_____________________________________________________________________ i Human Communication A Journal of the Pacific and Asian Communication Association Volume 10, Number 3 Human Communication publishes experimental, theoretical and speculative articles in the artistic, humanistic and scientific areas of communication. A blind review process is used with editorial associates reading and reviewing each manuscript. Manuscripts must conform to the APA Publications Manual (5th edition, 2001). Articles must be sent in Microsoft Word on a disk, with two hard copies, to Dr. Virginia Richmond, Department of Communication Studies, The University of Alabama at Birmingham, 1055 BLDG, Suite 117, 1530 3rd Ave, S., Birmingham, AL 35210-4480, USA. Each article should include a carefully planned abstract. Human Communication is free to download. Scholars are free to copy and distribute this journal to their students and colleagues under the “fair use” guideline of U.S. copyright law. EDITORIAL BOARD Editor Virginia Richmond University of Alabama at Birmingham, USA Layout and Design Jonathan Howard Amsbary, University of Alabama at Birmingham, USA PACA Liaison James C. McCroskey, University of Alabama at Birmingham, USA © 2007, Pacific and Asian Communication Association _____________________________________________________________________ ii Table of Contents __________ 163 Participation in Decision-making and Job Satisfaction: Ideal and Reality for Male and Female University Faculty in the United States Jerry L. Allen and Ben B. Judd __________ 187 Mayor, Cheerleader, Lawyer, or Juggler? An Exploratory Study of CAO Leadership Metaphors and Challenges G.L. Forward and Kathleen Czech __________ 205 Revisiting Teacher Immediacy in the HBCU and PWI Context: Do Teacher Immediacy and Interpersonal Communication Satisfaction Influence Student Retention? Mary L. Rucker and Joanna M. Davis-Showell __________ 219 Communication Experiences of Korean Expatriates in the U.S.: A Study of Cross-Cultural Adaptation Yang_Soo Kim __________ 239 Nollywood: The Influence of the Nigerian Movie Industry on African Culture Uchenna Onuzulike __________ 251 The Impact of Gender on Instructor Nonverbal Communication from the Perspectives of Learner Affect and Learners’ Perceptions of Instructor Melinda Findley and Narissra Punyanunt-Carter ___________________________________________________________________ iii __________ 267 Leader-Member Exchange and Cooperative Communication Between Group Members: Replication of Lee (1997, 2001) Study on Malaysia Respondents Hassan Abu Bakar, Che Su Mustaffa, and Bahtiar Mohamad __________ 285 Proxemics and its Relationship with Malay Architecture Abdul Muati Amad, Arbai’e Sujud, and Hamisah Zaharah Hasan __________ 299 Psychologically Abusive Relationships and Self-Disclosure Orientations Jacqueline A. Shirley, William G. Powers, and Chris R. Sawyer __________ 313 Extroversion versus Similarity: An Exploration of Factors Influencing Communication Accuracy of Social Cognitions David Dugas, William G. Powers, and Chris Sawyer __________ 321 Communicating with College Students about STIs: Assessing Message Effectiveness and Preferred Source and Channel Marian L. Houser, Michael E. Burns, and Nicholas R. Driver __________ 339 Audience Reactions to Negative Campaigns Spots in the 2005 German National Elections: The Case of Two Ads Called “The Ball” Jürgen Maier and Michaela Maier __________ ____________________________________________________________________ iv __________ 357 A Comparison of Role Models Among Japanese, Korean and Chinese University Students Masao Nakabayashi, Yumi Hasegawa, and Yang Yi __________ 387 The Relationship Between Self-Deception and Extrinsic-Personal Religiosity Amanda J. Norris, Larry Powell, and Mark Hickson, III __________ 395 Laughing Before Takeoff: Humor, Sex, and the Preflight Safety Briefing Jason S. Wrench, Brenda Millhouse, David Sharp __________ Human Communication. A Publication of the Pacific and Asian Communication Association. Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 153 – 186. Participation in Decision-making and Job Satisfaction: Ideal and Reality for Male and Female University Faculty in the United States Jerry L. Allen University of New Haven Ben B. Judd University of New Haven Jerry L. Allen (Ph.D., 1978, Southern Illinois University-Carbondale) is professor and chair, and Ben B. Judd (Ph.D., University of Texas-Arlington, 1978) is professor in the Department of Communication and Marketing at the University of New Haven. Address correspondence to the first author in the Department of Communication, University of New Haven, West Haven, CT 06516 (jlallen@newhaven.edu) (203-932-7132 telephone, 203931-6055 fax). Participation in Decision-Making 164 Abstract Communication literature has established that participative decision-making (PDM) influences job satisfaction. Discrimination against women in the workplace has also been well documented. This study investigated differences in how male and female faculty members in the U. S. view opportunities to participate in decision-making, and how such participation differently affects their job satisfaction. Results obtained from PDM and job satisfaction scales administered to male and female faculty indicate that female faculty members do not differ from males in desire for PDM but report lower actual PDM and lower job satisfaction than males. Contrary to expectation, it was found that increasing levels of actual PDM results in greater job satisfaction for female, but not for male faculty. This suggests that women faculty may view the role of PDM differently than men. Although women had taught for fewer years, held lower faculty rank and fewer had tenure, they had a slightly higher rate of refereed publications, and none of these status indices confounded the PDM-job satisfaction findings. Implications for diversity in the academic workplace are discussed. 165 Jerry L. Allen and Ben B. Judd Introduction “The rationale for extensive faculty participation in institutional decision-making rests on reasoning drawn from generic organization theory related to a broad range of organizations and on reasoning related more directly to the specifics of the faculty role in higher education” (Floyd, 1985, p. 1). Employee participation in organizational decision-making in the United States has been found to be directly related to increased job satisfaction and performance (Jablin, 1982; Redding, 1973; Richmond & McCroskey, 1979; Richmond, McCroskey, Davis, & Koontz, 1980; Richmond, Wagner, & McCroskey, 1983; Tannenbaum & Schmidt, 1958). Also, in the U. S., Faculty expertise in subject matter has historically been used to justify faculty supremacy in academic decision-making, and faculty habitually question the legitimacy of institutional decisions made without faculty input. However, faced with the tasks of controlling costs, increasing productivity, diversifying work forces, and streamlining operations, college and university administrations have resorted to tenure and promotion quotas, hiring and wage freezes, program and personnel cuts, and increased managerial decision-making based upon fiscal accountability in addition to educational efficacy (Kolodny, 1998). The support and cooperation of all constituencies is essential if U. S. institutions of higher learning are going to successfully compete for qualified students and necessary funding. However, in the face of the Yeshiva court decision which restricted faculty participation in unions and structural changes in institutional governance, many faculty members feel that they are being asked to support crucial organizational decisions that affect the quality of their work without opportunities for meaningful input. Analysts have noted that faculty disillusionment is reflected by a shift from concern with issues such as curriculum, class size, faculty load, etc. to issues related to job security and greater access to policy making power and autonomy (Callan, 1993; Floyd, 1985; Getman & Franke, 1988; Kolodny, 1998; Lazerson, 1997; Scott, 1996; Williams & Zirkel, 1989). In a 2000 report of the American Council on Education only 37 percent of the university administrators surveyed said that they would rate the level of faculty morale on their campuses as "excellent" or "very good." The lack of a participative role in organizational decisions would be expected to affect the job satisfaction and commitment to organizational goals for faculty generally, but such effects could be multiplicative in the case of female faculty. Numerous observers have indicated that sex or gender-role stereotyping often limits women’s participation in organizational decision-making. Given the current state of higher education, and efforts to dismantle affirmative action programs, it is propitious to examine women faculty member’s participation in decision-making in higher education. The purpose of the study reported here was to determine how male and female faculty members in the U. S. view their participation in university decision-making. Given previous conclusions that women have lower expectations and goals, differences in female and male faculty members' reports of desired and actual levels of participation in decisionmaking, and their affects on job satisfaction were also investigated. The results of this study have wider implications related to women’s participation in decision-making in the work force generally. Women's Participation in Higher Education In 1993, the annual survey of the American Council on Education revealed that women had less than "adequate" representation in decision-making in higher education, and some Participation in Decision-Making 166 claimed that estimates of female participation are grossly exaggerated (Leatherman). In 1981, the National Advisory Council on Women's Educational Progress concluded, "...the glass remains half-empty" relative to the full participation of females in academia in the U. S., and over the last two decades, data has shown that fewer women hold senior faculty ranks; they publish less; they are less likely to receive tenure; and they are paid lower salaries than men (Cole & Zuckerman, 1987; Davis, 2001; Fogg, 2003; Hickson, Stacks, & Amsbary, 1989, 1992, 1993; Moses, 1997; Sandler 1986, 1993; Schneider, 1998; West & Curtis, 2006; Williams , 2000a; Wilson, 2004). In the field of communication alone, women authors have been found to be underrepresented proportionally to the number of female faculty members, and few women have edited or served on the editorial boards of communication journals (Bodon, Powell, & Hickson, 1999; Cooper, Stewart, & Friedley, 1989; Davis, 2001; Hickson, Stacks, & Amsbary, 1992). In 1993 only 12 percent of the institutions of higher education in the United States had women presidents, and only four percent of the presidents at doctorate-granting institutions were women (Leatherman). By 1998, the percentage of women presidents had increased to only 19 percent, and recent data reflects that the numbers remain at that level (Dowdall, 2003). Most of the increase was at twoyear colleges, and fewer than a dozen women were presidents of doctorate-granting institutions (Lively, 2000). Data presented in the AAUP Report on Gender Equity Indicators 2006 reports that in 2005-06 women held only 39 percent of the full-time faculty positions at American colleges and universities, and that only 23 percent of the full professors were women. A disproportionate number of women teach full-time at doctoral-level and four year schools—34 percent are women to 66 percent men.. In 2005-06 women held only 31 percent of tenured academic posts in the U. S., and women occupied 52 percent of the non-tenure track positions (West & Curtis, 2006). Across academic departments, only ten percent of the chairs were women in 1993 (Gmelch and Miskin), and the situation is little better in the field of communication. DeWine (1987) reported that only 20 percent of the chairs of communication departments were women. Over the last two decades, a considerable body of communication research been concerned with differences in the ways that men and women communicate in the U. S., and conclusions from that research has supported that social and occupational outcomes in the U. S. often turn on perceptions directly related to expectations attached to those differences (Bem, 1993; Ivy & Backlund, 2000; Maltz & Borker, 1982; Mulac, Bradac, and Gibbons, 2001; Tannen, 1990). In 1974, Rosen and Jerdee in a study of 1500 Harvard Business Review readers reported that there was a prevailing bias toward sex stereotyping which precluded women's full participation in managerial decision-making. Kovach found an almost identical pattern of sex stereotyping among male and female senior college business students in 1985. The curious thing was that female respondents also stereotyped women so as to deny them full participation in decision-making. In a 1992 replication, Allen and O'Mara verified that such stereotyping still existed. Women were denied full participation in decision-making by both male and female middle managers in varied corporations. Others have also observed that gender-related differences have been significant factors in women not being placed in decision-making roles (Burrell, Dononhue & Allen, 1988; Dowdall, 2003; Guyot, 2001; Kushell & Newton, 1986; Richmond, McCroskey, & Roach, 1997; Weider-Hatfield, 1987); Winter & Green, 1987). LeClair and Steinblom (1989) concluded that sex-role socialization, organizational structures, and individual perceptions of men and women explained the dearth of women's participation in organizational decision-making roles. When it comes to women's participation in organizational leadership, Reardon (1995) argued that the few women who have positions allowing for 167 Jerry L. Allen and Ben B. Judd participation in decision-making “are supposed to look like they are not using power” (p. 124). In fact, it is argued that when it comes to advancing women into decision-making positions in higher education, interpretations related to more a more responsive communication style and “softer” presentation trumps “objective” qualifications, such as previous experience, academic accomplishments, etc. (Dowdell, 2003). Participation in organizational leadership and decisionmaking by women faculty members is likely more acerbic. Title IX legislation was passed by the U. S. congress in 1972 for the expressed purpose of prohibiting sex discrimination in educational programs receiving federal funding. However, currently, efforts are underway to dismantle affirmative action apparatus in higher education. Though not supported by prima facie evidence, the argument has often made that women and minorities are benefiting disproportionally to their qualifications. Further, the argument is advanced that women have been precluded from greater organizational advancement by mindsplits between mutually exclusive goals related to career and motherhood, or femininity and intellectual achievement. It has been contended, that women often sacrifice career goals for socially acceptable goals, and therefore lack the motivation to succeed (Evans, 2003; Fogg, 2003; Matthews, 1999; Sandler, 1993; Wilson, 2001). Testing the supposition that women are in disadvantageous bargaining positions in organizations because of sex-role stereotypes, goals and expectations, self-perceptions, and behavioral orientations, Nadler and Nadler (1989) found that women did, in fact, have lower expectations regarding satisfactory outcomes. They concluded, “(E)equivalent perceptions of success for men and women do not necessarily mean equivalent outcomes of success” (p. 8). Others have concluded that women’s career goals have been delayed, subordinated or lowered by their husbands’ career pursuits and women’s primary responsibilities to their families (Davis, 2001; Drago, 2007; Evan, 2003; Schneider, 1998; West & Curtis, 2006; Williams, 2000a, b; Wilson, 2004) Matthews (1999) suggested that women are socialized to be more supportive in relationships, and are likely to avoid administrative and decision-making roles because such roles are antithetical to their nature. This could be interpreted to imply that women in higher education may be participating in decision-making and achieving success at their desired level. Communication and Decision-making Participative decision-making (PDM) has been viewed as a communication dimension of managerial climate (Redding, 1973; Richmond & McCroskey, 2001), and has been traditionally conceptualized to include subordinate-superior collaboration, information sharing, and problemsolving (Gordon & Infante, 1991; Richmond et al., 1997), or interactionally as value and goal building and sharing process in which the focus is on the centrality of communication (Caldwell, Chatman, & O'Reilly, 1990; Allen, 1992). Harrison and Laux (1987) argued that the traditional approach to conceptualizing participation in decision-making holds that PDM is a management prerogative, and that participation occurs when superiors invite subordinates' opinions, and take those opinions into account when making decisions. Theoretically, their approach integrated two dimensions, both viewed as under the control of the supervisor: (1) the communication dimension where employee input is valued, and what subordinates say is considered as important; (2) and the influence dimension where employee input is viewed as an opportunity to fulfill the needs of subordinates to express themselves, though their opinions may be disregarded at the option of superiors. The superior unilaterally controls both dimensions. The degree to which the superior is participative Participation in Decision-Making 168 is determined by the quantity and quality of superior-subordinate communication and the extent to which subordinate's opinions are reflected in the final decision (Likert, 1967; Richmond & McCroskey, 1979, 2005; Richmond, McCroskey & Roach , 1997; Tannenbaum, Weschler, & Massarik, 1961; Vroom & Yetton, 1973). To the extent that work conditions allow and choice exists, participation may be limited by lack of subordinates' desire to share decision- making (Dienesch & Liden, 1986; Hespe & Wall, 1976). Additionally, some subordinates may lack the personality characteristics (Abdel-Halim & Rowland, 1976; Anderson, 1984; Stake & Stake, 1979; Shimanoff & Jenkins, 2003; Vroom, 1960), communication competency or skill (Richmond & McCroskey, 1979; 2005; Reich & Wood, 2003) to participate. However, the basic assumption of traditional approaches is that participation is a managerial style which is administered consistently across subordinates. It is presumed that managers' election to use participative approaches are operationalized through communication that energizes subordinates to be involved in decision-making, and that joint decision-making results in increases in interaction and information sharing (Allen, Judd, & Ceruzzi, 2003; Harrison, 1985; Harrison & Laux, 1987; Gouran & Hirokawa, 2003; McCroskey & Richmond, 2000; Richmond & McCroskey, 1979; Richmond et al., 1997). On the other hand, the interactional approach conceptualizes PDM as one aspect that emerges from the relationship between superiors and subordinates. From this perspective, control and power are seen as derived from the hierarchical component of the superior and subordinate relationship, which is objectively structured for both participants. The hierarchical component of the superior-subordinate relationship is viewed as secondary to the interpersonal, and it is held that issues emanating from the hierarchical component are negotiated interpersonally as definitions of the relationship are advanced through interaction (Kassing, 2000; McCroskey & Richmond, 2000; Myers & Kassing, 1998; Richmond & McCroskey, 1979; Richmond et al., 1997). The extent of PDM is dependent on formulation of a unique definition of the superior-subordinate role. It is contended that higher levels of PDM result from relational negotiation involving both superiors and subordinates. Harrison and Laux state, "Superiors cannot 'share' decision-making without the active contributions of the subordinate" (p. 8). The subordinates' definition of the situation is viewed as crucial, and it is argued that participation will not be realized unless subordinates perceive that a relationship is conductive to participative behavior (Richmond, McCroskey, & McCroskey, 2005). While the interactive approach accepts that organizational contingencies such as the personality traits of individuals involved and the characteristics of decision-making situations may affect participation norms, it is assumed that in large measure the variance in participation will be as a consequence of superiors and subordinates' unique understandings and differentiated responses because of different individual capabilities, communication styles, and personality characteristics (Harrison & Laux, 1987; McCroskey & Richmond, 2000; Richmond & McCroskey, 1979; 2005; Richmond et al., 1997). In the interactive model, communication has both relational and pragmatic functions. Through communication superiors and subordinates define their relationship and negotiate definitions of respective roles, and pragmatically speaking, it is through communication that decision-making is shared and influence is exercised. This process is framed by how much desire subordinates have to influence decisions, and how much influence subordinates perceive that superiors are willing to accept (Harrison & Laux, 1987). In communication research the tendency has been to view decision-making as a management prerogative, and the extent of subordinates participate has been viewed as dependent upon the communication style of the supervisor. Richmond and McCroskey (1979), 169 Jerry L. Allen and Ben B. Judd building on the work of Tannenbaum and Schmidt (1985) and Sadler (1970) conceptualized management communication style as a continuum ranging from boss centered to subordinate centered, with employees more satisfied with supervisors’ communication when a more participative style is employed (consults or joins) and less satisfied with supervisors’ communication when less participative styles (tell or sell) are employed. In a more interactive perspective, some researchers have advocated that in addition to the amount of participation in decision-making allowed by supervisors, management communication style may be valenced by the amount of participation desired by subordinates. Wheeless and his associates (1984) concluded that lower level employees may exactly desire less autonomy in order to avoid added responsibility, and therefore, a moderate amount of participation may result in maximum satisfaction. Communication and Job Satisfaction The study of job satisfaction, defined as an employee's affective response to various aspects of the work environment (Wheeless, Wheeless, & Howard, 1984), has been investigated from the perspective of need fulfillment (Schaffer, 1953), Herzberg's Two-Factory model (Herzberg, Mausner, & Synderman, 1959; Locke, 1973), discrepancy theory (Locke, 1969; Locke & Schweiger, 1979), and equity theory (Adams, 1963). The traditional approach is too measure job satisfaction by focusing on the attitudes of subordinates toward work, supervisor, pay, promotions, and co-workers as conceptualized by Smith, Kendall, and Hulin (1969). In the U. S., much of the research focusing on job satisfaction has been concerned with subordinates' perceptions of supervisors (Downs, 1979; Jablin, 1979; Pinus, 1986), and has concluded that open, trusting, and participative subordinate-supervisor relationships culminate in greater subordinate satisfaction (Falcione, 1974a, Falcione, 1978; Falcione, Daly, & McCroskey, 1979; McCroskey & Richmond, 2000; Muchinsky, 1977; Pincus, 1986; Richmond & McCroskey, 2000; Richmond, Wagner, & McCroskey, 1983). Relationships have been found between job satisfaction and openness in communication (Burke & Wilcox, 1969), supervisors’ perceived credibility and attractiveness (Falcione, 1974b; McCroskey & Richmond, 2000), listening and supervisor's receptivity (Daly, Falcione, & McCroskey, 1978), levels of employee communication apprehension and perception of supervisor (Falcione, McCroskey, & Daly, 1977; Richmond, 1999), tolerance for disagreement (Richmond & McCroskey, 1979; McCroskey, Richmond, & Davis, 1981), initiating personal communication (Cummings, Lewis, & Long, 1980), management communication style (Richmond & McCroskey, 1979; Richmond, McCroskey, Davis, & Koontz, 1980; Richmond, McCroskey, & Davis, 1982; Richmond, Wagner, McCroskey, 1983), communication style (Allen, Rybczyk, & Judd, 2006; McCroskey & Richmond, 2000), nonverbal immediacy (Richmond & McCroskey, 2000), and the use of power, compliance gaining, and affinity-seeking strategies (Richmond, Davis, Saylor, & McCroskey, 1984; Richmond, McCroskey, Davis, 1986). Communication climate is another variable that has been found to relate to job satisfaction (Jablin, 1980; Goldhaber, 1993), and decision-making has been conceptualized as a dimension of communication climate (Redding, 1970). When employees' perceive a discrepancy between a company's climate and their own values, they have been found to be less committed to their jobs; less satisfied with their jobs, career progress, pay; and less inclined to contribute to decision- making (O'Reilly, Chatman, & Caldwell, 1991; Shockley-Zalabak & Morley, 1989; Weatherly & Beach, 1996). Participation in Decision-Making 170 Participation in Decision-making and Job Satisfaction While the recent tendency has been to view PDM as a continuum, Wheeless and his associates (1984) contend that it should be viewed in terms of (1) the amount of participation allowed, and (2) the discrepancy between the amount of participation allowed, and the amount desired. Utilizing Richmond and McCroskey’s (1979) conceptualization of manager communication styles as an independent variable to examine the affects of decision-making on job satisfaction, they reported that PDM in consort with other communication variables accounted for a significant amount of the variance (76 percent) of job satisfaction, but PDM alone did not make a significant contribution to job satisfaction. They also examined the discrepancy between the amount of PDM reported, and the subjects' desired level of PDM. The expectation was that the discrepancy between the amount of participation allowed and the amount desired would be negatively related to job satisfaction, but neither actual nor desired participation contributed unique variance to job satisfaction. It was concluded that lower level employees may desire less autonomy, and, therefore, a moderate amount of participation may result in maximum satisfaction. Floyd (1985) arrived at a similar conclusion, maintaining that many faculty members, especially the very junior and the very senior, may not want to participate in institutional level decision-making. Her reasoning was that senior faculty often do not want to become involved because of time constraints. She surmises that younger faculty members and those with fewer years of service may be advised or may choose to avoid campus governance for fear that it would take time from professional development activities or cause difficulties at promotion and tenure time. The nonlinear relationship between PDM and satisfaction might have a more general alternate mechanism. Alutto and Belasco (1972) studied desired PDM, actual PDM, and the degree of discrepancy, and speculated that too much actual PDM compared to desired PDM results in decisional overload. Conversely, too little PDM compared to desired PDM results in decisional deprivation. Both of these conditions are likely to result in less satisfaction than decisional congruence, where actual and desired PDM are closely matched. A number of studies have found that the amount of PDM is related to job satisfaction in the U. S. (Vroom, 1964; Falcione 1974 a, b; Smolen, 1983; Gordon, Infante and Gordon, 1985a, b, 1987); Gordon, Infante, & Graham, 1988; Gordon, Infante, & Izzo, 1989; Allen, 1992), and Richmond and her associates (Richmond & McCroskey, 1979; Richmond, McCroskey, Davis, & Koontz, 1980) found management style to be a positive contributor to job satisfaction. It should be noted, however, that the management style instrument used in this study (Richmond & McCroskey, 1979) was intended to measure subordinates' perceptions of their supervisors' style of management (participative or non-participative), and not as a measure of how much subordinates actually see themselves participating in decision-making. Hypothesis The above rationale and review suggests that the contribution of PDM to employees’ job satisfaction in the U. S. has been perceived as heuristic. Though PDM has received considerable attention, the focus has not always been clear. This study was undertaken to clarify that focus. While previous studies have focused on subordinates impressions of their supervisors’ preferred decision-making styles, in this study the emphasis was on respondents' perceptions of their PDM. Moreover, previous studies have not investigated the possibility of sex differences in decision-making and their effects on job satisfaction. Given the urgent decision-making 171 Jerry L. Allen and Ben B. Judd imperatives, which exist in higher education today, the unique dilemmas faced by faculty generally, and women faculty in particular, the following hypotheses were tested: H1: Increased PDM by faculty results in increased job satisfaction. H2: Male and female faculty members will differ in their actual level of PDM. H3: Male and female faculty members will differ in their desired level of PDM. H4: Male and female faculty members will differ in satisfaction with their jobs. H5: The affect of PDM on job satisfaction will be different for male and female faculty members. H6: Actual and desired levels of PDM will vary with academic rank, years teaching, possession of tenure, number of publications, achievement of refereed publications. Faculty members with senior rank, more teaching experience, tenure and publications are accorded greater prestige in the academic world, and consequently it would be expected that they would entitle the holder to more involvement in university managerial processes. H7: Actual and desired levels of PDM will vary for ale and female faculty members will with academic rank, years teaching, possession of tenure, number of publications, achievement of refereed publications. Method Participants Questionnaires were mailed to 500 faculty members at four urban private universities located in the northeastern United States, and 269 responses were received back. Although the cover letter described the questionnaire as a study of participation in decision-making, no mention was made of any investigation of sex differences in PDM or job satisfaction. Measuring Instruments Participation in Decision-Making (PDM). Scales for academic PDM originally developed by Alutto and Belasco (1972) and modified by Conway (1976) were used to measure desired and actual PDM. Participants were asked to rate how frequently they were consulted (actual participation) and how frequently they wished to be involved (desired level of participation) relative to 11 types of decisions affecting faculty. Since each decision situation is scored from one to four, scores can range from 11 to 44. In this study, the coefficient alpha for actual participation was .80, and the alpha for desired participation was .75. Job Satisfaction. The short form of the Job Descriptive Index (JDI), developed by Smith, Kendall and Hulin (1975), was used to measure job satisfaction. This version is composed of 27 items used to measure satisfaction with work, supervision, co-workers, pay and opportunity for promotion. Participants are asked to assign each item a score ranging from 0 to 3, with higher numbers indicating greater satisfaction. Reliability coefficients ranged from .83 to .90 for the five scales. Demographics. Participants were asked to indicate their age, sex, years teaching, salary level, academic rank, tenure status, number of publications, and number of refereed publications. Participation in Decision-Making 172 Statistical Analysis. Hypothesis one was analyzed by examining Pearson Correlations, and hypotheses two, three, and four were analyzed with t-tests. Pearson correlations, with zscores to determine the strength of differences, were used to study hypotheses four and five. Hypothesis six was analyzed using Pearson correlations, and chi-square was employed to test hypothesis seven. Results Based on previous studies that found that participation in decision-making or job autonomy increased job satisfaction, greater actual participation was expected to be associated with greater job satisfaction (H1). As the correlations reported in Table 1 show, this hypothesis was moderately supported. Actual PDM was significantly related to each of the dimensions of job satisfaction. The correlations ranged from .214 to .350, indicating that actual PDM accounted for four to ten percent of the variance in job satisfaction. It was also expected that a faculty member's desired level of PDM would increase as their achieved PDM increased. There was moderate support for this speculated relationship (r = .381, p < .001). However, as can be seen in Table 1, faculty members’ level of desired PDM was unrelated to the dimensions of job satisfaction. Table 1: Correlations of Actual and Desired Participative Decision-Making and Job Satisfaction Job satisfaction ____________________________________________________________________________ Actual Participation Desired Participation ____________________________________________________________________________ Work .269* .068 Supervision .260* -.105 Pay .350* -.064 Opportunity for promotion .225* -.084 Co-workers .214* -.027 ______________________________________________________________________________ Note: *p < .001. Based on frequent reports that women are discriminated against in employment and higher education in the U. S., it was expected that women faculty members would report lower levels of actual PDM than men (H2). This hypotheses was supported by a significant difference of about six percent (of the scale range) in mean PDM scores, as reported in Table 2 [t(268) = 2.72, p < .01]. Because of social pressures for women to be more passive, women’s greater family responsibilities, and the greater instructional demands of lower level faculty positions in which many women might find themselves, it was expected that women faculty members would not desire as high a level of PDM as men (H3). Since, as is also reported in Table 2, the mean desired PDM scores for male and female faculty were not significantly different [t(268) = .29, p .73], hypothesis three was not supported. However, the desired level of PDM was significantly higher (by at least 10 percent) than the achieved level for males [t(179) = 11.27, p < .001] and females [t(88) = 9.5, p < .001] considered separately or combined, t(268) = 14.65, p < .001. 173 Jerry L. Allen and Ben B. Judd Table 2: Participative Decision-making means for male and female faculty Participative decision-making _____________________________________________________________________________ Male faculty Female faculty ______________________________________________________________________________ Actual participation 26.32 33.80* Desired participation 30.95 31.15 ______________________________________________________________________________ Note: *p < .01. Given frequent reports that women are paid less than men both in business and industry (U. S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 1994; Wage Gap, 1996) and in higher education in the U. S. (Fogg, 2003), it was expected women faculty would report less satisfaction than men with their faculty positions (H4). The mean scores for job satisfaction reported in Table 3 show that this hypothesis was partially supported. Women were significantly less satisfied with work [t(268) = 3.53, p < .001], pay [t(268) = 4.14, p < .001], and opportunity for promotion [t(268) = 2.67, p < .01], but male and female faculty members did not differ in satisfaction with supervision [t(268) = .19, p .87] or with co-workers [t(268) = 1.16, p .08]. Table 3: Means for Job Satisfaction for Male and Female Faculty Job satisfaction ______________________________________________________________________________ Male faculty Female faculty ______________________________________________________________________________ Work 38.46 33.80** Supervision 39.45 39.20 Pay 11.67 7.75** Opportunity for promotion 13.12 10.20* Co-workers 39.08 37.13 ______________________________________________________________________________ As Table 1 indicates, male and female faculty members combined reported that job satisfaction increased with actual PDM. Hypothesis five was constructed to determine whether the level of PDM afforded men and women faculty members differently affected their job satisfaction. Hypothesis five was supported in an unexpected manner by the findings reported in Table 4. For male faculty members, the correlation between actual PDM and pay was significant (r = .214, p < .05), but contrary to prediction, actual PDM was not significantly related to the other dimensions of job satisfaction. On the other hand, for women faculty members there were significant correlations between actual PDM and satisfaction with work (r = .449, p < .001), supervision (r = .535, p < .001), pay (r = .541, p < .001), opportunity for promotion (r = .305, p < .05), and co-workers (r = .397, p < .001). The differences in these correlations were tested by a method developed by Fisher (Ferguson, 1966, p. 188), in which r-values are converted to normal curve values (z-scores), subtracted, and compared by a t-test. The difference z-scores and significance level of the t-tests are reported in Table 4. The PDM to job satisfaction correlations Participation in Decision-Making 174 were significantly higher for women in satisfaction with work [t(268) = 3.45, p < .001], supervision [t(268) = 4.31, p < .001] and co-workers [t(269) = 3.17, p < .01], but men and women faculty members did not differ on PDM and satisfaction with pay or opportunity for promotion. Table 4: Correlations and z Scores between Actual Participation of Male and Female Faculty and Job Satisfaction ______________________________________________________________________________ Job satisfaction Male faculty Female faculty z scores Work .108 .449 .375** Supervision .175 .535** .420** Pay .214* .541** .003 Opportunity for promotion .146 .305* .168 Co-workers .095 .397** .325* _____________________________________________________________________________ Notes: * p < .05. ** p < .01. Hypothesis six predicted that actual and desired amounts of PDM for male and female faculty combined would vary with academic rank, years teaching, tenure, number of publications overall, and number of refereed publications. Table 5 reveals the correlations between actual and desired PDM and these indices of faculty status. Actual PDM was significantly related to academic rank (r = .281, p <. 001, years teaching (r = .277, p. <.001), and tenure (r = .225, p <.001), and together those variables accounted for 58 percent of the variance related to PDM. Surprisingly, the number of publications overall and the number of refereed publications were not related to perceptions of actual PDM. Even more surprising, the desire for PDM was not related to any of those variables usually used as indices of faculty status—rank, years of experience, tenure, number of publications, or number of refereed publications . To determine if discrimination against women faculty exists in the U. S., it was important to examine whether women faculty have achieved equality with men in academic status (H7). To evaluate this, a series of chi-square tests were performed with sex as the independent and the individual dimensions of faculty status—rank, years of teaching experience, tenure v. nontenure, number publications overall, and the number of refereed publications as the dependent variables. Since the variables are at least ordinal, the results are depicted in the last column of Table 5 as correlations to suggest the strength and direction of effect. In comparison to male faculty Table 5: Correlations between Participative Decision-Making or Sex and Indices of Faculty Status ______________________________________________________________________________ Participative Decision-Making ___________________________ Indices of faculty status Actual Desired Sex ______________________________________________________________________________ Academic rank .281** .050 .311** 175 Jerry L. Allen and Ben B. Judd Years teaching .277** .037 -.199* Tenure .225** .075 -.269** Number publications -.012 -.052 -.122 Refereed publications .064 -.025 .127 ______________________________________________________________________________ Note: Significance levels of X 2: *p < .05; **p < .01; **p < .001. members, women were found to have lower academic rank (χ2(3, N = 269) = 33.76, p <. 0001 ), fewer years of teaching (χ2(5, N = 269) = 14.44, p <. 01), less tenure (χ2(1, N = 269) = 19.78, p <. 0001), but a higher percentage of refereed publications (χ2(1, N =269) = 4.12, p <. 