, ATOM makes no specific claims about such a difference. There are two suggested explanations for this finding. One is that the agreement between the functional head T of TP and its subject in Spec-TP involves two features, namely the person [PERS] and number [NUM] features. Yet these two features are treated as one single composite [AGR] feature within the ATOM account (Radford 2005a). Since agreement involves valuation of two features, it may be a more difficult process for these SLI children, since tense marking is only controlled by one single [TNS] feature. The other explanation as suggested by Clahsen, Bartke & Göllner’s (1997) Agreement Deficit Model is that the [TNS] feature on T is an interpretable feature while the [AGR] feature on T is an uninterpretable feature, which causes major problems for SLI children. Nevertheless, a further theoretical problem with ATOM is that it assumes that the head T, which contains only the uninterpretable [AGR] feature and the interpretable [TNS] feature, can be underspecified for the [TNS] feature. Such an assumption violates Chomsky’s (1995) claim that syntactic structures must not 4 5 The data is analysed in the Pearson correlation using SPSS software. The data is analysed in a paired-samples t test using SPSS software. 140 Lin contain heads to which no semantic interpretation can be assigned. Since the interpretable [TNS] feature on T provides information about the time at which an event takes place, T cannot be assigned any semantic interpretation if the [TNS] feature is not specified, which causes the derivation to crash at the semantic interface (Radford 2005a). An alternative account, suggested by Radford and Ramos (2001) for ATOM and the omission of tense marking found in SLI grammar, is that T carries an interpretable feature other than the [TNS] feature, namely a mood [MOOD] feature. This feature correlates with the Nom case marking, and this will be discussed in the next section. That the [TNS] feature is underspecified but not the [MOOD] feature will not result in a crash at the semantic interface. Additionally, the notion of underspecification adopted in ATOM leads to a developmental question. That is, which feature carried by T is mastered before the other one if either [TNS] or [AGR] (or even both) features can be underspecified? A suggested answer to such a question is that tense is acquired before agreement since the [TNS] feature is interpretable and the [AGR] feature is uninterpretable. This answer is supported by Ingham’s (1998) longitudinal study of a single child (Radford 2005a). Tense Marking, Agreement Marking and Case Marking Since ATOM asserts that the case-marking of subjects is determined by whether clauses contain an auxiliary/main verb which agrees in person and number with the subject or not, it predicts that (i) Nom subject pronouns always co-occur with verbal forms specified for agreement and that (ii) the subject pronoun will be in the (Acc) default form with verbal forms not specified for agreement. However, it is shown that the SLI children in the Leonard corpus seldom use non-Nom subjects in their spontaneous speech, and the mean percentage suppliance of the correct Nom case is 97.26% (SD=2.67). These figures show that these children’s performance on case marking is almost adult-like. This suggests that the few case-marking errors found among the SLI children in the Leonard corpus may be case spellout errors rather than case assignment errors. If the Nom case of subjects is indeed assigned by the specification of [AGR] feature as claimed by ATOM, the overall high percentage suppliance of the correct Nom case found in the Leonard corpus indicates that a high percentage suppliance of agreement marking should be found among these SLI children. Nevertheless, this expectation is not borne out by the data presented. One way for ATOM to deal with this is to say that these SLI children have problems with verb morphology – i.e. with retrieving the s-inflections or irregular s-inflected forms rather than with marking agreement in the syntax. This is the core assumption of Gopnik and Crago’s (1991) Rule Deficit Model. However, an alternative Mood-and-Agreement model which argues that Nom case is assigned by a [MOOD] feature on T can also account for the data in this study. If the few case-marking errors are indeed case assignment errors but not spellout errors, it could be due to the underspecification of the [MOOD] and/or [AGR] feature(s) within the Mood-and-Agreement model. A deficit in specification of [TNS] and/or [AGR] feature(s) in SLI grammar will not cause any problem for this alternative account. On the other hand, ATOM successfully predicts that SLI children will not have 141 Lin any problem with the case marking on personal pronoun objects. A 100% of adult-like case marking on personal pronoun objects is found for all eleven SLI children in the Leonard corpus. Agreement Marking and A-movement Although ATOM does not make any