The Sport Psychologist, 2012, 26, 359-374 © 2012 Human Kinetics, Inc. Relationships Between Psychological Factors, RPE and Time Limit Estimated by Teleoanticipation Jérémy B. J. Coquart University of Rouen Yancy Dufour University of Lille Alain Groslambert University of Franche-Comté Régis Matran Lille Academic Hospital Murielle Garcin University of Lille The purpose was to study the relationships between psychological factors and perceptually-based values (Ratings of Perceived Exertion: RPE and Estimated Time Limit: ETL). The researchers obtained the scores of several psychological factors (anxiety, extraversion-introversion, neuroticism-stability, self-esteem, motivation, psychological resistance and endurance, desire for success, social desirability, dynamism, competitiveness, activity control, risk-taking, emotional control, aggressiveness, sociability, cooperation, acceptance of a judgment, and leadership) among 23 cyclists. The cyclists performed a graded exercise test in which the researchers collected RPE and ETL at 150, 200, 250 and 300W. Correlations between RPE/ETL and psychological factors were examined. RPE was correlated with leadership, psychological resistance and endurance. ETL was significantly correlated with psychological endurance. These results suggest a link between psychological factors, effort perception, and the time limits predicted by teleoanticipation. These relationships varied according to intensity. Coquart is with the Faculty of Sports Sciences and Physical Education, University of Rouen, Rouen, France. Dufour and Garcin are with the Faculty of Sports Sciences and Physical Education, University of Lille, Ronchin, France. Groslambert is with the Prevention Innovation and Sport Research Dept., University of Franche-Comté, Besancon, France. Matran is with Lille Academic Hospital, Dept. of Functional and Respiratory Explorations, Lille, France. 359 360 Coquart et al. Effort perception, defined as the intensity of subjective effort, stress, or discomfort felt during physical activity, is used for clinical, pedagogical and athletic applications (Robertson & Noble, 1997), to predict some physiological variables or even to regulate exercise intensities (Robertson, 2004). The most frequently used tool in measuring effort perception is Borg’s (1970) Ratings of Perceived Exertion (RPE) scale (Watt & Grove, 1993). The concurrent validity of this scale has been shown through numerous physiological variables (e.g., heart rate, ventilation, breathing frequency, oxygen uptake, carbon dioxide excretion and metabolic acidosis; Noble & Robertson, 1996). However, other nonphysiological factors must be integrated to explain the variance of RPE (Morgan, 1973). It was reported that situational, sociological and psychological factors could mediate RPE (Robertson & Noble, 1997). Extraversion, anxiety, neuroticism and depression were considered among the psychological factors that influenced effort perception (Morgan, 1973, 1994). Moreover, in his conceptual model, Rejeski (1981) considers the role of exercise intensity on the contribution of factors that shape RPE (physiological, psychological, sociological and situational factors). Indeed, when exercise intensity is high, effort perception is principally determined by physiological signals, whereas, when exercise intensity is low, nonphysiological factors make a major contribution to the determination of effort perception, and thus the perceptuallybased values (Hall, Ekkekakis & Petruzzello, 2005). A recent study by Hall et al. (2005) confirmed that the relationships between psychological factors and RPE changes according to exercise intensity were lower at higher exercise intensities. Indeed, in their experimentation, extraversion (p < .01; r = -.44), behavioral activation (p < .05; r = -.32), and self-efficacy (p < .01; r = -.52) were significantly and negatively correlated to RPE at lower (below the ventilatory threshold) but not at higher intensities (above the ventilatory threshold), whereas neuroticism was unrelated to RPE (whatever the exercise intensity) and behavioral inhibition was positively related to RPE across all exercise intensities (p < .05 ; range between r = .35 and r = .65). Using the concept of teleoanticipation (Ulmer, 1996), Noakes, Snow and Febbraio (2004) suggested that, at exercise onset, a central programmer estimates the time to volitional exhaustion (or time limit, Tlim) that can safely be performed without causing absolute whole body energy depletion. Then, this subjective prediction of the Tlim is associated with the maximal RPE that can be tolerated (Noakes et al., 2004). During exercise, it was proposed that the brain provides RPE which increases according to the subjective prediction of the Tlim until the attainment of the corresponding tolerated maximal RPE. Noakes et al. (2004) proposed a scalar linear relationship between RPE and exercise duration during a constant power test at 70% maximal oxygen uptake in both carbohydrate-depleted and carbohydratereplete conditions. Consequently, Noakes et al. (2004) propose that the brain increases RPE proportionally to the percentage of time remaining to complete the exercise. To verify this hypothesis, Eston, Faulkner, St Clair Gibson, Noakes and Parfitt (2007) compared RPE during an exhaustive constant power test at 75% maximal oxygen uptake between a fatigued condition (the constant power test was preceded by an exhaustive graded exercise test) and a nonfatigued condition (only the constant power test was performed). The results confirmed the Noakes et al. (2004) hypothesis because, the RPE increase rate under both conditions did not differ significantly when the exercise duration was expressed as a relative time to exhaustion (i.e., % Tlim); conversely, the Tlim was significantly lower during the Effects of Psychological Factors on RPE/ETL 361 fatigued condition. Therefore, one can conclude that the brain uses scalar rather than absolute time to set RPE (Eston et al., 2007; Noakes et al., 2004). Recently, Garcin, Vandewalle