05). Male and female faculty did not differ in the number of publications overall. As can be seen from the correlations for PDM and sex presented in Table 5, the relationships of academic rank, teaching experience, tenure, and the number of refereed publications accounted for 42 percent of the variance related to PDM. Years of teaching experience appears to be the primary variable, since fewer years of teaching reduces opportunities for tenure and promotion. In this sample, 43 percent of the women had been teaching for less than ten years compared to 25 percent of the men. Thus, status differences may disappear or become less important over time. Table 6: Means for Actual Participation in Decision-Making by Faculty Members’ Years Teaching and Sex Years teaching ______________________________________________________________________________ Sex of faculty 0–5 6 – 10 11 – 15 16 – 20 21 – 24 25+ ______________________________________________________________________________ Males 22.4* 25.0* 27.4* 27.9* 28.0* 27.1 Females 21.7* 23.1* 24.6* 25.1* 27.0* 27.4 Combineda 44.1 48.1 52.0 53.0 55.0 54.5 Notes: Means in the same column designated by an asterisk (*) are significantly different at p < .05. a Means in this row (Combined) are significantly different from each other at p < .05. The dual finding that female faculty had fewer years of teaching and less actual participation in decision-making suggests a possible confounding of the finding reported in Table 2. Was the lower participation of female faculty due to sexual bias or merely due to women having been faculty members for fewer years on average? To answer this question, a post hoc analysis was performed with a two-way ANOVA (2 x 6), with actual PDM as the dependent variable and sex and years of teaching as the independent variables. The relevant means are reported in Table 6. As expected, years of teaching was a significant influence on PDM, (F(1, 268) = 4.85, p < .001), but sex was also significant (F(1,268) = 3.77, p < .05). PDM was significantly less for females who had less than 25 years of teaching experience (Table 6). Discussion and Conclusions Participation in Decision-Making 176 For nearly three decades, evidence has indicated that women in the U. S. are constrained by a “glass ceiling” in the workplace, so that their participation in the upper echelons of management is out of proportion with either their actual numbers in the workplace or the proportion of males in management positions (Allen & O’Mara, 1992; Guyot, 2001; Ivy & Backlund, 2000; Kovach, 1985; Pepper, 1995; Rosen & Jerdee, 1974; Weider-Hatfield, 1987). In higher education, data indicates that proportionally fewer women hold full-time faculty positions; fewer women are granted tenure; they publish less; fewer women are likely to be employed in administration; and women faculty are paid less (Cole & Zuckerman, 1987; Davis, 2001; Dowdall, 2003; Fogg, 2003; Hickson, Stacks, & Amsbary, 1989, 1992, 1993; Leatherman, 1993; Sandler, 1993; West & Curtis, 2006; Williams, 2000a; Wilson, 2001, 2004). In the field of communication, fewer women have served on the editorial boards of journals (Bodon, Powell, & Hickson, 1999; Cooper, Stewart, & Friedley, 1989; Hickson, Stacks, & Amsbary, 1992), and as chairs of departments (DeWine, (1987; Hanson, 1996)). Speculation, supported by sparse research, has been that mind-splits related to motherhood, femininity, husband’s careers, and social standards reinforced by sex-role stereotyping results in lowered career expectations; and that women are likely to avoid administrative, decision-making roles because of less desire to participate in decision-making (Davis, 2001; Fogg, 2003; Matthews, 1999; Nadler & Nadler, 1989; Sandler, 1993; Schneider, 1998; Wheeless, Wheeless, & Howard, 1984; Williams, 2000a, b). The purpose of this study was to determine if male and female faculty members have different levels of desired and actual PDM, and whether either desired or actual levels of PDM differently impacts job satisfaction for either women or men. Results show that actual PDM is significantly, but modestly, related to all the dimensions of job satisfaction (work, supervision, pay, opportunity for promotion, and co-workers) for men and women faculty members combined, but surprisingly, desired PDM is not significantly related to the dimensions of job satisfaction (hypothesis one). This finding is particularly revealing in that some researchers have speculated that when actual levels of PDM exceed desired PDM job satisfaction will be lower because of decision-making satiation or overload (Alutto & Belasco, 1972; Wheeless, Wheeless, & Howard, 1984). In this study, only 10 percent of the males and seven percent of the females reported actual PDM exceeding desired PDM. The actual PDM level was on average somewhat lower than desired. Thus, neither male nor female faculty members seem to experience job dissatisfaction due to PDM saturation. This indicates that attention to increasing actual PDM is warranted for both male and female faculty. It is revealing that a moderate relationship (r = .38, p .001) was found between actual and desired PDM for men and women combined, and in analyzing hypotheses two and three, both men and women reported a desire for more PDM than they were actually allowed (Table 2). While men and women faculty members do not differ in the levels of PDM desired, men, in fact, report that they engage in more actual PDM than women. It is not possible to explain the suppression of female faculty member’s desire for more PDM, but it seems apparent that speculation that women have lowered career expectancies and consequently have achieved an optimum level of PDM is not the case. Not surprisingly, results indicate that female faculty are less satisfied with their jobs, pay, and promotion opportunities than are male faculty (hypothesis 4), but in stark contrast, female faculty report strong relationships between actual PDM and the dimensions of job satisfaction (hypothesis 5). The explanation for this finding is uncertain. Contrary to expectation, the variances (range) of the PDM scale and all of the job satisfaction scales do not significantly differ in homogeneity of variance tests. It may be that a ceiling effect has occurred for male but 177 Jerry L. Allen and Ben B. Judd not for female faculty, but examination of the scatter plots did not suggest this. In addition, since female faculty members do not report a lower desired level of PDM, women should differ from men in tolerance for the responsibilities of greater PDM. A remaining plausible explanation is that due to sensitization from a history of discrimination females are more sensitive to the value of participation in decision-making when judging job satisfaction. Still, this does not explain why male faculty members give less weight to PDM. Results of testing hypothesis 6 indicate that desire for PDM is not impacted by rank, years of teaching, number of publications, or refereed publications for men and women faculty members combined. Actual PDM was greater for faculty who had higher academic rank, more years of service, and/or tenure. However, the testing of hypothesis 7 reveals that women have lower academic rank, fewer years of teaching, and are less likely to be awarded tenure. Surprisingly, despite this, women report having more refereed publications. Years of teaching may the primary variable affecting this finding, since fewer years of teaching reduces opportunities for tenure and promotion. In this sample, 43 percent of the women had been teaching less than 10 years, and only 25 percent of the men had been teaching less than 10 years. This suggests that status differences may disappear over time. The question here is two-fold. First, are women in the field less than ten years because they have not been granted tenure and/or promoted, and secondly, since the women in this study reported more refereed publications, are men held to a lower standard relative to tenure and/or promotion? These questions should be explicated in a future study. In this study post hoc analysis revealed that women who have been in their teaching career less than 25 years consistently report less PDM. The results of this study provide additional evidence for the claim of discrimination against women faculty. Female faculty members have lower rank (and the associated pay), fewer achieve tenure, participation less in decision-making, and as a consequence, they are less satisfied with their work. The interpretation of the findings requires further consideration. One interpretation, which is inconsistent with some of the results, is that women have achieved less status and participation in decision-making due to outside pressures limiting their job performance. For example, women subjected to the "mommy track" may have delayed their studies, have been in the profession fewer years, and as a consequence, would have achieved less across the board (Drago, 2007; Fogg, 2003; Sandler, 1993; West & Curtis, 2006). Arguing against this interpretation is that women faculty members in this study indicate that they want the same levels of participation as men but are accorded lower PDM at all years of service. Further, women faculty members have produced a greater number of refereed publications than men, indicating, at least in the case of refereed publications, professional achievements comparable to or greater than male faculty. A second but non-novel interpretation is that women in the U. S. continue to be discriminated against in many forms. One of those forms is a management style in academia akin to that found in workplaces generally that denies women a chance to participate in decisions affecting the quality of their work life (Fogg, 2003; Ivy & Backlund, 2000; Pepper, 1995; Reardon, 1995). These data support such a conclusion and is consistent with other findings of slow progress for women in achieving job equality (Fogg, 2003; Leatherman, 1993; Wilson, 2001, 2004). Likely a better interpretation would be that women may actually view their jobs and the salient aspects of their working environment differently than men. Even without exhibiting a tendency for a higher level of desired PDM, women in this study gave much greater weight to participative decision-making in judging satisfaction with their work. This would imply a need to Participation in Decision-Making 178 more explicitly address the unique perceptions of women so that they are included more in decision-making (Ivy & Backlund, 2000; Pepper, 1995; Reardon, 1995; Reich & Wood, 2003; Shimanoff & Jenkins, 2003). An interactional model of PDM would likely more appropriately address the uniqueness of female faculty members and enhance their job satisfaction (Harrison & Laux, 1987). While a traditional approach assumes a generic, across-the-board approach to PDM, which is controlled by superiors (Harrison & Laux, 1987), an interactional approach views higher levels of PDM as emerging from relational negotiation between supervisors and subordinates. The primary assumption of such negotiation is that superiors and subordinates have to develop unique understandings and differentiated responses because of individual differences in personality, competency, and communication styles (Harrison & Laux, 1987, McCroskey & Richmond, 2000). It is obvious from casual observations, verified by the results obtained in this study, that women faculty members are de facto denied participation in the interactional equation by lack of full-time, tenure-track positions. As a consequence, the unique needs of female faculty members are not being addressed as would be expected if administrations were operating on an interactional model (Drago, 2007; West & Curtis, 2006). Alutto and Belasco (1972) argued that employees would report more actual PDM than desired PDM, and that this predicted difference would result in dissatisfaction from satiation or overload in decision-making. The results of this study do not support that contention. As was stated above, only ten percent of males and seven percent of the females reported actual PDM exceeding desired PDM, and the actual PDM level was on average somewhat lower than desired. Thus, job dissatisfaction due to PDM saturation does not seem to be a factor. Stated differently, attention to increasing actual PDM still seems warranted. Some have argued that in an evolutionary, if not, revolutionary sense more women are being admitted to the higher professional ranks, and that gradually salary gaps between men and women are declining. Still others have argued that much of the salary gap is related to the fact that more women go into the lower paying areas of the arts, humanities and education as opposed to engineering, sciences and mathematics (Fogg, 2003). An extension of these arguments is that women’s participation in decision-making in academe in the U. S. is directly related to either increased levels of satisfaction or career choices that lead to greater emphasis on teaching, advising, and grading papers, as opposed to “hard” research and institutional-related prestige markers (Drago, 2007; West & Curtis, 2007). Future research should explore this further. Specifically, it should be determined if the areas where women teach and the labor expectations in those areas affect their PDM. It has been argued that women participate less in management decision-making generally because of the masculine style manifest in management hierarchies generally (Pepper, 1995; Reardon, 1995; Richmond et al., 1997). The contention is that women’s communication style does not generally lend itself to challenging the more aggressive conflict and decision-making style of their male counterparts (Reich & Wood, 2003). Fogg (2003) points out, “Some female scholars feel that academe places too much value on factors that favor men” (p. A14). However, Reich and Wood (2003) counter, “Deeply ensconced cultural views of how women and men should communicate affect perceptions of how they actually communicate and how appropriate and effective they are” (p. 227). Future research needs to further investigate the communication styles of men and women specific to acceptance in a wide-range of decision-making scenarios in business as well as higher education. 179 Jerry L. Allen and Ben B. Judd Summary This study found differences in participation in decision-making and job satisfaction related to sex. Women faculty members in the U. S. participate less in decision-making, have lower academic status, and are less satisfied with their jobs than male faculty members. While women's lower participation in decision-making is closely tied to lower job satisfaction, surprisingly, differing degrees of participation in decision-making among male faculty members did not yield differences in job satisfaction. Differences between men and women in full-time appointments, tenure and faculty rank are hindrances and disincentives for women faculty to participate in decision-making. Women face as many or more obstacles as faculty in higher education as managers in corporations, and their lack of participation places serious limitations on the success of educational institutions. The implication of this study for all employers, and most especially those in higher education is that participation in decision-making needs to be recognized as an important aspect of employees’ job satisfaction, and that there is need to more explicitly address the unique perceptions of women so that they are included equally with their male counterparts in decision-making in their perspective organizations. Participation in Decision-Making 180 References Abdel-Halim, A. A., & Rowland, K. M. (1976). Some personality determinants of the effects of participation: A further investigation. Personnel Psychology, 29, 41-55. Adams, J. S. (1963). Toward an understanding of inequity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 67, 422-436. Allen, M. W. (1992). Communication and Organizational Commitment: Perceived organizational support as a mediating factor. Communication Quarterly, 40, 357-367. Allen, J. L., Judd, B. B., & Ceruzzi, F. (2003, April) The effects of anxiety, (dis)trust, and information quality on employees’ willingness to accept organizational change. Paper presented at the Eastern Communication Association Convention, Washington, D. C. Allen, J. L., & O'Mara, J. (1992, October). Sex stereotyping in business and professions: Across generations and cultures. Paper presented at the Organization for the Study of Communication, Language, and Gender Conference, Hempstead, NY. Allen, J. L., Rybczyk, E. J., & Judd, B. B. (2006). Communication style and the managerial effectiveness of male and female supervisors. Journal of Business & Economics Research, 4, 7-18. Alutto, J. A., & Belasco, J. A. (1972). A typology for participation in organizational Decisionmaking. Administrative Science Quarterly, 17, 117-125. American Council on Education. (1993). Campus Trends, 1993. Washington, D.C. Anderson, L. R., & Blanchard, P. N. (1982). Sex differences in task and social-emotional behavior. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 3, 109-139, Bem, S. (1993). The lens of gender. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Bodon, J., Powell, L., & Hickson III, M. (1999). Critiques of gatekeeping in scholarly journals: An analysis of perceptions and data. JACA, 28, 60-70. Burke, R. J., & Wilcox, D. S. (1969). Effects of different patterns and degrees of openness in superior-subordinate communication on subordinate job satisfaction. Academy of Management Journal, 12, 319-326. Burrell, N. A., Donohue, W. A., & Allen, M. (1988). Gender- based perceptual biases in mediation. Communication Research, 15, 447-469. Caldwell, D. F., Chapman, J. A. & O'Reilly, C. A. (1990). Building organizational commitment: A multifirm study. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 61, 245-36. Callan, P. M. (1993). Government and higher education. In A. Levine (Ed.), Higher Education in America, 1980-2999 (pp. 3-38). Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University Press. Cole, J. R., & Zuckerman, H. (1987). Marriage, motherhood and research performance in science. Scientific American, February, 119-125. Conway, J. A. (1976). Test of linearity between teachers' participation in decision making and their perceptions of their schools as organizations. Administrative Science Quarterly, 21,130-139. Cooper, P. J., Stewart, L. P., & Friedley, S. A. (1989). Twenty years of research by and about women in major communication journals. ACA Journal, 16, 46-61. Cummings, H. W., Lewis, M. L., & Long, L. W. (1980, April). Communication roles as predictors of job satisfaction. Paper presented at the Eastern Communication Association Convention, Ocean City, MD. Daly, J. A., McCroskey, J. C., & Falcione, R. L. (1978, April). Communication apprehension, 181 Jerry L. Allen and Ben B. Judd supervisor communication receptivity and satisfaction with superiors. Paper presented at the Eastern Communication Association Convention. Philadelphia, PA. Davis, S. (2001, July 13). Women and the tenure track. The Chronicle of Higher Education, pp. A7-9. DeWine, S. (1987). Female leadership in male-dominated organizations. ACA Bulletin, 61, 19-29. Dienesch, R. M., & Liden, R. C. (1986). Leader-member exchange model of leadership: A critique and further development. Academy of Management Review, 11, 618-634. Dowdall, J. (2003, June 20). Gender and the administrative search. The Chronicle of Higher Education, p. C3. Downs, C. W. (1979). The relationship between communication and job satisfaction. In R. C. Huseman, C. M. Logue, & D. L. Freshley (Eds.), Readings in interpersonal and organizational communication (3rd ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Dago, R. (2007, March 30). Harvard and the academic glass ceiling. The Chronicle of Higher Education, p. C3. Evans, C. (2003, May 9). So why are you really leaving? The Chronicle of Higher Education, p. C4. Falcione, R. L. (1974a). Communication climate and satisfaction with immediate supervision. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 2, 13-20. Falcione, R. L. (1974b). Credibility: Qualifier of subordinate participation. Journal of Business Communication, 11, 43-64. Falcione, R. L. (1978, April). Subordinate satisfaction as a function of communication climate and perceptions of immediate supervision. Paper presented at the Eastern Communication Association Convention, Boston, MA. Falcione, R. L., McCroskey, J. C., & Daly, J. A. (1974). Job satisfaction as a function of employee communication apprehension, self-esteem, and perception of their immediate supervisors. Communication Yearbook, 1, 363-375. Ferguson, G. A. (1966). Statistical analysis in psychology and education. New York: McGraw-Hill. Floyd, C. E. (1985). Faculty participation in academic decision-making: Necessity or Luxury (ASHE-ERIC Report 8). East Lansing, MI: National Center for Research on Teacher Learning. Fogg, P. (2003, April 18). The gap that won’t go away. The Chronicle of Higher Education, pp. A12-14. Getman, J. G., & Franke, A. H. (1988, February 8). The "Yeshiva" case revisited: Professors' right to bargain is entitled to statutory protection. The Chronicle of Higher Education, pp. B1-2. Gmelch, W. H,, & Miskin, V. D. (1993). Leadership skills for department chairs. Boston, MA: Anker Publishing Goldhaber, G. M. (1993). Organizational communication (6th ed.). Dubuque, IA: Brown & Benchmark. Gorden, W. I., & Infante, D. A. (1991). Test of a communication model of organizational commitment. Communication Quarterly, 39, 144-155. Guyot, J. F. (2001, April 20). The defining moment for gender equity. The Chronicle of Higher Education, p. A9. Gordon, W. I., Infante, D. A. & Izzo, J. (1988). Variations of voice pertaining to Participation in Decision-Making 182 dissatisfaction/satisfaction with subordinates. Management Communication, 2, 6-22. Gouran, D. S., & Hirokawa, R. Y. Effective decision-making and problem-solving in groups: A functional perspective. In R. Y. Hirokawa, R. S. Cathcart, L. A. Samovar, & L. D. Henman (Eds.), Small group communication: Theory and practice (pp. 27-38). Los Angeles, CA: Roxbury Publishing Company. Hanson, T. L. 1996). A comparison of leadership practices used by male and female communication department chairperson. Journal of the Association for Communication Administration, 1, 40-55. Harrison, T. M. (1985). Communication and participative decision making: An exploratory study. Personnel Psychology, 38, 93-116. Harrison, T. M., & Laux, J. L. (1987, May). Superior-subordinate participation : Tests of an interactional model. Paper presented at the International Communication Association Convention, Montreal, Canada. Hertzberg, F. (1966). Work and the nature of man. Cleveland: World Publishing. Hertzberg, F., Mausner, B. & Snyderman, B. (1959). The motivation of work. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. Hespe, G., & Wall, T. (1976). The demand for participation among employees. Human Relations, 29, 411-241. Hickson III, M., Stacks, D. W., & Amsbary, J. H. (1989). An analysis of prolific scholarship in speech communication, 1915-1985: Toward a yardstick for measuring research productivity. Communication Education, 38, 230-236 Hickson III, M., Stacks, D. W., & Amsbary, J. H. (1993). Active prolific female scholars in communication: An analysis of research productivity II. Communication Quarterly, 40, 350-356. Hickson III, M., Stacks, D. W., & Amsbary, J. H. (1993). Active prolific scholars in communication studies II. Communication Education, 42, 224-233. Hickson III, M., Stacks, D. W., & Bodon, J. (1999). The status of research productivity in communication: 1915-1995. Communication Monographs, 66, 178-197. Ivy, D. K., & Backlund, P. (2000). Exploring gender speak: Personal effectiveness in gender communication. Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill Jablin, F. M. (1979). Superior-subordinate communication: The state of the art. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 1,201-1,222. Jablin, F. M. (1980). Organizational communication theory and research: An overview of communication climate and network research. Communication Yearbook, 4, 327-347. Jablin, F. M. (1982). Formal structural characteristics of organizations and superior-subordinate communication, Human Communication Research, 8, 338-347. Kassing, J. W. (2000). Investigating the relationship between superior-subordinate relationship quality and employee dissent. Communication Research Reports, 17, 58-70. Kolodny, A. (1998). Failing the future: A dean looks at higher education in the twenty-first century. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Kovack, K. A. (1985). Sex stereotyping by tomorrow's executives. Personnel, December, 14-20. Kushell, E., & Newton, R. (1986). Gender leadership style, and subordinate satisfaction: An experiment. Sex-Roles, 14, 203-209. Lazerson, M. (1997, March/April). Who owns higher education? The changing face of governance. Change, 10-15. 183 Jerry L. Allen and Ben B. Judd Likert, R. (1967). The human organization: Its management and values. New York: McGrawHill. Leatherman, C. (1993, July 28). Disappointed with women's progress. The Chronicle of Higher Education, p. A16. LeClair, D., & Sherblom, J. (1989). Research on women as managerial leaders: A review and critique. Women and Language, 12, 31-36. Lively, K. (2000, September 15). Diversity increases among presidents. The Chronicle of Higher Education, pp. A10-12. Locke, E. A. (1969). What is job satisfaction? Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 4, 309-336. Locke, E. A. (1973). Satisfiers and dissatisfiers among white collar and blue collar employees. Journal of Applied Psychology, 58, 67-76. Locke, E. A., & Schweiger, D. M. (1979). Participation in decision making: One more look. In B. Staw and L. L. Cummings (Eds.) Research in organizational behavior, 1. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Maltz, D., & Borker, R. (1982). A cultural approach to male-female miscommunication. In J. Gumperz (Ed.), Language and social identity. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Matthews, P. (1999, July 9). Why some women shy away from college administration, The Chronicle of Higher Education, pp. A. 13-14. McCroskey, J. C., & Richmond, V. P. (2000). Applying reciprocity and accommodation theories to supervisor/subordinate communication. Journal of Applied Communication, 28, 278-289. McCroskey, J. C., Richmond, V. P., & Davis, L. M. (1981). Individual differences among employees, management communication style and employee satisfaction: Replication and extension. Paper presented at the Eastern Communication Association Convention, Pittsburgh, PA. Moses, Y. (1997, December 12). Salaries in academe: The gender gap persists. The Chronicle of Higher Education, p. A60. Muchinsky, P. M. (1977). Organizational communication: Relationships to organizational climate and job satisfaction. Academy of Management Journal, 20, 592-607. Mulac, A., Bradac, J., & Gibbons, P. (2001). Empirical support for the gender as culture hypothesis. Human Communication Research, 27, 121-152. Myers, S. A., & Kassing, J. W. (1998). The relationship between perceived supervisory communication behaviors and subordinates organizational identification. Communication Research Reports, 15, 71-81. Nadler, L. B., & Nadler, M. K. (1986). The role of sex in organizational negotiation ability. Women’s Studies in Communication, 9, 1-11. O’Reilly, C. A., III, Chatman, J., & Caldwell, D. F. (1991). People and organizational culture: A profile comparison approach to assessing person-organization fit. Academy of Management Journal, 34, 487-516. Pepper, G. L. (1995). Communicating in organizations: A cultural approach. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. Pincus, J. D. (1986). Communication satisfaction, job satisfaction, and job performance, Human Communication Research, 12, 395-419. Redding, W. C. (1973). Communication within the organization. New York: Industrial Participation in Decision-Making 184 Communication Council; Lafayette, IN: Purdue Research Foundation. Reich, N. M., & Wood, J. T. (2003). Sex, gender, and communication in small groups. In R. Y. Hirokawa, R. S. Cathcart, L. A. Samovar, & Linda D. Henman (Eds.), Small group communication: Theory and practice (pp. 27-38). Los Angeles, CA: Roxbury Publishing Company. Richmond, V. P. (1999). Communication, people, and change. In G. Stone, M. Singletary, & V. Richmond (Eds.), Clarifying communication theories: A hands-on approach (pp. 132139). Ames, IA: Iowa State University Press. Richmond, V. P., Davis, L. M., Saylor, K., & McCroskey, J. C.(1984). Power in organizations: Communication techniques and messages. Human Communication Research, 11, 85-108. Richmond, V. P., & McCroskey, J. C. (1979). Management communication style, tolerance for disagreement, and innovativeness as predictors of employee satisfaction: A comparison of single-factor, and multiple-factor approaches. Communication Yearbook, 3, 359-373. Richmond, V. P., & McCroskey, J. C. (2000). The impact of supervisor and subordinate immediacy on relational and organizational outcomes. Communicaiton Monographs, 67, 85-95. Richmond, V. P., McCroskey, J. C., & Davis, L. M. (1982). Individual differences among employees, management communication style, and employee satisfaction: Replication and extension. Human Communication Research, 8, 170-188. Richmond, V. P., McCroskey, J. C., & Davis, L. M. (1986). The relationship of supervisor use of power and affinity-seeking strategies with subordinate satisfaction. Communication Quarterly, 34, 178-193. Richmond, V. P., McCroskey, J. C., & McCroskey, L. L. (2005). Organizational communication for survival: Making work, work (3rd ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon. Richmond, V. A., McCroskey, J. C., Davis, L. M. & Koontz, K. A. (1980). Management communication style and employee satisfaction: Replication and extension. Paper presented at the International Communication Association Convention. Acapulco, Mexico. Richmond, V. A., McCroskey, J. C., & Roach, K. D. (1997). Communication and decisionmaking styles, power base usage, and satisfaction in marital dyads. Communication Quarterly, 45, 410-426. Richmond, V. P., Wagner, J. P., & McCroskey, J. C. (1983). The impact of perception of leadership style, use of power, and conflict management style on organizational outcomes. Communication Quarterly, 31, 27-36. Rosen, B., & Jerdee, T. H. (1974). Sex stereotyping in the executive suite. Harvard Business Review, March-April, 45-58. Sandler, B. R. (1986). Chilly climate revisited: Chilly for women faculty, administrators, and graduate students. Washington, D. C.: Association of American Colleges. Sandler, B. R. (1993). Women faculty at work in the classroom, or why it still hurts to be a woman in labor. Washington, D. C.: Center for Women Policy Studies. Schaffer, R. H. (1953). Job satisfaction as related to need satisfaction in work. Psychological Monographs, 67, 1-29. Schneider, A. (1998, September 11). Why don’t women publish as much as men? The Chronicle of Higher Education, pp. A7-9. 185 Jerry L. Allen and Ben B. Judd Schneider, B. (Ed.). (1990). Organizational culture and climate. San Francisco, CA: JossyBass. Scott, J. W. (1996, August 9). Defending the tradition of shared governance. The Chronicle of Higher Education, pp. B1-3. Shimanoff, S. B., & Jenkins, M. M. (2003). Leadership and gender: Challenging assumptions and recognizing resources. In R.Hirokawa, R. Carthcart, L. Samovar, & L. Henman (Eds.). Small group communication: Theory & practice (pp. 184-198). Los Angeles, CA: Roxbury Publishing Company. Shockley-Zalabak, P., & Morley, D. D. (1989). Adhering to organizational culture: What does it mean, why does it matter? Group and Organizational Studies, 14, 483-500. Smith, P. C., Kendall, L. M. & Hulin, C. L. (1975). The measurement of satisfaction in work and retirement. Chicago: Rand-McNally & Company. Smolen, B. W. (1983). Effect of faculty participation in the decision-making process upon perceptions of organizational climate and job satisfaction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland. Tannen, D. (1990). You just don’t understand. New York, NY: Ballantine. U. S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (1994). Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York, NY: Wiley. The wage gap. (1996, March-April). Ms., 46-37. Weatherly, K. A., & Beach, L. A. (1996). Organizational culture and decision making. In L. Beach (Ed.). Decision making in the workplace: A unified perspective (pp. 117-132). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Weider-Hatfield, D. (1987). Differences in self-reported leadership behavior as a function of biological sex and psychological gender. Women's Studies in Communication, 10, 1-4. West, M. S., & Curtis, J. W. (2006). AAUP Faculty Gender Equity Indicators 2006. Washington, D.C. American Association of University Professors. Wheeless, L. R., Wheeless, V. E., & Howard, R. D. (1984). The relationships of communication with supervisor and decision-participation to employee job satisfaction. Communication Quarterly, 32, 222-232. Williams, G. P., & Zirkel, P. A. (1989). Shift in collective bargaining issue in higher education: A review of the literature. Journal of Collective Negotiations in the Public Sector, 18, 73-86. Williams, J. (2000a, October 27). How the tenure track discriminates against women, The Chronicle of Higher Education, p. B10. Williams, J. (2000b, December 15). What stymies women’s academic careers? It’s personal, The Chronicle of Higher Education, pp. B5-6. Wilson, R. (2001, November 9). For women with tenure and families: Moving up the ranks is challenging. The Chronicle of Higher Education, pp. A11-13. Wilson, R. (2004, January 23). Women underrepresented in sciences at top research universities, study finds. The Chronicle of Higher Education, p. A9. Winter, D. A., & Green, S. B. (1987). Another look at gender-related differences in leadership behavior. Sex-Roles, 16, 41-55. Participation in Decision-Making 186 Human Communication. A Publication of the Pacific and Asian Communication Association. Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 181 –204. Mayor, Cheerleader, Lawyer, or Juggler? An Exploratory Study of CAO Leadership Metaphors and Challenges G.L. Forward Point Loma Nazarene University Kathleen Czech Point Loma Nazarene University G. L. Forward (Ph.D., The Ohio State University) is a Professor in the Department of Communication and Theatre at PLNU. Kathleen Czech (Ed.D, University of San Diego) is an Associate Professor in the same department. Address all correspondence to: GLForward@pointloma.edu . The authors wish to thank Patrick Allen, former Chief Academic Officer and Provost of PLNU for financial support and encouragement in the completion of this project. CAO Leadership 188 Abstract Two-hundred-twenty-four CAO members of the Council of Independent Colleges completed a survey suggesting metaphor to describe their position and assessing their own leadership style and communication behavior. The solicited metaphors (N=148) were sorted into four clusters labeled hierarchy, paradox, competence, and relational. These four clusters were then arranged in a 2 x 2 matrix reflecting ontological orientation (realist or idealist) as well as approach to interpersonal influence (macro or micro). CAO’s were also asked to identify the greatest challenge to their leadership effectiveness. The resultant answers were sorted into a four category typology consisting of political, relational, competence, and resource challenges to effectiveness. KEY WORDS: Leadership, Metaphors, Communication, Higher Education Administration 189 G.L. Forward and Kathleen Czech Introduction Colleges and universities are a ubiquitous, integral, and influential part of modern society and the nexus of many individual aspirations. In spite of this importance personally and socially, schools in the U.S. according to Ackerman and MaslinOstrowski (2002, p. 5) “are facing a dearth of leaders capable of providing good leadership.” Compounding the problem is the fact that the study of management in higher education has been relatively neglected in favor of studies focusing on the business sector (Mech, 1997). However, nascent research in higher education administration suggests that academic leaders specifically find themselves in a unique position with a leadership role that has no clear parallel in business or industry due to its dualistic nature. Although most organizations require shared governance between individuals or groups, with sometimes inconsistent and even incompatible goals, “dualism” is the defining characteristic of academic leadership as the responsibilities and interests of trustees, administrators, and faculty are continuously negotiated through bureaucratic maneuvers (Edelstein, 1997, p. 58). As a result, according to Gmelch (2000), academic leaders may occupy the least studied and most misunderstood management position in America. The foregoing is troubling indeed given the pervasive belief that leadership is the single most critical component of organizational success (Birnbaum, 1992). Fortunately, disillusionment with traditional leadership models and the realization that leadership must be practiced in a troubled, complex, and crisis-ridden context has led to a new wave of leadership studies in academe. In this project we will contribute to this literature in two distinct ways. First, we solicited academic leadership metaphors from our sample in an effort to create a leadership typology and uncover taken-for-granted assumptions about the CAO role and the interpersonal dynamics inherent in that role. This creation of a leadership metaphor typology, and a resultant 2 x 2 matrix predicated on underlying ontological assumptions and influence strategy, helps to bring these dynamics into sharper focus. Secondly, we solicited a list of leadership challenges that enabled us to create a second typology and relate that typology of challenges to the leadership metaphor types previously identified. Literature Review University Governance Organizational types. Martin and Semels (1997, p. 64) suggest that US universities tend to cluster into four organizational types that they label “collegial, archaic, political, and bureaucratic.” The collegial model is most likely found in small, liberal arts colleges with very direct and immediate communication linkages between organizational members. The archaic structural style represents the “organized anarchy” sometimes found in huge, “flagship” state universities with massive numbers of students and a long, rich history which sometimes ossifies historic practices. The political model is distinguished by a diffusion of formal power, the dominance of unions and entrenched committees, and the proliferation of special interest groups often found in complex state university systems. Finally, the bureaucratic university is characterized by centralized decision making, the proliferation of rules and policies, and a highly structured organizational environment. This last organizational type is by far the most prevalent CAO Leadership 190 model of university governance in the US and often is a dominant characteristic of the other three organizational types as well (Martin & Semels, 1997). The Chief Academic Officer. The position of the chief academic officer (CAO) is especially challenging and deserving of attention. Traditionally the titles Academic VP, Provost, and Dean of Faculty were used interchangeably to identify broadly those responsible for academic oversight at US colleges and universities. The more recent use of titles such as VP for Academic Affairs and Chief Academic Officer (CAO) reflects the more complex organizational structure and management responsibilities currently expected of academic leadership (Martin & Samels, 1997). This increasing complexity of the CAO role often results in high job stress, role ambiguity, and high rates of turnover. As a result, CAO’s frequently report low job satisfaction and leave administration to return to the classroom. One explanation for this phenomenon concerns the scope of the CAO’s position which is often wider and more complex than that of the president (Bright & Richards, 2001). CAO’s, more than any other leader, link the central administration with academic departments and become the crucial backbone of university decision-making (Wolverton, Wolverton, & Gmelch, 1999). Every CAO must be prepared to deal with a bewildering variety of developing relationships, priorities, and problems. CAO’s are frequently caught between the expectations of individual faculty, college departments, and those of the central administration (Wolverton, 1984). According to Bright and Richards (2001), the CAO is “invoked as the cause and explanation of unpopular campus policies, as the reason for a failed promotion, and for other sources of discontent. In short, the provost is “like a dean but even more remote and terrible” (Bright & Richards, 2001, p. 233). A role of this scope and imagery requires a leadership style that will ultimately cultivate these working relationships and motivate and maintain continued development of faculty and staff (Mech, 1997). Unfortunately there is a temptation to say more than we know about leadership in higher education when our assumptions are predicated on models inappropriately gleaned from other organizational contexts. Leadership Paradigms in Higher Education Administration Leadership in academia. Following Wolverton and Gmelch (2002, p. 33), we define academic leadership as “the act of building a community of scholars to set direction and achieve common purposes through the empowerment of faculty and staff.” Leadership of this type requires movement away from coercion and bureaucratic manipulation toward a collaborative orientation that holds everyone accountable to a higher vision of the organization. In the process of working collaboratively, opportunities will also be created to involve, reward, resource, recognize, and empower faculty in ways that transcend traditional bureaucratic hierarchy. Lastly, this conceptualization of leadership entails the mutual negotiation of a joint vision that elevates priorities above the ordinary and mundane in a way that inspires our best efforts. Next we will contrast bureaucratic approaches to leadership with the possibilities revealed by transformational leadership when applied to higher education administration. Traditional bureaucratic models. The leadership dilemma noted previously stems both from the nature of the job itself as well as the leadership model routinely embraced in higher education. Leadership in colleges and universities is problematic because of the dual control systems, conflict between professional and administrative authority, unclear goals, and other professional organizations (Bensimon, Neumann, & 191 G.L. Forward and Kathleen Czech Birnbaum, 1989). However, many institutions seem by default, to develop a bureaucratic model of leadership in an effort to establish “strict boundaries” and keep things “neat and tidy” (Ackerman & Maslin-Ostrowski, 2002, p. 5). Leaders who employ a bureaucratic framework emphasize setting priorities, making orderly decisions, and communicating through established lines of authority (Birnbaum, 1992). In a study by Lees, Smith, and Stockhouse (1994), higher education administrators most often defined leadership as “a one way approach whose purpose was getting others within the organization to conform to or comply with the leader’s directives by using various sources of social power” (p.12). The bureaucratic leader can control the institution, but this style of leadership does not motivate the faculty and the staff, who must approve or at least implement new programs and other changes if they are to be successful (Wolverton, 1984). Until a new leadership paradigm can be implemented, CAO’s will be placed in the position of needing to create change in an environment with little motivation or commitment to do so (Montez & Wolverton, 2000). Transformational leadership. Over the past several years, much attention has been given to the notion of transformational leadership as a contrast to traditional bureaucracy (Tracey & Hinkin, 1998). Burns (1978) characterizes transformational leadership as a process that motivates followers by appealing to higher ideals and moral values. Transformational leadership seeks to raise followers’ levels of consciousness about the importance and value of specified and idealized goals and moving followers to address higher-level needs (Covrig, 2000). This type of leadership is concerned with follower’s values and beliefs, and asks followers to respond to a higher level of moral and ethical conduct (Northouse, 2001). Transformational leadership utilizes this commitment to emphasize the inspirational aspects of the relationships between leaders and followers (Brown & Moshavi, 2002). Communication in Higher Education Administration In order to create organization change and motivate the constituents of any organization, competent communication is essential. According to Zorn and Violanti (1996) communication is central to organizational function and to the daily goals of individuals in the organization. A major part of the CAO’s role is establishing effective relationships and a major part of that task involves effective communication (Hickson & Stacks, 1992). When viewing leadership as an influential relationship, a communicationbased perspective of leadership becomes paramount. Hackman and Johnson (2000) define leadership as human communication that modifies the attitudes and behaviors of others in order to meet shared group goals and needs. Leadership styles inherently display a distinct set of communication behaviors. Many CAO’s have expressed a desire to become more competent communicators. In a study by Townsend and Bassoppo-Mayo (1996), almost half of the respondents desired skills and knowledge in communication competence. The need for traditional communication skills of listening, speaking and writing, as well as the ability to mediate and resolve conflicts were expressed. Administrators must be able to arbitrate between several possible moral codes, and at the same time, portray a unified moral front to keep the organization on course (Covrig, 2000). If administrators can combine communication competencies with a transformational leadership style that inspires and elevates faculty CAO Leadership 192 and staff to higher levels of innovation, critical thinking, and morality, the revolution that higher education is calling for may begin. Therefore, in this study we will include several measures of communicator style, social support, and role negotiation in an effort to foreground “how” CAO’s are doing what they are doing as they interact with others. Metaphors and Leadership Perusal of the relevant literature reveals a rich history of discourse analysis in the organizational communication discipline (Bantz, 1993). As Fairhurst (2001, p. 407) notes, communication scholars are especially interested in analyzing symbolic forms of communication in order to identify recurrent themes, tensions, goals, and sense-making devices in organizational life. As such, a useful method for understanding the leadership role of the CAO is through the use of metaphor analysis (Forward, 2001). The importance of metaphor analysis as a methodology is highlighted by Foss (1989, p. 359) who summarized the arguments of I. A. Richards and Kenneth Burke with the assertion that “all thought is metaphoric.” Metaphor, according to these scholars, is an omnipresent attribute of thought and is logically prior to meaning and indeed is necessary to the generation of ideas and the discovery of “truth.” Metaphors serve as structuring principles that enable us to construct a particular reality based on the language we use to describe that reality (Foss, 1989). As such, metaphor analysis can be used as a diagnostic tool to analyze what is happening interpersonally in an organizational context and to surface taken-for-granted assumptions about normative behavior and expectations (Morgan, 1997). According to Fairhurst and Sarr (1996) leadership is preeminently “a language game.” Therefore a linguistic analysis provides a meaningful look at how individuals view thier roles, relationships, and orientation toward power (Linstead, 2001). The metaphors that individuals use to describe their roles may be indicators of deeply held organizational meanings, values, and proscribed actions (Putnam & Fairhurst, 2001). They are significant indicators of how CAO’s view themselves as leaders and the relationships that characterize leadership within higher education. Therefore, CAO’s were asked to describe their position in terms of a metaphor (simile) by completing the phrase: “A CAO is like a _______________ .” Research Questions To gain more insight into the unique dynamics of the CAO role, the following research questions are presented. RQ 1: What types of metaphors do CAO’s use to describe their role? RQ2: What types of leadership challenges do CAO’s identify in their work? 193 G.L. Forward and Kathleen Czech Method Research Participants The subjects in this study (N = 225) ranged in age from 31 to 76 with a mean age of 54 (SD = 8.7). Sixty-two percent (n = 139) were male and 38 % (n = 85) were female. The sample was predominately white (90.2 %, n = 202) but included 14 (6.3 %) individuals who identified themselves as Black/African-American. Many of the CAO’s were relative neophytes. Examination of the descriptive statistics revealed that a majority of the respondents (73.5 %, n = 165) had been in their present assignment for five years or less. The entire sample had a mean tenure in their present assignment of 4.8 years (SD = 5.5, Median = 3.0) with an average of 15 years teaching experience (SD = 8.3) prior to moving into an administrative post. A plurality of respondents (42.2 %, n = 95) serve as Chief Academic Officer in institutions that enroll between one to two thousand students. Only 14 (6.3 %) work in institutions that enroll five thousand or more students whereas 51 (22.7 %) serve