specific prediction about SLI children’s performance with A-movement, it claims that they frequently underspecify the [AGR] feature in obligatory contexts, which should result in frequent A-movement errors, since Chomsky (1998, 1999) proposes that agreement plays an important role in A-movement. In more detail, an active Probe in T by virtue of its uninterpretable and unvalued [PERS] and [NUM] features searches for an active nominal Goal (by virtue of its uninterpretable and unvalued [CASE] feature) to match and delete its [PERS] and [NUM] features and to delete its uninterpretable [EPP]6 feature by movement. Additionally, Chomsky claims that the [EPP] feature on T can attract an expletive such as there which carries only the [PERS] feature. Hence, he assumes that T attracts a constituent agreeing with T in at least one of its φ-features. In other words, the movement of the nominal from Spec-vP, Spec-VP or VP-complement to Spec-TP requires T to attract the closest nominal with which it agrees in respect of all φ-features carried by the nominal. However, it is found that the SLI children in the Leonard corpus do not commit any word order errors in obligatory contexts where A-movement is required. These children correctly move subjects to the Spec-TP position even when they commit other errors such as tense/agreement marking errors and auxiliary-omission errors in the sentences below: (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) ‘It not work’ (Child B) ‘He not clean’ (Child G) ‘It not working’ (Child H) ‘This be not the doctor’ (Child H) ‘It not this one’ (Child J) According to Chomsky’s claim, the observation that these SLI children do not have any problem with A-movement suggests that they specify person and/or number agreement between T and the moved subject. Moreover, since these children correctly assign Nom case to the subjects, it indicates that agreement is fully specified. However, agreement marking errors and auxiliary-omission errors are found in the sentences above. ATOM cannot provide an explanation for these findings. An alternative account is that all these agreement marking errors and auxiliary-omission errors are merely spellout errors. The agreement marking errors are not due to the underspecification of the [AGR] feature (or the [PERS] and [NUM] features). 6 It is ‘originally an abbreviation for the Extended Projection Principle, which posited that every T constituent must be extended into a TP projection which has a specifier. In more recent work, the requirement for a T constituent like will to have a specifier is said to be a consequence of T carrying an [EPP] feature requiring it to project a specifier. The EPP Generalisation specifies the conditions under which the [EPP] feature carried by a head is deleted via use of an expletive or movement’ (Radford, 2004: 450). 142 Lin Additionally, this account indicates that these SLI children’s low percentage suppliance of the suffix –s in obligatory contexts may be the result of a problem retrieving the regular s-inflection or irregular s-inflected forms. Tense Marking, Agreement Marking and Auxiliary Inversion Although ATOM does not make any specific prediction about SLI children’s performance with auxiliary inversion, it maintains that SLI children sometimes underspecify [TNS] and/or [AGR] feature(s) on T, which should lead to problems with auxiliary inversion, since current thinking within Minimalism suggests that auxiliary inversion is triggered by either an affix on C carrying a [TNS] feature requiring it to attract the closest head marked for tense (Pesetsky & Torrego 2001, Radford 2004) or an affix on C carrying a set of strong agreement features requiring it to attract the closest head specified for agreement (Radford 2005b). More specifically, if tense is the factor triggering auxiliary inversion, these children’s performance on auxiliary inversion is expected to reflect their performance on tense marking. If agreement is the factor triggering inversion, their performance on auxiliary inversion should have the same frequency as agreement marking and Nom case assignment, for Chomsky (2005) claims that C hands over its agreement properties to T so that T must be underspecified for agreement and leads to a non-nominative subject if the agreement features on C are not specified. It is interesting to explore the extent to which the Leonard children’s performance on auxiliary inversion can be explained in terms of ATOM and the theories for the mechanism of auxiliary inversion. It is found that the overall frequency with which these children supply auxiliary inversion (52.45%) is more similar to their tense suppliance rate (53.87%) than to their agreement suppliance rate (34.78%) and nominative case suppliance rate (97.26%). Additionally, according to the finding in the previous section, the [AGR] feature seems to be unimpaired in these SLI children’s grammars. Therefore, their problems with auxiliary inversion may be due to the underspecification of the [TNS] feature. 