and Monod (1999) proposed associating the RPE scale with a second scale based on subjective estimation of exhaustion time (Estimated Time Limit scale, ETL), to have further information on the psychological load (intensity and duration) of exercise. These scales are complementary tools because the RPE scale is concerned with the current status of the participant (how hard he/she feels the exercise currently is) whereas the ETL scale deals with a subjective prediction of how long the current exercise level can be maintained (Garcin et al., 2006). In other words, the RPE scale measures the degree of heaviness and strain experienced during exercise, while the ETL scale allows a subjective estimation of the Tlim that can be maintained at a given intensity and at a given instant. Consequently, the ETL scale may be used to quantify the predicted Tlim from the teleoanticipation (i.e., the duration of exercise that may be performed before the participant stops the exercise). Just as some psychological factors influence RPE (Morgan, 1973, 1994), ETL, which may represent the predicted time limit from the teleoanticipation, could also be affected by psychological factors. To our knowledge, only one study has already attempted to examine the relationships between psychological factors and RPE/ ETL scales (Garcin et al., 2006). This study revealed that ETL was only linked by neuroticism or even agreeableness. However, this study was only performed at the intensity corresponding to the lactate concentration threshold, whereas Hall et al. (2005) suggested that the relationships between psychological factors and the perceptually-based values changed with the exercise intensity. As a consequence, the main purpose of the current study was to examine the relationships between psychological factors and the perceptually-based values (RPE and ETL), at different intensities, during a graded exercise test (GXT). In accordance with the results of Hall et al. (2005), we hypothesized that there would be a correlation between the psychological factors and RPE and ETL, but only during lower intensities in GXT. Methods Participants Twenty-three French male competitive cyclists of different physical ability levels (among these 23 cyclists, 5 were competitors at a local level, 12 at a regional level, and 6 at the semiprofessional level) volunteered to take part in this study (Tables 1 and 2). The MAP were respectively equal to: 340 ± 30W, 346 ± 21W and 350 ± 25W (these values were not significantly different between the groups). Only competitive cyclists were recruited because this population is able to precisely determine the RPE and ETL during GXT. Moreover, to facilitate perceptual anchoring, all participants were experienced (9.1 ± 4.7 yr) and well-trained (10.9 ± 3.5 hr.wk-1) in cycling. None of the cyclists reported respiratory or cardiac disease, or were known to be suffering from any chronic illness. None were taking medication. All were nonsmokers and presented normal electrocardiograph, hemoglobin concentration and spirometric values at rest. Before the test, the participants filled out a written consent form concerning the purposes and procedures of the experiment. This present study was approved by the consultative committee for participants’ protection in clinical research of Lille, France. Table 1 Anthropometric Characteristics and Maximal Aerobic Power Measured During the Incremental Test Age (yr) Body mass (kg) Height (cm) Body fat (%) Maximal aerobic power (W) Mean SD 22.8 70.1 178 12.2 346 ± 5.1 ± 5.6 ±6 ± 1.8 ± 25 Values are means ± SD (SD). Table 2 Scores on the Psychological Factors Obtained Before the Incremental Test State anxiety Trait anxiety Extraversion-introversion Neuroticism-stability Overall self-esteem Social self-esteem Family self-esteem Professional self-esteem Intrinsic motivation to know Intrinsic motivation to accomplish things Intrinsic motivation to experience stimulation Extrinsic motivation by an identified regulation Extrinsic motivation by introjected regulation Extrinsic motivation by external regulation Absence of motivation Psychological resistance Psychological endurance Desire for success Social desirability Dynamism Competitiveness Activity control Risk-taking Emotional control Aggressiveness Sociability Cooperation Acceptance of a judgment Leadership Values are means ± SD (SD). 362 Mean SD Range (observed) Range (possible) 43.7 40.0 14.0 9.5 21.6 6.8 7.1 6.8 18.6 20.4 24.1 10.5 7.5 3.0 4.5 3.8 1.7 1.9 1.4 5.9 3.9 2.9 27–66 29–55 6–19 2–19 9–25 1–8 1–8 3–8 7–26 10–28 19–28 20–80 20–80 0–24 0–24 0–26 0–8 0–8 0–8 4–28 4–28 4–28 16.7 3.6 8–23 4–28 21.7 14.0 5.7 5.1 6.1 3.9 4.7 4.1 4.9 5.1 4.4 5.0 4.6 4.8 5.3 4.6 4.7 3.7 4.7 2.3 1.8 1.6 2.4 2.2 2.1 2.8 2.3 2.5 2.2 1.5 2.4 2.1 2.3 2.2 12–28 8–24 4–12 2–8 3–9 0–7 0–8 0–7 0–10 1–10 0–9 0–9 2–7 0–8 2–9 2–9 0–9 4–28 4–28 4–28 0–10 0–9 0–10 0–10 0–10 0–10 0–10 0–10 0–9 0–10 0–10 0–8 0–10 0–10 Effects of Psychological Factors on RPE/ETL 363 Materials The perceptually-based values were expressed with two valid and reliable scales: The Ratings of Perceived Exertion (RPE) scale of Borg (1970) and the Estimated Time Limit scale (ETL; Garcin et al., 1999). The RPE scale consists of fifteen numerical ratings (between 6 and 20) associated with verbal cues, from “7 = very very light” to “19 = very very hard”. The cyclist was asked, “How hard do you feel this exercise is?” The ETL scale is comprised of twenty numerical ratings (between 1 and 20) from “1 = more than 16 hours” to 20 = less than 2 min. This scale is designed as a function of the logarithm of the estimated Tlim (ETL = 21–2n, with n = log2 × Tlim, where Tlim is expressed in min). A base 2 logarithm was chosen to allow a large range of Tlim. However, to facilitate the use of the ETL scale, Garcin et al. (1999) used a simplified version. For example: ETL equal to 13 and 11 correspond to 15 and 30 min instead of 16 and 32 min, respectively. Similarly, ETL equal to or lower than 9 are expressed in multiples of 1 hr. The cyclist was