schools that enroll fewer than one thousand students. Most survey respondents taught full-time prior to assuming administrative responsibilities. They listed more than fifty academic specialties which cluster broadly into nineteen disciplinary domains (Table 1). However, five academic disciplines including Literature, Education, History, Psychology, and Chemistry/Biology, account for over half of the sample (55 % n = 123). Research Procedures This research focuses on Chief Academic Officers in higher education. The sample frame was established by securing a membership directory from the Council of Independent Colleges headquartered in Washington, D.C. The CIC is a professional organization comprised of private, four-year colleges and universities in the U.S. One of the authors of this paper is a CIC member and attends their annual meeting. The membership directory identified 479 affiliated CAO’s at the time of this study. A survey packet was constructed consisting of a cover letter, survey, SASE, and separate response card so that names could be removed from the mailing list in preparation for a second mailing to non-respondents. The mailing list was prepared by removing the names of the 30 CAO’s included in a pilot test, as well as removing the CIC member co-author and two others who had vacated their positions. This resulted in an initial mailing sent to the 446 CAO’s remaining. One-hundred-eighty surveys were returned (40.4 %) following this first mailing. An identical second packet was sent to those remaining on the list approximately 5 weeks later. This mailing generated an additional 46 (10.3 %) surveys for an accepting sample of 50.7 % (N = 226 with 1 unusable survey). This is an acceptable response rate for survey research in general and is especially robust given the top management tier of a university CAO (Baruch, 1999). As part of a larger project, respondents completed a four- page survey measuring variables of interest and soliciting leadership metaphors and challenges to leadership in academia. The final section of the survey collected demographic data about the respondents (e.g., age, sex, ethnicity), their personal history (e.g., discipline, teaching history, tenure as CAO), and institutional enrollment. CAO Leadership 194 Table 1: Academic Discipline of Origin for CAO’s (n = 223) Academic Discipline N % Humanities 90 40.4 39 20 13 12 6 17.5 9.0 5.8 5.4 2.7 53 23.7 Education/Administration/Curriculum 28 Business/Management/Operations 10 Nursing/Medical/Physiology 9 Physical Education 3 Food Nutrition 2 Law 1 12.6 4.5 4.0 1.3 .9 .4 Literature/English History Bible/Ethics/Philosophy Music/Art/Photography Languages Professional Studies Social Sciences Psychology/Counseling Political Science Communication Sociology Natural Sciences Chemistry/Biology Mathematics Physics/Engineering Geology/Geography 48 21.5 19 14 9 6 8.5 6.3 4.0 2.7 32 14.3 17 11 2 2 7.6 4.9 .9 .9 195 G.L. Forward and Kathleen Czech Results CAO Role Metaphors The first research question sought to identify the types of metaphors CAO’s use to describe their academic role. Survey recipients responded to the following prompt: “A CAO is like a _______________ .” One hundred forty eight CAO’s supplied a metaphor and a brief explanation in response to this request. Each metaphor and explanation was placed on a separate card with the intention of identifying any thematic patterns that might emerge (Boyatzis, 1998). Using the constant comparative method (Rubin & Rubin, 1995), the entire stack of cards was sorted into clusters. The first author utilized key terms and ideas from the raw information to inductively construct four data-driven categories focusing either on hierarchical placement, relational influence, expert knowledge or competence, or irony and paradox. (Boyatzis, 1998). The second author then re-sorted all 148 cards into these previously supplied categories. Following McGee and Cegala (1998), a conservative measure of inter-rater reliability was employed yielding a coefficient of .90 for the entire data set. Table 2 lists the four metaphor types, representative examples, frequencies, and inter-rater reliabilities for each individual category. Table 2: CAO Metaphors ________________________________________________________________________ Metaphor Type N % Reliability ________________________________________________________________________ Hierarchy/Legitimate Authority (Including COO, Senior Manager, CEO, and Mayor) 52 35.1 .88 Relational Influence (Including Coach, Pastor, Parent, and Cheerleader) 47 31.8 .88 Competence/Expert Knowledge (Including Attorney/Judge, Broker, and Teacher) 26 17.6 .80 Irony/Paradox 23 15.5 .76 (Including Herder of Cats, Juggler, Circus Ringmaster, and Playground Monitor) ________________________________________________________________________ Note. Inter-rater reliability was calculated using the formula: Agreements minus Disagreements Agreements plus Disagreements CAO Leadership 196 A plurality of responses (n = 52, 35.1%) identified the CAO role in terms of placement in the organizational hierarchy either as the central figure or “number two” person in support of the president. Central figure metaphors included images like CEO, mayor, hospital president, and high school principal. These metaphors tended to focus on the enormous responsibility for budgeting, daily decisions, and internal management of the organization. The most common metaphor highlighting the role of a “number two” person was COO. Explanations for this designation tended to focus on managing the academic life of the university on behalf of the President or Board of Trustees who establish university policy and vision. Another large subset (n = 47, 31.8 %) focused on relationships and facilitating faculty member growth by building interpersonal relationships and encouraging the development of others. This relational emphasis was expressed in two ways in this subset. The first used metaphors that expressed relational and developmental functions like servant or cheerleader. The second cluster described the same functions but did so in terms of specific jobs like coach or conductor. However, the explanations for these metaphors highlighted a concern for the feelings, gifts, and needs of others. An additional 26 (17.6 %) CAO’s described their role in terms of a knowledgebased expertise. These metaphors emphasize skills, credentials, or professional roles (e.g., lawyer) that allow CAO’s to utilize specialized knowledge in the service of others and yet remain somewhat independent. Finally, twenty-three respondents (15.5 %) took an ironic view by using something “inappropriate” to capture the unique ethos of the job. This group used humor and paradox to highlight the unpredictable and uncontrollable aspects of life in an academic bureaucracy. Herder of cats, juggler, and playground supervisor were representative images of this fourth group of metaphors. Challenges to Effective Leadership In analyzing the second question the same procedures were utilized, placing each challenge on a card, and sorting the cards for themes. Key terms were identified and again themes emerged among the plurality of responses. While not originally predicted, clear parallels were discovered between the role metaphor categories and the CAO’s reported greatest challenge to their leadership. The same category types used to conceptualize leadership were utilized to classify the CAO’s leadership challenges. The leadership challenge types, examples, and frequencies are summarized in Table 3 and their relationship to the leadership metaphor types are represented in Figure 1. The first set of challenges dealt with issues relating to organizational hierarchy and legitimate authority (n=47, 22.9%). From this cluster several sub-categories were identified. Several CAO’s listed the president as their single greatest challenge to their leadership. Often in this context the president was further described as a “micromanager”. The organizational structure also referred to specific interactions with the Board of Trustees. The final subset dealt with the overall power and politics in the organization as manifested in traditional policies and practices. These challenges included knowing the “law of the land” and dealing with “unwritten policies and procedures”. CAO’s also reported several interpersonal relationship aspects (n = 70, 34.1%) that effected their leadership. These relational issues tended to deal almost exclusively 197 G.L. Forward and Kathleen Czech with faculty relationships. Three sub categories were apparent within the relational issues identified. CAO’s repeatedly reported the development of trust as one of their greatest challenges. CAO’s felt that earning the “trust of the faculty” was a crucial dynamic affecting their leadership. The next relational issue CAO’s dealt with was conflict. Conflict cumulated in many forms, including conflict among faculty, departments, and staff. Most conflict issues expressed a fragmented view of the purpose of the academic institution as a whole. Dealing with change was the third reoccurring theme. CAO’s felt that “faculty resistance to change” was a major hurdle in their quest for effective leadership. . Figure 1: CAO Leadership & Metaphor Matrix Challenges President, Trustees, Tradition Challenges Preparation, Training, Personality Techno-Social (Realism) Hierarchy Competence Systemic (Macro-Influence) Referent (Micro-Influence) Paradox Challenges Money, Time, Resources Extreme Relational Moral-Aesthetic (Idealism) Mid-Range Challenges Interpersonal Trust, Conflict, Resistance to Change Balanced CAO Leadership 198 As with the leadership metaphors, CAO’s expressed competency based concerns with the demands of their position (n = 16, 8%). This third category addressed CAO’s challenges in two specific areas. The first was with job preparation and training. Some felt that little had been done to prepare them for their role, especially if they only had a “teaching background”. Along with preparation, many cited their own personality traits as hindering their leadership potential. For example, one CAO noted his own sense of “humor and irony” as off-putting to some people and a subsequent hindrance to his performance and ability to relate to those people The final category we developed for greatest CAO leadership challenge revolved around limited resources, both tangible and intangible. This resource category was compared to the fourth metaphor role category of paradox. The rationale for this was simply the irony of needing more by way of resources than one will ever be able to obtain. This theme was mentioned quite often by our respondents (n = 72, 35.1%). The issues here centered on inadequate money or time to do the job. Furthermore, the workload of the job was reported as one that is simply “impossible to do”. A general lack of support was also identified by several as impacting effective CAO leadership. Discussion The purpose of this study was to specifically explore CAO conceptualizations of their leadership role through metaphor analysis. In addition, we sought to contribute to the extant literature by identifying challenges to leadership effectiveness as experienced by those responsible for providing direction in academic administration. CAO Role Metaphors and Challenges One of the most frequently studied sense-making devices in qualitative communication research concerns the use of metaphor (Lindlof, 1995). The metaphors that individuals use to describe their roles and are indicators of deeply held organizational meanings, values, and proscribed actions (Putnam & Fairhurst, 2001). As such, they are significant indicators of how CAO’s view themselves as leaders and the relationships that should characterize their leadership in the academy. All metaphors tend to foreground certain aspects of organizational life, while simultaneously minimizing and deemphasizing others (Putnam & Fairhurst, 2001). The four metaphor clusters identified in this research can be meaningfully arranged in a 2 x 2 matrix on the basis of two underlying dimensions including ontological orientation and focus of power and influence (see Figure 1). The first dimension, as explicated by Gowler and Legge (1996), contains two contrasting ontological orientations. The techno-social or “realist” orientation tends to foreground issues relevant to the economic, mission-related goals of the organization. This approach focuses on serving the legitimate interests of the organization as traditionally understood in bureaucratic institutions like higher education. The hierarchy metaphor cluster does this by highlighting the CAO’s position and authority in the bureaucratic structure. The competence cluster does so by focusing specifically on the knowledge, skills, and abilities most likely to add value to the organization and further organizational interests. 199 G.L. Forward and Kathleen Czech The moral-aesthetic or “idealist” ontological orientation tends to foreground issues related to truth, beauty, and implicational meanings. The paradoxes and irony inherent in modern organizational life have long been obscured by the “myth of rationality” or treated as dysfunctional behavior to be corrected (Trethewey & Ashcraft, 2004). Instead, paradox brings into focus incongruous structures, policies, and practices that exist in all organizations and may even be necessary to function effectively (Tracy, 2004). The paradox cluster of metaphors does this by bringing practical dilemmas into sharp focus and highlighting the limitations inherent in supposed rationality. Pearce (2004, p. 175), reflecting on her tenure as an interim dean, concluded that typical academic governance “…puts adjectives like Byzantine, red tape, bureaucratic, ossified, and Kafkaesque to shame.” The use of paradoxical metaphors can bring into sharp focus the inevitable disparity between the real and ideal in everyday organizational practices (Stohl & Cheney, 2001). According to Putnam and Fairhurst (2001), irony and paradox can be liberating, empowering, a source of counterintuitive insight, and a means of encouraging discussion and diversity. The relational metaphor cluster represents moral-aesthetic idealism by focusing on interpersonal dynamics highlighting nurture, servanthood, and personal supportiveness in CAO-faculty interaction. A relational style foregrounds the people in the organization and how they actualize and grow, in addition to the ways they can contribute to the organization and corporate goals. The assumption here is that investing in the development and growth of individuals will both motivate and enable them to make greater contributions to their department and the university. Table 3: Challenges to CAO Leadership ______________________________________________________________________ Challenge Type N % _______________________________________________________________________ Political Challenges 47 22.9 % (President, Trustees, Tradition) Relational Challenges (interpersonal trust, conflict, change) 70 34.1 % Competence Challenges ( preparation, training, personality traits) 16 7.8 % Resource Challenges (money, time) 72 35.1 % The second underlying dimension in the 2 x 2 matrix reflects the ground of power and influence in an organizational context. This dimension is parsed on the basis of orientation toward impersonal organizational structures and processes or orientation toward personal relationships, in exercising influence. Both the hierarchy and paradox CAO Leadership 200 metaphor clusters operate at the macro-level of organizational structure either by endorsing or resisting the implications of bureaucracy. The competence and relational clusters focus on the micro-level of interpersonal dynamics focused either on self (competence) or other (interpersonal relationship). This matrix can also be used to help create a more complete picture of the challenges, role, and system in which CAO’s operate on a daily basis (Table 3). When used in this way, the findings of the matrix resonate with the literature characterizing the CAO role as one full of conflict and ambiguity (Wolverton, Wolverton, & Gmelch, 1999). The matrix has further implications for the effectiveness of the CAO’s leadership style. The matrix (see Figure 1) can be viewed through three additional lenses borrowed from family communication research (Olson, Russell, & Sprenkle, 1989). The first lens is located in the center of the diagram which calls for a balance of all four dimensions. In this inner circle, the goal of the CAO is to find a balance between the realist and idealist by using both legitimate and referent forms of influence and power. This balance would indicate a leadership style that is able to focus on the ideal vision of the institution while simultaneously maintaining a realistic or pragmatic orientation that may temper the ideal. This perspective would further recognize the use of both systemic and individual referent power to maintain relationships, build trust, and pursue needed change. This area of balance would incorporate all of the numerous roles of the CAO while still dealing as effectively as possible with the challenges identified. As one moves out of the area of balance on the figure into mid-range, the leadership and role challenges CAO’s face become more difficult to overcome. This is the area in which CAO’s exhibit over-reliance on the behaviors of certain dimensions and the concomitant under-utilization of the opposite dimension (Swenson & HenkelJohnson, 2002). Finally the extreme areas of the figure represent a system where leadership is possibly pre-defined and influence and power only come from one source resulting in either rigid or chaotic, and enmeshed or disengaged leadership as behavior gravitates toward the extremes (Swenson & Henkel-Johnson, 2002). These extremes would likely exacerbate the challenges of the opposite quadrant and lead to little to no effectiveness for the CAO. This phenomenon lends support to the provocative notion that the skills organizations reward with promotion to higher office may not be the skills needed to keep one there (Richmond & Martin, 1998). Conclusion Johnson (1999) cites data suggesting that a majority of employed adults work for someone with poor leadership skills. There is no reason to conclude that academics are an exception to this generalization since many CAO’s are selected on the basis of skills and criteria that do not reflect the demands of their administrative assignment (Bogue, 1994). As such, there are three practical implications that can be drawn from this research to aid in understanding how CAO’s function in their administrative role. First, this project highlights the fact that metaphor analysis is a useful method for exploring meaning in organizational life. One way it does this is by explicating metaphors in order to surface sub rosa conceptualizations of how CAO’s view themselves as leaders and the dynamics that should characterize their interactions (Forward, 2000). Metaphor analysis 201 G.L. Forward and Kathleen Czech also helps in developing conceptual frames and typologies necessary to understand the inherent tensions and paradox in organizational life (Ashcraft & Trethewey, 2004). Secondly, communication matters for educational leaders and their followers. A brochure produced by the Educational Management Network (2002) concludes that the skills most essential for academic administrator success are effective interpersonal communication and team-building abilities. Effective communication creates an organizational environment in which CAO’s can receive needed emotional and information support and where followers are listened to and engaged in an active process of role negotiation. Lastly, this research hints at a link between leadership style and challenges to leadership. Pascarella (1996, p. 9) has argued: “We need a spiritual foundation for working together to manage our technical capabilities and our human faults.” Unfortunately, long emergent assumptions and practices have accumulated to shape workplaces that often stifle the human spirit and discourage displays of our humanity and spirituality (Pascarella, 1996). As Bogue (1994, p. xi) notes, “a few collegiate leaders would appear to suffer not only from a paucity of ideas but a poverty of ideals” including integrity, candor, stewardship, humility, and compassion. This project makes a beginning contribution to the literature by suggesting there is a link between communication, challenges, and leadership. CAO Leadership 202 References Ackerman, R. H., & Maslin-Ostrowski, P. (2002). The wounded leader: How real leadership emerges in times of crisis. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Ashcraft, K. L., & Trethewey, A. (2004). Developing tension: An agenda for applied research on the organization of irrationality. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 32(2), 171-181. Bantz, C. R. (1993). Understanding organizations: Interpreting organizational communication cultures. Columbia: University of South Carolina Press. Baruch, Y. (1999). Response rate in academic studies: A comparative analysis. Human Relations, 52(4), 421-438. Bensimon, E., Neumann, A., & Birnbaum, R. (1989). Making sense of administrative leadership: The “L” word in higher education. ASHE-ERIC Higher Education Report No. 1. Washington, D.C.: School of Education and Human Development, The George Washington University. Birnbaum, R. (1992). How academic leadership works: Understanding success and failure in the college presidency. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Bogue, E. G. (1994). Leadership by design: Strengthening integrity in higher education. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Boyatzis, R. E. (1998). Transforming qualitative information: Thematic analysis and code development. Thousand Oaks: Sage. Bright, D.F., & Richards, M.P. (2001). The academic deanship: Individual careers and institutional roles. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Brown, F. W., & Moshavi, D. (2002). Herding academic cats: Faculty reactions to transformational and contingent reward leadership by department chairs. Journal of Leadership Studies, 8(3), 79-93. Burns, J. (1978). Leadership. New York: Harper & Row. Covrig, D.M. (2000). The organizational context of moral dilemmas: The role of moral leadership in administration in making and breaking dilemmas. The Journal of Leadership Studies, 7(1), 40-59. Edelstein, M. G. (1997). Academic governance: The art of herding cats. In J. Martin and J. E. Samels (Eds.), First among equals: The role of the Chief Academic Officer (pp. 58-78). Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press. Educational Management Network. (2002). Success factors for academic Deans [Brochure]. Oak Brook, IL: Witt/Kieffer. Fairhurst, G. T. (2001). Dualisms in leadership research. In F.M. Jablin & L.L. Putnam (Eds.), The new handbook of organizational communication (pp.379439). Thousand Oaks: Sage. Fairhurst, G., & Sarr, R. (1996). The art of framing. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Forward, G. L. (2001). Servant or CEO? A metaphor analysis of leadership in a nonprofit context. The New Jersey Journal of Communication, 9(2), 145-165. Forward, G. L. (2000). Clergy stress and role metaphors: An exploratory study. The Journal of Communication and Religion, 23(2), 158-184. Foss, S. K. (1989). Rhetorical criticism: Exploration & practice (2nd ed.). Prospect 203 G.L. Forward and Kathleen Czech Heights, IL: Waveland. Gmelch, W.H. (2000, February). The new dean: Taking charge and learning the job. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Association of Colleges for Teacher Education, Chicago, IL. Gowler, D., & Legge, K. (1996). The meaning of management and the management of meaning. In S. Linstead, R. G. Small, & P. Jeffcutt (Eds.), Undrstanding management (pp. 34-50). London: Sage. Hackman, M., & Johnson, C. (2000). Leadership: A communication perspective. Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland. Hickson, M., & Stacks, D. (Eds.). (1992). Effective communication for academic chairs. Albany: State University of New York Press. Johnson, W. B. (1999). Personality characteristics of future military leaders. Military Medicine. Retrieved April 28, 2002 from http://proquest.umi.com Lees, K., Smith, D., & Stockhouse, J. (1994, October). Implicit theoretical leadership frameworks of higher education administration. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Northern Rocky Mountain Educational Research Association, Sun Valley, ID. Lindlof, T. R. (1995). Qualitative communication research methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Linstead, S. (2001). Rhetoric and organizational control: A framework for analysis. In R. Westwood & S. Linstead (Eds.), The language of organization (pp. 217-240). London: Sage. Martin, J., & Samels, J. E. (Eds.) (1997). First among equals: The current roles of the Chief Academic Officer. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press. McGee, D. S., & Cegela, D. J. (1998). Patient communication skills training for improved communication competence in the primary care medical consultation. Journal of Applioed Communication Research, 26, 412-430. Mech, T. (1997). The managerial roles of chief academic officers. Journal of Higher Education, 68(3), 282-298. New York: Academic Press. Montez, J., & Wolverton, M. (2000, April). The challenge of the deanship. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA. Morgan, G. (1997). Imaginization: New mindsets for seeing, organizing, and managing. San Francisco: Berrett-Koehler. Northouse, P. (2001). Leadership theory and practice. Thousand Oaks: Sage. Olson, D.H., Russell, C.S. & Sprenkle, D.H. (1989). Circumplex Model: Systemic assessment and treatment of families. New York: Haworth. Pascarella, P. (1996). The ten commandments of the workplace. Grand Rapids, MI: Zondervan. Putman, L., & Fairhurst, G. (2001). Discourse analysis in organizations: Issues and concerns. In F.M. Jablin & L.L. Putnam (Eds.), The new handbook of organizational communication (pp.78-136). Thousand Oaks: Sage. Richmond, V. P., & Martin, M. M. (1998). Sociocommunicative style and sociocommunicative orientation. In J. C. McCroskey, J. A Daly, M. M. Martin, & CAO Leadership 204 M. J. Beatty (Eds.), Communication and personality: Trait perspectives (pp. 133148). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton. Rubin, H. J., & Rubin, I. S. (1995). Qualitative interviewing. Thousand Oaks: Sage. Stohl, C., & Cheney, G. (2001). Participatory practices/paradoxical practices: Communication and the dilemmas of organizational democracy. Management Communication Quarterly, 14(3), 349-407. Swenson, D. X., & Henkel-Johnson, G. (2002, March). Dealing with juvenile sex offenders with impaired families. Paper presented at the 6th Annual Conference of the MnTASA, Minneapolis, MN. Townsend, B. K., & Bassoppo-Moyo, S. (1996). If I’d only known: Administrative preparation that could have made a difference. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New York: NY. Tracy, S. J. (2004). Dialectic, contradiction, or double bind? Analyzing and theorizing employee reactions to organizational tension. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 32(2), 119-146. Tracey,B., & Hinkin, T. (1998). Transformational leadership or effective managerial practices? Thousand Oaks: Sage. Trethewey, A., & Ashcraft, K. L. (2004). Practicing disorganization: The development of applied perspectives on living with tension. Journal of Applied Communication, 32(2), 81-88. Wolverton, M., & Gmelch, W. (2002). College deans: Leading from within. Westport, CT: Oryx Press. Wolverton, M., Wolverton, M.L., & Gmelch W. (1999). The impact of role conflict and ambiguity on academic deans. The Journal of Higher Education, 70, 80-106. Wolverton, R.E. (1984). The chief academic officer: Argus on the campus. In D.G. Brown (Ed), Leadership roles of chief academic officers (pp. 7-17). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Zorn, T.E., & Violanti, M.T., (1996). Communication abilities and individual achievement in organizations. Management Communication Quarterly, 10(2), 139-167. Human Communication. A Publication of the Pacific and Asian Communication Association. Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 205 – 218. Revisiting Teacher Immediacy in the HBCU and PWI Context: Do Teacher Immediacy and Interpersonal Communication Satisfaction Influence Student Retention? Mary L. Rucker Wright State University Joanna M. Davis-Showell Central State University Mary L. Rucker (Ph.D., Purdue University, 2000), Associate Professor in the Department of Communication, Wright State University, Dayton, OH. Joanna M. Davis-Showell (Ph.D., Howard University, 1998), Assistant Professor in the Communication Department, Central State University, Wilberforce, OH All correspondence regarding this study should be addressed to Dr. Rucker, mary.rucker@wright.edu. Revisiting Teacher Immediacy 206 Abstract The purpose of this study was to investigate the influence verbal and nonverbal teacher immediacy and interpersonal communication satisfaction have on student retention in two historically black colleges and universities (HBCUs) and a predominantly white (PWI). Two hundred seventy four students (140 HBCU students, 134 PWI students) participated in this study. The results indicated that verbal and nonverbal immediacy were not significant predictors of student retention. Hypotheses 1 and 2 were not supported. The paired t-test for the combined sample produced some significant results between interpersonal communication satisfaction and student retention. 207 Mary L. Rucker & Joanna M. Davis-Showell Introduction The purpose of this study was to investigate the relative influence verbal and nonverbal teacher immediacy and interpersonal communication satisfaction have on student retention in both predominantly white and historically black universities. In particular, four communication constructs were the focus of this study: 1) verbal teacher immediacy, 2) nonverbal teacher immediacy, 3) interpersonal communication satisfaction, and 4) student retention. Teacher Immediacy Extant literature extends our understanding of the different factors that influence communication satisfaction, and there is a growing demand in higher education to investigate and address issues of student retention and faculty-student communication satisfaction. Even though research has addressed teacher immediacy in both the HBCU and PWI context (Gendrin & Rucker, 2002, 2004), it has not addressed the relationships between teacher immediacy and interpersonal communication satisfaction and student retention. Rucker and Gendrin (2003) assert that affective and cognitive behaviors of instructors influence interactions between teachers and students, and the importance for instructors to know the impact their communication behaviors have on students. Communication theory and research suggest that verbal and nonverbal messages function differently in social interactions. Verbal messages function to convey the content of the message, whereas nonverbal messages function to establish the relationship. Thus, verbal messages appear to have their primary impact on cognitive responses, whereas nonverbal messages have their primary impact on affective responses. (Mottet, Beebe, Raffeld, & Paulsel, 2004, p. 2) Teacher verbal and nonverbal communication behaviors and students’ reactions to them can influence relationships (Paulsel & Chory-Assad, 2004). Verbal immediacy focuses on speech that provides “feedback, uses humor, gives personal examples in discussions, and addresses the individual student by name” (Gorham, 1988 as cited in Mottet, Beebe, Raffeld, & Paulsel, 2004, p.2). Nonverbal immediacy includes using eye contact, smiling, body positioning and movement or lack thereof, and instructors’ use of a variety of vocal expressions while talking to the class (Arbaugh, 2001; Gorham, 1988). Beyond this, some studies have reported that “communicators who engage in nonverbally immediate behaviors with others are seen by those others in a more positive way than they see people who do not engage in those communication behaviors” (Richmond, McCroskey, & Johnson, 2003, p.1). Immediacy is therefore conceptualized by Mehrabian (1966) as a behavior that communicates approachability and closeness between interactants (Mottet & Richmond, 1998), where the interactants construe the meaning of the interaction. Simonds (2001) contends that teacher immediacy, noted as behavior that can signal approachability and warmth, is relative to student communication satisfaction initiated in the cognitive and affective behaviors. In a classroom context, teacher immediacy is evident in the verbal and nonverbal processes by which teacher-student relationships begin to form, which suggests that communication satisfaction can begin to be realized when interactants’ verbal and nonverbal communication behaviors are accepted. Overall, this research determines that teacher immediacy underscores the vital premise for understanding the behaviors that create and maintain teacher/student relationships. As interpersonal communication satisfaction is inherent in the communication Revisiting Teacher Immediacy 208 event or activity and the social interactions in which teachers and students are engaged, this research adheres to the concept of interpersonal communication satisfaction to provide orientation into the messages that devise relationships. Interpersonal Communication Satisfaction Hecht (1978) “conceptualized [interpersonal] communication satisfaction as the positive reinforcement provided by a communication event that fulfills positive expectations” (p. 217) and argues that interpersonal satisfaction is a communication outcome. Hecht goes on to say that immediacy is a contributing factor to liking. Other scholars’ studies make a similar claim. That is, Hess and Smythe (2001) claim that immediacy is the causal mediator of relationship development between teachers and students. Graham (2004) contributes to this line of thinking and contends that teacher immediacy positively correlates with student satisfaction. She also argues that “instructors who help students feel good about themselves . . . contribute to the communication satisfaction of their students” (Graham, 2004, p. 217). By doing so, instructor interpersonal communication satisfaction provides the landscape for predetermining positive immediacy. Furthermore, positive immediacy is implicit in behaviors that engage satisfying communication. Congruent with this view, Hecht devised the Interpersonal Communication Satisfaction Inventory (Com-Sat Inventory) to assess one’s communication satisfaction in actual and recalled conversations. While the Com-Sat Inventory ascertains the verbal behavioral perspective of communication satisfaction, Proctor II & Wilcox (1993) assert the importance of understanding that we express our awareness of meanings in messages. They further argue that communication satisfaction is reflected in our thoughts and statements. Overall, Hecht developed an important point connecting the circumstances surrounding communication satisfaction that attends to the continuum of immediacy behavior and meanings in messages and notes that communication satisfaction is embedded in the “communication event” also known as the social interaction. Therefore, the social interactions between teachers and students facilitate relationship development. Student Retention Student retention is a major problem for the academic community (Lau, 2003). Instructors do not necessarily bear understanding of affective behaviors relative to students’ communication satisfaction and immediacy needs. Both faculty and student interactions and relationships are very significant to student retention (Schulte, Franklin, Hayes, Noble, & Jacobs, 2001). Pruitt (2005) defines retention as “retaining a student for consecutive regular semesters [and quarters]. Retention refers to students who enroll at a college or university and stay there until they graduate. Retention rates are generally measured by the percentage of first-time, full-time students who return for the following semester [or quarter]” (p. 50). Speaking to the issue of retention performance indicators for institutions, Pruitt also informs us that retention is Everything the institution undertakes to improve the quality of student life and learning for its students. It is a measure of how much student growth and learning occurs. It is a measure of how valued and respected students feel on your campus. It is a measure of how effectively your campus delivers what students expect, need, and want. (p. 53) 209 Mary L. Rucker & Joanna M. Davis-Showell Retention is important to institutional image, faculty/student morale, improved recruitment and retention of faculty and staff, and improved learning outcomes for students. Beyond this, retention is a contributing factor to students’ satisfaction with a school’s responsiveness to diverse populations, computer technology, campus life, support services, campus climate, concern for the student, registration effectiveness, instructional effectiveness, academic advising, and students’ overall college experience (Noel-Levitz, 2004). Lau’s (2003) research on factors affecting student retention suggests that teacher/student relationships can significantly motivate students to stay in school. Conversely, Tinto’s (1987) dynamic “Model of Institutional Departure maintains that retention is dependent on student satisfaction” (as cited in Lau, 2003, p.1). While documented research validates that student communication satisfaction and learning correlate with faculty interaction (Cascarilla & Terenzini, 1991), this research determines that teacher immediacy and interpersonal communication satisfaction are useful constructs in depicting factors that contribute to student retention. Classroom interaction and social interaction between instructors and students are important correlates to student retention. Communication scholars have examined that verbal and nonverbal communication behaviors are instrumental to facilitating positive interpersonal relationships between faculty and students, but they have not focused their research on whether teacher immediacy and interpersonal communication satisfaction influence student retention. This study extends the literature to include and/write in student retention in the teacher immediacy and interpersonal communication satisfaction literature. Therefore, the following hypotheses constitute this study: African American students will report that verbal and nonverbal teacher immediacy influences student retention at HBCUs. H2: Euro-American students will report that verbal and nonverbal teacher immediacy influence student retention at PWIs. RQ1: What is the relationship between verbal and nonverbal teacher immediacy and interpersonal communication satisfaction for both African American and EuroAmerican students? RQ2: What is the relationship between interpersonal communication satisfaction and student retention for both African American and Euro-American students? H1: Method Participants Two hundred seventy four students (140 African American students attending two HBCUs in the Midwest, 134 Euro-American students attending a PWI located in the Midwest) participated in this study. Ages ranged from 17 to 38 (m = 19.9, sd = 2.79). Student participation in this study was one means of receiving extra credit from their professors. Procedure During the last two weeks of the 2006 Summer Session B at the predominantly white institution and the end of the first week during Fall Semester 2006 at the historically black universities, participants were asked to complete a questionnaire asking their perceptions of Revisiting Teacher Immediacy 210 verbal and nonverbal teacher immediacy, interpersonal communication satisfaction, and student retention. The authors asked their fellow colleagues’ permission to survey students in 10 different classes at both HBCUs and the PWI. One of the authors teaches at one of the HBCUs and the other author teaches at the PWI. The authors collected 422 survey questionnaires, 200 from the two HBCUs and 222 from the PWI. Data was collected from two HBCUs located in the Midwest because each school has a small population of students. After the data were collected, the authors entered the gender and race of the HBCU and PWI students into separate SPSS file and ran a random sample procedure. If a number 2 appeared after the student’s name, then that student’s survey data was used for this study. As a result, 140 African American students and 134 Euro American students’ names were drawn from the sample. Measures Teacher Immediacy Scales: Teacher immediacy was measured using Gorham’s (1988) 20-item measure of verbal immediacy such as “My instructor ask questions that solicit view points or opinions” and “My instructor asks questions or encourages students to talk,” and Richmond et al.'s (1987) 14-item measure of nonverbal immediacy such as “My instructor sits behind the desk while teaching” and “My instructor smiles at individual students in the class.” Participants were asked to evaluate the frequency with which the instructor teaching the course immediately preceding the one for which they were filling the questionnaire used the immediacy behaviors. Each item was rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 = never to 4 = very often. The reliability estimates for the verbal and nonverbal immediacy scales were: α = .81 and α = .92, respectively. Interpersonal Communication Satisfaction Scale: Communication satisfaction was measured using Hecht’s (1978) 16-item measure of interpersonal communication satisfaction such as “The other person let me know that I was communicating effectively” and “I was very satisfied with the conversation.” Each item was rated on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 = disagree to 7 = agree. The reliability estimate for the interpersonal communication satisfaction scale was: α = .87. Student Retention Scale: Student retention was measured using Noel-Levitz (2004) national conference on student retention materials 11-item measure of retention such as “I am satisfied with the school’s responsiveness to diverse populations,” “I am satisfied with academic advising,” and “I am satisfied with my overall college experience.” Each item was measured using a 5-point likert scale ranging from 1 = not satisfied at all to 5 = very satisfied. The reliability estimate for the scale was α = .90. Data Analysis Hypotheses 1 and 2 were tested using simple regression analyses. The first research question was answered using a simple regression analysis, and the second research question was answered using t-tests. Results Regression Analyses Hypothesis 1 predicted that African American Students will report that verbal teacher 211 Mary L. Rucker & Joanna M. Davis-Showell immediacy influences student retention at the two historically black colleges and universities (HBCUs), and hypothesis 2 predicted that Euro-Americans will report that verbal and nonverbal teacher immediacy influences student retention at the predominantly white institution (PWI). To explore the relative influence of verbal and nonverbal teacher immediacy on student retention, simple regression analyses were conducted. Verbal immediacy and nonverbal immediacy were the explanatory variables (independent) and student retention was the response variable (dependent). The results of the analyses are presented in Table 1 for the African American sample and Table 2 for the Euro-American sample. For the African American sample, the results indicated that verbal immediacy (standardized b = .24, t = 1.764, p. > .05) and nonverbal immediacy (standardized b = -.25, t = -1.847, p. > .05) were not significant predictors of student retention at the two HBCUs, and explained three percent of the variance. The hypothesis was not supported (see Table 1). Table 1: Regression Analysis: Verbal and Nonverbal Immediacy and Student Retention: African American Sample Variables Student Retention Verbal Immediacy Nonverbal Immediacy Beta .24 -.25 t 1.764 -1.847 Sig. Adj. R2 Model Sig. .12 .03 p. > .05 F 2.193 p. > .05 p. > .05 For the Euro-American sample, the results indicated that verbal immediacy (standardized b = .09, t = .669, p. > .05) and nonverbal immediacy (standardized b = .08, t = .580, p. > .05) were not significant predictors of student retention at the PWI, and explained less than one percent of the variance. The hypothesis was not supported (see Table 2). Table 2: Regression Analysis: Verbal and Nonverbal Immediacy and Student Retention: Euro-American Sample Variables Beta t Sig. Adj. R2 Model Sig. F .48 -.008 p > .05 .750 Student Retention Verbal Immediacy .09 .669 p. > .05 Nonverbal Immediacy .08 .580 p. > .05 ______________________________________________________________________ The first research question investigated the relationship between verbal and nonverbal teacher immediacy and interpersonal communication satisfaction for both African American and Euro-American students. The results are indicated in Table 3. For the African American sample, the results indicated that instructor verbal immediacy (standardized b = -.05, t = -.326, p. > .05) and nonverbal immediacy (standardized b = -.10, t = -.683, p. > .05) behaviors were not significant predictors of interpersonal communication satisfaction for students attending the two Revisiting Teacher Immediacy 212 HBCUs. However, for the Euro-American sample, the results indicated that instructor verbal immediacy (standardized b = .61, t = 5.804, p. < .01) and nonverbal immediacy (standardized b = -.386, t = -3.320, p. < .01) behaviors were significant predictors of interpersonal communication satisfaction for students attending the PWI. The results are indicated in Table 4. Table 3: Regression Analysis: Verbal and Nonverbal Immediacy and Interpersonal Communication Satisfaction: African-American Sample Variables Beta t Interpersonal Communication Satisfaction Verbal Immediacy .