4. Conclusion The Agreement and Tense Omission Model for syntactic errors produced by SLI children has been evaluated on the basis of the Leonard corpus in the CHILDES. In the previous section, it was shown that the SLI children in the Leonard corpus did experience difficulties in the use of the 3SgPres –s and the preterite verb forms as predicted by ATOM. However, it was found that these SLI children mark tense better than agreement. This discrepancy cannot be explained by ATOM. In addition, it was shown that case marking is unimpaired in these children’s grammars. Therefore, an alternative account which claims that Nom case is assigned by an interpretable [MOOD] feature on T was adopted in order to account for the findings. Furthermore, it was found that these SLI children do not have any problem with A-movement. This suggests that agreement marking errors are not due to the underspecification of the [AGR] feature on T as proposed by ATOM. They may instead be merely spellout errors. By contrast, it was shown that these SLI children’s problems with auxiliary inversion may be due to the underspecification of 143 Lin the [TNS] feature. Hence, this indicates that their tense marking errors are caused by the underspecification of the [TNS] feature on T. However, the proposal that the interpretable [TNS] feature may sometimes be underspecified requires the functional head T to have another interpretable feature: [MOOD]. Such an amendment prevents the derivation of sentences from crashing at semantic interface. In summary, the primary deficit in SLI children is the specification of the [TNS] feature, and this is arguably due to the fact that tense is a conceptually complex notion. References Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Chomsky, N. (1998). Minimalist inquiries: the framework. In Martin, R., Michaels, D. & Uriagereka, J. (eds.), Step by step: essays on minimalism in honor of Howard Lasnik. Cambridge: MIT Press. 89 – 155. Chomsky, N. (1999). Derivation by phase. In Kenstowicz (ed.), 1 – 52. Chomsky, N. (2005). On phases. Ms., MIT. Clahsen, H. (1989). The grammatical characterization of developmental dysphasia. Linguistics 27. 897 – 920. Clahsen, H., Bartke, S., & Göllner, S. (1997). Formal features in impaired grammars: A comparison of English and German SLI children. Journal of Neurolinguistics 10. 151 –171. Eisenbeiss, S., Bartke, S., & Clahsen, H. (2005). Structural and lexical case in child German: evidence from language-impaired and typically-developing children. Language Acquisition 13. 3 – 32. Gopnik, M., & Crago, M. (1991). Familial aggregation of a developmental disorder. Cognition 39. 1 – 50. Ingham, R. (1998). Tense without agreement in early clause structure. Language Acquisition 7. 51 – 81. Kenstowicz, M. (ed.) (2001). Ken Hale: A life in language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Leonard, L. (1998). Children with specific language disorders. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Leonard, L. (2004). Specific language impairment in children. In Kent, R. (ed.), The MIT encyclopedia of communication disorders. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 402 – 404. Pesetsky, D., & Torrego, E. (2001). T-to-C movement: Causes and consequences. In Kenstowicz (ed.), 355 – 426. Radford, A. (2004). Minimalist syntax: exploring the structure of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Radford, A. (2005a). Children’s English: principles-and-parameters perspectives. Ms., University of Essex, Colchester. Radford, A. (2005b). English syntax revisited: an introduction. Ms., University of Essex, Colchester. Radford, A., & Ramos, E. (2001). Case, Agreement and EPP: evidence from an English-speaking Child with SLI. Essex Research Reports in Linguistics 36. 42 – 81. 144 Lin Rice, M., Wexler, K., & Cleave, P. (1995). Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38. 850 – 863. Schütze, C. & Wexler, K. (1996). Subject case licensing and English root infinitives. In Stringfellow, A., Cahana-Amitay, D., Hughes, E. & Zukowski, A. (eds.), Proceedings of the 20th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 2). Somerville: Cascadilla Press. 670 – 681. Wexler, K. (1994). Optional infinitives, head movement and the economy of derivations in child grammar. In Lightfoot, D. & Hornstein, N. (eds.), Verb movement. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 305 –350. Wexler, K. (1996). The development of inflection in a biologically based theory of language acquisition. In Rice, M. L. (ed.), Toward a genetics of language. Mahwah: Erlbaum. 113 –144. Wexler, K., Schütze, C. & Rice, M. (1998). Subject case in children with SLI and unaffected controls: evidence for the Agr/Tns Omission Model. Language Acquisition 7. 317 – 344. Yi-An Lin Department of Linguistics Faculty of Modern and Medieval Languages University of Cambridge Sidgwick Avenue, Cambridge, CB3 9DA United Kingdom yal23@cam.ac.uk 145