asked, “How long would you be able to perform an exercise at this intensity until exhaustion?” Anxiety was assessed with Spielberger’s state-trait inventory (STAI; Spielberger, Gorsuch & Lushene, 1970). This inventory measures state anxiety and trait anxiety using a 40-question survey with 4 possible responses to each question. The STAI is often used to examine the influence of anxiety on perceptually-based values (Garcin et al., 2006; Morgan, 1973). Extraversion-introversion and neuroticism-stability, which are two pervasive, independent dimensions of personality, were evaluated with the Eysenck Personality Inventory (EPI; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1975). This is an inventory consisting of 57 “Yes/No” questions about one’s feelings, thoughts, and behavior. Some authors (Hall et al., 2005; Morgan, 1973) have already tested the influence of extraversionintroversion and neuroticism-stability on RPE using EPI. Motivation was evaluated by the sports motivation scale (Brière, Vallerand, Blais & Pelletier, 1995). This scale is based on the tenets of the self-determination theory and is composed of 28 questions divided into seven categories, assessing three types of intrinsic motivation (intrinsic motivation to know, to accomplish things, and to experience stimulation), three types of extrinsic motivation (external, introjected, and identified regulation), and the absence of motivation. The sports motivation scale was used because it is specifically designed for athletes, and because motivation is a psychological factor linked to performance (Le Scanff, 2003). The adult form (for participants over 17 years of age) of the personality questionnaire for athletes (Thill, 1983) was used to determine psychological resistance, psychological endurance, competitiveness and leadership. A participant with a high psychological resistance score is an athlete who resists high pressure, psychological and that imposed by the environment, the bad luck, criticism and stress. A high psychological endurance score may be found in a persistent, determined and obstinate participant. An athlete with a high score on the competitiveness scale is a participant with a high capacity to surpass himself. A high leadership score corresponds to great persuasive ability and a great ability to command. The personality questionnaire used for athletes (Thill, 1983) was written in French. This questionnaire is composed of 340 “True/False” items. The questions deal with specific situations 364 Coquart et al. that the athletes regularly encounter. Similar to the sports motivation scale, this questionnaire is formulated purely for athletes. Moreover, the French version of this questionnaire was used because some authors (Thill, 1983; Thill & Brenot, 1985) have already attested to its validity. Height, body mass, and skinfold thickness were measured for each cyclist with a wall stadiometer (model 220, Seca, Hamburg, Germany), a calibrated scale (TBF 543, Tanita, Tokyo, Japan) and a skinfold caliper (HSK-B1, Body Care, Warwickshire, United Kingdom), respectively. Respiratory gas analysis was carried out via a breath-by-breath system using an open-circuit metabolic card (Ergocard, Medisoft, Sorinnes, Belgium). This system was calibrated just before the test, in accordance with the manufacturer’s guidelines, using a 3 L syringe (Calibration pump, Medisoft, Sorinnes, Belgium) for volume calibration, and using ambient air and a gas of known oxygen and carbon dioxide concentrations (16% and 4%, respectively) for gas calibration. The software used was Exp’air (Medisoft, Sorinnes, Belgium). Heart rate was recorded with a 12-lead electrocardiogram (Medcard, Medisoft, Sorinnes, Belgium) linked to the respiratory gas analysis system. Lactate concentration was determined at the end of each exercise by means of the ABL800Flex (Radiometer Medical, Copenhagen, Denmark). The exercise test was carried out on an electromagnetically braked cycle ergometer (Ergoselect, Medisoft, Sorinnes, Belgium) which maintained the power output by adjusting the resistance according to variations in pedal rate. Procedures About 1 month before the test, the instructions for both perceptually-based scales were read by the participants and they were each provided with a copy. Moreover, the cyclists were familiarized with the RPE and ETL scales using the memory anchoring procedure (Robertson, 2004). During this procedure, the cyclist was asked to think of a time when he reached an effort perception that was equal to verbal cues at the bottom and top of both scales (Robertson, 2004). Then, during his training sessions before the study, the cyclist was asked to estimate RPE and ETL using the memory anchoring procedure on a continuum set between the high and low anchor effort levels on both scales. Furthermore, instructions on the scales were read again by the participants before the test (i.e., definition of RPE and ETL, explanation of the nature and use of the scales, opportunity for participants to ask questions about the scales), to help them to link their full exercise stimulus range with their full perceptual response range. These precautions were taken because some authors have suggested that a lack of information on RPE and ETL scales may lead to a slight but not significant overestimation of perceptually-based values during GXT (Skinner, Hutsler, Bergsteinova & Buskirk, 1973; Coquart et al., 2009). Moreover, other authors have suggested reading the instructions on the scales (Gearhart, Becque, Hutchins & Palm, 2004), and performing a memory or exercise anchoring procedure (Robertson, 2004) before each test. The cyclists were informed they should maintain their normal diet during the last two days preceding the test, refrain from alcohol consumption 24 hr before the Effects of Psychological Factors on RPE/ETL 365 test, and caffeine consumption on the test day. These precautions were performed to avoid a possible influence of these factors on RPE and ETL (Hadjicharalambous et al., 2006; Lecoultre & Schutz, 