-.05 -.326 Nonverbal Immediacy -.10 -.683 Sig. Adj. R2 .48 -.01 Model Sig. p > .05 F .519 p. > .05 p. > .05 Table 4: Regression Analysis: Verbal and Nonverbal Immediacy and Interpersonal Communication Satisfaction: Euro-American Sample Variables Beta t Interpersonal Communication Satisfaction Verbal Immediacy .61 5.084 Nonverbal Immediacy -.39 -3.230 Sig. Adj. R2 .000 .27 Model Sig. p < .01 F 13.33 p. < .01 p. < .01 Paired T-tests The second research question investigated the relationship between interpersonal communication satisfaction (ICSI) and student retention for both African American and EuroAmerican students. Paired t-tests were used to answer this question for the combined sample. The results of the analyses are presented in Tables 5. The results indicated that ICSI3 [I would like to have another conversation like this one with my instructor] (t = 7.153, p < .001); ICSI 4 [The instructor genuinely wanted to get to know me] (t = 2.856, p < .01); ICSI5 [I was very dissatisfied with the conversation with my instructor] (t = -1.693, p < 001); ICSI6 [I had something else to do besides personally communicating with my instructor after class] (t = 7.330, p < .001); and ICSI10 [The instructor expressed a lot of interest in what I had to say] (t = -4.418, p < .001) produced significant results between interpersonal communication satisfaction and student retention 213 Mary L. Rucker & Joanna M. Davis-Showell Table 5: Paired t-tests between Interpersonal Communication Satisfaction and Student Retention African Americans Euro-Americans ___________________ ___________________ Student Retention M SD n M SD n 1. The other person let me know that I was communicating effectively 2. Nothing was accomplished 3. I would like to have another conversation like this one** 4. The other person genuinely wanted to get to know me*** 5. I was very dissatisfied with the Conversation*** 6. I felt that during the conversation 7. I felt that during the conversation I was able to present myself as I wanted the other person to view me 8. I was very satisfied with the conversation. 9. The other person expressed a lot of interest in what I had to say. 10. I did not enjoy the conversation* 11. The other person did not provide support for what he/she was saying* 12. I felt that we could laugh easily together. 13. We each got what we wanted. 3.29 1.009 140 3.37 .813 134 2.03 3.71 1.103 .950 140 140 2.06 3.55 .625 .840 134 134 4.83 6.833 140 3.52 .877 134 2.20 1.211 140 3.74 1.099 134 3.74 3.83 1.099 .884 140 140 3.87 3.82 .757 .695 134 134 3.80 .910 140 3.84 .828 134 2.06 1.062 140 2.13 1.057 134 2.56 3.96 1.137 3.308 140 140 2.46 3.66 1.020 .993 134 134 3.87 .833 140 3.85 .783 134 4.01 .843 140 4.01 .707 134 14. The conversation flowed smoothly 3.90 15. The other person frequently said 2.81 things which added little to the conversation 16. We talked about something I was 2.26 not interested in. *p < .01, **p < .001, *** p < .0001 3.90 1.195 140 140 3.70 2.76 .888 1.129 134 134 .988 140 2.24 .854 134 . Discussion This study investigated the perceptions of African American students in two HBCUs and Euro-American students in a PWI on verbal and nonverbal teacher immediacy and student retention, the relationship between verbal and nonverbal immediacy and interpersonal Revisiting Teacher Immediacy 214 communication satisfaction, and the relationship between interpersonal communication satisfaction and interpersonal communication satisfaction and student retention. To test the first hypothesis, regression analyses indicated that instructor verbal and nonverbal immediacy behaviors did not influence student retention. Specifically, instructor verbal and nonverbal immediacy behaviors were not predictive of African American or EuroAmerican students’ retention in college, and only a small amount of the variance in student retention was accounted for by instructor verbal and nonverbal immediacy behaviors (3 percent for the African American sample, and less than one percent for the Euro-American sample. As a result, it will be important for future research to consider other variables such as students’ motives for communicating with their instructors in both HBCUs and PWI contexts, which may contribute to African American and Euro-American students' self perceptions of their academic performance and interpersonal communication satisfaction with their instructors, since these variables are important constructs to student retention. Perhaps the results were not significant for both samples because the study used a matched-race method, which delimits students’ interpersonal experiences with instructors, in most case, who might share the same ethnicity. That is, when African American students attend an HBCU and Euro Americans attend a PWI, students have certain expectations of their instructors. For example, Gendrin and Rucker (2006, in press) conducted a matched-race study and their results indicated that African American students, in particular, have certain expectations of their instructors in the HBCU context than their Euro counterparts. Therefore, the results are not surprising. Gendrin and Rucker’s study also indicated no significant results for Euro-American students attending a PWI. To test the second hypothesis, regression analyses did not predict a link between instructor verbal and nonverbal immediacy behaviors and African American students’ perceptions of interpersonal communication satisfaction, but predicted a link between these constructs for EuroAmerican students. Woodside, Wong, and Wiest (1999) claim that “a possible explanation for [these] result may be that out-of-class self-concept domains such as social acceptance, intellectual ability, and [student] overall self-worth cannot be predicted by in-class instructor behavior” (p. 730) for African Americans. A second possible result may be that institutional structures and classroom interpersonal communication satisfaction based on interpersonal relations play an important part in shaping Euro-American students’ roles, feelings, attitudes, norms, and societal expectations toward learning (Giroux, 1997). The first research question investigated the relationship between verbal and nonverbal teacher immediacy and interpersonal communication satisfaction for both African American and Euro-American students. The results indicated no relationship between these two constructs. According to research, “Euro American students attending PWIs and African American students enrolled in HBCUs apply different sets of formal and informal rules to interact successfully with instructors” (Gendrin & Rucker, 2002, 2004; Neuliep, 1995; Rucker & Gendrin, 2003, as cited in Gendrin & Rucker 2006, in press), “to manage academic and social expectations effectively, and to take advantage of educational opportunities” (Brower & Ketterhagen, 2004; Pascarella & Terenzini, 1991, as cited in Gendrin & Rucker, 2006, in press). The second research question investigated the relationship between interpersonal communication satisfaction and student retention for both African American and Euro-American students. The t-tests indicated that interpersonal communication satisfaction for the combined sample produced significant results between interpersonal communication satisfaction and student retention. When students say “I would like to have another conversation like this one” or “The other person wanted to genuinely get to know me,” the results indicated that both African 215 Mary L. Rucker & Joanna M. Davis-Showell American and Euro-American students perceived having positive interpersonal communication satisfaction which influenced their retention at their respective universities. One possible explanation for students’ interpersonal satisfaction is that Chen (2002) argues that derived “communication satisfaction . . . [is] an affective construct that reflects [individuals’] emotional reaction toward their interaction in terms of the degree it had met or failed to meet their expectations. The more [an individual’s] communicative expectations were met in an interaction, the more the person reports feeling satisfied” (p. 134). The results of this study are consistent with previous work that highlights the importance of faculty-student relationships with respect to interpersonal communication satisfaction. This study also provides some evidence that faculty-student interpersonal communication satisfaction is significantly associated with students' overall college experience. The approaches to using these communication constructs was first to discern instructor verbal and nonverbal immediacy with respect to overall interpersonal interactions with students. Second, the interpersonal communication satisfaction scale delineates how meaning derives from the interactions initiated from an interpersonal context between instructors and students. Third, this study suggests that when students are satisfied with teachers’ communication practices and behavior toward them, then relationships develop. Therefore, as student satisfaction is determined in the development of interpersonal communication relationships with instructors, interpersonal communication satisfaction becomes the core contributor to student retention. Future research should investigate out-of-class interpersonal relational communication between faculty and student and how it influences student retention. If scholars examine both inclass and out-of-class interactions between instructors and students, perhaps a comprehensive understanding of how instructor-student interactions affect student retention. Moreover, future work should examine traditional and non-traditional students individually since student retention may differ significantly between these two groups at HBCUs and PWIs (c.f., Donohue & Wong, 1997). Since this study appears to be the first direct study on the influence of instructor verbal and nonverbal immediacy and interpersonal communication satisfaction and their relative influence on student retention in both HBCU and PWI settings, these results should be interpreted with caution. Even though extant literature has addressed instructor verbal and nonverbal immediacy in both the HBCU and PWI contexts, but have yet to address these constructs in relation to student retention, these research findings will need to be confirmed or disconfirmed. Revisiting Teacher Immediacy 216 References Arbaugh, J. B. (2001). How instructor immediacy behaviors affect student satisfaction and learning in Web-based courses. Business Communication Quarterly, 64(4), 42-58. Brower, A. M., & Ketterhagen A. (2004). Is there an inherent mismatch between how black and white students expect to succeed in college and what their colleges expect from them? Journal of Social Issues, 60, (1), 95-116. Cascarilla, E., & Terenzini, P. (1991). How college affects students: Findings and insights from twenty years of research. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Chen, L. (2002). Perceptions of intercultural interaction and communication satisfaction: A study on initial encounters. Communication Reports, 15(2), 133-143). Chory, R. M., & McCroskey, J. C. (1999). The Relationship between teacher management communication style and affective learning. Communication Quarterly, 47(1), 1-10. Chory-Assad, R. M. (2002). Classroom justice: Perceptions of fairness as a predictor of student motivation, learning, and aggression. Communication Quarterly, 50(1), 58-69. Chory-Assad, R. M. & Paulsel, M. L. (2004). Antisocial classroom communication: instructor influence and interactional justice as predictors of student aggression. Communication Quarterly, 52(2), 98-115. Comstock, J. & Higgins, G. (1997). Appropriate relational messages in direct selling interaction: Should salespeople adapt to buyers’ communicator style? The Journal of Business Communication, 34(4), 401-426. Donohue, T. L & Wong, E. H. (1997). Achievement motivation and college satisfaction in traditional and nontraditional students. Education, 118, 237-243. Gendrin, D. M., & Rucker, M. L. (2002). The impact of gender on teacher immediacy and student learning in the HBCU classroom. Communication Research Reports, 19, 291-299. Gendrin, D. M., & Rucker, M. L. (2004). Revisiting the relationship between teacher immediacy and student learning in the HBCU classroom: Does instructor immediacy make a difference? The Atlantic Journal of Communication, 12, 77-92. Gendrin, D. M., & Rucker, M. L. (2007, in press). The Relationship between Students’ Motives for communicating and instructors’ immediacy behaviors: A Matched race institutional comparison. Atlantic Journal of Communication, 14, (4). Gorham, J. (1988). The relationship between verbal teacher immediacy behaviors and student learning. Communication Education, 37, 40-53. Graham, E. E. (2004). Interpersonal communication satisfaction inventory. In Rebecca B. Rubin, Philip Palmgreen, and Howard E. Sypher (eds.), Communication research measures: A sourcebook. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Hecht, M. L. (1978a). The conceptualization and measurement of interpersonal communication satisfaction. Human Communication Research, 4, 253-264. Hess, J. A., & Smythe, M. (2001). Is teacher immediacy actually related to student cognitive learning? Communication Studies, 52(3), 197-211. Lau, L. K. (2003). Institutional factors affecting student retention. Education 124(2), 243-253. Mehrabian, A. (1966). Immediacy: An indicator of attitudes in linguistic communication. Journal of Personality, 34, 26-34. Menges, R., Weimar, M., & Associates (1996). Making the transition to college: Teaching on solid ground: Using scholarship to improve practice. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass 217 Mary L. Rucker & Joanna M. Davis-Showell Publishers. Mottet, T. P., Beebe, S. A., Raffeld, P. C. & Paulsel, M. L. (2004). The Effects of student verbal and nonverbal responsiveness on teachers’ liking of students and willingness to comply with student requests. Communication Quarterly, 52(1), 27-43. Mottet, T. P., & Richmond, V. P. (1998). An inductive analysis of verbal immediacy: alternative conceptualization of relational approach/avoidance strategies. Communication Quarterly, 46(1), 25-41. Neuliep, J. W. (1995). A comparison of teacher immediacy in African-American and EuroAmerican college classrooms. Communication Education, 44, 267-277. Noel-Levitz. (2004). National Conference on Student Retention materials. Pascarella, E. T., & Terenzini, P. T. (1991). How college affects students: Findings and insights from twenty years of research. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Paulsel, M. L., & Chory-Assad, R. M. (2004). The Relationships among instructors’ antisocial behavior alteration techniques and student resistance. Communication Reports, 17(2), 103-117. Pruitt, D. (2005). Enrollment management. College Business Management Institute, PowerPoint presentation. Proctor, R.F. &Wilcox, J.R. (1993). An Exploratory analysis of responses to own messages in interpersonal communication. ETC.: A Review of General Semantics, 50(2), 210-228. Richmond, V. P., Gorham, J. S., & McCroskey, J. C. (1987). The relationship between selected immediacy behaviors and cognitive learning. In M. L. McLaughlin (Ed.), Communication Yearbook 10 (pp. 574-590). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Richmond, V.P., McCroskey, J. C. & Johnson, A. D. (2003). Development of the nonverbal immediacy scale (NIS): Measures of self-and other-perceived nonverbal immediacy. Communication Quarterly, 51(4), 504-525. Rucker, M. L., & Gendrin, D. M. (2003). The Impact of ethnic identification on student learning in the HBCU classroom. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 30(3), 207-214. Schulte, L. E., Thompson, F., Hayes, K., Noble, & Jacobs, E. (2001). Undergraduate faculty and student perceptions of the ethical climate and its importance in retention. College Student Journal, Vol. 35. Simonds, C. J. (2001). Central States outstanding teaching award winners: Reflecting on the relationship between instructional communication theory and teaching practices. Communication Studies, 52(4), 260-287. Tinto, V. (1987). Leaving college: Rethinking the causes and cures of student attrition. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Tubbs, S. L., & Moss, S. (2000). Human Communication. McGraw-Hill Companies. Woodside, B. M., Wong, E. H., Wiest, D. J. (1999). The Effect of student-faculty interaction on college students' academic achievement and self Concept. Education, 119(4), 730-735. Human Communication. A Publication of the Pacific and Asian Communication Association. Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 219 – 238. Communication Experiences of Korean Expatriates in the U.S.: A Study of Cross-Cultural Adaptation Yang_Soo Kim Middle Tennessee State University Yang-Soo Kim, Ph. D. (University of Oklahoma, 2003) is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Speech & Theatre at Middle Tennessee State University. Direct all correspondence to: yskim@mtsu.edu Cross-Cultural Adaptation 220 Abstract The present study examines the communication experiences of Korean expatriates in the United States. Y. Y. Kim’s (1988, 2001) Cross-cultural Adaptation Theory provides the basis for offering an explanation of the linkage between communication competence and psychological health of Korean expatriates vis-à-vis American sociocultural milieu. The analysis uses portions of verbal transcripts obtained through 20 in-depth personal interviews between February and September 2002. The results show that host language competence, cultural knowledge, and cultural differences reflected on verbal behavior and work styles are important sources of psychological challenge for the Korean expatriates. While Korean expatriates are involved in different communication activities with host nationals and deal with different realities in and outside work, the positive and genuine relationship with co-workers contribute to their positive life experience overseas. As Kim’s theory predicts, the overall outcome of the study affirms that communication is the central force in the adaptation of expatriates by promoting psychological health in an unfamiliar host cultural environment. 221 Yang_Soo Kim Introduction The business environment is becoming increasingly global. To implement global corporate strategies and manage subsidiaries, many companies dispatch employees, particularly managerial and professional personnel, on overseas assignments. The number of expatriate assignments has been increasing and this trend is expected to continue (Windham International, 1998). In 1999, 80% of midsize and large companies sent professionals abroad, and 45% planned to increase the number of employees on foreign assignment (Black & Gregersen, 1999). Like other sojourners, business expatriates immerse themselves in a new, unfamiliar cultural environment. For such employees and their families, adjusting to life overseas poses a significant hurdle (Black & Gregersen, 1991; Tung, 1988). Nearly 40% of American expatriates return early (Kealey, 1996), owing to the inability to adjust to a foreign cultural environment rather than to a lack of technical competence (Stroh, Dennis, & Cramer, 1994; Kramer, Wayne, & Jaworski, 2001). This retention failure incurs serious costs to both companies and individual employees. Early termination of just one expatriate costs an American company as much as $1 million (Shannonhouse, 1996), in addition to various non-financial costs such as damaged reputation, lost business opportunities, and lost market or competitive shares (Black & Gregersen, 1991; Copeland & Griggs, 1985; Naumann, 1992; Shannonhouse, 1996). Withdrawal from international assignments is found to be costly for expatriates and their families as well, resulting in diminished self-esteem, impaired relationships, and interrupted careers, in addition to a possible adverse impact on qualified coworkers (Stroh, 1995; Tung, 1988). Practical concerns such as these have been the primary driving force behind an extended body of sojourner studies. Many studies have approached the phenomenon of expatriate adjustment from the perspective of “culture shock” (e.g., Oberg, 1960; Ward, Bochner, & Furnham, 2001). Some studies have generated conceptual models to examine the process and mechanisms of expatriate adjustment, highlighting the roles of attribution, uncertainty reduction and expectations (e.g., Black & Mendenhall, 1991; Black, Mendenhall, & Oddou, 1991; Black, 1992). Other studies have identified specific factors that promote expatriate adjustment, including personality traits (e.g., Harrison et al., 1996; Shaffer et al., 2006;), spousal or family adjustment (e.g., Black & Gregersen, 1991; Torbiorn, 1982), cultural novelty (e.g., Dunbar, 1994; Stroh, Dennis, & Cramer, 1994; Van Vianen et al., 2004), organizational support (e.g., Black & Gregersen, 1991; Gomez-Meija & Balkin, 1987; Kramer & Wayne, 2004; Krell, 2005), previous international experience (e.g., Selmer, 2002; Takeuchi et al., 2005), and job characteristics (e.g., Aycan, 1997; Guy & Patton, 1996). However, driven primarily by practical needs and interests such as selection, training and repatriation, most expatriate adjustment studies have been descriptive and atheoretical (Aycan, 1997). Factors of interpersonal and mass communication activities and of the host environment itself have not been examined. As noted by Shaffer, Harrison, and Gilley (1999), such studies have tended to address only a small number of factors as antecedents while excluding many other factors that are likely to influence the sojourner adaptation process. The present study investigates the communication experiences of Korean business expatriates in the United States to explore qualitative insight into their process of cross-cultural adaptation. The study examines how the Korean expatriates’ communication experiences are related to their psychological well-being vis-à-vis their host cultural milieu (i.e., American society). Specifically, the important related issues were posed as the following three research Cross-Cultural Adaptation 222 questions: 1) What kinds of contact and communication activities do Korean expatriates have with local people?; 2) What kind of communication-related difficulties do Korean expatriates face?; and 3)What is the overall feeling and life experience of Korean expatriates in their host cultural milieu? The present analysis utilizes verbatim transcripts from face-to-face, in-depth interviews, conducted in the United States between February and September 2002. Theoretical Grounding Guiding this investigation is Y.Y. Kim’s (1988, 2001, 2005) Cross-cultural Adaptation Theory. Grounded in an open systems perspective, Y. Y. Kim’s theory (1988, 2001, 2005) approaches cross-cultural adaptation not as a specific analytic unit (or variable) but as the entirety of the evolutionary process an individual undergoes vis-à-vis a new and unfamiliar environment. Cross-cultural adaptation is therefore explained in terms of a dynamic interplay of the person and the environment. By placing adaptation at the intersection of the person and the environment, Y. Y. Kim defines cross-cultural adaptation as “the entirety of the phenomenon of individuals who, upon relocating to an unfamiliar sociocultural environment, strive to establish and maintain a relatively stable, reciprocal, and functional relationship with the environment” (Y. Y. Kim, 2001, p. 31). Implicit in this definition is the goal of achieving an overall personenvironment “fit” that entails “almost always a compromise, a vector in the internal structure of culture and the external pressure of environment” (Sahlins, 1964, p. 136). Based on this systemic conception of cross-cultural adaptation, the theory addresses two basic questions: (1) What is the essential nature of the adaptation process individual settlers undergo over time? and (2) Why are some settlers more successful than others in attaining a level of fitness in the host environment? The first question is addressed in the form of a process model that presents a three-pronged psychological movement Y. Y. Kim refers to as the stressadaptation-growth dynamic — a movement in the generally forward and upward direction of increased chances of success in meeting the demands of the host environment. The stressadaptation-growth dynamic is not explained as playing out in a smooth, steady, and linear progression, but in a dialectic, cyclic, and continual "draw-back-to-leap" pattern. The spiral model explains that humans, as open systems, have the natural tendency to resist evolution accompanied by the destruction of the old structure. This tendency manifests itself in various forms of psychological resistance, such as selective attention, denial, avoidance and withdrawal, as well as in compulsively altruistic behavior, cynicism and hostility. Yet, no open system can stabilize itself forever. If it were so, nothing would come of evolution. The state of misfit and a heightened awareness in the state of stress serve as the very same forces that propel individuals to overcome the predicament and partake in the active development of new habits. What follows the dynamic stress-adaptation disequilibrium, according to the theory, is subtle growth. Periods of stress pass as settlers work out new ways of handling problems, owing to the creative forces of self-reflexivity of human mentation. Building on the process model, the theory turns to the second basic question: “Why do some settlers adapt faster than others?” or “Given the same length of time, why do some settlers attain a higher level of adaptation?” Integrating various factors addressed by different investigators as constituting and/or predicting differing levels or rates of adaptive change, Y. Y. Kim (1988, 2001, 2005) addresses this question in a structural model, depicted in Figure 1. The core of this structure is the dimension of personal communication, or host communication competence (Dimension 1), which is defined as the cognitive, affective and operational capacity 223 Yang_Soo Kim to communicate in accordance with the host communication symbols and meaning systems. This dimension serves as the very engine that pushes individuals along the adaptive path. Inseparably linked with host communication competence are the activities of host social communication (Dimension 2), through which strangers participate in interpersonal and mass communication activities of the host environment. Activities of ethnic social communication (Dimension 3) provide distinct, subcultural experiences of interpersonal and mass communication with fellow co-ethnics. Interacting with the personal and social (host, ethnic) communication activities are the conditions of the host environment (Dimension 4), including the degrees of receptivity and conformity pressure in the local population as well as the strength of the ethnic group. The individual’s predisposition (Dimension 5) — consisting of preparedness for the new environment, proximity (or distance) of the individual’s ethnicity to that of the natives, and the adaptive personality attributes of openness, strength, and positivity — influences the subsequent development in personal and social communication activities. Together, all of the factors identified above directly or indirectly contribute to explaining and predicting differential rates or levels of intercultural transformation (Dimension 6) within a given time period. The theory identifies three key facets of intercultural transformation: increased functional fitness, psychological health and the emergence of an identity orientation that reaches beyond a single culture. The level of intercultural transformation, in turn, helps to explain and predict the levels of all other dimensions. The six dimensions of factors together constitute an interactive and functional model, in which all the linkages indicate mutual stimulations (and not unidirectional causations), identified in 21 theorems (see Y. Y. Kim, 2001, pp. 91-92). Methods The present study is based on an analysis of verbatim transcripts from in-depth personal interviews with Korean expatriates conducted in the U.S. between February and September 2002. Participants The participants in this study were Korean expatriate employees working in the U.S. The Korean expatriate group for this study was comprised of Korean-born employees of U.S. subsidiaries, joint ventures, overseas branch offices, and multinational corporations located in the United States. This study’s aim was to investigate individual expatriates and their communication and adaptation experiences when they were new to a different host culture; thus, Korean Americans who were born and raised in the U.S. were not considered for this study. For the interview, participants were selected by using a quota sampling method. Twenty Korean respondents were chosen from among those who had completed a questionnaire survey, based on the respondents’ age and length of stay. (A survey of 106 Korean expatriates was the first stage of research; findings from the survey are not included for this analysis) (See Y.S. Kim, 2003; Kim & Kim, 2005; Kim & Kim (in press) for findings from the survey). All twenty Korean interviewees were male. The average age of the Korean interviewees was 39.7 years old (SD = 5.2 years; Range: 32–50 years) while the average length of stay was 3.5 years (SD = 2.1 years; Range: 5 months - 10 years). Fifteen interviewees had a bachelor’s degree (75%) and five had a master’s degree (25%). Nine interviewees (45%) had lived in a foreign country before coming to the U.S. and six had had prior intercultural training (30%). Cross-Cultural Adaptation 224 The Interview Procedures All interviews were conducted by the author in Korean, based on a Korean version of the interview questionnaire. The shared background of the interviewer with the interviewees (ethnic origin and experience as an expatriate) helped to draw more frank and candid opinions and Figure 1: Y. Y. Kim’s Structural Model: Factors Influencing Cross-Cultural Adaptation (Source: Y. Y. Kim, 2001, p. 87). impressions of American people and society from the respondents. All interviews took place in a conference room or reception hall at their place of work, during working hours, and took approximately 40 minutes each. Interviews began with an exchange of personal information (i.e., age, gender, length of stay, education, etc.) after each interviewee had signed the consent form. All interviews were audiotaped and transcribed in their entirety, with the written consent of the interviewees. Interview questions were originally written in English. Interview questions were then translated into Korean and the Korean version was back-translated into English by a bilingual Korean. Among the topics covered in the interview, open-ended interview questions dealt with the main research variables: communication ability and communication-related difficulties with local people (“host communication competence”), experiences of interacting with local people (“interpersonal communication”), and overall feelings and positive/unpleasant life experiences in the host country (“psychological health”). Regarding host communication competence, questions included different communication styles the interviewees found between communicating with co-ethnics and communicating with host nationals: “It is likely you have opportunities to interact with American people both in and outside of your work. Do you find any differences between communicating with Koreans and communicating with Americans?” and communication-related difficulties they experienced when interacting with local people both at work and outside the workplace: “Have you ever experienced difficulties in communicating with American people in or outside of the work environment?”. Based on the responses, follow-up questions were used to elicit specific incidents and typical experiences illustrating these differences and difficulties, along with coping strategies used to deal with these difficulties. With respect to interpersonal communication, one question was asked to indicate the amount of daily interaction with host nationals both at work and outside the workplace: “Of all your daily conversations (at work or outside work), approximately what percentage of them do you have with American people?” Another question was asked about the purpose and nature of these interactions: “In what capacities and for what reasons, both in and out of work, do you interact with American people?” followed by a question about the types of social activities with host nationals: “What kinds of socializing do you do with American people?” 225 Yang_Soo Kim . Concerning psychological health, the interviewees were asked to describe their positive and/or unpleasant life experiences while living in the host country: What are some of the Cross-Cultural Adaptation 226 positive/unpleasant experiences you have had while living in the U.S. so far?” their general feelings about their life in the host country: “Overall, how are you feeling about your present life in the U.S. as regards your life experiences interacting with Americans in and outside work?” and their desire to return to the country after this assignment: “If you have another chance to work overseas in the future, would you like to come back to the U.S.?”. Follow-up questions were used to allow the respondents to provide specific reasons why they wanted or did not want to return to the host country. Results Interview data were analyzed based on a portion of the qualitative verbal responses which were relevant to the interviewees’ personal adaptation experiences in the host environment. The interviewees’ comments and testimonials in response to the interview questions serve as the basis for addressing the three research questions posed above: 1) to identify the communicationrelated difficulties that Korean expatriates experience in relating to local people; 2) to investigate the kinds of contact and communication activities that Korean expatriates have with local people; 3) to identify the overall feelings that Korean expatriates have in their life overseas. In analyzing qualitative interview data, all questions and responses to open-ended questions were transcribed in their entirety by the investigator in Korean. After transcription, the verbatim data were grouped into common categories based on emerging themes of communication difficulties, interpersonal contact and communication, and overall feelings and intercultural experiences in the U.S. Before being presented, the findings from the Korean interviews were translated back into English by the investigator and verified by a Korean bilingual. Profiles of Interviewees A brief profile of five of the twenty interviewees is presented in the following. These five respondents represent all 20 respondents in terms of geographical area and line of business. Interviewee #1. He is a male in his mid-forties. He has been in the U.S. for more than three years. He has a bachelor’s degree and is general manger of a Korean shipping company and in charge of general administration. He lived in the Netherlands for two years before his career in the U.S., but has not taken any intercultural training. He commented that his interaction with local people is limited to coworkers at the shipyard. In addition, the fact that his American coworkers are all employees under his supervision might affect his relationship with them and their mutual interaction. He says that as short-term sojourners, expatriates have an experience merely on the surface, unlike long-term immigrants. Interviewee #2. He is a forty-year-old male. He earned his master’s degree in the U.S. and has been in the U.S. for four years. He is the general manager of a shipping company and is in charge of coordinating the shipping between the Seoul headquarters and overseas customers. He had never been in a foreign country and never received any intercultural training before he came to the U.S. He mentioned that one of the most interesting things in his intercultural experiences is the strong and clear boundary between private and public affairs in business. Interviewee #3. He is a male in his late forties. He had been in the U.S. for three years during his first international assignment prior to this one, and had been in the U.S. for nine months at the time of this interview. He has a bachelor’s degree and is general manager of a Korean bank, where he handles loans and letters of credit. He had never lived in other foreign 227 Yang_Soo Kim countries before coming to the U.S. and had not received any intercultural training prior to this international assignment. He has a positive perception of American society. He commented that to be mainstreamed into the American society, migrants need to master the host language and culture. He gave insightful comments on the interview questions as well. Interviewee #4. He is a male and forty years old. He has a bachelor’s degree and is a manager of a Korean company. His job includes procurement within the aerospace industry. He had been in the U.S. for almost three years at the time of the interview. He had never been in foreign countries and had not received any intercultural training prior to this international assignment. Due to his work assignment, he mostly interacts with American coworkers daily. He pointed out that one of the most positive things in his life in the U.S is the well-developed public education system. Interviewee #5. He is a male in his early forties. He is a general manager of a Korean electronics company, in charge of the business and technology division. He had been in the U.S. more than three years at the time of the interview. Before his current job assignment, he took part in a company-sponsored overseas language program and on-the-job training in the U.S. for three months. Although during that overseas program he described his interaction with local people as “a bad experience,” he described his current overseas life as “a really positive experience.” Like other Korean interviewees, his interaction with local people is largely limited to coworkers at work and business relationships. Communication-related Difficulties Almost all of the Korean interviewees reported that cultural differences, reflected in verbal behavior and work style, contribute strongly to difficulties in their interactions with host nationals. Regarding verbal behavior, one Korean interviewee commented on the informality in interactions with his supervisor: Here [in the U.S.], people call each other by their name. I saw people addressing their supervisor directly [calling their first name]. . . . I am envious of them. . .In Korea, we almost forget about our name and we go by our titles. . .Besides when they sit talking with their boss. . .with their feet on the table and listening to their boss. . .and they finish when they are done. . .There is a clear-cut boundary between private and public setting [in supervisor and subordinate relations]. One interviewee mentioned the individualistic/direct communication style of American coworkers. This communication style is frustrating because this is different from a Korean style: I have lived in Korea more than 30 years and I am used to Korean styles [indirect communication]. . .Sometimes. . . .even if I need him [American coworkers] and want to ask him questions. . . . if he has no time and it’s not in his scope of work. . . .he turns down my request very coldly. . . .I wouldn’t be able to follow this part. . . .because this is not our [Korean] style that we are used to. Along with different verbal behaviors, cultural differences are reflected in work styles. According to the Korean interviewees, the American approach to management, based on logic rather than hierarchy, creates conflicts between Koreans and Americans. One interviewee commented: Cross-Cultural Adaptation 228 This might be a cultural difference. In Korean business style, sometimes, prompt changes should be done by the direction from the top management. It is kind of one way communication without any explanation. . .However, Americans constantly ask questions (why?) and seek logical explanation or justification on the issue. . .They [Americans] want us to follow American business style. . .This might cause some conflict between two sides. . .Different business practice and way of thinking [Asian style] – Americans have difficulty understanding it. . .. Different styles of maintaining business relationships were reported as well. The abrupt attitude change of an American partner was a great surprise to Koreans, who are familiar with relationship-oriented business. One interviewee explained: Even though Americans okayed whatever I say, in some point [there is no more consensus], they turned away and ceased the business relationship. . .We have worked together and had a business relationship for a couple of years. . .Koreans tend to try to work on the contract and business relationship. . .Americans look okay and get along with business partners well. . .All of sudden. . .they turn away. . .To me it is very cold and merciless. . .It is different. Culture differences were also seen in a clear sense of time dividing work from other times. One interviewee stated, “They [Americans] have a strong sense of privacy. . .a clear boundary between their private life after work and public life at work. . .So, I should not violate this. . .For example, after five, it is not a good idea to ask for people [American workers] to go to dinner even if I am a boss.” This different work value provides a kind of shock to typical Koreans, who are accustomed to a corporate system which values hierarchy and has less rigid boundaries between in- and out-ofwork contexts. Another difference was reported by one Korean interviewee regarding attitudes about how to deal with an unclear job situation. He explained his difficulty in dealing with American co-workers: While Koreans are likely to cope with unexpected situations once they receive job orders, the Americans always request clear and complete information such as instructions or job descriptions. For example, they ask, “give me a clear job description. The clear scope of work or boundary of my authority is not given to deal with things under these circumstances.” Even though this might be attributed to cultural differences, I think there is considerable difference in work style between Americans and Koreans. In addition to verbal behaviors/work styles, another source of intercultural challenges reported by Korean interviewees is insufficient host language competence and cultural knowledge. One interviewee explained: “Most of all, it [communication difficulty] is English [language competence]. Even though expatriates who have a good English competence were dispatched to the U.S. and had language training, we [expatriates] cannot say our English is perfect. Setting aside discussing business issues with co-workers at work, we cannot fully express ourselves when we have a social conversation at the party, which poses a hurdle for us to get involved in more in-depth conversation.” One interviewee revealed his frustration related to this language issue: [W]hen I take the driving license test or when I go to a market. . .Americans believe that if you are here you should be able to speak English. . .In Korea, if an American speaks 229 Yang_Soo Kim Korean, Koreans pay more attention to him and try to help him. . .Just because an American tries to speak Korean, Koreans try to figure out the meaning based on the context. . .Americans, here when they interact with Koreans or me, they speak English with the same native tone and accent without considering my position [as a foreigner]. . .If I don’t understand their English, this is my problem. . . . Host language competence is not only related to stress and frustration but also related to how expatriates are treated by local people. One interviewee explained: “As I told you before. . .if we have trouble communicating with each other. . .then they are unwilling to. . .interact with people who cannot speak fluent English.” Another interviewee explained his experience: When I went to visit the southern part, I could feel that. . .it is an unfamiliar place. . . . Because of my [accented] English, I feel like they look down on me. . .particularly at the hotel. I believe that this treatment comes from mainly the lack of language competence. Of course, there are other factors like skin color. But language is the prominent factor. If you do not speak the host language [English] fluently, along with your appearance [ethnic marker], you will be treated based on that. Along with host language competence, the degree of cultural knowledge is related to difficulty in Korean expatriates’ interaction with host nationals. One interviewee reported: In doing business in Korea, we don’t have much social talk after getting into contract. Here in the U.S., before making a business deal, we need common knowledge to have social conversation. . . .When I am talking about the current issues that most Americans are interested in, such as sports, politics, and culture. . . if I do not know much about these issues, I feel like I am having a wall between me and them [Americans] and our conversation becomes boring. . . So, I think I should have knowledge about those issues. Host Interpersonal Communication Contact and Communication Activities As short-term sojourners, Korean expatriates reported relatively limited interactions and personal relationships involving host nationals. They reported that their contact with host nationals was generally limited to coworkers at work or with business partners (e.g., buyer or seller). They socialized during activities such as golf, a company party or going to church. (See Kim & Kim, 2005). Since expatriates work at the company and carry their daily lives outside the organization, their interaction with host nationals occurs in two different contexts—i.e., in and outside work. While the Korean interviewees reported positive relationships with coworkers mostly at work, they also reported different challenging experiences when they interacted with local people outside work. One interviewee commented about his positive life experience associating with his coworkers or business partners: I believe America is the nice place to live. . . .It is a free country. As long as you keep the boundaries, you have freedom to do anything. . . . Another thing is our staffs and coworkers. . . . I don’t know whether my higher position in this company might contribute to this nice treatment. . .but I know there are many nice people. I am really grateful for them. . .They [American staff and co-workers] treat me really well. They Cross-Cultural Adaptation 230 never perceive me as a foreigner but take care of me as part of the team/family at work. I think that I have such good people at my work. Another Korean interviewee observed: I heard Americans are individualistic. So, I presumed that it is very difficult to have a relationship with Americans because of cultural differences. . . .It takes a while to build up a close personal relationship apart from business. On the contrary, in my staying here, if I open my heart first, they open their heart too. I met many people like that. . . becoming friends. . . .Once we get acquainted. . .become friends. . .I cannot feel any difference between Americans and Koreans. . . .If I do my best in treating others, that will work anywhere. It is the same here [in the U.S.]. Our co-workers are like that. Along with their positive and genuine feelings about interaction with Americans at work, some other interviewees reported different attitudes and treatment from local people outside work. One interviewee commented on his life experience when his status is or isn’t disclosed: In my relationship with people [Americans], in most cases, I meet with people on business and we exchange our business cards. They are very generous. . . .I could not have feeling that they treat me differently. If I meet people in another context [outside work without identifying my position as business person]. . .probably I could sense that [different treatment]. For example, when I went to play golf by myself, I happened to team up with some Americans that I have not known.. . . .I could not join their conversation. . . .something different. After I disclosed myself and position at the company, they seemed to show interest to me. Some other interviewees reported unpleasant experiences with local people outside of work. One interviewee explained: I have no problem at all when I interact with Americans at work on business. Most of them treat me favorably and kindly. However, outside work, it might be different. For example, I had a car wreck. I was turning left on my signal and the other car hit my car carelessly. It was his fault. Nonetheless, he treated me real badly. I sensed that he looked down on me and mistreated me because I am an Asian. Another interviewee reported a similar unpleasant life experience: . . .At the airport, when I go through the immigration process. . . .well. . . .I believe it is the same process applicable to everybody. On the one hand, however, if I were an American, they would not be treating [bothering] me like this, I assume. I felt like they treat me as if I was desperate to come to this country. . . .I was one of those people [refugees]. . .As a stranger. . . .as a Korean living in the U.S., this is the part I should embrace as long as I live in this country. . .. Most of the Korean interviewees enjoyed their status as expatriates and this played an important part in their relationship with people at work. However, outside work, when their minority status with relatively less powerful ethnic group strength stood out, they faced the reality of unexpected or different attitudes from local people. This might suggest that in communication activities involving host nationals, expatriates dealt with dualistic worlds – in and outside work – and they had different feelings depending on the context. 231 Yang_Soo Kim Psychological health Most of the interview respondents had very positive impressions about American society/culture in general. They described their images toward American society/culture as: “a land of opportunity that hard work should be paid off,” “not corrupted society,” “convenient societal system for everybody, including minorities,” and “positive individualistic culture respecting individual rights and privacy.” Coupled with these positive images about the host society, regarding their overall life in the U.S., the Korean interviewees generally perceived their intercultural life experience overseas as rewarding and positive. Almost all of the interviewees replied that they would come back if they have a chance in the future, because of the positive aspects of American culture/systems and the good living conditions. As one of the positive experiences, almost all of the interviewees highly appreciated the good public education system. One interviewee explained: I am hoping to come back. . . .The most positive thing is public education. . . .When I drive my kid to school, I have a chance to talk with teachers. . . .They are so nice. I don’t know if they treat me kindly because I am a parent. . . .I could not find any discrimination against my child. . . .If my kid is pretty good at something, they [teachers] always give compliments and encourage him. There is a big difference in teaching styles between the U.S. and Korea. Thus, we can raise our kid very ideally in the U.S. here, I believe. . . .For educational environment. . . it is worthwhile to work overseas like this. Along with public education, one Korean interviewee indicated the better quality of life in the U.S. as a rewarding life experience: I wish I could have a chance to come back again. . .Speaking of the life in the U.S., I would like to say this, in Korea, with relation to the life and work, I could not have the chance to self-actualize myself. . . .too much tied up with work. . . .no time to look back over myself. . . .spend time with coworkers drinking after work. So, the life was very in a rush. Here, even though I work as much as I did in Korea, I have more time with family. This is the precious opportunity for me. Another interviewee reported his adaptive experience: I think my life is very positive. . . .First time I began my life here in the U.S., I was feeling uncomfortable. But when I get to know about societal system and adjust myself well, it is very convenient living. The American lifestyles and customs are very convenient and comfortable in many aspects. There is no reason for us to reject it. Along with the predominantly positive experiences, some expatriates commented on their concerns, such as family (spouse) adjustment, as important challenges during their sojourn: My wife had a hard time [when she was here first]. . .She is happy now upon hearing that we will be leaving soon. . . .She is more adapted now. . . .She had a hard time in her first six months. . . .We lived in a place where there were no friends nearby. . . .She stayed at home by herself. . . .It is best for Koreans to live in Korea. . . .I see many [Korean] immigrants. . . .They are rich and own good homes. . . .but I think their life is hard. . . .Even if I have a chance to immigrate, I would not do that. Cross-Cultural Adaptation 232 Other interviewees showed concern about the reentry issue for himself and his family after completing his assignment. Two respondents explained their concerns: This [America] is the best place for education for children to spend their childhood. One thing is that they should go back home someday. . . .If they continue to live here, it would be good. . . .I am really worried about my child at the thought of their readjustment to the school system in Korea. . . .I am afraid my kid. . .has more difficulty readjusting than me. . . .This is my concern. It was a great experience to me. . . .It is positive [about the life in the U.S.]. . .One issue is that I spent 3 years overseas. . . .connected to [people in] Korea via internet or phone. . . .In 3 years I have to return, not live here permanently. . . .There is some disconnection. . . .how this will affect my life at work back home. . . .This is my concern. Discussion The purpose of the present analysis has been to examine the communication experience of Korean expatriates in the U.S. in the process of individual adaptation. Y. Y. Kim’s (1988, 2001) Cross-cultural Adaptation Theory has served as the basis for examining communication activities which have been posed as three research questions: 1) What kinds of contact and communication activities do Korean expatriates have with local people? 2) What kind of communication-related difficulties do Korean expatriates have? and 3) What is the overall feeling and life experience of Korean expatriates in their host cultural milieu? The present analysis is based on the data from face-to-face in-depth personal interviews conducted in the United States between February and September 2002. The results of the study show that for Korean interviewees, cultural differences have played out in different verbal behaviors and work styles, which present intercultural challenges. In verbal behaviors, individualistic/direct communication and informality are revealed as sources of communication difficulty. Culture differences are also reflected in work styles as there is a clear distinction between life in and outside work, which presents a challenge to Korean interviewees who are more used to a collectivistic/relationship-oriented culture which has less-clear boundaries between private life and public affairs (i.e., work). This cultural orientation even played out during the interview process. When this investigator conducted the interviews, most of them took place at the respondent’s workplace during their working hours. This reflects the Korean culture, which sees private life and organizational tasks holistically. In addition, two important sources of psychological challenges were revealed – host language competence, which is related to the quality of treatment by local people, and cultural knowledge, which could inhibit effective business communication. In their interaction with host nationals, Korean expatriates seemed to deal with two worlds in their relationship to host nationals: within- and outside work. Coupled with their expatriate status, these two different settings presented different realities: Korean expatriates perceive their interaction with co-workers inside work as meaningful, which contributed to the positive life experience in their life overseas, but they reported more frustration when they dealt with Americans/host environment outside of work. This status was also an important factor influencing the limited perception of expatriates as short-term sojourners, which was reported by some of the Korean interviewees. One interviewee commented about this limited perception colored by a “traveler’s mentality”: My perception [about host interpersonal ties and the host environment] is very positive. 233 Yang_Soo Kim However, my viewpoint may be skewed. I mainly interact with American managers at work and have no interaction with the Korean community. The reality other immigrants face in daily context might be totally different from mine. . . .In 2-3 years, as short-term sojourners, we are going back home [Korea]. We do not take the life here serious that much regardless of life situations. . .kind of life of sojourners. . . .Therefore, we might be in the position to perceive the American society in rather more positive light. Most of the Korean interviewees perceived their life overseas as very positive and rewarding. They reported on the public education system and privileges they could enjoy from the host system as expatriates, as well as on some concerns, like family adjustment and reentry shock. Theoretically, even though expatriates as short-term sojourners might have different motivations as compared with long-term immigrants, the study clearly shows that cross-cultural adaptation occurs through communicative interaction vis-à-vis their host cultural milieu. Most of the Korean interviewees said that the promotion of their host language competence and host cultural knowledge is related to their overall psychological well being in their life overseas. In addition, although the context and interaction partner might be important factors for Korean expatriates’ interaction with local people (i.e., in and outside business), meaningful ongoing relationships with co-workers at work is related to the overall positive life experiences of Korean expatriates. As predicted by Kim’s cross-cultural adaptation theory, it shows the centrality of communication in the process of adaptation in their expatriation. There is a reciprocal relationship between communication and psychological health. One Korean interviewee explained his adaptive change in the process of adaptation: It has been changed a lot. At that time [when he began his life in the U.S.], the life in the U.S. . . well. . .I was scared. It has not been long since I came to the U.S. . . uncomfortable. . . . When I interacted with Americans, if something unexpected happens, how could I deal with the situation?. . .I was nervous. What should I do? Now two years have passed. I might be able to deal with the unexpected situation. . . .I can express myself clear. . . .I [can] get what I want to get. . . .Even in hard situations, I can explain my situation effectively. So, as compared with before, I cannot feel any difficulty anymore. Methodologically, the present study uses interviews to describe the communication and life experiences of Korean expatriates in the American cultural milieu. This emic perspective of using in-depth personal interviews yields richer information on the practical aspects of participatory experience in the field and allows the researcher to have a clearer understanding of the participants’ reality, i.e., the personal and authentic experiences of expatriates living in a different host cultural milieu. This provides accounts from the expatriates themselves, which is closer to the interviewees’ own reality and helps illuminate some relevant facets of the concrete everyday reality in which expatriates find themselves. Practically, the present study provides some insights into the adaptation experiences in a different host cultural environment. The findings show that the expatriates’ knowledge and understanding of their host culture and active involvement in interpersonal communication with host nationals will help them to meet intercultural challenges arising from the process of adjustment. Given that for Koreans host language competence constitutes an important factor in successful adjustment, and enhancement of active involvement in host interpersonal communication and treatment by local people beyond a work context, training programs for Cross-Cultural Adaptation 234 international firms should focus on knowledge and understanding of the host culture and communication systems, particularly with respect to the language and cultural practices of the host society. As some of the interviewees reported, family adjustment is another significant issue in their life overseas. Therefore, companies should take this factor into account when they dispatch their employees—such as by incorporating it into the training program. Reentry shock has also been revealed as one of the concerns that Korean expatriates have. Thus, international companies should seriously consider long-term planning in expatriates’ management, which incorporates reentry and repatriation. The globalizing trend in business organizations makes the need for cross-cultural competency more important than ever before (Gertsen, 1990; Mendenhall & Oddou, 1985; Nauman, 1992). International firms could improve retention by offering comprehensively designed, extensive training programs to their employees, leading to more successful overseas business experiences. Because of the small sample size, the present findings are to be interpreted and generalized with some caution. They can, however, be made less problematic as consistent findings are obtained over time across different studies involving a wider ranger of nationalities working in different countries (e.g., American employees in Indonesia or Swedish employees in Japan). Furthermore, studies can be done regarding different types of sojourner groups (e.g., refugees, immigrants, and international students) in different countries (particularly, a nonWestern cultural context). Also, in addition to communication activities, the study would be more complete if predisposition and environmental factors were included in future studies. 235 Yang_Soo Kim References Aycan, Z. (1997). Acculturation of expatriate managers: a process model of adjustment and performance. In D. M. Saunders (Series Ed.) & Z. Aycan (Vol. Ed.), New approaches to employee management: Vol. 4. Expatriate management: Theory and research (pp. 1-40). Greenwich, CT: Jai Press, Inc. Black, J. (1992). Coming home: the relationship of expatriate expectations with repatriation adjustment and job performance. Human Relations, 45, 177-192. Black, J., & Gregersen, H. (1991). Antecedents to cross-cultural adjustment for expatriates in Pacific Rim assignments. Human Relations, 44, 497-515. Black, J., & Gregersen, H. (1999). The right way to manage expatriates. Harvard Business Review, 77, 52-62. Black, J., Mendenhall, M., & Oddou, G. (1991). Toward a comprehensive model of international adjustment: an integration of multiple theoretical perspectives. Academy of Management Review, 16, 291-317. Copeland, L., & Griggs, L. (1985). Going International. New York: Random House. Dunbar, E. (1994). The German executive in the U.S. work and social environment: Exploring role demands. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 18, 277-291. Gertsen, M. (1990, December). Intercultural competence and expatriates. International Journal of Human Research Management, 341-361. Gomez-Meija, L., & Balkin, E. (1987). The determinants of managerial satisfaction with the expatriation and repatriation process. Journal of Management Development, 6, 7-17. Guy, B., & Patton, W. (1996). Managing the effects of culture shock and sojourner adjustment on the expatriate industrial sales force. Industrial Marketing Management, 25, 385-393. Harrison, J., Chadwick, M., & Scales, M. (1996). The relationship between cross-cultural adjustment and the personality variables of self-efficacy and self-monitoring. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 20, 167-188. Kealey, D. (1996). The challenge of international personnel selection. In D. Landis & B. S. Bhagat (Eds.), Handbook of intercultural training (2nd ed.) (pp. 383-399). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Kim, Y.S., & Kim, Y. Y. (in press). Communication patterns and psychological health in the process of cross-cultural adaptation: a study of American and Korean expatriate workers. International and Intercultural Communication Annual. Cross-Cultural Adaptation 236 Kim, Y.S., & Kim, Y. Y. (2005). Communication patterns and psychological health in the process of cross-cultural adaptation: a study of American and Korean expatriate workers. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Communication Association, Boston, MA. Kim, Y. Y., & Kim, Y.S. (2004). The role of host environment in cross-cultural adaptation: a comparison of American expatriates in South Korea and their Korean counterparts in the United States. Asian Communication Research, 3, 5-25. Kim, Y. Y. (1988). Communication and cross-cultural adaptation: an integrative theory. Clevedon, Philadelphia: Multilingual Matters. Kim, Y. Y. (2001). Becoming Intercultural: An integrative theory of communication and crosscultural adaptation. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Kim, Y. Y. (2005). Adapting to a new culture: An integrative communication theory. In W. Gudykunst (Ed.), Theorizing about intercultural communication (pp. 375-400). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Kramer, M., & Wayne, S. (2004). An examination of perceived organizational support as a multidimensional construct in the context of an expatriate assignment. Journal of Management, 30, 209-237. Krell, E. (2005). Budding Relationships. HRMagazine, 50, 114-118. Kramer, M., Wayne, S., & Jaworski, R. (2001). Sources of support and expatriate performance: The mediating role of expatriate adjustment. Personal Psychology, 54, 71-101. Mendenhall, M., & Oddou, G. (1985). The dimensions of expatriate acculturation: A review. Academy of Management Review, 10, 39-48. Naumann, E. (1992). A conceptual model of expatriate turnover. Journal of International Business Studies, 23, 499-531. Oberg, K. (1960). Cultural Shock: Adjustment at new cultural environments. Practical Anthropology, 7, 177-182. Sahlins, M. (1964). Culture and Environment: The Study of Cultural Ecology. In S. Tax (Ed.), Horizons of Anthropology (pp. 132-147). Chicago: Aldine. Selmer, J. (2002). Practice makes perfect? International experience and expatriate adjustment. Management International Review, 42, 71-88. Shaffer, M., Gregersen, H., & Harrison, D. (2006). You can take it with you: individual differences and expatriate effectiveness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 109-125. 237 Yang_Soo Kim Shaffer, M., Harrison, D., & Gilley, K. (1999). Dimensions, determinants, and differences in the expatriate adjustment process. Journal of International Business Studies, 30, 53-73. Shannonhouse, R. (1996, November 8). Overseas-assignment failures. USA Today/International edition, p. 8A. Stroh, L. (1995). Predicting turnover among repatriates: Can organizations affect retention rates? International Journal of Human Resource Management, 6, 443-457. Stroh, L., Dennis, L., & Cramer, T. (1994). Predictors of expatriate adjustment. The International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 2, 176-192. Takeuchi, R., Tesluk, P., & Yun, S. (2005). An integrative view of international experience. Academy of Management Journal, 48, 85-100. Torbiorn, I. (1982). Living abroad: Personal adjustment and personnel policy in the overseas setting. New York: Wiley. Tung, R. (1988). The new expatriates. Cambridge: Balinger. Van Vianen, A., De APater, I., & Kristof-Brown, A. (2004). Fitting-in: surface- and deep-level cultural differences and expatriates’ adjustment. Academy of Management Journal, 47, 697-709. Ward, C., Bochner, S., & Furnham, A. (2001). The Psychology of Culture Shock. Philadelphia: Routledge. Windham International and National Foreign Trade Council, Inc. (1998, June). Global relocation trends: 1998 survey report. New York: Windham International. Human Communication. A Publication of the Pacific and Asian Communication Association. Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 239 – 250. Nollywood: The Influence of the Nigerian Movie Industry on African Culture Uchenna Onuzulike Clark University Uchenna Onuzulike ia a Candidate for Masters of Science in Professional Communication at Clark University. Please direct all correspondence to: Uchelike@hotmail.com Nollywood Influence 240 Abstract Since its emergence at the turn of the twenty-first century, the Nigerian movie industry “Nollywood” has had a profound influence on African culture. The Nigerian accents, style of dress, and behavioral idiosyncrasies, all of which are distinctly Nigeria, are now being transmitted as images around the globe. The medium of film has come to be directly associated with the culture industry. In Nigeria such a role for the film industry is still evolving. However, certain factors are altering the profile of what could be regarded as the country's culture, while the film industry itself is undergoing a crucial transition. The corpus of songs and oral literature, festivals, rituals, the traditional religion, performing arts, music, dance, and indeed, the entire range of artifacts constituting traditional oral performance of Nigerian culture are represented in Nigerian movies. 241 Uchenna Onuzulike Introduction This work highlights the Nigerian movie industry and demonstrates how it impacts and influences African culture. “We are often reminded that film is a powerful medium of entertainment and the transmission of cultural values” (Orewere, p.206). In this study, Nigerian films will be viewed through a relevant theoretical framework deeply rooted in Nigerian cultural traditions and social texts and a comprehensive methodology that focuses on the intervening mediations between community life and representation. Cultures are the creation of human interaction. Culture is something we learn; we are not born with it. He writes that changes in human society reflect the dynamism of culture. This dynamism is responsible for constant change in patterns associated with given cultures, and the multicultural character of most, if not all, societies substantially widens the range for influence on such cultures (Hall, 2005). The meeting of cinema and television has created a new reality called videofilm. There are numerous reasons why Nigerian movie production shifted from celluloid to videofilm including political, cultural, and economic reasons. However, a primary reason was concern for safety, as Faris (2002) of Time International noted, “With cities plagued by armed robbers, few wanted to risk a nighttime outing just to see a movie” (p.1). Celluloid roll film, also known as motion picture film or raw film stock, consists of long strips of perforated cellulose acetate on which a quick succession of still photographs known as frames can be recorded (Barsam, 2004). As the term videofilm implies, Haynes (2000) says “they are something between television and cinema, and they do not fit comfortably within the North American structures of either” (p.1). In many ways, videofilm itself stands for an example of technology that can be used for cultural explorations and representations mostly for the individuals or groups who cannot afford celluloid. Nigerian videofilms provide videofilmic (I define videofilmic as relating to or resembling motion pictures just like cinematic or filmic) representations of Nigeria. Nigerian videofilms are deeply rooted in Nigerian cultural traditions and social texts that focus on Nigerian community life. Nigerian videofilm stories are told using African idioms, proverbs, costumes, artifacts, cultural display, and the imagery of Africa. The common Nigerian videofilm genres include horror, comedy, urban legend, mythic parable, love and romance, juju, witchcraft, melodrama, and historical epic. Movie production helps to determine the differences and transformations that have occurred in Nigeria. As Roger (1995) states that every society changes over time. Some change rapidly; others seem to stay virtually unchanged for generations. However slowly, change does occur. Haynes (2000) affirms, “The study of Nigerian video films does not fit easily into the structures of African film criticism in still another way. Studies of African film have tended to be pan-African, for marketing as well as ideological reasons” (p.9). Pearson (2001) notes that Nigeria is one of only three countries, alongside India and the U.S., where domestically produced movies dominate local viewing. The emergence of the Nigerian videofilm industry “Nollywood” is a cultural phenomenon and Nigerian movies serve as a representation of Nigerian culture. The movie Living in Bondage produced by Ken Nnabue in 1992 set the pace for emergence of Nollywood (Haynes, 2005; Mbamara, 2004; Onuzulike, 2007; Servant, 2001). Nollywood is the name given to the Nigeria film industry and it is the highest grossing Nollywood Influence 242 movie-making industry behind Hollywood and the Indian film industry, “Bollywood,” respectively (Mbamara, 2005). Though no one could claim exactly how and when the first time the name “Nollywood” was conceptualized or used to describe the Nigerian movie industry, according to Haynes (2005), the name “Nollywood” was invented by a non-Nigerian, first appearing in an article by Matt Steinglass in the New York Times in 2002 and continued to be imposed by foreigners to Nigeria. Haynes (2005) states that Nollywood is an example of Nigeria living up to its potential role as the leader of Africa. According to the Nigeria Censor Board, 1,080 videos have been marketed between 1997 and 2000. Most are shot in Pidgin, Ibo, Yoruba, or Hausa - the main languages of Nigeria’s 250 or so ethnic groups-and then subtitled in English. In this regard, some locally made films are known to fit this cultural agenda. While Amadi - a 1975 product of Ola Balogun - demonstrated the beauty of the Igbo Language, Ajani Ogun, also by Ola Balogun, demonstrated the richness of the Yoruba language and Sheu Umar by Adamu Halilu, that of the Hausa language (Owens-Ibie, 2005). Nigerian movies hold a very prominent place in the minds and hearts of most Africans and among the broad variety of Africans or those of African descent that have been exposed to Nigerian videofilms. Influence on Ghanaians Faris (2002) in his article, Hollywood, Who Really Needs It? quotes Chico Ejiro, who says that Ghanaians and Nigerians are like cousins in terms of their common experience of being colonized by the British. Nigerian and Ghanaian cinemas are usually grouped together because of their common colonial history and because their industries developed in a somewhat similar manner. Nwachukwu Ukadike (1994) in his work Black African Cinema, states: Ghana and Nigeria are identical twins. In times of prosperity both countries have competed against each other or rallied together for a common cause in the pan-African spirit. In times of adversity both have expelled each other’s nationals, but even in hard times they still celebrate an annual soccer competition. Both have experienced successive military coups and coups d’etat, inept government, and unprecedented looting of government treasuries by officials, whether clothed in civilian garb or military uniforms. (p.127) In his remarks, Chico Ejiro says that in eight years he has directed 80 "home videos,” and declares that Nigeria also exports its videos, with particular success in Ghana. The names of the actors and actresses, as well as the roles that they play, are well known. Norimtitsu Onishi (2002) of the New York Times in his article “Step side, L.A. and Bombay, for Nollywood,” a popular Nigerian actress, Kate Henshaw-Neattall, says that when she visited Ghana, she was surprised about her popularity outside Nigeria and she said, “I was shocked. People came up to me and said. Aren’t you the Nigerian actress?” (p. 10). This is evidence of the diverse African audience who patronize Nigerian movies. This illustrates the impact and the influence of Nollywood on Africans and its culture. Not all the response has been favorable, McLaughlin (2005) notes, “Nollywood's influence is so strong across Africa that there's been a backlash against Nigerian movies 243 Uchenna Onuzulike in nearby Ghana, where police have reportedly been raiding shops selling Nollywood videos, though it's not clear what laws have been breached. In interview with Williams says, “They're struggling not to be colonized by Nigerian movies’ (p.1). McLaughlin states that other countries in Africa are hustling to copy Nigeria: “Uganda is trying to jumpstart ‘Ugandawood’” (p.1). Influence on Africans in Africa Ofe Motiki (2006) of In Mwegi, Botswana's only independent daily newspaper says that many people love Nigerian videofilms and find them irresistible mostly because of their familiar story lines. He says that Nigerian movies are a household name all over Botswana. “Although the whole cinematography of the movies is not of the best quality, a lot of people are in love with them” (p.1). He notes that the common incorrect use of adjectives, nouns and verbs are all ignored and laughed at as in most homes people remain glued to their television sets when these movies are showing. Some say that the reason they love them is that they can easily relate to them. Motiki writes: All the movies that are aired on Mnet Africa can be found in various shops in Francistown [Botswana, a country in Africa] and are so in demand that even the street hawkers at the bus ranks [bus stations] are cashing in. The names of the movies are not only appealing but catchy too, names like, the Corridors of Power, Father and Son, Sharon Stone and many others. (p.1) Motiki (2006) quotes a Francistown, Botswana resident Kobamelo Mosheno who says that she began watching them last year and has never stopped: I don't think I will ever stop watching them and I now know the real names of all the actors and actresses. I have quite a collection of Nigerian DVDs at home and when I have enough money to subscribe to DSTV, I always make it a point to watch Channel 102 because of all the channels that DSTV offers that is where action happens. (p.1) Commenting on Nollywood’s influence on African culture on BBC Focus On Africa magazine, Muchinba (2004) notes: Nollywood films are packed with simple but dramatic storylines "Ah, you want to kill me now!" - the woman yanks her hair wildly, her facial expression alive and contorted dramatically. "No, not me, not today!" Her body shudders and in seconds, she is transformed into a vicious sleek mongrel, emitting blood-curdling growls. (p.12) She goes on to say that this is a scene from a typical Nigerian movie - and in many subSaharan countries, their popularity is growing tremendously, leaving fans burning with a longing for more. Muchinba adds that the stories tend to be rather plain although very dramatic and full with emotions: the women wail and are covetous money lovers; the men are just as emotional and very revengeful: Throw in a gibbering bone-rattling juju man and Bible-waving preacher and what you have is a brew of conflict, revenge, trials and tribulations - Nollywood Influence 244 the likes of which are keeping most Zambians, especially in the capital city, Lusaka, glued to TV screens for hours on end. (p.12) Oliver Mbamara (2000), a native of Nigeria and an Administrative Law Judge with the State of New York, notes that the film industry in Nigeria has grown tremendously and has established a presence in many African countries. He says that today, the industry is exporting itself strongly to other parts of Africa. Mbamara writes that a group of Nigeria actors and filmmakers including Fred Amata, Olu Jacobs, Genevieve Nnaji, and Omotola Jolade Ekeinde, visited Sierra Leone and were hosted by the President, Ahmad Tejan Kabbah, and the people of the country. It was the country’s way of showing appreciation for the role the Nigerian movie industry has played in helping the Sierra Leoneans heal from the scars of the civil war they went through in the past several years. Mbamara further states that recent reports show Sierra Leonean affection for Nigerians and they hold the Nollywood stars in high esteem. Nigerian filmmakers are now extending their activities to Sierra Leone and other African countries in order to help build the film industry in any of these countries. Mbamara states: “Reports have it that the Nigerian film industry is the second highest revenue earner in Nigeria today” (p.3). During a BBC (2006) interview with Martin Mangenda, a Zambian citizen, he said that the main problem with Nigerian movies is that they show too much witchcraft and black magic, adding that he did not think that all Africans are like that. He stated, “Mind you, these films are watched by children. Their minds get affected. He said he has stopped his family from watching them” (p.1). Influence on the African Diaspora In an article, Nigerian Film Industry Grows, Adebambo Adewopo (2005), director general of the Nigerian Copyright Commission, reported that Nigerian movies are showing real promise...away from home. Nigerian movies provide an alternative to Western-made movies. The Nigerian movie industry has generated over $200 million in international sales between 1992 and 2005. Washington Post staff writer Steven Gray (2003) in his article “Nollywood Films’ Popularity Rising Among Emigrants” quotes Joy Oreke-Arungwa, a Nigerian-born consultant now living in Laurel, who has written extensively on the evolution of sub-Saharan African media, ‘“For us parents, it becomes a reference book,” she recalled scenes in various Nigerian movies she made a point of showing her own children. Our kids, when they get here, they get lost, too Americanized,” she says “These movies show them the other side”’ (p.1). Gray (2003) resonates with the fast growing emergence of Nollywood in the Diaspora. He attests: These English-language Nigerian movies are gaining popularity among the nation's fast-growing African immigrant population, offering their very Americanized children a glimpse of African life, particularly the clash of modernity and traditionalism. (p.1) 245 Uchenna Onuzulike Interview In May 2006, I conducted an interview with Ms. Vida Causey, a Ghanaian native, who resides in the United States, in order to get a perspective on how Nigerian movies influence African culture. Causey, says that Nigerian movies are fun and interesting. She states that Nigerian traditional attire is very influential. “I like the elegance of it and I can relate to Nigerian culture”, she declares. “We discuss about the movies all the time”, she adds. Furthermore, she claims “the influence of Nigerian movies is too much; even the pastors are becoming afraid. She says her pastor (an African in the Unites States) preaches against watching Nigerian movies because that is what everybody is doing now.” She attests: A pastor advised us instead of watching Nigerian movies; we should buy the Sunday sermon and listen to it or keep them to give as a gift to others. There is competition over there, the pastors want to sell their church CDs and preaching tapes. The pastors know that their items do not sell as fast as they wish; therefore, they condemn anything that is not anybody’s fault. (V. Causey, personal communication, May 27, 2006) She says further that “100 percent of my friends are dying for the latest Nigerian movies. You call them on the phone that is what they have been watching,” she declares. She adds: I’m concerned about my husband watching those Nigerian movies because he thinks that everything in the movies is real. Since my husband is from the United States and does not speak my native language, he thinks that watching Nigerian movies will teach him and help him connect with African cultures. (V. Causey, personal communication, May 27, 2006) Causey says that she does not interact with Nigerian people as much (in the United States), so Nigerian movies give her an insight about Nigerian culture. She adds, “Nigerian movies are just like soap opera. Most of the movies depict lives beyond our reach. Like riches and how to make quick money. They influence our way of thinking and out way of life.” This shows how citizens from African countries admire and cherish Nigerian creativity. Causey says that Nigerian movies are fun and interesting, and give insight about Nigerian culture. She says that the Nigerian traditional way of dressing is very influential. She acknowledges that she like the classiness and she can relate to it. She also says that even the pastors are becoming afraid that Nigerian movies’ influence would deprive church congregations of time to perform other activities. The effect Nigeria has on the rest of African culture through the videofilm medium, which is a unique means of communication and social transformation, cannot be underestimated. Movie Analysis Many people believe that Nigeria is just the way it’s depicted in these Nigerian videofilms. Most of the videofilms have a supernatural and religious theme including “juju” and the clash of modern religion with African Traditional Religion. Juju is a cult Nollywood Influence 246 that is consulted for one reason or another when in need. It is largely psycho-medical buttressed with the power of the supernatural (Kwabena-Essem, 2006). According to Ukadike (2002), “Certain elements and codes resonating from a particular culture may influence film form and film style” (p.22), for example, Mbiti (1999) says “Africans are notoriously religious and each group has its own religious system of life so fully that it is not always easy or possible to isolate it” (p.14). The movies such as Christian Marriage (2002) and Pestilence (2004) show that the Nigerian movie industry is saturated with religious overtones and undertones. In the movie Christian Marriage the theme has religious connotations with Christian songs throughout the movie. Also, the same movie shows commercials and clips of upcoming movies and recently released movies, all of which show some implications of religion and cults. Christian Marriage The movie Christian Marriage (2002) is based on religious and parental influence in regard to their children’s marriages. Uche, Loretta, and Angela are sisters and their parents reject any suitor who is not a Roman Catholic. The mother says that that they must never marry a man who is not a Roman Catholic and that if they marry outside their church she will die. Uche and Loretta agree and abide to their parents’ view and marry men they do not love because they worship at the same church and to make their mother happy. Their marriages later disintegrate and they are unhappily married. Angela refused to let her parent stop her from marrying a man of her dreams, because of church affiliation. Despite her parent’s objection, she went ahead and married a man of her choice. Other denomination’s parents, such as Anglican and Grace of God have similar problem by refusing their children’s from marring out of love instead church affiliation. Since Christianity came to Africa, this kind of problem has been an issue. At the end of the movie, one of the actresses who plays the mother speaks that the lesson of the movie is parents not to intervene in their children’s marriages and relationships based on religion. Pestilence Pestilence (2004) is said to be based on a true story about a village called Obinuzo, which was drenched by calamity and disasters because the village had gone contrary to the law of their ancestors and gods. The villagers were concerned about the disaster that had befallen them. Consequently, the men of the village met at the village square to discuss what to do about the mishaps. The village sends ten men to the “evil forest” to inquire from the gods what they should do to stop the disaster. Out of ten men who are sent to the evil forest, only three make it back. The other seven died on their way back from attacks of some sort of spirit in the forest. The content of these movies show that Nigerian movies are embedded with religious themes. This is due to the African cultural heritage, which is rooted in religion. According to Hall (2005), the fact that culture is a symbolic system gives culture both the power to change and the power over change. 247 Uchenna Onuzulike Symbolic Convergence Theory The symbolic convergence theory is used to explore the religious overtones in the Nigerian movie industry. It offers a possible explanation for similarities between movie “myths” and opinion trends. The symbolic convergence theory, often known as fantasytheme analysis, is a well-developed theory by Ernest Bormann, John Cragan, and Donald Shields dealing with the use of narrative in communication (Littlejohn & Foss, 2005). According to Griffin (1991), "Through symbolic convergence, individuals build a sense of community or a group consciousness” (p. 34). Since it is difficult to make a causal link between the fantasy themes of the movies and the parallel trends in public opinion, the symbolic convergence theory offers a potential explication with at least face validity (Littlejohn & Foss, p.158). This theory suggests that repeated exposure to the myths and themes of the movies on Nigerian cultures should influence an individual's perceptions of Nigerians and African as a whole. Perhaps the better way to evaluate fantasy themes, then, is in their artistry the creativity, novelty, and wisdom with which they are used, combined, and formed into visions. One can recognize a fantasy theme because it is repeated again and again. In fact, some themes are so frequently discussed and so well known within a particular group or community that the members no longer tell the whole episode, but abbreviate it by presenting just a “trigger” or symbolic cue, for example, the depiction of juju in Nigerian movies. This theory suggests that the repeated exposure to the myths and themes of the Nigerian movies should eventually influence individual perception of Nigerians and the reality of Nigerian culture. Conclusion Outside Africa’s shores, many stores, web sites, magazines, community newspapers, and journals are making a remarkable endeavor to promote African culture in the lands alien to Africans and create awareness to Africans abroad so that while in their sojourn abroad, Africans would not lose touch with their heritage. This work shows that Africans and the Diaspora are embracing Nigerian cultural heritage through watching Nigerian videofilms. The analysis of this work suggests that Nigerian movies are capable of influencing Africans, impacting their lives. Given the great influence that Nollywood has over African culture, such effect is reinforced by a massive consumption of Nigerian movies by Africans living in Africa and off the shores of Africa. It was proved in this study that many Africans all over the world watch Nigeria movies. For example, according to the interviews I conducted and the interviews gathered by the BBC radio that were used in this study, there is interest in watching Nigerian movies by Nigerians and other Africans both within African and outside the African continent. Many Nigerian videofilms are filled with religious overtones; therefore, many Africans see most Nigerians as juju people and other negative attributions based on what are portrayed in Nigerian movies. These interviews show that the rest of African countries believe that Nigerians are what are depicted in Nigerian movies. As would be expected, these films portray values rooted in their countries of origin. Non-Nigerians (other Africans than Nigerians) think that Nigerian movies are realistic and they assume that Nigerian movies depict what all of Nigeria really is like. Nollywood Influence 248 Nigerian videofilms as a representation of Nigerian culture have a great influence on an entire population of Africans due to similar cultural practices. Africans are proud of their artistic culture and heritage; as a result, they are showing appreciation for the significance of their cultural elements by acknowledging and patronizing the Nigerian movie industry and other related interests of Africa. Movies that are made in Africa and about Africa help to bridge continents and the people who live on them, providing critically important points of reference for immigrant people who are struggling to reconcile dual identities as citizens of their countries of origin and the new society in which they are trying to adapt and build new lives. 