2009; Lima-Silva et al., in press). Similarly, the cyclists were asked to avoid strenuous physical activity during the two days before the test and not to exercise on the test day. They were also informed that a urine sample could be collected to determine if performance-enhancing drugs had been taken. No urine analysis was completed, but this warning was intended to avoid the possibility of doped cyclists participating in the study. On the morning of the tests, the cyclists’ cardiovascular health was evaluated with a 12-lead electrocardiogram. Anthropometric data, including height, body mass and percentage of body fat were also measured. Then, all cyclists individually filled out the psychological questionnaires (for about 1.5 hr). Before the test, an identical standardized meal of approximately 3027 kJ (74.4% carbohydrate, 22.6% protein, 3.0% fat) was consumed by each participant, between 11:30 a.m. and 12:30 p.m., within the laboratory and only water was drunk. This meal included pasta, turkey, yogurt and apple. A digestion period of at least 2 hr was then respected. Just before GXT on a cycle ergometer, the seat and handlebar heights were adjusted by each cyclist. GXT was preceded by a 3 min resting period (on the cycle ergometer) followed by an 8 min warm-up at 100W, then a 5 min passive recovery period. At the end of the warm-up period (during the last 30 s of the warm-up period), overall RPE and ETL were checked so that the participants would have a reference for the perceptually-based values. During GXT, the initial power output was set at 150W for 4 min then increased by 50W every 4 min, until the power output reached 300W. The duration of 4 min was chosen so that the participant could integrate the different signals shaping the perceptually-based values. Once the power output reached 300W, an increment of 25W was administered every 2 min until volitional exhaustion (i.e., participant failed to maintain a pedalling rate above 70 rpm), to more accurately determine MAP and limit the exercise duration. The cyclists were instructed to develop the highest possible level of power. RPE and ETL were collected during the last 30 s of the first 4 stages and at the end of the test (for RPE). The presentation order of the RPE and ETL scales was counterbalanced among the participants. During GXT, the respiratory variables (i.e., oxygen uptake and respiratory exchange ratio) and heart rate were recorded to check the exhaustion at MAP. A blood lactate concentration was determined immediately at the GXT end, with the same objective. Blood sampling took 15–20 s The primary criterion used to define the exhaustion was a plateau phenomenon in oxygen uptake (oxygen uptake increase < 2.1 mL.kg-1.min-1 between the last and penultimate stages). If this criterion was not noted, exhaustion was verified by at least 3 of the 4 following criteria: 1) a respiratory exchange ratio ≥ 1.1; 2) reaching a heart rate within ± 10 bpm of the age-predicted maximum (220—age); 3) blood lactate concentration after test cessation > 8 mmol.L-1; and 4) RPE ≥ 18 at test cessation. The cyclists were not verbally encouraged until after they had entirely completed the first 4 stages of GXT. The temperature of the air-conditioned room was always maintained between 20 and 24 °C. The tests were conducted at the same time of day and were carried out under medical supervision. 366 Coquart et al. Statistical Analysis Individual linear regressions between the perceptually-based values (i.e., RPE and ETL) and exercise intensity (expressed as a percentage of maximal aerobic power, MAP) were calculated from the perceptually-based values obtained at 150, 200, 250 and 300W. From these regressions, RPE and ETL at 25, 50, 75 and 100% MAP were computed, to avoid the differing physical ability levels of the ­participants being a confusing variable in the research. The correlations between these perceptually-based values and the scores obtained for the different psychological factors were examined with the Bravais-Pearson test and quantified by Pearson correlation coefficients. Statistical analysis was performed using Statistica software (version 6.0, Statsoft, Tulsa, USA). P values < .05 were considered statistically significant. Results The mean power output at 25, 50, 75 and 100% MAP corresponded to 86 ± 6W, 173 ± 12W, 259 ± 18W and 345 ± 25W, respectively. The Pearson correlation coefficients between the psychological factors and the perceptually-based values during the different workloads are presented in Tables 3 and 4. The results of the Bravais-Pearson test showed a significant correlation between psychological resistance and RPE at 25, 50 and 75% MAP (p < .05, r ≤ -.51, Table 3). Similarly, psychological endurance was significantly correlated with RPE from 25% MAP up to 50% MAP (p < .05, r ≤ -.51, Table 3). A significant correlation was also found between leadership and RPE at 25 and 50% MAP (p < .05, r ≤ -.48, Table 3). The results showed a significant correlation between psychological endurance and ETL, but only from 75% MAP (p < .05, r ≤ -.46, Table 4). Discussion The purpose of this study was to examine the relationships between psychological factors and perceptually-based values (RPE and ETL), at different intensities during GXT while cycling. Our results showed no significant relationship between anxiety and RPE/ ETL (Tables 3 and 4), whereas Morgan (1973, 1994) found higher RPE in anxious participants. This lack of similarity may be linked to differences in the anxiety scores of the recruited participants. Indeed, in Morgan’s study (1973), RPE were compared between high anxiety participants and low anxiety participants, whereas in the current study, the scores on the STAI were homogenous. Consequently, the greater heterogeneity in anxiety scores in the Morgan study (1973) most likely led to a significant correlation between anxiety scores and the perceptually-based values. Moreover, some authors have reported that the anxious participants had lower physical fitness than the subjects who were less anxious (Morgan, Roberts & Feinerman, 1971). Therefore, to avoid a possible influence of the fitness level on anxiety, we suggested expressing the exercise intensity in terms of percentage Effects of Psychological Factors on RPE/ETL 367 Table 3 Pearson Correlation Coefficients Between the Ratings of Perceived Exertion and Several Psychological Factors According to the Exercise Intensity Ratings of Perceived Exertion (Borg, 1970) State anxiety Trait anxiety Extraversion-introversion Neuroticism-stability Overall self-esteem Social self-esteem Family self-esteem Professionnal self-esteem Intrinsic motivation to know Intrinsic motivation to accomplish things Intrinsic motivation to experience stimulation Extrinsic motivation by an identified regulation Extrinsic motivation by introjected regulation Extrinsic motivation by external regulation Absence of motivation Psychological resistance Psychological endurance Desire for success Social desirability Dynamism Competitiveness Activity control Risk-taking Emotional control Aggressiveness Sociability Cooperation Acceptance of a judgment Leadership 25% MAP -.03 -.05 -.01 -.17 .03 .04 .07 -.12 .14 .13 -.25 -.26 -.29 -.39 .35 -.51 * -.59 * -.10 .14 -.24 -.16 -.01 .15 -.06 -.32 .28 -.16 -.27 -.48 * 50% MAP .04 -.06 -.02 -.20 .09 .03 .21 -.15 .19 .20 -.12 -.30 -.30 -.40 .27 -.57 * -.59 * -.09 .06 -.22 -.19 -.04 .12 -.10 -.25 .29 -.23 -.27 -.50 * 75% MAP .16 -.07 -.04 -.19 .19 .01 .41 -.16 .22 .26 .17 -.28 -.22 -.29 .04 -.51 * -.39 -.04 -.13 -.11 -.20 -.11 .01 -.15 -.03 .21 -.30 -.19 -.38 100% MAP .21 -.04 -.03 -.09 .20 -.02 .43 * -.09 .16 .21 .40 -.14 -.03 -.04 -.22 -.20 -.01 .03 -.27 .06 -.12 -.12 -.10 -.13 .20 .03 -.23 -.02 -.07 MAP: Maximal aerobic power, * Significant correlation between the perceptually-based values and the psychological factors. of maximal aerobic power, as opposed to Morgan’s studies where power was expressed in watts. However, it has already been stated that highly anxious participants may reduce their state of anxiety during high exercise intensities, and inversely the exercise may increase the state of anxiety in low anxiety participants (O’Sullivan, 1984). Consequently, the interaction of psychological state and trait may influence the perceptually-based values, and provide an additional interpretation of the results 368 Coquart et al. Table 4 Pearson Correlation Coefficients Between the Estimated Time Limit and Several Psychological Factors According to the Exercise Intensity Estimated Time Limit (Garcin et al., 1999) State anxiety Trait anxiety Extraversion-introversion Neuroticism-stability Overall self-esteem Social self-esteem Familyself-esteem Professionnal self-esteem Intrinsic motivation to know Intrinsic motivation to accomplish things Intrinsic motivation to experience stimulation Extrinsic motivation by an identified regulation Extrinsic motivation by introjected regulation Extrinsic motivation by external regulation Absence of motivation Psychological resistance Psychological endurance Desire for success Social desirability Dynamism Competitiveness Activity control Risk-taking Emotional control Aggressiveness Sociability Cooperation Acceptance of a judgment Leadership 25% MAP -.04 -.04 -.05 -.36 .28 .31 .25 .28 -.26 -.08 .35 .15 .04 -.01 .04 .34 -.08 .17 .45 * .04 -.08 .02 -.24 .37 -.13 .08 .16 .30 .26 50% MAP .14 .05 -.14 -.40 .27 .24 .36 .30 -.12 .08 .29 .23 -.06 -.13 .14 .09 -.31 -.02 .31 -.05 -.14 -.04 -.35 .22 -.09 .10 .08 .25 .34 75% MAP .29 .13 -.20 -.34 .17 .10 .38 .25 .07 .24 .14 .24 -.15 -.22 .20 -.20 -.46 * -.22 .06 -.13 -.16 -.11 -.36 -.01 -.02 .09 -.04 .12 .32 100% MAP .34 .17 -.20 -.24 .08 -.01 .33 .17 .19 .31 .02 .21 -.19 -.25 .20 -.37 -.49 * -.32 -.11 -.16 -.15 -.13 -.31 -.16 .03 .07 -.11 .01 .26 MAP: Maximal aerobic power, * Significant correlation between the perceptually-based values and the psychological factors. (O’Sullivan, 1984). In other words, participants may characteristically function in one way (trait-dependent), but under certain circumstances (e.g., during a high exercise intensity), may change the way they characteristically function, thereby becoming state-dependant (O’Sullivan, 1984). This may be an explication of the lack of significant correlations between the anxiety (state and trait) and the perceptually-based values. Effects of Psychological Factors on RPE/ETL 369 The lack of a significant link compared with Morgan’s studies (1973, 1994) may be explained by the difference in the population studied. In the current study, all the participants were experienced and well-trained cyclists, to facilitate the assessment of their effort perception and feelings of fatigue with the perceptually-based values (Pandolf, 1983). Conversely, the participants in Morgan’s studies (1973, 1994) were not experienced or well-trained. Consequently, the higher physical ability level and greater sporting experience, among the cyclists in the current study, probably produced lower anxiety and hence a lack of significant correlation. Based on the experiments carried out by Morgan (1973, 1994), several authors proposed that extraversion was inversely related to RPE (O’Sullivan, 1984; Pandolf, 1983; Robertson & Noble, 1997). According to these authors, this effect may be linked with a higher pain tolerance in the extraverted participants. However, other studies did not indicate a link between extraversion and the perceptuallybased values (Garcin et al., 2006). Similarly, Hall et al. (2005) showed that the negative correlation between the extraversion and the RPE was significant only when the exercise intensity was lower than or equal to the ventilatory threshold (p < .05; Pearson product–moment correlation coefficients between -.32 and -.51). According to these authors, the difference in results with Morgan’s works (1973, 1994) may be explained by the active or retrospective RPE measures. Indeed, in Morgan’s studies, RPE were given immediately following each workload (i.e., during the recovery period: retrospective RPE measure). Inversely, in the current study and