249 Uchenna Onuzulike References Adewope, A. (2005, Dec.). Nigerian film industry grows. 25.7, Article 1. Retrieved January 24, 2005, from http://find.galegroup.com/itx/infomark.do/$contentSet African Movie Channel (2006). Retrieved on July 7, 2006 from http:/www.afrol.com/articles/19229 Bormann, E. (1972). Fantasy and rhetorical vision: The rhetorical criticism of social reality. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 58,396-407. Bormann, E. (1982a) “Colloquy: Fantasy and rhetorical vision: Ten years later.” Quarterly Journal of speech, 68,288-305. Bormann, E. (1982b). A fantasy theme analysis of the television coverage of the hostage release and the Reagan inaugural. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 68, 133-145. Causey, V. Personal interviews May 27, 2006. Cisse, S. (2002). Questioning African cinema (Frank N. Ukadike) [Interview]. University of Minnesota Press, 19 - 28. Faris, S. (2002). Time International, "Hollywood, who really needs it? 159 (19) (May 20, 2002) Retrieved January 24, 2006, from <http://find.galegroup.com/itx/infomark.do?&contentSet=IACFoss, K. A., & Littlejohn, S.W. (2005). Theories of human communication. CA: Thomson Wadsworth. Grey, S. (2003, November 8). Nigeria on-screen 'Nollywood' films popularity rising among émigrés. The Washington Post-Washington, D.C, p. E01 Griffin, E. (1997). A first look at communication theory. New York: McGraw-Hill. Hall, B. J. (2005). Among cultures: The challenge of communication. CA: Thomson Wadsworth. Haynes, J. (Ed.). (2000). Nigeria video films. Ohio: Ohio University Center for International Studies. Haynes, J. (2005). Nollywood: What's in a name? Retrieved May 12, 2006, from http://www.nollywood.net/Essays Kwabena-Essem, N. A, (2006). A new look at juju. Djembe Magazine. Retrieved July 7, 2006 from http://www.afgen.com/juju.html Mbamara, O. (2005, May). Ban on Nigerian movies in Ghana. Retrieved August 7, 2005, from http://www.africanevents.com/EssaysTalesSpeeches.htm McLaughlin, A. (2005, December). Africans, camera, action: Nollywood catches world's eye. The Christian Science Monitor World. Retrieved August 7, 2005, from http://www.csmonitor.com/2005/1220/p01s02-woaf.html Mbiti, J. S. (1999) African religions and philosophy. Oxford, England: Heinemann Educational Publishers. Motiki, O. (2006, June 21). Botswana: Nigerian movies prove popular. Retrieved on July 7, 2006 from http://allafrica.com/stories/200606210094.html Muchinba, H. (2004, September 23). Nigerian film lights Zambia's screens. Retrieved July, 15 2006 from http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/africa/3674322.stm Nwabulu, V. (Producer), & Okpala, C. (Writer). (2002). Christian Marriage [Motion picture]. Nigeria: Valseco Production. Odunukwe, A., & Odunukwe. U. (Producers), & Prospect, N. & Oge-Kalu, C. (Writer) (2004). Pestilence [Motion picture]. Nigeria: Ulzee Films. Nollywood Influence 250 Onishi, N. (2002, September 16). Step Aside, L.A. and Bombay, for Nollywood. The New York Times, p.10. Onuzulike, U. (2007). The birth of Nollywood: The Nigerian movie industry. Black Camera, 22(1), 25 – 26. Orewere, B. (1992). Towards a film policy for Nigeria. In U. Ekwuazi., & Y. Nasidi. Y (Ed.), Film for development: Theory and practice at the crossroads (pp. 308-319). Jos, Nigeria: Nigerian Film Corporation. Owens-Ibie, N. (1998) How video films developed in Nigeria. Retrieved October, 15 2006from Person, B. (July 9, 2001). Rebounding biz goes straight to video. (Nigerian motion picture industry) Retrieved 9, October, 2007, from http://findarticles.com/p/articles/mi_hb1437/is_200107/ai_n5929667 Rogers, E.M. (1995). Diffusion of innovations (4th ed.). New York: The Free Press. Servant, J.C. (2001) Nigeria: Straight to video. [Electronic version]. World Press Review, 48(5), 40 - 41. Ukadike, N. F. (1994). Black African Cinema. Berkeley, California: University of California Press. Human Communication. A Publication of the Pacific and Asian Communication Association. Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 251 – 266. The Impact of Gender on Instructor Nonverbal Communication from the Perspectives of Learner Affect and Learners’ Perceptions of Instructor Melinda Findley Texas Tech University Narissra Punyanunt-Carter Texas Tech University Melinda Findley (E.Ed. Texas Tech. University), was a Doctoral Candidate at Texas Tech University. Narissra Punyanunt-Carter (Ph.D. Kent state University) is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Communiation Studies at Texas Tech University. Direct all correspsondence to Melinda Findley; melinda.findley@ttu.edu Nonverbal Instructional Communication 252 Abstract Past research on instructor nonverbal behaviors has focused on the effects of immediacy and dominance on cognitive learning, affective learning, and learners’ perceptions of instructor. This study built on that research by removing the focus on immediacy and dominance behaviors; rather, the authors compiled a broad list of nonverbal behaviors representative of those instructors use in the classroom. Furthermore, student respondent gender and instructor gender were coded to determine their impact on findings. Key findings included few gender differences in the student responses; instead, the major difference occurred in the nonverbal behaviors students identified for male and female instructors. 253 Melinda Fin and Narissra Punyanunt-Carter Introduction A girl bats her eyelashes at a boy she’s interested in. A man puts his arm around his elderly father’s shoulders. A mother raises her eyebrows at her unruly child. Each of these behaviors exists within the context of a relationship and has meaning in that relationship. In the same way, a teacher’s nonverbal behaviors have meaning within the context of a teacher-student relationship (Grant & Hennings, 1971). In fact, “because the teacher is vitally concerned with the communication of meanings, in the encounter between teacher and student physical motions are even more significant than in the non-structured encounter between persons” (Grant & Hennings, 1971, pgs. 3-4). In their 2001 study, Semykina and Rsyhova looked at the extent to which teachers understand the specific features of nonverbal behaviors. They found that 56 percent of the teachers in their study recognized only one feature of nonverbal communication, while ten percent of the teachers could not recognize any features of nonverbal communication. This demonstrates a need for teachers to better understand what nonverbal behavior is, and how it impacts their classroom communication. According to Witt and Wheeless (2001, p. 327), “the communication behaviors employed by teachers play a strategic role in student learning outcomes.” In the past, teachers’ nonverbal behaviors have been explored for the role they have on affective learning, cognitive learning, and student perception of teacher (Andersen, 1979, 1985; Richmond et al., 1987; Witt and Wheeless, 2001; Witt, Wheeless, & Allen, 2004; Mehrabian, 1969, 1971, 1981; Rodriguez, Plax, & Kearney, 1996; Burgoon, 1994; Coker & Burgoon, 1987; Patterson, 1983; Chamberlin, 2000; Cappella, 1985; Dovidio & Ellyson, 1985; Harper, 1985; Schwartz, Tesser, & Powell, 1982; Spiegal & Machotka, 1974; Chesebro, 2003; Thweatt, 1999; Rocca, 2004; and Hendrix, 1997). For instance, Witt and Wheeless (2001) looked at both verbal and nonverbal immediacy and their effect on recall, learning loss, and affective learning. Witt, Wheeless, and Allen (2004) conducted a meta-analysis of 81 studies to examine teachers’ verbal and nonverbal immediacy and the effect on learning outcomes. Rocca (2004) studied instructor nonverbal immediacy and verbal aggressiveness as it applied to student attendance. Chamberlin (2000) considered both immediacy and dominance nonverbal behaviors and the way they affect a teachers’ perceived trustworthiness. These studies seem to have a narrow focus on nonverbal immediacy (sometimes paired with a verbal variable) and / or nonverbal dominance (sometimes paired with a verbal variable). While the findings of these studies definitely advance scholars’ understanding of the effects of instructor nonverbal behaviors, the narrow focus of the studies may have caused certain nonverbal behaviors and their effects to be ignored. The field seems to be lacking in studies that look at universal nonverbal behaviors, affective learning, cognitive learning, and students’ perceptions of instructor. Furthermore, there appear to be few studies that consider demographic characteristics of instructors and how those characteristics differentiate the way their nonverbal behaviors are received. There are a few studies of note that have branched out into this area: Hendrix (1997) explored nonverbal communication and students’ perceptions of black and white professors; Arbuckle and Williams (2003) considered age and gender stereotypes when they looked at the way students evaluate verbal expressiveness and nonverbal vocal qualities of speakers of varying age and gender; Menzel and Carrell (1999) studied perceived learning, willingness to talk, verbal immediacy, and nonverbal immediacy and grouped their responses by student and teacher gender. Nonverbal Instructional Communication 254 This research is intended to build on the earlier research, while stretching to allow new information to emerge. The authors followed the lead of earlier scholars in focusing on learner affect and learners’ perceptions of instructors. The authors veered from previous research in that the nonverbal behaviors studied were not limited to immediacy or dominance behaviors; rather, the authors attempted to compile a broad list of nonverbal behaviors that were representative of those any instructor might use in the classroom. The authors also coded the gender of the student respondent and the instructor gender so that those variables could be analyzed for their impact on the findings. It is the authors’ hope that the findings presented in this paper will serve the scholarly community by adding in a constructive way to the body of knowledge about the effects instructor nonverbal behaviors. Furthermore, the intent of this research is to provide instructors with the knowledge to become aware and manage their nonverbal behaviors more effectively. In effect, instructors may be able to use the findings to develop a prioritized growth plan for their nonverbal behaviors, based on the emotions they want to foster or avoid, as well as the perceptions they want to foster or avoid. Review of Literature In the following review of literature, previous research has been organized into the following categories: nonverbal behaviors, instructor nonverbal behaviors and learner affect, and instructor nonverbal behaviors and learner perceptions of teaching effectiveness. Nonverbal Behaviors As humans interact, they commit a steady stream of verbal and nonverbal actions that send messages about their attitudes, feelings, and personality; conversely, those they communicate with are left to figure out what the messages mean (Knapp & Hall, 2002). The spoken word falls into the category of verbal communication and its related studies; all other actions, including the way verbal language is expressed, fall into the category of nonverbal studies. Nonverbal communication includes the way humans use social and personal space; the way humans move their body parts; the way the vocal cords change the quality of the human voice; and the sounds emitted from the pharyngeal, oral, or nasal cavities (Knapp & Hall, 2002). Argyle (1988; as cited by Knapp & Hall, 2002) explained that humans use nonverbal behaviors to express emotion, express an interpersonal attitude, portray their personality, and manage conversational elements such as demonstrating attention and taking turns in conversation. Even when a person has an intended message to send using nonverbal behaviors, the receiver may interpret the meaning in a different way. Mehrabian (1969, 1971; as cited by Knapp & Hall, 2002) noted three categories of meaning that could be applied to nonverbal behaviors. The first was immediacy, which he described as a human tendency to evaluate the behavior as good or bad, positive or negative. The second category of meaning was status; receivers determine our status, or social position, based on our nonverbal behaviors. The final category of meaning Mehrabian described was responsiveness; this category describes the human perception of activity as slow or fast, active or passive. Grant and Hennings (1971) specifically looked at teachers, and divided teacher nonverbal behaviors into two categories: instructional and personal. Instructional behaviors are those which facilitate teaching, such as reading materials for teaching or directing student attention (Grant & Hennings, 1971). Instructional motions can be further broken down into subcategories: conducting motions, such as those which control participation or get attention; acting motions, such as those which emphasize, illustrate, or pantomime; and wielding motions, such as those 255 Melinda Fin and Narissra Punyanunt-Carter which include “interaction with objects, materials, or parts of the room” (Grant & Hennings, 1971, p. 13). Personal behaviors, on the other hand, include individualized mannerisms or adjustments, such as tugging on an earlobe when nervous or adjusting a skirt that has ridden too high (Grant & Hennings, 1971). Nonverbal Behaviors and Learner Affect Mottet (2000) discussed learners’ nonverbal responsiveness in terms of immediacy, because immediacy has been shown to improve affective learning in the classroom (Andersen, 1979; as cited by Mottet, 2000). Mottet (2000) referenced Richmond et al. (1987) as having identified 14 nonverbal immediacy behaviors including smiling at students, moving around the classroom while teaching, using a variety of vocal expressions, and having a relaxed body position while teaching. Andersen, 1985; Burgoon, 1994; Coker & Burgoon, 1987; Mehrabian, 1971; Patterson, 1983 (as cited by Chamberlin, 2000) identified physical closeness, increased direct body and facial orientation, eye gaze, smiling, head nods, and frequent and animated gesturing as behaviors that elevate immediacy and involvement in the classroom. Witt and Wheeless (2001) explained that immediacy has its roots in approach-avoidance theory. Approach-avoidance theory states that “people approach what they like and avoid what they don’t like” (Mehrabian, 1981, p. 22; as cited by Witt & Wheeless, 2001). Rodriguez, Plax, & Kearney (1996) conducted a study to find out how immediacy behaviors impact affective learning and cognitive learning. The authors found support for the Affective Learning Model, which states that the connection between teacher nonverbal immediacy and cognitive learning is mediated by the students’ affective learning (Rodriguez et al., 1996). In effect, teachers use immediacy nonverbal behaviors in order to get students to like them, which opens the door for approaching students and increasing their learning (Rodriguez et al., 1996). Chamberlin (2000) explained that while immediacy nonverbal behaviors can have positive consequences for teachers, dominance nonverbal behaviors can have the opposite effect. The author described dominance nonverbal behaviors as those “that reinforce the traditional hierarchical nature of the teacher-supervisor relationship” (Chamberlin, 2000, p. 355). Nonverbal behaviors associated with dominance are using a relaxed posture, indirect body orientation, physical placement (at front of classroom, behind a lectern, etc.), using larger amounts of physical space, limited eye contact, protection of territory (Cappella, 1985; Dovidio & Ellyson, 1985; Harper, 1985; Mehrabian, 1971; Schwartz, Tesser, & Powell, 1982; and Spiegal & Machotka, 1974; as cited by Chamberlin, 2000). Menzel and Carrell (1999) studied perceived learning, willingness to talk, verbal immediacy, and nonverbal immediacy and grouped their responses by student and teacher gender. They conducted research with 256 undergraduate students and found that a student’s willingness to talk in class was positively associated with instructor verbal immediacy and not related to gender. The amount of learning the student perceived to have taken place, however, was positively associated with both instructor verbal immediacy and the sex of the instructor being the same as the student. Based on the research literature, research question one was posed: RQ1: If learners are given a list of emotions they experience while in a classroom, what nonverbal instructor behaviors would they identify as causing those emotions? Nonverbal Instructional Communication 256 Nonverbal Behaviors and Learner Perceptions of Teaching Effectiveness Chamberlin (2000) reported that teachers who displayed nonverbal behaviors of immediacy and involvement were perceived as likeable, warm, accessible, approachable, having a positive attitude. Chamberlin (2000) expanded on those findings, and linked nonverbal behaviors of immediacy and involvement to positive student-teacher relationships, positive impressions of instructor competence, increased student motivation, increased teacher credibility, positive teacher evaluations. Thweatt, 1999 (as cited by Rocca, 2004) found instructor immediacy behaviors to positively affect students’ perceptions of an instructor as competent, caring, and trustworthy. Hendrix (1997) explored nonverbal communication and students’ perceptions of black and white professors. The study participants defined credible instructors as either knowledgeable or knowledgeable and a good teacher (Hendrix, 1997). In terms of perceived differences in black and white instructors, the students listed the same verbal and nonverbal behaviors demonstrating credibility for both black and white professors (Hendrix, 1997). Hendrix (1997) did report that students looked for more evidence of academic and experiential credentials before deeming black instructors credible. Arbuckle and Williams (2003) considered age and gender stereotypes when they looked at the way 352 undergraduate students evaluate verbal expressiveness and nonverbal vocal qualities of speakers of varying age and gender. The authors controlled for visual cues by using an audiotape of a professor speaking, and presenting the material identically (Arbuckle & Williams, 2003). Students’ evaluations of speaking enthusiastically and using a meaningful voice tone during class lecture were higher for instructors they perceived as young and male than they were for instructors they perceived as young and female, old and male, and old and female (Arbuckle & Williams, 2003). One concept that has not been discussed with relevance to students’ perceptions of teachers is that of communal and agentic characteristics. These terms come from the study of sex stereotypes and social roles. Traditionally, women have been thought to exhibit communal characteristics which cause them to be viewed as warm, giving, cooperative, and focused on interpersonal relationships; men have been thought to exhibit agentic characteristics which cause them to be viewed as independent, task-oriented, dominant, and competitive (Bem, 1974, Eagly, 1987, and Gilligan, 1982; as cited by Myers et al., 2005). This raises questions about whether students’ perceptions of teachers can be limited to an immediacy/dominance categorization without a sub-categorization for teacher sex. From this body of research, research question two was created: RQ2: If students are given a list of perceptions they might have of an instructor, what nonverbal instructor behaviors would they identify as causing those perceptions? 257 Melinda Fin and Narissra Punyanunt-Carter Method Participants Participants in the present study were 149 students from a large Southwestern university. Of the 149 subjects, 79 (53.0%) were men, 70 (47.0%) were women. The sample consisted of 30 (20.1%) first year students, 37 (24.8%) sophomores, 34 (22.8%) juniors, 33 (22.2%) seniors, and 15 (10.1%) that identified themselves as “other”. In addition, 5 (3.4%) were African-American, 120 (80.5%) were Caucasian, 15 (10.1%) were Hispanic, 3 (2.0%) were Asian/Pacific Islander and 6 (4.0) classified themselves as “other”. All participants received course credit for their participation. Measures Instructor Nonverbal Communication Questionnaire. In order to assess the importance of nonverbal communication in the classroom, a questionnaire was created to address learners’ emotions based on instructor nonverbal behavior and learners’ perceptions of the instructor based on the instructor’s nonverbal behavior. Participants were randomly given a questionnaire concerning either a male or female instructor. In addition, participants were instructed to reflect on how their instructor’s nonverbal communication affects their feelings and their view of their instructor. Students were asked 20 fill-in-the-blank statements about their perceptions of their instructor. The authors used a ten-item, bi-polar design to assess learner affect (feel important/feel unimportant, feel frustrated/feel content, feel engaged/feel bored, feel nervous/feel confident, feel comfortable/feel uncomfortable) and a ten-item, bi-polar design to assess learners’ perceptions of instructor (competent/incompetent, organized/disorganized, caring/uncaring, trustworthy/untrustworthy, effective/ineffective). For instance, one statement on the questionnaire stated: “I feel important when instructors _____, _____, and _____.” Each question had three blanks so that students could pick the top three nonverbal behaviors that they perceived to answer that particular question. Each statement dealt with one of the feelings or perceptions listed above. Students were given a word bank that contained 39 nonverbal behaviors that teachers typically display in the classroom. The word bank included both instructional motions, such as moving around the classroom while teaching and nodding head at a student, and personal motions, such as blushing and drumming fingers. Students could pick from any of the 39 nonverbal behaviors to complete their questionnaire. Cronbach’s alpha for the scale was .70. Data Analysis In order to identify differences for each of the statements, a multiple response analysis was conducted. Each of the three nonverbal behaviors were categorized together in order to develop frequencies for each statement. In addition, multiple response cross-tabs were computed to look at the differences between the sex of the instructor and perceptions of the nonverbal behavior. Nonverbal Instructional Communication 258 Results The first research question looked at which nonverbal behaviors instructors use to make the students feel a certain way. When students were asked what nonverbal instructor behaviors make them feel important, the top three answers were show attention on face when I speak, make frequent eye contact with me, and smile at me. When the students were asked about feelings of unimportance, the top three answers were frown at me, drum their fingers, and mumble. When students were asked about feelings of frustration, the top three answers were mumble, drum their fingers, and use non-words. When students were asked about feeling content, the top three answers were speak with varied tone, speak loudly, and stand with relaxed body posture. When students were asked about feelings of being engaged, the top three answers were moving around the classroom while teaching, speak with a varied tone and show attention on face when I speak. When students were asked about feelings of boredom, the top three answers were speak with the same tone, stand behind desk or podium, and pause in speech. When asked about feelings of nervousness, the students’ top three answers were shrug, make extended eye contact with me, and touch my shoulder/arm. When asked about feelings of confidence, the top three answers were smile at me, show attention on face when I speak, and nod their head at me. When asked about what nonverbal behaviors instructors use to make students feel comfortable, the top three answers were smile at me, stand with relaxed body posture, and more around classroom while teaching. When asked about what nonverbal behaviors instructors use to make students feel uncomfortable, the top three answers were make extended eye contact with me, touch my shoulder/arm, and frown at me. The responses of male and female students were very similar across all emotions. Some differences did exist, however, in the data gathered for female instructors and male instructors. In reference to feeling important, unimportant, engaged, bored, confident, and uncomfortable, the differences between the nonverbal behaviors of male and female instructors were minor. In all of these cases, students agreed on two of the three top nonverbal behaviors they used to describe male and female instructors. For two of the emotions, frustration and comfortable, students listed the exact same top nonverbal behaviors for both male and female instructors. The biggest differences were found in the nonverbal behaviors students referenced as causing feelings of contentment and nervousness. These differences are illustrated in Figures 1 and 2. 259 Melinda Fin and Narissra Punyanunt-Carter Male / Female Learner Affects Content Top three nonverbals (overall): • Speak with varied tone • Speak loudly • Stand with relaxed posture • • • Female Instructors Move around room Smile at student Speak loudly • • • Male Instructors Move eyes around room Move around room Stand with relaxed back Figure 1. Differences in the nonverbal behaviors students identify from male and female instructors as causing students to feel content. Nonverbal Instructional Communication 260 Male / Female Learner Affects Nervous Top three nonverbals (overall): • Shrug • Extended eye contact • Touch my shoulder/arm • • • Female Instructors Point at student Extended eye contact Tap feet • • • Male Instructors Lean toward student Point at student Touch my shoulder/arm Figure 2. Differences in the nonverbal behaviors students identify from male and female instructors as causing students to feel nervous. The second research question looked at the nonverbal behaviors instructors demonstrate, and the resulting perceptions students form of those instructors. When asked about perceiving an instructor as competent, the students’ top three answers were move around the classroom while teaching, speak loudly, and speak with a varied tone. When asked about perceiving an instructor as incompetent, the top three answers were pause in speech, use non-words, and grunt. When asked about perceiving an instructor as organized, the students’ top three answers were move around the classroom while teaching, stand up straight, and speak loudly. When asked about perceiving an instructor as disorganized, the top three answers were mumble, use non-words, and drum their fingers. When asked about perceiving an instructor as caring, the top three answers were smile at me, show attention on face when I speak, and touch my shoulder/arm. When asked about perceiving an instructor as uncaring, the top three answers were frown at me, grimace, and drum their fingers. When asked about perceiving an instructor as trustworthy, the top three answers were smile at me, show attention on face when I speak, and make frequent eye contact. When asked about perceiving an instructor as untrustworthy, the top three answers were mumble, grunt, and smirk at me. When asked about perceiving an instructor as effective, the top three answers were speak loudly, move around the classroom, and gesture with their arms/hands while talking. When asked about perceiving an instructor as ineffective, the top three answers were mumbling, speak with same tone, and use non-words. The responses of male and female students were very similar across all perceptions. Some differences did exist, however, in the data gathered for female instructors and male instructors. In reference to perceiving instructors as incompetent, organized, trustworthy, and untrustworthy, the differences between the nonverbal behaviors of male and female instructors were minor. For each of these perceptions, students agreed on two of the three top nonverbal 261 Melinda Fin and Narissra Punyanunt-Carter behaviors they used to describe male and female instructors. For five of the perceptions – competent, disorganized, caring, effective, and ineffective – students listed the exact same top nonverbal behaviors for both male and female instructors. The biggest difference was found in the nonverbal behaviors students referenced as causing a male or female instructor to be perceived as uncaring. These differences are illustrated in Figure 3. Male / Female Learners’ Perceptions of Instructor Uncaring perceived as uncaring. Discussion • • • Top three nonverbals (overall): • Frown at student • Grimace • Drum fingers Female Instructors Frown at student Tap feet Drum fingers • • • Male Instructors Grimace Drum fingers Shrug Figure 3. Differences in the nonverbal behaviors students identify from male and female instructors as causing instructors to be Findings Overall, participants in this study indicated there were certain instructor nonverbal behaviors that students perceive as beneficial and some that are unhelpful, regardless of the gender of the instructor. Instructor behaviors that were generally perceived as positive are smiling, vocal variation, moving around the classroom, showing attention on face, making eye contact with students, and standing with a relaxed posture. Instructor behaviors that were generally perceived as negative are frowning at students, drumming fingers, mumbling, and using non-words. Students were generally more varied in the nonverbal behaviors they identified as negative. These findings seem to partially support the body of research on immediacy, perhaps providing richer information and insight on behaviors that have not been considered immediate nonverbal behaviors previously. These findings also support the consideration of a category of behaviors connected to instructor incompetence; this would encompass the nonverbal behaviors students referenced when perceiving an instructor as incompetent, disorganized, or ineffective. The behaviors students listed were neither wholly immediate nor wholly dominant in their characteristics. Nonverbal Instructional Communication 262 Also, results revealed that students perceived differences among the nonverbal behaviors that male and female instructors display in the classroom and the influences of these behaviors on students’ feelings and perceptions. These differences were particularly evident for students’ feelings of contentment and nervousness and students’ perceptions of instructors as uncaring. While the gender differences in these areas are striking, so is the lack of difference that was evident in students’ feelings of frustration and comfort and students’ perceptions of instructors as competent, disorganized, caring, effective, and ineffective. Limitations This study is limited in its generalizability. Due to the fact that all participants were enrolled in a university at either the undergraduate or graduate level, these findings are limited to the relationship between college students and their instructors. Furthermore, the results are limited to the southwest, as nonverbal behaviors and the resulting learner affect and learner perceptions of instructor may be regional. Finally, the participants in this study were overwhelmingly Caucasian. There may be differences in the perception of nonverbal cues among students of other races or cultures. This should be considered in future studies. This study is also limited in the statistical inferences that can be drawn from its data. The researchers purposely developed a multiple response survey to increase the depth of data gathered for each variable. The result of this choice, however, was an inability to analyze the data using any statistical method more complicated than frequencies and crosstabs. This lessens the researchers’ ability to speak to strength and power in relation to the findings. Implications This study builds on previous research in the areas of nonverbal immediacy and dominance (Richmond, Gorham, & McCroskey, 1987; Rocca, 2004). Behaviors, such as smiling at students, moving around the classroom while teaching, using a variety of vocal expressions, and having a relaxed body position, were verified as being positive by virtue of their connection to positive learner affects and positive perceptions of instructors. Conversely, this study casts a shadow of doubt on some behaviors that were previously considered to be immediate nonverbal behaviors. Participants in this study were conflicted on behaviors such as moving nearer a student, touching a student on the arm or shoulder, eye contact with students, and gesturing. It seems these behaviors may sometimes have a negative connotation. This study also raises questions about whether immediate and dominant nonverbal behaviors alone constitute the full range of important nonverbal behaviors that instructors should consider. There seemed to be similarities in the nonverbal behaviors that caused students to perceive an instructor as incompetent, disorganized, or ineffective. This study also points to important differences in the gender of the instructor sending the nonverbal messages. Although the findings of this study don’t point to the reasons the genders are viewed differently, the findings do cast doubt on the idea that immediacy behaviors have universal appeal. These findings demonstrate that the gender of the instructor has some bearing on whether the nonverbal behaviors have positive or negative effects. Future Research In order to build on the findings of this study, qualitative research should be conducted to gain a better understanding of the way students perceive gender differences in instructor nonverbal behaviors. In-depth interviews or focus groups would provide more specific information to describe this phenomenon. Furthermore, qualitative methods could also shed light 263 Melinda Fin and Narissra Punyanunt-Carter on the reason students perceive some nonverbal behaviors in both positive and negative lights. In order to determine the strength and significance of these findings, future research should attempt to triangulate findings with quantitative research that can be examined with a Chi-Squared analysis or by determining strength of correlation or causation. Summary Overall, this study raises questions about the practice of labeling a set of behaviors as positive or negative. Participants in this study indicated that certain instructor nonverbal behaviors could actually work in both positive and negative ways to impact learner affect and learner perceptions of instructors. Furthermore, participants in this study revealed important differences in the way they perceive differences among the nonverbal behaviors that male and female instructors display in the classroom. These findings indicate a need for further research into college students’ perceptions of the nonverbal messages male and female instructors display in the classroom. Nonverbal Instructional Communication 264 References Andersen, J. F. (1979). Teacher immediacy as a predictor of teaching effectiveness. In D. Nimmo (Ed.), Communication Yearbook 3, (pp. 543-559). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books. Andersen, P. A. (1985). Nonverbal immediacy in interpersonal communication. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Multichannel integrations of nonverbal behavior (pp. 136). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Arbuckle, J. & Williams, B. D. (2003). Students’ perceptions of expressiveness: Age and gender effects on teacher evaluations. Sex Roles, 49(9/10), 507-516. Argyle, M. (1988). Bodily communication(2nd. Ed.). London: Methuen. Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45, 196-205. Burgoon, J. K. (1994). Nonverbal signals. In M. L. Knapp & G. R. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal communication (2nd ed., pp. 229-285). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Cappella, J. N. (1985). Production principles for turn-taking rules in social interaction: Socially anxious vs. socially secure persons. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 4, 193212. Chamberlin, C. R. (2000). Nonverbal behaviors and initial impressions of trustworthiness in teacher-supervisor relationships. Communication Education, 49(4), 352-364. Chesebro, J. L. (2003). Effects of teacher clarity and nonverbal immediacy on student learning, receiver apprehension, and affect. Communication Education, 52(2), 135-147. Coker, D. A. & Burgoon, J. K. (1987). The nature of conversational involvement and nonverbal encoding patterns. Human Communication Research, 13, 463-494. Dovidio, J. F. & Ellyson, S. L. (1985). Patterns of visual dominance in humans. In S. L. Ellyson & J. F. Dovidio (Eds.), Power, dominance, and nonverbal behavior (pp. 129-149). New York: Springer-Verlag. Eagly, A. E. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women’s development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Grant, B. M., & Hennings, D. G. (1971). Teacher moves: An Analysis of nonverbal activity. New York: Teachers College Press. Harper, R. G. (1985). Power, dominance, and nonverbal behavior: An overview. In S. L. Ellyson & J. F. Dovidio (Eds.), Power, dominance, and nonverbal behavior (pp. 29-48). New York: Springer-Verlag. Hendrix, K. G. (1997). Student perceptions of verbal and nonverbal cues leading to images of black and white professor credibility. The Howard Journal of Communications, 8(3), Knapp, M. L. & Hall, J. A. (2002). Nonverbal communication in human interaction (5th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Mehrabian, A. (1969). Some referents and measures of nonverbal behavior. Behavioral Research Methods and Instrumentation, 1, 213-217. Mehrabian, A. (1971). Silent messages. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Mehrabian, A. (1981). Silent messages (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. 265 Melinda Fin and Narissra Punyanunt-Carter Menzel, K. E. & Carrell, L. J. (1999). The impact of gender and immediacy on willingness to talk and perceived learning. Communication Education 48(1), 31-40. Meyers, R. A, Berdahl, J. L., Brashers, D., Considine, J. R., Kelly, J. R., Moore, C., Peterson, J. L., & Spoor, J. R. (2005). Understanding groups from a feminist perspective. In M. S. Poole and A. B. Hollingshead (Eds.), Theories of small groups: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 241-276), Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Mottet, T. P. (2000). Interactive television instructors’ perceptions of students’ nonverbal responsiveness and their influence on distance teaching. Communication Education, 49(2), 146-164. Patterson, M. L. (1983). Nonverbal behavior: A functional perspective. New York: SpringerVerlag. [Chapter 5 on “Intimacy”]. Richmond, V. P., Gorham, J., & McCroskey, J. C. (1987). The relationship between selected immediacy behaviors and cognitive learning. In M. L. McLaughlin (Ed.) Communication yearbook, 10 (pp. 574-590). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Rocca, K. A. (2004). College student attendance: impact of instructor immediacy and verbal aggression. Communication Education, 53(2), 185-195. Rodriguez, J., Plax, T. G., & Kearney, P. (1996). Clarifying the relationship between teacher nonverbal immediacy and student cognitive learning: Affective learning as the central causal mediator. Communication Education, 45(4), 293-305. Schwartz, B., Tesser, A., & Powell, E. (1982). Dominance cues in nonverbal behavior. Social Psychology Quarterly, 45(2), 114-120. Semykina, E. Y. & Rzyhova, V. K. (2001). Nonverbal communication in school-students reception. Sotsiologicheskie Issledovaniya, 27(12), 116-121. Spiegal, J. P. & Machotka, P. (1974). Messages of the body. New York: The Free Press, Macmillan. Thweatt, K. S. (1999). The impact of teacher immediacy, teacher affinity-seeking, and teacher misbehaviors on student-perceived teacher credibility. Paper presented at the National Communication Association, Chicago, IL. Witt, P. L. & Wheeless, L. R. (2001). An experimental study of teachers’ verbal and nonverbal immediacy and students’ affective and cognitive learning. Communication Education, 50(4), 327-342. Witt, P. L., Wheeless, L. R., & Allen, M. (2004). A meta-analytical review of the relationship between teacher immediacy and student learning. Communication Monographs, 71(2), 184-207. Human Communication. A Publication of the Pacific and Asian Communication Association. Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 267 – 284. Leader-Member Exchange and Cooperative Communication Between Group Members: Replication of Lee (1997, 2001) Study on Malaysia Respondents Hassan Abu Bakar Universitiy Utara Malaysia Che Su Mustaffa Universitiy Utara Malaysia Bahtiar Mohamad Universitiy Utara Malaysia Cooperative Communication 268 Abstract This study attempts test the affects of superior-subordinate relationships on group cooperative communication. Based on the Tukey’s HSD multiple comparisons test indicates that there are significant differences in subordinates’ perceptions of cooperative communication between in-group and out-group; mid-group and out-group; and between mid-group and in-group. This study also reveals that the impact of superior-subordinate relationships were grater on cooperative communication behavior between group members in Malaysia compared to US respondents. Thus, in a relation-oriented society like Malaysia, the supervisor may be a more important factor in influencing employee behavior and attitudes at work group than organization as an impersonal entity. The consequences of these results were elaborated in details. 269 Hassan Abu Bakar, Che Su Mustaffa and Bahtiar Mohama Introduction Very little research has attempted to empirically test the applicability of management theories such as LMX theory, developed in the US, to other nations. A good theory is described as having the ability to replicate. However, replication has not been adequately recognized as a valuable part of theory development (Tsang & Kwan, 1999). Tsang and Kwan (1999) offer six types of replications along with two dimensions. The first dimension concerns whether replication employs the same method of measuring constructs and analyzing data. The second dimension is about the source of data. In this dimension researchers may conduct a replication just on the data generated or employed by the study. A combination of the two dimensions produces six type of replication, which is: 1. Checking of analysis. In this type of replication, the researcher employs exactly the same procedures used in a past study to analyze the latter’s data set. 2. Reanalysis of data. The researcher uses different procedures to reanalyze the data of a previous study. 3. Exact replication. This is the case where a previous study is repeated on the same population by using basically the same procedures. 4. Conceptual extension. This type of replication involves employing procedures different from those of the original study and drawing a sample from the same population. 5. Empirical generalization. Repeating a past study on a different population, a researcher conducting an empirical generalization tests how far the results of the study are generalizable to another population. 