other studies (Hall et al., 2005; Garcin et al., 2006), RPE were measured when the participants were still exercising (i.e., during the effort period: active RPE measure). These methodological differences may explain the difference in results. The present study suggests thus that RPE measured during exercise is not related to extraversion. Similarly to anxiety, the lack of significant correlation between extraversion and RPE/ETL may be explained by the population studied. The participants in Morgan’s studies (1973, 1994) were most likely less experienced and not as well trained as the cyclists in the current study, limiting the possible effect of extraversion on RPE and ETL. Moreover, similarly to anxiety, the participants in the current study perhaps had close extraversion scores, whereas the participants in the Morgan study had more heterogeneous scores, producing a significant correlation. The link between neuroticism and RPE/ETL is a controversial topic. Based on Morgan’s studies (1973, 1994), some authors suggested that neurotic participants presented higher RPE than nonneurotic participants (O’Sullivan, 1984; Watt & Grove, 1993), whereas more recent experiments found no significant effect (Garcin et al., 2006; Hall et al., 2005). Garcin et al. (2006) showed that ETL was significantly correlated to the degree of neuroticism at the ventilatory threshold. This result was not confirmed in the current study at different intensities (i.e., 25, 50, 75 and 100% MAP). In the study by Garcin et al. (2006), although the participants had different athletic backgrounds (i.e., runners, sprinters or handball players), all were tested while running. ETL may have been influenced by neuroticism in the handball players, because they have little experience of the exercise mode used during the test. Inversely, in the current study, the participants were all cyclists who performed GXT on a cycle ergometer, thus no significant correlation between neuroticism and ETL was noticed. 370 Coquart et al. Motivation for exercise can be expected to link with RPE (Pandolf, 1983). Nevertheless, when the tests are performed in a laboratory, this link may be reduced (Borg, 1977). To decrease the variance in RPE from motivation, the recruitment of trained participants is recommended (Pandolf, 1983). Therefore, the lack of a significant relationship between motivation and the perceptually-based values is probably linked to the fact that the current study was performed with trained cyclists in a laboratory. Although psychological resistance, which may be defined as the capacity to resist undesirable events, criticism and stress (Thill, 1983), does not seem to correlate with ETL; the results suggested that RPE was significantly lower in cyclists with a high psychological resistance, especially at light intensities. This result confirms the hypothesis proposed by Hall et al. (2005), which supposes that some psychological factors directly influence RPE, but only during weaker workloads. In accordance with these studies, when the internal signals (e.g., coming from cardiovascular and respiratory systems) were strong (because the exercise intensity was high), the link between certain psychological factors such as the psychological resistance and RPE could restricted. Cyclists with high psychological resistance are probably able to continue physical tasks over a longer time period than cyclists with a weak psychological resistance. If this is confirmed, high psychological resistance could decrease the sensations of stress and discomfort which shape effort perception. Because RPE is correlated to psychological resistance, the prescription of exercise intensity according to the RPE scale, as is sometimes proposed in training or rehabilitation programs, may be inadequate for participants with a high psychological resistance, since they develop higher exercise intensity than the control participants. This higher exercise intensity is not necessarily influenced from a psychological point of view, but it may produce a physiological overload, which must be taken into account in the scheduling of training programs, to avoid a possible physiological overtraining state. This study shows that the cyclists with high psychological endurance (i.e., persistent, strong-minded, and stubborn) produced lower RPE when the exercise intensity was lower than 50% MAP, and rated lower for ETL toward the end of the exercise (i.e., from 75% MAP), compared with cyclists with lower psychological endurance. Thus, cyclists with high psychological endurance would perceive lower exercise intensity as light and feel that they could endure more when the exercise intensity was high. This means that high psychological endurance would decrease the effort perception at light intensity, and would increase the estimated exhaustion time at high intensity. The fact that ETL was lower at moderate to high exercise intensities may be explained by the specificity of training programs and competitions that require road cycling. Indeed, according to Faria, Parker and Faria (2005), in well-trained competitive cyclists, the competitions are performed at approximately more than 50% of maximal oxygen uptake during 75% of the competition time. Therefore, the cyclists are accustomed to performing moderate to high exercise intensities for a long time. Consequently, it may be supposed that, the more a cyclist carries out long training sessions at high exercise intensity, the greater is his psychological endurance, and the lower the ETL. Social desirability is often defined as a tendency for participants to provide responses that they believe to be consistent with social norms and expectations Effects of Psychological Factors on RPE/ETL 371 (Adams et al., 2005). The present study is the first to have examined the relationship between social desirability and RPE/ETL; it is thus impossible to compare our results. However, Hardy, Hall and Prestholdt (1989) have already suggested that team mate presence influenced the RPE. In their study, RPE were lower