6. Generalization and extension. The researcher employs different research procedures and draws a sample from a different population of subjects. They further argue that replications do help to support or discredit theories, although they do not lead to conclusive verification of falsification. This study attempt to retest the effect of Leader-member exchange theory (LMX) and cooperative communication based on Lee (1997) study on Malaysia respondents. We are aware that there are several attempts to test LMX in a country context outside the US, such as in China (Hui & Graen, 1997; Hui, Law, & Chen, 1999), and Japan (Wakabayashi & Graen, 1984). For example in Hui et al.’s (1999) study, the authors contrasted the effects of LMX with the effects of perceived job mobility on in-role performance and organizational citizenship behavior (OCB) through matching data from supervisor and subordinates in a major battery manufacturing company in southern China. Their study replicates studies conducted by Settoon et al. (1996) and Wayne et al (1997). In both of these studies LMX was found to be related to OCB (Settoon, Bennett, & Liden, 1996; Wayne, Shore, & Liden, 1997). In another study, based on structural equation analysis Hui et al. (1999) found that both LMX and perceived job mobility predicted OCB. This finding is also in line with Tsang and Kwan (1999) argument that replication of a study could support or disconfirm theories. The impact of LMX quality and communication behaviors in Malaysian organizations is under researched. This study will follow Lee (1997) construct and methodology in order to find the generalizability of his study. In particular this study seeks to verify the LMX and cooperative communication construct. Similarly this study tests the cross-cultural external validity of findings reported by Lee (1997) that LMX has a direct impact on cooperative communication. Cooperative Communication 270 Review of Literature Superiors are limited in their time and resources and share their personal and positional resources differently with their subordinates for job performances (Graen & Scandura, 1987; Graen & Uhl-Bien, 1995; Graen & Wakabayashi, 1994; Graen, Wakabayashi, Graen, & Graen, 1990; T. A. Scandura, Graen, & Novak, 1986). As a result, leaders tend to develop and maintain LMX relationships with their subordinates that vary in quality ranging from high (in-group), medium (mid-group) and low (outgroup) (Dansereau, Graen, & Haga, 1975; Terri A. Scandura & Graen, 1984; T. A. Scandura et al., 1986). High-quality exchange relationships sometimes called in-group, cadre, or partnership are demonstrated by a high degree of mutual positive affect, loyalty, contribution or obligation to exchange, professional respect, and trust. In contrast, the opposite is observed in low-quality exchange relationships sometimes called out-group, hire hands or manager ship (Liden & Maslyn, 1998). Leader-member exchange (LMX) theory indicates that subordinates in highquality LMX relationships received and report more favorable outcomes than their peers in low-quality LMX relationships. Duarte et al. (1994) found that poorly performing, high LMX employees were given favorable ratings, despite their actual performance. Moreover, Dinesech and Liden (1986) found that high LMX members consistently received more formal and informal rewards than low LMX members. Low-quality exchange members may experience a sense of unfairness which gives rise to feelings of second-class status (Dienesch & Liden, 1986; Duarte, Goodson, & Klich, 1994). In a separate study, findings suggest that supervisors may afford differing behavior, and thus possibly use different communication tactics with subordinates in higher quality exchange relationships than with those in lower quality relationships (Yrle, Hartman, & Galle, 2003). Subordinates perceptions of superior communication behavior can be influenced by the outcomes individuals receive. Therefore, compared to subordinates involved in low-quality LMXs, subordinates in high LMXs are likely to report favorable communication behavior in their interactions with superiors. Hypothesis Development In Lee (1997) study cooperative communication in the work group refers to the message exchange behaviors designed to facilitate the joint achievement of work group goals. Tjosvold, Andrews and Jones (1983) suggest that as individual become more cooperative in attaining work-related goals with other group members, they exchange more information, more cooperative in resources, show concern and interest in what others want to accomplish, and provide assistance (Tjosvold, Andrews, & Jones, 1983). Further it is also argued that individuals’ personalities also affect the cooperative behavior in working group (Lepine & Dyne, 2001; LePine & Van Dyne, 1998). In addition, Lee (1997) argued that the one potential influence for cooperative communication between group members is the quality of leader-member exchange (LMX) between superiors and subordinates. The cooperative behavior among organizational members plays an important role in contributing organizational effectiveness and social relationships. For example, LePine and Van Dyne (2001) study found that employee initiative through constructive change-oriented behavior contribute 271 Hassan Abu Bakar, Che Su Mustaffa and Bahtiar Mohama to contextual performance. In another study in health organization settings a cooperative communication skills intervention program among nursing home staff found to improve relationships with patient family members (Pilemer et al., 2003). These studies suggest that the important of cooperative communication behavior in influencing various organizational outcomes. Findings from several empirical studies suggest that LMX quality has a strong impact on social interaction. The differential qualities of LMX have been found to affect variety communication behaviors between superior and subordinate (Fairhurst, 1993; Krone, 1992; Mueller & Lee, 2002; Waldron, 1991; Yrle, Hartman, & Galle, 2002). Earlier research explicated how the quality of LMX affects subordinates’ and superiors’ communication areas such as discourse patterns, upward influence, communication expectations, cooperative communication, perceived organizational justice, and decisionmaking practices (Fairhurst, 1993; Fairhurst & Chandler, 1989; Krone, 1992; Lee, 1997, 2001; Lee & Jablin, 1995; Mueller & Lee, 2002; Yukl & Fu, 1999). In addition to this, a recent study indicates LMX and the magnitude of change interact to predict employee rationality (Olufowote, Miller, & Wilson, 2005). Thus, the quality of relationships that subordinate have with their superior will determine the types of interactions in the relationships. Sias and Jablin (1995) found that differences in the quality of a superior’s communication exchanges with his or her subordinates have an impact on co-worker communication. Co-workers are aware of the differential treatment and, in fact, talk about it. Furthermore, individuals in low versus high quality LMX relationships with their superior have more conversations about differential treatments with their peers. Sias (1996) also found that a co-worker conversation about differential treatment by their superiors serves to create and reinforce social perceptions about differential treatment in the work group (Sias, 1996; Sias & Jablin, 1995). Superior interaction patterns in highquality LMX relationships typify “open” communication exchanges (leadership) in which subordinates are afforded greater amounts of trust, confidence, and attention, inside information, negotiating latitude, and influence without resources to authority. In contrast, low-quality LMX are “closed” communication systems (supervision) in which superior use formal authority to force member to comply with prescribed role (Jablin, 1987; Terri A. Scandura & Graen, 1984). As a result, subordinates in low-quality LMX are restricted in their opportunities to influence decisions, and, hence complain of their superior’s resistance and unresponsiveness in their attempts to affect change. Based on these findings it is likely that LMX could affect cooperative communication among group members. Although the majority of these studies on the impact of LMX and communication behaviors take place in the US, a number of studies have also been conducted outside the US. The findings from these studies have mixed results on the impact of LMX quality and communication. For example in a cross-country comparisons study based on communication satisfaction and organizational commitment, Downs et al. (1995) compared results obtained in US, Australian, and Guatemalan organizations. The measurement instruments used for this cross-cultural study were: The Down’s Communication Satisfaction Questionnaire, and the Cook and Wall’s Organizational Commitment Instrument. The findings of this study suggest that satisfaction with supervisory communication is a much greater predictor of organizational commitment Cooperative Communication 272 than that of any other factor such as satisfaction with horizontal communication, communication climate and top management communication (Downs et al., 1995). These findings suggest that supervisory behavior is a one of the major factors in effecting various organizational outcomes, such as organizational commitment. Findings from another study between communication practices and organizational commitment in Guatemalan organizations were significant only in an education industry context. (Varona, 1996). Also, a cross-country study investigating supervisory style and supervisors’ upward influence with worker satisfaction in the US, Spain, and Mexico found that there was a relationship between perceived upward influence of the immediate supervisor and satisfaction of the employees. In this study, however, only Mexican supervisor influence had an impact on workers satisfaction regardless of the immediate supervisor’s particular style of management (Page & Wiseman, 1993). Although these findings based on Spanish speaking countries, but it provides support for the assumptions that there are cultural differences in superior-subordinate relationships. These inconclusive findings raise the important issue of the use of research instruments in crosscultural studies. As Varona (1996) argues, more cross culture studies are needed to develop a better understanding of the influence that cultural factors and organizational characteristics may have on the internal reliability and factor structure of research instruments created in a another culture. Malaysia is a multicultural country in which the major ethnic groups are Malays, Chinese, and Indians. They cooperate harmoniously in their everyday living. Each of these ethnic groups maintains its own ethnic identity, practicing their own cultures, customs, behavior, the language they speak, norms, values and beliefs (Abdul Rashid & Ho, 2003). Malaysia is fully engaged in the international economy, it retains a distinctive mix of Asian cultural values. Malaysia cultures are mainly influenced by the Malay social structure that overlaid by Islamic principles and leavened with British colonial management philosophies and Chinese and Indian religious and cultural values (Kennedy, 2002). Generally, all these ethnics groups in Malaysia are motivated by their affiliation to groups, families, and individuals. Studies indicate that Malay, Chinese, and Indian Malaysians do not differ significantly. For example there is no significant differences between Malaysian Malay and Chinese in work related values (Lim, 2001); in term of cultural differences between ethnics in Malaysia only religiosity construct were reported to be different (Asma & Lim, 2001); and all Malaysian respond better to productivity increases if they see benefits ensuing not only to the organization but also to their family, community and nation (Asma, 1992). In Malaysian organizational setting study it shows that there is significant relationships between participation in decision making and organizational commitment (Razali, 1996); two factors of quality work-life which is development and pay and benefit were significant in explaining organizational commitment (Razali, 2004); perceived information cues substantially contributed to job satisfaction; task content properties of identity, significance and autonomy as well as the interpersonal task attribute of dealing with others were significant contributors of organizational commitment (Pearson & Chong, 1997); and the ‘mercenary’ and network culture positively influence the attitudes toward change among manufacturing employees in Malaysia (Abdul Rashid, Sambasivan, & Johari, 2003). It also argued that interpersonal relationship and collective behavior plays an important role in a Malaysian country context than in a US country, 273 Hassan Abu Bakar, Che Su Mustaffa and Bahtiar Mohama thus we predict that LMX are more likely to influence cooperative communication behavior among group members (Kennedy, 2002). Lee’s (1997, 2001) studies among 279 students with work experiences and 270 respondents from various organization found LMX quality has a significant impact on perceived use of cooperative communication among co-workers. We are also aware that a few studies based on multiple comparison analysis, found that the quality of LMX has a direct impact on communication behavior (Lee, 1997, 2001; Lee & Jablin, 1995; Mueller & Lee, 2002; Yrle et al., 2002, 2003). Based on these findings, it is predicted that LMX quality may have a similar impact within the Malaysian country context. Thus, in this study based on Lee’s (1997) study we hypothesize that: Hypothesis: LMX quality in the Malaysian country context as perceived by subordinates will have a direct impact on subordinates’ perceptions of a cooperative communication. Method Participants in this study are employees of a media organization in Kuala Lumpur. The organization is allowed to sell its services to various organizations in Malaysia. The majority of this organization’s clients are from the local and international media. It involves in media activities such as providing in-depth reporting that is sold to other media and business organizations such as banking and information provider agencies. We choose this organization because English is used as a medium of interactions among its members. Survey packets were sent directly to 312 to executives and journalists. The sample is made up of 205 journalists (65.7%) mainly located in Editorial and Audio Visual Department and 107 (34.3%) executives who are drawn from various departments. The rationale for choosing this sample is that all respondents reporting to a specific supervisor perform essentially identical roles. These roles include covering and reporting events for journalists and performing various marketing and administration job for executives. A total of 200 respondents (64.4% rate of return) returned the survey packet. 108 (54%) respondents were journalists and 92 (46%) were executives. Approximately 52.5% (n = 105) were male and 47.5% (n = 95) were female. Approximately 7% (n = 14) of respondents worked for the organization for less than one year, 13% (n = 26) have worked for one to three years, 23.5% (n = 47) have worked between four to six years, 28% (n = 56) have worked between seven to ten years and 28.5% (n = 57) have worked for more than ten years. Measurement The measurement of LMX quality in this study relies primarily on scales from LMX theory (Liden & Graen, 1980). LMX-7 scale items are used as a measure of LMX quality. This scale is widely used in the communication literature (Fairhurst & Chandler, 1989). Each item in the scale is measured with a five-point Likert type scale. Pre-test results for this measurement show that Cronbach’s alphas are .87. Subjects were asked to indicate the extent to which they agreed with each item on a scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree. Overall the higher the score, the higher the quality of Cooperative Communication 274 LMX. The mean score of LMX in the present study was 3.38 (Median = 3.43, SD = .72). Following Lee (1997, 2001) we inspect of the frequency distribution to three equal proportions. The LMX quality as perceived by subordinates is grouped into three levels—in-group, middle-group, and out-group. 28.5% respondents are in out-group (n = 57), 34% are in middle-group (n = 68) and 37.5% in in-group (n = 75). Details of this comparison are shown in Table 1. In addition to this data were test for coding/data entry errors and tests for normality were conducted for each of the survey items as well as the constructs that are created by computing individual items. Tests for normality include kurtosis measures, skewness measures, and visual inspection of histograms. The majority of items appear to be within normality with kurtosis measures below one, skewness measures around zero, and normal-shaped histograms. Table 1: Mean and Standard Deviation Of Out-Group, Mid-Group and In-group Based on LMX-7 items in Lee (1997, 2001) Studies Group N (Lee 1997,2001) M (Lee 1997,2001) SD (Lee 1997,2001) Out-group 57 (97, 88) 2.86 (22.19, 3.88) .37 Mid-group 68 (87, 80) 3.14 (25.38, 4.98) .41 In-group 75 (95, 92) 3.50 (27.37, 5.56) .62 The instrument used to quantify cooperative communication in this study was sourced from Lee (1997). The participants were asked to indicate the degree to which they agreed with the statement that describe cooperative communication in their overall interactions with peers in the work group on a scale 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree. Pre-test results for this measurement show that Cronbach’s alphas are .78. The mean score of LMX in the present study was 22.4 (Median = 22.00, SD = 4.36). The correlation matrix on LMX and cooperative communication is presented in Table 2 along with alpha coefficients. All the measures in this study and in the context of this paper both of LMX constructs and cooperative communication demonstrate good internal consistency ranging from .87 to .78. Table 2: Means, Standard Deviations, Reliabilities and Inter-correlations among study variables Variables M SD 1 2 1. Leader-member exchange (LMX) 23.6 5.04 (.87) 2. Cooperative communication 22.4 4.36 .41* (.78) *p<.05 Results This research hypothesis is concerned with the impact of LMX quality on perceptions of cooperative communication among group members. The participants’ responses were analyzed by an ANOVA procedure. Analyses showed a significant effect of the quality of LMX on group cooperative communication behaviors, F (3, 197) = 26.79, p < .005 (see Table 3). Result based on raw score of LMX also shows a similar results, F93, 197) = 17.64, p < .005 (see Table 3). 275 Hassan Abu Bakar, Che Su Mustaffa and Bahtiar Mohama Lee’s (2001, 1997) studies based on various US organizations (e.g., financial, education, chemical, communications, high-tech, retail and entertainment) found that members of in-group LMX relationships perceived their cooperative communication differently than their middle-group counterparts who in turn, reported significantly Table 3: Analysis Of Variance Sum of Squares Between Group Within Groups 13.65 45.87 df Mean Square F 6.83 .255 26.79* 3 197 greater amounts of cooperative communication than their out-group peers. Tukey’s HSD multiple comparisons test is applied to gauge the level of similarity between Lee’s (1997, 2001) findings for the US context and the findings in this study in large a Malaysian organization. The Tukey’s HSD method enables researchers to keeps the Type1 error rate at .05 for the entire sets of comparison (Pagano, 2001). Result of Tukey’s HSD multiple comparison test at .05 (see Table 4) indicates similar findings to Lee’s (2001, 1997) findings, that there are significant differences in subordinates’ perceptions of cooperative communication between in-group and out-group t (200) = .644, p<. 05, mid-group and out-group t (200) = .266, p <. 05 and between mid-group and in-group t (200) = .378, p <. 05. Table 4: Mean Difference Between Group Members Using Tukey’s HSD Multiple Comparison Compared to Lee’s ( 1997, 2001) Studies Present Study Group Mean 1 Mean 2 Mean 3 Out-group (1) .266* .644* Mid-group (2) .378* In-group (3) *p < .05. Discusion As predicted, the quality of LMX affected cooperative communication among group members. The hypothesis is accepted as predicted and one-way ANOVA test indicates that the LMX quality has a direct, positive impact on subordinates’ perceptions of cooperative communication among group members. Lee (2001) describes this finding as subordinates in low-quality LMX perceived fewer exchanges or less sharing of information, ideas, and resources with their work group colleagues. On the other hand, subordinates in high-quality LMX perceived themselves sharing information, ideas and resources with their colleagues. In addition, this finding enhances and reconfirms our understanding that perceptions about quality of relationship between subordinate and their superior do affect frequency of information exchange, sharing ideas and resources, showing concern and interest in what others want to accomplish, being supportive and open to each other’s needs, and consulting and discussing issues to reach mutually satisfying agreements in the work group. Additionally, this finding also endorses Cooperative Communication 276 pervious findings that suggest in-group and out-group members would form strong relationships among themselves (Sherony & Green, 2002) The findings for this study have broadened our understanding of the impact of cross-country contexts for LMX. Similar to the US country context, our findings suggest that LMX quality in the Malaysian country context, as perceived by subordinates, will have a direct impact on subordinates’ perceptions of cooperative communication among group members. The higher the quality of LMX, the more subordinates report favorable cooperative communication behavior. These findings are consistent with the theoretical perspective and prior empirical findings of LMX to communication behaviors and activities in the superior-subordinate relationship (Graen & Uhl-Bien, 1995; Yrle et al., 2002, 2003). As with the results from the US country context, subordinates in high quality relationships enjoy frequent information exchange, sharing ideas and resources, showing concern and interest in what others want to accomplish, being supportive and open to each other’s needs, and consulting and discussing issues to reach mutually satisfying agreements In addition, similar to findings in the US country context, the respondents in this Malaysian study also experience frequent information exchange, sharing ideas and resources, showing concern and interest in what others want to accomplish, being supportive and open to each other’s needs, and consulting and discussing issues to reach mutually satisfying agreements. As a matter of fact, the quality of LMX appears to compare greatly to that in the US country context with respect to how individual subordinates feel about their cooperative communication experiences. Additionally, this finding raises some interesting possibilities for theoretical extension of LMX. LMX researchers has not made clear what balance of high and low quality LMX relationship is optimal for a leader (Graen & Uhl-Bien, 1995; Sparrowe & Liden, 1997). Although a supervisor might best use their limited time by fostering highquality relationships with only a handful of work group members, there is also an argument to be made for each member of a team being afforded equal time and similar opportunities to form high quality relationships. In fact as indicated in this study, LMX influences the relationship quality among group members, thus it would seem advisable for managers to develop high-quality LMX relationships with all subordinates. In doing so, leaders would foster positive work group cooperation such as better information exchange, sharing ideas and resources, showing concern and interest in what others want to accomplish, being supportive and open to each other’s needs, and consulting and discussing issues to reach mutually satisfying agreements which favorably affect other work attitudes such as organizational commitment or job satisfaction (Harris, Kacmar, & Witt, 2005) The finding in this study is also consistent with Kennedy (2002) argument that Malaysian people seem to put emphasis on interpersonal relationships and collectiveness. These interpersonal relationships are important determinants of interpersonal interactions for Malaysian people. Kennedy (2002) asserts that Malaysians are known to value interpersonal relationships. Therefore, the finding in this study affirms Kennedy (2002) notation that interpersonal relationships play an even more important role in a Malaysian country context than in a US country context. This is because this finding suggests that it is possible that in the Malaysian country context, there is a stronger relationship between LMX and cooperative communication behaviors than for the US country context (Kennedy, 2002). 277 Hassan Abu Bakar, Che Su Mustaffa and Bahtiar Mohama This result also provides external validity for Lee’s (2001, 1997) studies. In particular, the results of this study support the proposition that LMX exchange quality has a direct impact on cooperative communication in the Malaysian country context. In contrast to this finding, some authors argue against external validity across country. For example, Hui et al. (1999) argued that patterns of relationships identified in one country cannot be assumed to be invariant across country. The similar findings in both the US and Malaysia suggest that LMX quality and cooperative communication can transcend country boundaries. Future, research would benefit from similar studies using samples from various cultures and nationalities. Further, we also echoing Tsang and Kwan (1999) that good theory should have an ability to replicate as a valuable part of theory development that has been demonstrated in this study. Leader-member interaction is important to organizations. Unfortunately, such exchanges can also be a leading cause of employee distress. Recent research calls for leaders to develop higher quality relations with their members, which in turn can increase communication satisfaction. Scholars have also suggested additional research that focuses on interaction between leaders and members to better understand how leaders can and should manage such relationships. However, there is considerable evidence that leaders and members do not agree about the quality of their relationship. In this article, we address how leaders can build better relations with their members. Specifically, we explain how leaders might improve the quality of their relationship with members by focusing on an interpersonal communication strategy. First, we focus on a competing values framework to clearly establish the importance of relationship building in effective management Second, we define communication strategies for building better leadermember relationships by borrowing concepts from rapport management in sociolinguistics. Finally, we demonstrate how our model of leadership communication (based on a synthesis of competing values and rapport management theory) explains the quality of leader-member exchanges. Conclusions and Suggestions Findings reported in this study have important practical and theoretical implications that extend LMX to an Asian context. LMX theory suggests that superiors are largely responsible for the development of their superior-subordinate exchange relationships (Danserau, Graen, & Haga, 1975; Danserau & Markham, 1987; Graen & Scandura, 1987; Graen & Uhl-Bien, 1995). Accordingly, superiors strongly influence types of communication experiences that subordinates will have by developing and sustaining different LMX relationships and thus, are primarily responsible for subordinates’ affective responses to them. Therefore, to improve effective communication among their subordinates, a superior could offer opportunities to develop and maintain higher-quality LMX relationships with as many subordinates as possible (Mueller & Lee, 2002). For example a superior can increase feedback on jobs done by subordinates. A superior could also facilitate more upward communication and open communication with all subordinates regardless of their relationship quality or cultural background. Cross-culturally, subordinates too can learn about and actively engage in communication behaviors that positively affect the quality of the LMX relationship with Cooperative Communication 278 their superiors. They could provide greater competence and performance in tasks by asking for feedback on their performance from their superiors. They may also do things that may increase liking and trust and utilize impression management strategies (Mueller & Lee, 2002). Since the results of this study support the previous findings on LMX especially in the US country context, they also indicate that in the Malaysian country context, by improving the quality of LMX with superiors, subordinates are likely to experience more informal rewards—in this case quality communication behavior from their superior as an exchange for high perceived LMX quality from subordinates. Although our study supports LMX and communication research in the US country context, there are at least two limitations in the current study. First, communication literature relies heavily on the uni-dimensional LMX-7 scale—that focuses on latitude— as an indicator of LMX quality. Future research could explore the multidimensionality issues of LMX—for example communication in organizations and LMX dynamics. As suggested by Uhl-Bien, Grean & Scandura (2000) and Maslyn and Uhl-Bien (2001), researchers must begin to empirically test issues related to social exchange in leadermember relationship and by nature communication. This suggestion relates especially to dyadic communication that is a means of social exchange in the daily operation of organizations (Maslyn & Uhl-Bien, 2001; Uhl-Bien, Graen, & Scandura, 2000). Furthermore Dienesch and Liden (1986) describe leader-member relationship development as a series of steps that begins with initial interaction between the members of the dyad. Therefore exploring other LMX dimensions such as contribution, loyalty and affect can guide us to better understanding how LMX evolves in a Malaysian organization setting. Secondly, this study was conducted in an organization that has direct involvement with the Malaysian government. Future research could also consider conducting a study in a multinational corporation (MNC) or private organization in Malaysia. Such study can explore whether comparisons between government and private organizations will help us to better understand cross-cultural effect of LMX. In sum, the evidence of LMX theory and relationship communication patterns based on the Malaysian country context in this study improves our fundamental understanding of LMX effects in communication in differing country contexts. Results from this study further extend previous findings by empirically testing the cross-country external validity of existing LMX quality and communication behaviors scales. The result suggests that LMX quality and communication behaviors are similar in differing country contexts. Even though the US and Malaysia organization are different—the US has more emphasis on the individual while in Malaysia there is greater emphasis on collective culture—this result suggests that the quality of the relationship between superior and subordinate will have an impact on communication regardless of the country context and cultural background. The cross-country finding further emphasizes the importance of communication behavior to an effective work. This finding also presents some interesting similarities with findings of US studies. In terms of the LMX association and communication behavior, this finding supports the results of earlier studies (Lee, 1997, 2001; Lee & Jablin, 1995; Yrle et al., 2002, 2003). These earlier studies indicated that the quality of a relationship between superior and subordinate has an impact on a subordinate’s perceptions of their leader’s communication behavior. In a relation-oriented society like Malaysia, the supervisor 279 Hassan Abu Bakar, Che Su Mustaffa and Bahtiar Mohama may be a more important factor in influencing employee behavior and attitudes at work than organization as an impersonal entity. We hope that our study may contribute to universal understanding of LMX and superior-subordinate communication in two ways: 1. The LMX-7 scale employed in the current study may serve as a comprehensive and meaningful measure of leader-member exchange quality. 2. A supervisor as an agent of a system may be as important as an organization in influencing employees’ perceptions. In any organization, regardless of the context of institutionalism, a supervisor may still be psychologically and physically more proximal to employees than impersonal system. In turn, employees’ attitude towards a supervisor will have stronger impact on interpersonal communication than employee attitude towards overall communication system in organization. In an increasingly globalize economic and social environment, systematic research in different country contexts is needed to test the generality or limits of existing theories and models that were largely developed in countries like the US with Anglo-Saxon Western culture and tradition. Cooperative Communication 280 References Abdul Rashid, M. Z., & Ho, J. A. (2003). Perceptions of business ethics in a multicultural community: The case of Malaysia. Journal of Business Ethics, 43, 75-87. Abdul Rashid, M. Z., Sambasivan, M., & Johari, J. (2003). The influence of corporate culture and organizational commitment on performance. Journal of Management Development, 22, 708-728. Asma, A. (1992). The influence of ethnic values on managerial practices in Malaysia. Malaysian Management Review, 27, 3-18. Asma, A., & Lim, L. (2001). Cultural dimensions of Anglos, Australians and Malaysian. Malaysian Management Review, 36, 1-17. Dansereau, F., Graen, G., & Haga, W. J. (1975). A vertical dyad linkage approach to leadership within formal organizations: A longitudinal investigation of the role making process. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 13, 46-78. Danserau, F., & Markham, S. E. (1987). Superior-subordinate communication. In L. Porter (Ed.), Handbook of organizational communication (pp. 343-388). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications. Dienesch, R. M., & Liden, R. C. (1986). Leader-member exchange model of leadership: A critique and further development. Academy of Management Review, 11, 618634. Downs, C. W., Downs, A., Potvin, T., Varona, E., Gribas, J. S., & Ticehurst, W. (1995, May). A cross-cultural comparison of relationship between organizational commitment and organizational communication. Paper presented at the International Communication Association Convention, Albuquerque, NM. Duarte, N. T., Goodson, J. R., & Klich, N. R. (1994). Effects of dyadic quality and duration on performance appraisal. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 499521. Fairhurst, G. T. (1993). The leader-member exchange patterns of woman leaders in industry: A discourse analysis. Communication Monographs, 60, 321-351. Fairhurst, G. T., & Chandler, T. A. (1989). Social structure in leader-member interaction. Communication Monographs, 56, 215-239. Graen, G. B., & Scandura, T. A. (1987). Toward psychology of dyadic organizing. In S. B. M & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 9, pp. 175-208). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Graen, G. B., & Uhl-Bien, M. (1995). Relationship-based approach to leadership: Development of leader-member exchange (LMX) theory of leadership over 25 years: Applying a multi-level multi-domain perspective. Leadership Quarterly, 6, 219-247. Graen, G. B., & Wakabayashi, M. (1994). Cross-cultural leadership making: Bridging American and Japanese diversity for team advantage. In H. C. Triandis, M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 415-446). New York: Consulting Psychology Press. Graen, G. B., Wakabayashi, M., Graen, M. R., & Graen, M. G. (1990). International generalizability of American hypotheses about Japanese management progress: A strong inference investigation. Leadership Quarterly, 1, 1-23. 281 Hassan Abu Bakar, Che Su Mustaffa and Bahtiar Mohama Harris, K. J., Kacmar, K. M., & Witt, L. A. (2005). An examination of the curvilinear relationship between leader-member exchange and intent to turnover. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 363-378. Hui, C., & Graen, G. (1997). Guaxi and professional leadership in contemporary SinoAmerican joint ventures in Mainland China. Leadership Quarterly, 8, 451-465. Hui, C., Law, K. S., & Chen, Z. X. (1999). A structural equation model of the effects of negative affectivity, leader-member exchange, and perceived job mobility on inrole and extra role performance: A Chinese case. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 77, 3-21. Jablin, F. M. (1987). Organizational entry, assimilation, and exit. In F. M. Jablin, L. L. Putnam, K. H. Roberts & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Handbook of organizational communication: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 679-740). Newbury Park: Sage Publication. Kennedy, J. C. (2002). Leadership in Malaysia: Traditional values, international outlook. Academy of Management Executive, 16, 15-26. Krone, K. J. (1992). A comparison of organizational, structural, and relationship effects on subordinates' upward influence choices. Communication Quarterly, 40, 1-15. Lee, J. (1997). Leader-member exchange, the 'Pelz effects' and cooperative communication between group members. Management Communication Quarterly, 11, 266-287. Lee, J. (2001). Leader-member exchange, perceived organizational justice, and cooperative communication. Management Communication Quarterly, 14, 574589. Lee, J., & Jablin, F. M. (1995). Maintenance communication in superior-subordinate work relationship. Human Communication Research, 22, 220-258. Lepine, J. A., & Dyne, L. V. (2001). Voice and cooperative behavior as contrasting forms of contextual performance: Evidence of differential relationships with big five personality characteristics and cognitive ability. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 326-336. LePine, J. A., & Van Dyne, L. (1998). Predicting voice behavior in work groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 853-868. Liden, R. C., & Graen, G. B. (1980). Generalizability of the vertical dyad linkage model of leadership. Academy of Management Journal, 23, 451-465. Liden, R. C., & Maslyn, J. M. (1998). Multidimensionality of leader-member exchange: An empirical assessment through scale development. Journal of Management, 24, 43-73. Lim, L. (2001). Work-related values of Malays and Chinese Malaysians. International Journal of Cross Cultural Management, 1, 209-226. Maslyn, J. M., & Uhl-Bien, M. (2001). Leader-member exchange and its dimensions: Effects of self-effort and other's effort on relationship quality. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 697-708. Mueller, B. H., & Lee, J. (2002). Leader-member exchange and organizational communication satisfaction in multiple context. Journal of Business Communication, 39, 220-244. Cooperative Communication 282 Olufowote, J. O., Miller, V. D., & Wilson, S. R. (2005). The interactive effects of role change goals and relational exchanges on employee upward influence tactics. Management Communication Quarterly, 18, 385-403. Pagano, R. R. (2001). Understanding statistics in the behavioral sciences (6th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning. Page, N. R., & Wiseman, R. L. (1993). Supervisory behavior and worker satisfaction in the United State, Mexico and Spain. Journal of Business Communication, 30, 161-174. Pearson, C. A. L., & Chong, J. (1997). Contribution of job content and social information on organizational commitment and job satisfaction: An exploration in a Malaysian nursing context, Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology (Vol. 70, pp. 357-374). Pilemer, K., Suitor, J. J., Henderson, C. R., Meador, R., Schultz, L., Robinson, J., et al. (2003). A cooperative communication intervention for nursing home staff and family members of residents. The Gerontologist, 43, 96-106. Razali, M. Z. (1996). Moderating effects on the relationship between participation in decision-making and organizational commitment: A Malaysian case. Singapore Management Review, 18, 65-82. Razali, M. Z. (2004). Perception of professional engineers toward quality of work life and organizational commitment. Gadjah Mada International Journal of Business, 6, 323-334. Scandura, T. A., & Graen, G. B. (1984). Moderating effects of initial leader-member exchange status on the effects of leadership intervention. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 428-436. Scandura, T. A., Graen, G. B., & Novak, M. A. (1986). When managers decide not to decide autocratically: An investigation of leader-member exchange and decision influence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 579-584. Settoon, R. P., Bennett, N., & Liden, R. C. (1996). Social Exchange in Organizations: Perceived Organizational Support, Leader–Member Exchange, and Employee Reciprocity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 219-227. Sherony, K. M., & Green, S. G. (2002). Coworker exchange: Relationships between coworkers, leader-member exchange, and work attitudes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 542-548. Sias, P. M. (1996). Constructing perceptions of differential treatment: An analysis of coworker discourse. Communication Monographs, 63, 171-187. Sias, P. M., & Jablin, F. M. (1995). Differential superior-subordinate relations, perceptions of fairness, and co-worker communication. Human Communication Research, 22, 5-38. Sparrowe, R. T., & Liden, R. C. (1997). Process and structure in leader-member exchange. Academy of Management Review, 22, 522-552. Tjosvold, D., Andrews, I. R., & Jones, H. (1983). Cooperative and competitive relationships between leaders and subordinates. Human Relations (Vol. 36, pp. 1111-1124). Tsang, E. W. K., & Kwan, K.-M. (1999). Replication and theory development in organizational science: A critical realist perspective. Academy of Management Review, 24, 759-780. 283 Hassan Abu Bakar, Che Su Mustaffa and Bahtiar Mohama Uhl-Bien, M., Graen, G. B., & Scandura, T. (2000). Implications of leader-member exchange (LMX) for strategic human resource management systems: Relationship as social capital for competitive advantage. In G. R. Ferris (Ed.), Research in personnel and human resource management (Vol. 18, pp. 137-185). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Varona, F. (1996). Relationship between communication satisfaction and organizational commitment in three Guatemalan organizations. Journal of Business Communication, 33, 111-129. Wakabayashi, M., & Graen, G. B. (1984). The Japanese career progress study: A 7-year follow up. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 603-614. Waldron, V. R. (1991). Achieving communication goals in superior-subordinate relationships: The multi-functionality of upward maintenance tactics. Communication Monographs, 58, 289-306. Wayne, S. J., Shore, L. M., & Liden, R. C. (1997). Perceived organizational support and leader-member exchange: A social exchange perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 40, 82-111. Yrle, A. C., Hartman, S. J., & Galle, W. P. (2002). An investigation of relationship between communication style and leader-member exchange. Journal of Communication Management, 6, 257-269. Yrle, A. C., Hartman, S. J., & Galle, W. P. (2003). Examining communication style and leader-member exchange: Considerations and concerns for managers. International Journal of Management, 20, 92-97. Yukl, G., & Fu, P. P. (1999). Determinants of delegation and consultation by managers. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20, 219-232. Human Communication. A Publication of the Pacific and Asian Communication Association. Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 285 – 298. Proxemics and its Relationship with Malay Architecture Abdul Muati Amad Universiti Putra Malaysia Arbai’e Sujud Universiti Putra Malaysia Hamisah Zaharah Hasan Universiti Putra Malaysia Abdul Muati Amad, Ph.D ,Arbai’e Sujud, Ph.D, and Hamisah Zaharah Hasan are faculty of Modern Languages and Communication in the Communication Department at Universiti Putra Malaysia. Direct all correspondence to: abmuati@putra.upm.edu.my Hasan Proxemics and its Relationship with Malay Architecture 286 Abstract Proxemics refers to the use of physical space in interactions between communicators. Obviously, it is fascinating to see how people use the physical space surrounding them when they communicate or interact in their house. This is especially true since most communication interactions occur in a place where they inhabit. The same goes to the Malays, the predominant inhabitant of Malaysia. Traditional Malay houses were built with distinctive characteristics. The division of the compartments in the house, the arrangement of the furniture and decorations provide huge space for the inhabitants to move. Each space determines the content of communication and who are the participants. For instance what kind of interactions can take place at the porch, the verandah, the living room, the kitchen and the passageway? This paper attempts to discuss the relationship between the space in a Malay house and their proxemics. 