during an exercise on a cycle ergometer at low intensities (i.e., 25–50% maximal oxygen uptake) when a team mate was present (Hardy et al., 1989). Inversely, Sylva, Byrd and Mangum (1990) did not find a significant effect of team mate presence on RPE in high level athletes. As suggested by Hardy et al. (1989), this influence could be reduced in trained athletes). Complementary studies must thus be performed to know the relationship between social desirability, team mate presence and the perceptually-based values. The cyclists who had high leadership scores rated lower for RPE than the cyclists with low scores, but only at low intensities (less than 50% MAP). According to Rejeski (1981), when physiological signals are strong, the link between the nonphysiological factors and RPE may be restricted. In this case, it can be hypothesized that, above 75% MAP, exercise intensity produces a strong reaction of the cardio-respiratory and muscular systems, indicated by strong physiological signals such as high heart rate and ventilation levels which the cyclist could not ignore. Consequently, when the exercise intensity is high (above 75% MAP for leadership), the psychological traits probably explain less than 33% of the variance in RPE, as proposed by Morgan (1973). Inversely, when the exercise intensity is very low, the psychological traits make a strong contribution to the perceptually-based values. Nevertheless, leadership does not seem related to ETL. To our knowledge, no one has performed an experiment to study the relationship between competitiveness and RPE/ETL, probably because a valid English translation of the personality questionnaire for athletes does not exist. The present study suggests that competitiveness does not correlate to the perceptually-based values in the male competitive cyclists during GXT in their athletic domain. Nevertheless, this result must be confirmed by further studies with different populations and various exercise modalities. It would be interesting to develop a valid English translation of the Thill personality questionnaire (1983) because this questionnaire is especially adapted to athletes. Morgan (1973) recommended the use of a test with random workloads, rather than GXT, to measure the perceptually-based values. However, GXT was used in the current study because it is very often executed in clinical and sporting settings, and it permits the assessment of the physical fitness. Nevertheless, GXT might inflate correlations due to the biased effect of knowing that the workload is increased (Morgan, 1973). Consequently, the lack of significant correlations between some psychological factors (e.g., motivation) and the perceptually-based values (i.e., RPE and ETL) may be linked to GXT, used in the current study. To avoid this effect, it would be preferable to use a test with randomized workloads where the main purpose is not to assess physical fitness, but to measure RPE and ETL. The present study demonstrated that some psychological factors such as leadership, psychological resistance and endurance are linked to RPE and/or ETL in trained athletes during GXT in cycling. Based on the concept of teleoanticipation (Ulmer, 1996), authors suggest that the brain calculates (before and continuously during exercise) the metabolic cost required to entirely complete a given exercise (St Clair Gibson, Lambert, Lambert, Hampson & Noakes, 2001). 372 Coquart et al. According to this estimation, the brain computes how this will be influenced by the prevailing environmental conditions and the current physical state. However, this overall estimation, which can be evaluated by the RPE scale, is also influenced by past exercises (Ulmer, 1996). The participants in the current study were all well-experienced in cycling. Therefore, they should determine RPE and ETL with only a slight impact of the psychological states and/or traits. However, the results show that, even with experienced cyclists, the psychological states and/or traits are correlated to the perceptually-based values. However, this link was more or less significant according to the imposed exercise intensity and the psychological variable which was studied. The present study indicates that a link exists between the perceptually-based values and some psychological factors. This result must be considered when a training program based on RPE or ETL is proposed. Indeed, to prescribe the same adapted exercise intensity (e.g., 70% maximal oxygen uptake) from RPE or ETL values in two cyclists who have the same maximal oxygen uptake, it is necessary to perform a prior GXT to precisely determine the RPE or ETL value corresponding to the required exercise intensity, because these cyclists do not necessarily have the same psychological states or traits. References Adams, S.A., Matthews, C.E., Ebbeling, C.B., Moore, C.G., Cunningham, J.E., Fulton, J., & Hebert, J.R. (2005). The effect of social desirability and social approval on selfreports of physical activity. American Journal of Epidemiology, 161, 389–398. PubMed doi:10.1093/aje/kwi054 Borg, G. (1970). Perceived exertion as an indicator of somatic stress. Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 2, 92–98. PubMed Borg, G. (1977). General introduction: psychophysiological studies of the three effort continua. In G. Borg (Ed.), Physical work and effort (pp. 1–11). New York, USA: Pergamon press. Brière, N.M., Vallerand, R.J., Blais, M.R., & Pelletier, L.G. (1995). Développement et validation d’une mesure de motivation intrinséque, extrinséque et d’amotivation en contexte sportif: l’échelle de motivation dans les sports (E.M.S.). International Journal of Sport Psychology, 26, 465–489. Coquart, J.B.J., Legrand, R., Robin, S., Duhamel, A., Matran, R., & Garcin, M. (2009). Influence of successive bouts of fatiguing exercise on perceptual and physiological markers during an incremental exercise test. Psychophysiology, 46, 209–216. PubMed doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00717.x Eston, R., Faulkner, J., St Clair Gibson, A., Noakes, T., & Parfitt, G. (2007). The effect of antecedent fatiguing activity on the relationship between perceived exertion and physiological activity during a constant load exercise task. Psychophysiology, 44, 779–786. PubMed doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00558.x Eysenck, H.J., & Eysenck, S.B.G. (1975). Manual for the Eysenck Personnality Questionnaire, junior and adult. San Diego, USA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service. Faria, E.W., Parker, D.L., & Faria, I.E. (2005). The science of cycling: factors affecting performance - part 2. Sports Medicine (Auckland, N.Z.), 35, 313–337. PubMed doi:10.2165/00007256-200535040-00003 Garcin, M., Mille-Hamard, L., Duhamel, A., Boudin, F., Reveillere, C., Billat, V., . . .. (2006). Factors associated with perceived exertion and estimated time limit at lactate threshold. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 103, 51–66. PubMed doi:10.2466/pms.103.1.51-66 Effects of Psychological Factors on RPE/ETL 373 Garcin, M., Vandewalle, H., & Monod, H. (1999). A new rating scale of perceived exertion based on subjective estimation of exhaustion time: a preliminary study. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 20, 40–43. PubMed doi:10.1055/s-2007-971089 Gearhart, R.F., Becque, M.D., Hutchins, M.D., & Palm, C.M. (2004). Comparison of memory and combined exercise and memory anchoring procedures on ratings of perceived exertion during short duration near-peak intensity cycle ergometer exercise. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 99, 775–784. PubMed Hadjicharalambous, M., Georgiades, E., Kilduff, L.P., Turner, A.P., Tsofliou, F., & Pitsiladis, Y.P. (2006). Influence of caffeine on perception of effort, metabolism and exercise performance following a high-fat meal. Journal of Sports Sciences, 24, 875–887. PubMed doi:10.1080/02640410500249399 Hall, E.E., Ekkekakis, P., & Petruzzello, S.J. (2005). Is the relationship of RPE to psychological factors intensity-dependent? Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 37, 1365–1373. PubMed doi:10.1249/01.mss.0000174897.25739.3c Hardy, C.J., Hall, E.G., & Prestholdt, P.H. (1989). The mediational role of social influence in the perception of exertion. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 8, 88–103. Lecoultre, V., & Schutz, Y. (2009). Effect of a small dose of alcohol on the endurance performance of trained cyclists. Alcohol and Alcoholism (Oxford, Oxfordshire), 44, 278–283. PubMed doi:10.1093/alcalc/agn108 Lima-Silva, A.E., Pires, F.O., Bertuzzi, R.C., Lira, F.S., Casarini, D., & Kiss, M.A. (in press). Low carbohydrate diet affects the oxygen uptake on-kinetics and rating of perceived exertion in high intensity exercise. Psychophysiology. PubMed Le Scanff, C. (2003). Manuel de psychologie du sport. Tome 2: les bases psychologiques de la performance sportive. Paris, France: Revue EPS Morgan, W.P. (1973). Psychological factors influencing perceived exertion. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 5, 97–103. PubMed Morgan, W.P. (1994). Psychological components of effort sense. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 26, 1071–1077. PubMed Morgan, W.P., Roberts, J.A., & Feinerman, A.D. (1971). Psychologic effect of acute physical activity. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 52, 422–425. PubMed Noakes, T.D., Snow, R.J., & Febbraio, M.A. (2004). Linear relationship between the perception of effort and the duration of constant load exercise that remains. Journal of Applied Physiology, 96, 1571–1573. PubMed doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.01124.2003 Noble, B.J., & Robertson, R.J. (1996). Perceived exertion. Champaign, USA: Human Kinetics. O’Sullivan, S.B. (1984). Perceived exertion. A review. Physical Therapy, 64, 343–346. PubMed Pandolf, K.B. (1983). Advances in the study and application of perceived exertion. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 11, 118–158. PubMed doi:10.1249/00003677-19830100000005 Rejeski, J.W. (1981). The perception of exertion: a social psychophysiological integration. Journal of Sport Psychology, 4, 305–320. Robertson, R.J. (2004). Perceived exertion for practitioners: rating effort with the OMNI picture system. Champaign, USA: Human Kinetics. Robertson, R.J., & Noble, B.J. (1997). Perception of physical exertion: methods, mediators, and applications. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 25, 407–452. PubMed doi:10.1249/00003677-199700250-00017 Spielberger, C.D., Gorsuch, R.L., & Lushene, R.E. (1970). Manual for the state trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, USA: Consulting Psychologists Press. St Clair Gibson, A., Lambert, E. V., Lambert, M. I., Hampson, D. B., & Noakes, T. D. (2001). Exercise and Fatigue-Control Mechanisms. International SportMedicine Journal, 2, no page. 374 Coquart et al. Skinner, J.S., Hutsler, R., Bergsteinova, V., & Buskirk, E.R. (1973). The validity and reliability of a rating scale of perceived exertion. Medicine and Science in Sports, 5, 94–96. PubMed Sylva, M., Byrd, R., & Mangum, M. (1990). Effects of social influence and sex on rating of perceived exertion in exercising elite athletes. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 70, 591–594. doi:10.2466/pms.1990.70.2.591 Thill, E. (1983). Questionnaire de personnalité pour sportifs (Q.P.S.). Paris, France: Les Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée. Thill, E., & Brenot, J. (1985). Le modèle de mesure des traits reconsidéré. Validité des interprétations descriptives et prédictives d’un questionnaire de personnalité. Revue de Psychologie Appliquée, 35, 175–200. Ulmer, H.V. (1996). Concept of an extracellular regulation of muscular metabolic rate during heavy exercise in humans by psychophysiological feedback. Experientia, 52, 416–420. PubMed doi:10.1007/BF01919309 Watt, B., & Grove, R. (1993). Perceived exertion: antecedents and applications. Sports Medicine (Auckland, N.Z.), 15, 225–241. PubMed doi:10.2165/00007256-199315040-00002