287 Abdul Muati Amad, Arbai’e Sujud, and Hamisah Zaharah Introduction Who are the Malays? The Malays are ethnic group who inhabit the Malay Archipelago. This region covers the Peninsula of Malaysia, part of Indonesia, South Thailand, Brunei Darussalam, and South of the Philippine. Some sources recorded that the ancestors of the Malays were originally from the lowlands of Cambodia and the Mekong River Delta of South Vietnam, migrated down south due to shortage of cultivation land and natural resources, population overcrowding and opportunity to settle in new lands. The Malays formed the largest population group of multi-racial Malaysia. Out of the total of 23 million people, the Malays account for about 60 percents, the Chinese 30 percents and the Indians 10 percents. Malaysia’s constitution penned after gaining its independent from the Great Britain in 1957 decrees that all Malays are born Muslim. Thus the Malays are devout Muslims. Even before the formation of Malaysia, Islam has influenced the life of the Malays for a long time. In fact, the history of Islam in the Malay Archipelago goes back to the fourteen century. With such a long influence of the religion in their lives, the traces of Islamic teachings and decrees can be found obviously in their daily routines and activities. This includes their names, foods, language, attire, arts, songs, dances and architecture especially their mosques and houses. The traditional Malay houses Traditional Malay architecture employs relatively sophisticated architectural processes ideally suited to tropical conditions such as wide roof overhangs and highpitched roofs. Building on stilts allows cross-ventilating breezes beneath the dwelling to cool the mosque or house whilst mitigating the effects of the occasional flood. Additionally this will also ensure safety from possible attack by wild animals such as snakes and insects in the vicinity of the village. Figure 1: The Traditional Malay house in a village. (Source: Lim Jee Yuan, The traditional Malay house. http://tcdc.undp.org/sie/experiences/vol4/Malay house.pdf ) Hasan Proxemics and its Relationship with Malay Architecture 288 Traditional Malay houses are mainly constructed from natural materials such as various kinds of timber and bamboo and thatched palm-leaves. The Malays often constructed their dwellings without any use of metal including nails. Instead they used pre-cut holes and grooves to fit the timber building elements into one another. A traditional Malay timber house is almost always in at least two parts: the main house and the kitchen annex. There is also at least one raised veranda attached to the house for seated working or relaxation or where non-intimate visitors would be entertained, thus preserving the privacy of the interior. For ventilation purposes, the elevation of the house on the stilts and also the wide windows, holed carvings and slatted panels around the walls plus the high thatch or clay tile roofs all contribute to the cooling ambience. Hitherto, these traditional Malay houses can still be seen forming typical scenery in the rural areas of Malaysia. However, some of these houses in the sub-urban areas have been renovated to suit modern living. Normally, additional annexation has been added to the main structure to get larger built-up area. Thus, more rooms could be added to accommodate bigger and more extended families. Proxemics The term proxemics was coined by Edward T. Hall in 1963 (Sheppard, 1996). Proxemics basically refers to the use of space around us. Ickinger noted that Hall has defined proxemics in various ways over the years. For instance, in 1963 Hall regarded proxemics as “… the study of how man unconsciously structures microspace – the distance between men in the conduct of daily transactions, the organization of space in his houses and buildings, and ultimately the layout of his towns.” Later in 1964, Hall redefined proxemics as “… the study of the ways in which man gains knowledge of the content of other men’s minds through judgements of behavior patterns associated with varying degrees of spatial proximity to them”. Two years later in 1966 he defined proxemics as “… the interrelated observations and theories of man’s use of space as a specialized elaboration of culture.” Finally in 1974, Hall redefined proxemics as “… the study of man’s transactions as he perceives and uses intimate, personal, social and public space in various settings while following out of awareness dictates of cultural paradigms”. In short from Hall’s definitions, we can conclude that proxemics bring together the concept of use of space around us and the cultural influence of such usage. Hence different culture will have different manifestations of proxemics. Nevertheless, there are two types of proxemics, i.e. physical territory and personal territory. Physical territory refers to any area controlled and defended by an individual or group of individuals with emphasis on physical possession (Leather, 1978 and Vargas, 1986). Public territories are places that anybody can enter such as restaurants and libraries. Home territories are premises that can be entered only by members such as family members and affiliates of a club or organization. Physical territory also includes how a boundary is set around a space and the way furniture is arranged in the space. On the other hand, personal territory refers to “… an area with invisible boundaries surrounding a person’s body into which intruders may not come” (Sommer, 1979:26). Gershaw (1986) quoted Hall’s four divisions of personal territories. The four 289 Abdul Muati Amad, Arbai’e Sujud, and Hamisah Zaharah areas are public space, social space, personal space and intimate space. Public space ranges from 12 to 25 feet. This space is suitable for formal speeches, lectures and business meetings. Social space refers to distance from 4 to 12 feet, and is used for communication amongst business associates. This space is also meant for social gatherings and separating strangers in public areas such as beaches and bus stops (Sheppard, 1996). Personal space ranges between 1.5 to 4 feet. We normally use this space to talk to friends and family members. Queuing up at banks and post-office counters are normally done in this distance. Intimate space however refers to distance within 1.5 feet. This close range involves a high probability for touching. Thus, the distance is reserved for telling secrets, whispering, embracing, comforting others and lovemaking (Gershaw, 1986 and Sheppard, 1996). Note that physical territory come with visible boundaries whereas personal territory come with the invisible one. Proxemics and the Malay Architecture Researches found that different cultures will have different use of space. One of the most frequent used of space is definitely in a person’s house. Matela stated that some cultures believe that space inside the house is important. A house is not only a private place for the house owner and his family but also more often than not they receive guests. Thus there is the consideration of social zones within a personal environment in a house. Therefore in various cultures, the houses will have various designs to produce such zones. Hence there are rooms for public gatherings, rooms for close friend and relatives, and private rooms that are even preserved for certain family members only. The same Figure 2: A layout of a typical traditional Malay house. (Source: Lim Jee Yuan, The traditional Malay house. http://tcdc.undp.org/sie/experiences/vol4/Malay house.pdf ) Hasan Proxemics and its Relationship with Malay Architecture 290 goes to the Malays. In constructing their houses, the Malays took into considerations the various compartments and their functions. A sketch below shows rooms or sections in a typical traditional Malay house and its functions. From the layout we can see that a Malay house can be divided into two main parts, namely the main house and the kitchen annex. The main house is the most important building block. It consists of the main room, the verandah and the landing. Activities wise, the main room is used for congregation. Since the Malays are devout Muslim, they normally pray together in the house. Muslims must offer prayer five times a day. Besides praying, the main room is also used for meeting and discussions with family members or relatives, feasts, and adult sleeping. The Malays normally have several feasts a year like ushering new Muslim year, paddy harvesting, wedding, circumcision etc. All these important occasion are held in the main room. However, the verandah, normally attached to the main house is used for entertaining guests and child (boy) sleeping. Landing, the outer most section of a house is used for relaxing and also entertaining strangers. Since the Malays like to sit on the floor and pray in their house, the floor of their houses must be kept exceptionally clean. Therefore, they must take off their shoes before getting into the landing area. Shoes are normally left at the staircase. The main house is mainly for men. Figure 3: A traditional Malay house showing a main house, a kitchen annex and a washing place. The kitchen annex is a place strictly for women. Activities taken place in this section are like preparing food, cooking and family dining. Guests will be served at the main house. Kitchen is a restricted area for men. Even the house owner cannot enter the kitchen when there are women guests in the kitchen. When a couple (husband and wife) come for a visit, the husband will enter the house from the main entrance at the landing and be entertained at the verandah by the husband or the eldest son of the house owner. 291 Abdul Muati Amad, Arbai’e Sujud, and Hamisah Zaharah The wife however will enter the house through the kitchen and be entertained by the wife of the house owner or the eldest daughter in the family. Figure 4: A floor plan of a typical Malay house. The main house is connected to the kitchen by a passage way. This passageway is used by the ladies for chit-chatting or for some casual discussions between husband and wife. More often than not this small section of the house is also a place for kids to play indoor games. The Malay houses in different areas in Malaysia will have slightly different design. Nevertheless, the functions of every section remain the same. These differences even though are not so significant have formed some sort of uniqueness and created special identity to the regional architecture. The houses from the northern region for instance have elevated floors and partitioned hallways to mark the boundaries of certain sections. Nonetheless, the hallways partitions are opened without any doors, except for the private rooms. Figure 5: A floor plan of a Malay house from the northern region. Hasan Proxemics and its Relationship with Malay Architecture 292 Figure 6: A typical Malay house from the northern region. Figure 7: A dining place in a Malay house from the northern region. Note the hallways without doors leading to another section of the house. However, Malay houses in the eastern region of Peninsular Malaysia were built with more apparent elevated floors and partitioned with wall and doors. It is noted that the people from this part of the country are considered more close minded, conservative and tend to be more closed communities. Thus they prefer to have more privacy in their Figure 8: A floor plan of a Malay house from the eastern region. 293 Abdul Muati Amad, Arbai’e Sujud, and Hamisah Zaharah own homes. Nevertheless the houses in this region have a kitchen annex combined to the main house and seemed like formed of only one block. Figure 9: A verandah of a Malay house from the eastern region. Note the elevated floor and wall with doors to separate sections. On the other hand, the Malay house from the central region shares more or less the same layout with the houses in the north. The different sections in the house were separated only by elevated floors and in some cases partitioned hallways. Similar to those from the northern region, the partitioned hallways are left open without doors except for the private rooms such as the bedrooms. However houses in this part of the country have the kitchen annex attached to the main house. Thus the main house and the kitchen annex looked like one unit. Figure 10: A floor plan of a Malay house from the central region. Hasan Proxemics and its Relationship with Malay Architecture 294 Figure 11: A hallway leading to a free area from a verandah in a Malay house from the central region. This is an example of a hallway without door or partition separating sections in the house. Nonetheless, the Malay houses in the south show some notable differences from those in the north and central peninsular. In fact the houses in the south are more similar to those in the eastern peninsular when come to separation of sections. The houses in the south use walled partitions with doors to separate sections in the main house. However elevated floors are used to separate sections in the kitchen. The kitchen annex is also attached to the main house, thus made the two sections integrated. Figure 12: A floor plan of a Malay house from the southern region. 295 Abdul Muati Amad, Arbai’e Sujud, and Hamisah Zaharah Figure 13: A Malay house from the southern region. Note the grand stairway to the house. Most of the houses in the south are equipped with an attic. The attics were used to store foods and other household necessities. At times girls were asked to ‘hide’ in the attic when the family was having feasts. This is because in the past girls were supposed to be less social and should not be seen mingling around with the guests especially strangers and outsiders. Figure 14: A staircase leading to the attic. Another distinct feature of a Malay house from the south is the concrete main stairway. The stairways are normally constructed with bricks and cements and decorated with clay tiles. In the past, some rich house owners even imported the tiles from China. Thus the main stairway served as a symbol status. The richer the house owner, the bigger and more elaborate the stairway of his residence. At any rate, when a person visits a Malay house, he must realize his status. If he is a stranger to the house owner, he can only be at the landing section. If he is a friend to the house owner, he can advance up to the verandah. If he has some family ties with the house owner, he can enter the main room. At any rate, the kitchen remains as a restricted area unless he is a family member to the house owner or join the family as a son in-law. Hasan Proxemics and its Relationship with Malay Architecture 296 A stranger visiting a Malay house will be entertained in the landing area. If he is alone, he will be sitting cross legged on the floor facing the house owner. If the strangers come in a group of three or more, they will be sitting in a circle. They can even sit next to the house owner, close enough until their knees touches each others’ knees. For the Westerners especially Americans this probably has violated their intimate space. But for the Malays sitting cross legged this close even with strangers are acceptable. This shows the Malay’s hospitality as Islam urges Muslims to respect and be kind to their visitors and to treat their guests openhandedly. But still the boundary is the landing area. Figure 15: The Malays sit cross legs on the floor in their house. Conclusion Observably people from different culture communicate differently. The Malays are not spared from this uniqueness. The Malays has shaped their culture since the past thousand years, incorporating several influences such as the Hindus during the Sri Vijaya and Majapahit Era, the Muslims from the Arab world and India, the Chinese from mainland China, and finally the Europeans from the West. These cultural influences can be obviously evidenced in their verbal and non-verbal communication, including their proxemics. Since most of their communication interactions occur in their living place, the Malays have their own way of setting up rules and norms patented by the architectural designs of their houses. Understanding the designs will lead us to appreciating the way the Malays communicate. 297 Abdul Muati Amad, Arbai’e Sujud, and Hamisah Zaharah References A. Najib Ariffin (2005). "A Disappearing Heritage: The Malaysian Kampung House". In Heritage Asia (September 2005, pp 6-8). Kuala Lumpur: Mediahub. Bremer, Jill. Proxemics – How We use Space. Available at: http://www.bremercommunications.com/Proxemics_How_We_Use_Space.htm (Access date: 10 September 2006) Gershaw, David A. Proxemics – Too Close for Comfort. Available at: http://members.cox.net/dagershow/lol/Proxemics.html (Access date: 10 September 2006) Hall,E.T. (1959). The Silent Language. Garden City, NY: Doubleday. Hall, E.T. (1976). Beyond Culture. Garden City, NY: Doubleday. Ickinger, W.J. Proxemics Research. Available at: http://sharktown.com/proxemics/intro.html (Access date: 10 September 2006) Leather, D. (1978). Nonverbal Communication Systems. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, Inc. Lee Ho Yin (2003). "The Kampong House: An Evolutionary History of Peninsular Malaysia's Vernacular Houseform". In Ronald G. Knapp (Ed.), Asia's Old Dwellings: Tradition, Resilience, and Change. New York: Oxford University Press. Lim Jee Yuan, The traditional Malay house. Available at: (Access date: 7 http://tcdc.undp.org/sie/experiences/vol4/Malay house.pdf September 2006) Matela, AJ. COMA 104 Proxemics Written Report. Available at: http://www.angelfire.com/tv/ajmatela/coma104a.html (Access date: 10 September 2006) Nasir, Abdul Halim and Wan Teh, Wan Hashim (2004). The Traditional Malay House. Shah Alam: Penerbit Fajar Bakti. Sheppard, M. Proxemics. Available at: http://www.cs.unm.edu/~sheppard/proxemics.htm (Access date: 12 September 2006) Sommer, R. (1979). Personal Space. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall. The body language of Proxemics. Available at: http://members.aol.com/katydidit/bodylang.htm (Access date: 12 September 2006) Vargas, M. (1986). Louder Than Words. Ames, IA: Iowa State University Press. Human Communication. A Publication of the Pacific and Asian Communication Association. Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 299 – 312. Psychologically Abusive Relationships and Self-Disclosure Orientations Jacqueline A. Shirley Texas Christian University William G. Powers Texas Christian University Chris R. Sawyer Texas Christian University Jacqueline A. Shirley (M.S., Texas Christian University, 2004) is an adjunct Instructor in the Department of Communication Studies at Texas Christian University, Fort Worth, TX 76129. William G. Powers (Ph.D., University of Oklahoma, 1973) is a Professor, and Chris R. Sawyer (Ph.D., University of North Texas, 1992)is an Associate Professor in the same department. Direct all correspondence to: w.powers@tcu.edu. Abusive Relationships 300 Abstract This study examines gender differences with respect to the relationships between perceived psychological abuse experienced in dating relationships and several dimensions of self-disclosure. One hundred sixty-six (62 male, 104 female) undergraduate students completed the Revised Self Disclosure Scale (RSDS) and the Psychological Abuse Scale (PAS) referring to their most recent former unpleasant dating partner. Openness, control of depth, positiveness, and honesty/accuracy dimensions of self-disclosure were negatively related to the level of psychological abuse experienced during dating. Moreover, the correlation between the level of psychological abuse and the amount of self disclosure differed for male and female respondents. Implications for future research in the area of psychological abuse and communication are presented. 301 Jacqueline A. Shirley, William G. Powers, and Chris R. Sawyer Introduction Relational abuse frequently involves inherently communicative acts, such as verbal and physical expressions of dominance, that result in feelings of guilt, fear and powerlessness among the victims of abusers (Tolman, 1992). Likewise, social humiliation, hostile interaction, and issuing orders are other communication actions considered to represent relationally abusive activity (Hoffman, 1984; Kasper, 1982; Walker, 1979). Described as the “most powerful coercive technique experienced in a battering relationship” (Walker, 1979, p.172; see also Okun, 1986; Tolman & Bhosley, 1991; Walker, 1984), verbal battering and threats of humiliation or violence can only occur through acts of communication (O’Leary, 1999). Thus, it is clear that the psychological dimension of abusive relationships is communicative in its very nature. Although many researchers agree that effective communication is essential for healthy intimate relationships (Babcock, Waltz, Jacobson, & Gottman, 1993, Noller 1981; Sabourin, 1991), the lack thereof may permit abusiveness in dating relationships. Consequently, understanding the association between communicative orientations and perceived relational abuse would have extensive heuristic significance relative to the initiation and evolution of abuse in interpersonal relationships. The specific purpose of this study is to examine gender differences relative to the relationship between perceptions of psychological abuse in relationships and self-disclosure orientations. Theoretical Perspective Psychological Abuse Recently, a substantial number of studies have examined the communication implications of psychological abuse (Marcus and Swett, 2002; Harned, 2002; Follingstad and Dehart, 2000; Katz, Arias and Beach, 2000; O’Leary, 1999). While numerous definitions have been offered for this construct, psychological abuse is often described as a form of mind control that employs threats to the physical and social well being of the victim (Hoffman, 1984). For example, Follingstad and Dehart (2000) report that the psychological abuse displayed by males to their female spouses tend to cluster into five categories: threats to physical health, control over physical freedoms, general destabilization (intimidation, degradation, isolation/restriction/monopolizing, destabilizing perceptions), dominating-controlling (jealousy/suspicion, isolation/restriction/monopolization, control of personal behavior, emotional withholding/blackmail, verbal abuse, treatment as inferior), and ineptitude (rigid gender roles, role failure). Clearly, while there are numerous manifestations of psychological abuse most reflect “direct infliction of mental harm” or “limits to the victim’s well-being” (Gondolf, 1987, p. 97). Specifically, Loring (1994) indicates that psychological abuse occurs when the essential ideas, feelings, perceptions, and personality characteristics of the victim are constantly belittled. Psychological abuse research has focused on a range of relationship levels: high school/ courtship relationships (Foshee, 1996; Bergman, 1992), college-level dating partners (Pipes and LeBov-Keeler, 1997; Kasian and Painter, 1992; White and Koss, 1991), pre-marital couples (McLaughlin, Lenord, Senchak, 1992; Makepeace, 1989), and married couples (O’Leary and Jouriles, 1994; Marshall, 1994). White and Koss (1991) report 80% to 90% of women experience some type of psychological abuse. Harned (2002) further reports that 82% of women and 87% of men reported experiencing psychological aggression from a dating partner. For 18- Abusive Relationships 302 24 year olds, 20% of the dating population reported the presence of violent behaviors (Stets and Straus, 1990). Further, prevalence of abuse is noted more in relationships where couples are more committed and have dated longer (Sugarman and Hotaling, 1989; Sharp and Taylor, 1999). Psychologically abusive relationships are said to have a direct negative impact on women’s emotional health (e.g., Molidor, 1995; Raymond and Bruschi, 1989). Women in abusive relationships often report that the psychological impact of belittling and threatening verbal behaviors have a continuing negative impact on their emotional well-being (Greenfield et al., 1998; McFarlane, Wilson, Malecha, & Lemmey, 2000; Follingstad, Rutledge, Berg, Hause and Polek, 1990; Foster, Veale, and Fogel, 1989). In addition, many victims report that the level of psychological abuse they experienced in these relationships was more damaging than the physical abuse they endured. Tolman and Bhosley (1991) found that psychological abuse was a powerful predictor of women’s psychosocial problems. Reports from battered women suggest that the harmful effects of psychological abuse on self-esteem and recovery are more prolonged and emotionally devastating (Dobash and Dobash, 1981; Okun, 1986; Walker, 1984). Straus, Sweet and Vissing (1989) report the more verbal aggression a woman experiences from her spouse, the greater the probability that she will be depressed. In essence, victims of psychological abuse have lower self-esteem and negative self-views (Katz, Street and Arias, 1997). Self-Disclosure Self-disclosure occurs when one person reveals thoughts, feelings and ideas to another (Rosenfeld, 1979; Cozby, 1973; Wheeless & Grotz, 1976). The tendency to engage in these behaviors is predicated on the notion that the target of disclosure is a person of good will (Jourard, 1971), as typically found in successful dating relationships. Self-disclosure tends to build trust and increases the likelihood of intimacy in relationships (Rosenfeld, 1979). Trust in one’s dating partner is clearly antecedent to a willingness to disclose personal information (e.g. Jourard, 1971; Wheeless and Grotz, 1977; and Wheeless, 1978). Intimacy involves revealing positive or negative feelings in relationships (Prager, 1995). Intimate relationships are based on high degrees of depth and intent of self-disclosure. Self-disclosure is associated with many benefits that are essential to the development and maintenance of good interpersonal relationships (Rosenfeld, 1979). Self-disclosure has been associated with need fulfillment and maintaining harmony in close relationships (Prager, 1995). Additionally, self-disclosure promotes mental health by decreasing self-alienation (Gergen, 1971). Other benefits of self-disclosure include increased attraction, liking, loving (Egan, 1970). Self-disclosure also involves risks and may result in avoidance. Perceived harmful consequences form a basis for avoiding self-disclosure. Rosenfeld (1979) found that women avoid self-disclosure when they wish to avoid personal hurt and problems with the relationship; and men may avoid self-disclosure to maintain control that would be hampered by selfdisclosure. Baxter and Montgomery (1996) identified four risks associated with self-disclosure: rejection, reduction of personal autonomy and integrity, loss of control or self-efficacy and hurting or embarrassing the listener. Thus, it would be reasonable to assert that generally self-disclosure indating relationships is precipitated by positive perceptions of one’s dating partners and expectations ofpositive outcomes from self disclosure. Moreover, this can be further delineated by the five factors associated with the self-disclosure concept by Wheeless and Grotz (1977): intention (or 303 Jacqueline A. Shirley, William G. Powers, and Chris R. Sawyer openness), amount, control of depth, positive/negative, and honesty/accuracy. The authors go on to conclude that higher individualized trust in targets of disclosure (as opposed to lesser trust) is related to more consciously intended disclosure and greater amounts of disclosure. Wheeless (1978) found that the amount, depth, and honesty factors were positively related to the perceived trustworthiness of the partner. Varying degrees of disclosure are clearly related to varying degrees of perceptions of trustworthiness. Reasoning that as psychological abuse escalates, the self-disclosure pattern will be appropriately adjusted as a function of decreased trust, the following hypotheses covering the five factors of self-disclosure were generated: H1: Perceived psychological abuse and openness of self-disclosure will be negatively correlated. H2: Perceived psychological abuse and amount of selfdisclosure will be negatively correlated. H3: Perceived psychological abuse and depth of selfdisclosure will be negatively correlated. H4: Perceived psychological abuse and positiveness of self-disclosure will be negatively correlated. H5: Perceived psychological abuse and honesty/accuracy of self-disclosure will be negatively correlated. Sex Differences Historically, psychological abuse literature has focused on women’s’ experiences of abuse. Foshee (1996) indicates that females report more victimization than males. No gender differences in the amount of victimization from threatening behaviors were found. For males and females, the most common type of psychological abuse received was emotional manipulation followed by monitoring (Foshee, 1996). Additionally, females reported more perpetration than males of emotional manipulation. Reports of psychological abuse may be minimized by women as a mechanism to reduce stress and anxiety or in fear of retaliation from her partner (Tolman, 1992). Also, non-traditional women perceived more abuse than traditional women (DeGregoria, 1987). Men also claim to be victims of abuse, when confronted with their abusive behavior towards their partner (Foshee, 1996). Tolman (1989); Straus et al. (1989) report that men are more likely than women to minimize reports of psychological maltreatment at intake, as compared with their partner’s reports. Rubin, Hill, Peplau, and Dunkel-Schetter (1980) generally support the notion that women self-disclose more than men. Additionally, men were found to reveal their strengths while women are more likely to reveal their fears. Clearly, women are more frequently negatively impacted by experienced psychological abuse than men. On the basis of the preceding literature review reflecting the potential impact of sex differences in the context of psychological abuse, the following generalized research question was generated: RQ: To what extent do males and females differ with respect to the relationship between perceived psychological abuse and self-disclosure factors? Abusive Relationships 304 Method Procedures As part of a larger study, participants were asked to write down the initials of their most recent former, unpleasant dating partner. Responses to questions about their self-disclosure patterns and the perceived psychological abusive behaviors displayed by their former dating partner were then answered relative to that specific relationship. Participation was voluntary and took place during regular class time. Credit toward a departmental research requirement in the aforementioned course was awarded for participation in this study. Participants Participants (N=166) were recruited from an undergraduate communication studies class at a southwestern university. There were 104 females and 62 males volunteered for the study. The mean age for these survey respondents was 19.2 years. The average time since terminating the negative relationship described above was 12.9 months. The distribution of study participants by class was; Freshman = 95, Sophomore = 50, Junior = 15 and Senior = 6. Measures Psychological Abuse Scale (PAS). The PAS is a 15-item instrument constructed by Pipes and LeBov-Keeler (1997) reflecting one partner’s perception of the amount of psychologically abusive activity by their partner. This scale contains items from Straus’ (1979) Conflict Tactic Scales (CTS) and those published by Hoffman (1984)and Howard, Blumstein, and Schwartz (1986). The PAS allows for the participants to endorse, on a seven-point Likerttype scale (0 = never; 1 = once; 2 = twice; 3 = three to five times; 4 = six to ten times; 5 = eleven to twenty times; more than twenty times) how many times their recent, unpleasant former dating partner behaved in psychologically abusive manners. Pipes and LeBov-Keeler (1997) reported acceptable reliability for the PAS ( = .89). In the current study, internal consistency was also acceptable ( = .88). Revised Self-Disclosure Scale (RSDS). The RSDS measured each respondent’s report of their self-disclosure to a targeted former dating partner during/following psychological abusive activity. RSDS is a five-factor self-disclosure instrument with a 7-step Likert-type response pattern where 1 = Strongly Disagree and 7 = Strongly Agree (Wheeless, 1978). In the Wheeless (1978) study, internal consistency for the five dimensions ranged from .84 to .91. In the present study, coefficients for each scale were as follows: Openness of Self-Disclosure = .71, Amount of Self-Disclosure = .78, Positive/Negative Self-Disclosure = .82, Control of Depth of SelfDisclosure = .76, Honesty/Accuracy of Self-Disclosure = .81, and Psychological Abuse = .87. Results Openness of self-disclosure (see table 1) was significantly, negatively correlated with perceived psychological abuse for the entire sample (r = -.31, df = 165, p < .05), and for males (r = -.28, df = 65, p < .05) and females (r = -.35, df = 103, p < .05). No significant difference was 305 Jacqueline A. Shirley, William G. Powers, and Chris R. Sawyer indicated between male and female correlations (Z = .85, p > .05). The analysis provided substantial support for H1. Table 1: Openness of Self-Disclosure ________________________________________________________________ Open-All Open-Males Open-Females Abusive Actions Insulting -.16* -.07 -.21* Spiteful -.18* -.22* -.17* Critical -.18* -.02 -.29 Sulked -.04 -.02 -.05 Stomped Out -.06 -.16 -.02 Your Guilt -.21* -.31* -.17* Restrict Yr Interactions -.30* -.38* -.27* Disregarded Yr Feelings -.27* -.13 -.24* Denied Your Perceptions -.23* -.15 -.26* No tenderness -.08 -.04 -.11 Accused You of Cheating -.12 -.34* .00 Issued Orders -.29* -.33* -.30* Degraded You -.27* -.23* -.30* Took No Responsibility -.18* -.20 -.16* Unreliable -.25* -.17 -.29* Total Psychological Abuse -.31* -.28* -.35* * p< .05 ________________________________________________________________ Amount of self-disclosure (see table 2) was not Table 2: Amount of Self-Disclosure _________________________________________________________ Abusive Actions Amt.-All Amt.-Males Amt.-Females Insulting .02 .20 -.09 Spiteful .01 .13 .07 Critical -.04 .19 -.20* Sulked .10 .20 .04 Stomped Out .01 .08 -.04 Your Guilt -.00 .18 -.10 Restricted Yr Interactions .06 .12 .03 Disregarded Yr Feelings .06 .29* -.09 Denied Your Perceptions .05 .18 -.02 No tenderness -.02 .18 -.15 Accused You of Cheating -.01 .13 -.09 Issued Orders -.01 .03 -.05 Degraded You .03 .28* -.15 Took No Responsibility .14* .25* .08 Unreliable .10 .29* -.01 Total Psychological Abuse .06 .28* -.10 __________________________________________________________ • p< .05 Abusive Relationships 306 significantly, negatively correlated with perceived psychological abuse for the entire sample (r = .06, df = 165, p > .05) nor for females (r = -.10, df = 103 p > .05). However, a significant positive correlation was indicated for males (r = .28, df = 65, p < .05). A significant difference was indicted between male and female correlations (Z = -2.31, p < .05). Thus H2 was not supported. Control of Depth of self-disclosures (see table 3) was Table 3: Control of the Depth of Self-Disclosure _________________________________________________________ Cont-All Cont-Males Cont-Females Abusive Actions Insulting -.09 -.10 -.07 Spiteful -.10 -.09 -.11 Critical -.10 -.13 -.08 Sulked -.24* -.30* -.20* Stomped Out -.11 -.11 -.12 Your Guilt -.03 -.04 -.02 Restricted Your Interactions -.08 .04 -.15 Disregarded Your Feelings -.06 -.22* .05 Denied Your Perceptions -.07 -.16 -.02 No tenderness -.05 -.23* .07 Accused You of Cheating -.04 -.11 .01 Issued Orders -.01 .04 -.05 Degraded You .01 -.08 .09 Took No Responsibility - .17* -.18 -.18* Unreliable -.15* -.18 -.13 Total Psychological Abuse -.15* -.19 -.12 ______________________________________________________ * p< .05 significantly, negatively correlated with perceived psychological abuse for the entire sample (r = -.15, df = 165, p < .05) and for males (r = -.19, df = 65, p > .05). The female correlation (r = .12) was directionally supportive but did not approach significance (df = 103, p > .05). No significant differences were indicated between male and female correlations (Z = .42, p > .05). The analysis provided support for H3. Positive self-disclosure (see table 4) was significantly, negatively correlated with perceived psychological abuse for the entire sample (r = -.22, df = 165, p < .05) and for females (r = -.26, df = 103, p < .05). The correlation for males (r = -.18, df = 65, NS) was not significant. No significant differences were detected between male and female correlations (Z = .85, p > .05). The analysis provided support for H4. 307 Jacqueline A. Shirley, William G. Powers, and Chris R. Sawyer Table 4: Positive/Negative Self-Disclosure _______________________________________________________________ Abusive Actions Pos-All Pos-Males Pos-Females Insulting -.13* -.10 -.14 Spiteful -.10 -.14 -.08 Critical -.19* .00 -.31* Sulked -.12 -.04 -.15 Stomped Out -.20* -.15 -.25* Your Guilt -.18* -.07 -.23* Restricted Yr Interactions -.16* -.05 -.22* Disregarded Yr Feelings -.11 -.12 -.09 Denied Your Perceptions -.09 -.13 -.05 No tenderness .03 -.13 .10 Accused You of Cheating -.11 -.25* -.04 Issued Orders -.16* -.21* -.16* Degraded You -.24* -.22* -.27* Took No Responsibility -.07 .00 -.08 Unreliable -.20* -.21* -.19* Total Psychological Abuse -.22* -.18 -.26* _______________________________________________________________ * p< .05 The honesty/accuracy of self-disclosure (see table 5) was Table 5: Honesty/Accuracy of Self-Disclosure _________________________________________________________ Abusive Actions Hon-All Hon-Males Hon-Females Insulting -.07 -.12 -.04 Spiteful .00 -.13 .07 Critical -.15* -.19 -.13 Sulked -.01 .02 -.15 Stomped Out -.15* -.09 -.19* Your Guilt -.12 -.09 -.13 Restricted Yr Interactions -.13* -.14 -.14 Disregarded Yr Feelings -.02 .02 -.03 Denied Your Perceptions -.06 -.04 -.07 No tenderness .01 -.02 .03 Accused You of Cheating -.14* -.20 -.11 Issued Orders -.09 -.01 -.17 Degraded You -.16* -.21* -.12 Took No Responsibility -.09 -.09 -.08 Unreliable -.17* .07 -.30* Total Psychological Abuse -.15* -.12 -.17* _________________________________________________________ p< .05 Abusive Relationships 308 significantly, negatively correlated with perceived psychological abuse for the entire sample (r = -.15, df = 165, p < .05) and for females (r = -.17, df = 103, p < .05). The male correlation (r = .12, df = 65, p > .05) was not significant. No significant differences were indicated between male and female correlations (Z = -1.90, p > .05). The analysis provided support for H5.On four of the five correlations between self-disclosure factors and perceived psychological abuse no significant differences were detected in the correlations for males and females. However, males and females differed with respect to the amount of disclosure. For males the correlation between the amount of disclosure and perceived psychological abuse by a dating partner was positive (r = .28) while the correlation for females was weak and negative (r = -.10). Discussion As predicted, there were significant negative relationships between the amount of perceived psychological abuse and the amount of openness of self disclosure, the degree that one controls the depth of disclosures, the nature of the positive-negative orientation of self-disclosures, and the extent of the honesty/accuracy in one’s disclosures. Only the amount of self-disclosure was not correlated with the amount of abuse over the entire sample, likely the result of the differences in directionality for male and female correlations. It appears that some aspects of disclosure provide both an opportunity and rationale for abuse by a partner so inclined. Conversely, the abusiveness by a partner may produce a pattern of caution in self-disclosure by the abuse target. The relationship between self-disclosure patterns and psychological abuse deserves further investigation. The effect of abuse on related constructs, such as the impact of abuse on the selfesteem of the abused partner, and the role of coping styles such as repression-sensitization, merit further examination in this context. Although four of the five self-disclosure factors were significantly and negatively correlated with the amount of psychological abuse, the degree of correlation was quite modest. To further explore this unexpected result, a more detailed examination of the different types of psychological abuse contained in the measure was conducted by examining each abuse item in the measure. Each of the four self-disclosure factors related to overall perceptions of abuse was significantly related to only some of the abusive behaviors—and rarely the same abusive behaviors. Table 6 displays the number of Table 6: Self-Disclosure and Psychologically Abusive Actions ______________________________________________________ Disclosure Number of Related Overall Factor Factor Abusive Actions Correlation Openness 10 -.31* Amount 1 .06 Control 3 -.15* Positive 8 -.22* Honesty 6 -.15* _________________________________________________________ * P<.05 309 Jacqueline A. Shirley, William G. Powers, and Chris R. Sawyer individual abuse actions that maintained a significant negative correlation with each selfdisclosure factor relative to the possible number of 15 abuse actions. A more detailed analysis is contained in Tables 1—5 in which individual scale items were correlated with the number of times each abusive action occurred, indicate that some facets of self-disclosure orientation are associated with particular abuse actions than are others. Future research should attempt to examine the impact of psychological abuse on particular types of self disclosure behavior. In addition, future researchers should shift from cross-sectional and descriptive research, such as the one employed in this study, to more elaborate and experimental designs. The focus of these studies would be to more fully explicate the causal relations between abuse and selfdisclosure. Moreover, differences in male and female reactions to the abuse of a dating partner merits further detailed examination. Finally, the emergence of same-sex dating partners as well as opposite-sex dating partners requires further research to pursue the extent to which the sex combinations within a dating relationship may impact the evolution and response pattern associated with abuse and self-disclosure. Abusive Relationships 310 References Babcock, J. C., & Waltz, J., Jacobson, N. S., & Gottman, J. M. (1993). Power and violence: The relation between communication patterns, power, discrepancies, and domestic violence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 40-50. Baxter, L. A., & Montgomery, B. M. (1996). Relating: Dialoguesand dialectics. New York: Guilford.Bergman, L. (1992). Dating violence among high school students. Social Work, 37, 21-27. Cozby, P. C. (1973). Self-disclosure: A literature review. Psychological Bulletin, 79, 73-91. DeGregoria, B. (1987). Sex role attitude and perception of psychological abuse. Sex Roles, 16, 227-235. Dobash, R. E., & Dobash, R. P. (1981). Violence against wives.New York: MacMillian Free Press. Egan, G. (1970). Encounter: Group processes for interpersonal group. California: Brooks and Cole. Follingstad, D. R., & Dehart, D. D. (2000). Definingpsychological abuse of husbands toward wives: Contexts, behaviors, and typologies. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 15, 891920. Follingstad, D. R., Rutledge, L. L., Berg, B. J., Hause, E. S., & Poleck, D. (1990). The role of emotional abuse in physically abusive relationships. Journal of Family Violence, 5, 107-120. Foshee, V. (1996). Gender differences in adolescent dating abuse prevalence, types and injuries. Health Education Research, 11, 275-286. Foster, L. A., Veale, C. M., & Fogel, C. I. (1989). Factors present when battered women kill. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 10, 273-284. Gergen, K. J., (1971). The concept of the self. New York: Holt, Reinhart, and Winston. Greenfield, L. A. Rand, M. R., Craven, D., Flaus, P. A., Perkins, C. A., Ringel, C., Warchol, G., Maston, C. & Fox, J. A. (1998). Violence by intimates: An analysis of data on crimes by current or former spouses, boyfriends and girlfriends. Washington, DC: Department of Justice Bureau of Justice Statistics. violence. Washington: DC. Bureau of Justice Statistics, US Department of Justice. Gondolf, E. W. (1987). Evaluating programs for men who batter:Problems and prospects. Journal of Family Violence, 2, 95-108. Harned, M. S. (2002). A multivariate analysis of risk markers for dating violence victimization. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 17, 1179-1197. Hoffman, P. (1984). Psychological abuse of women by spouses and live-in lovers. Women and Therapy, 3, 37-47. Howard, J. A., Blumstein, P., & Schwartz, P. (1986). Sex, power, and influence tactics in intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 102-109. Jourard, S. M. (1971). The transparent self. New York: Van Nostrand. Kasian, M., & Painter, S. L. (1992). Frequency and severity of psychological abuse in a dating population. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 7, 350-364. Kasper, S., (1982). Judgments of professional and nonprofessional men and women about psychological abuse in marriage. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Hofstra University. 311 Jacqueline A. Shirley, William G. Powers, and Chris R. Sawyer Katz, J., & Arias, I., & Beach, S. R. H., (2000). Psychological abuse, self-esteem, and women’s dating relationship outcomes: A comparison of the Self-verification and selfenhancement perspectives. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, 349-357. Katz, J., Street, A., & Arias, I. (1997). Individual differences in self-appraisals and responses to relationship violence. Violence and Victims, 12, 3-14. Loring, M. T. (1994). Emotional abuse. New York: Lexington. Makepeace, J. (1989). Dating, living together and courtship violence. In M. Pirog-Good & J. Stets (Eds.), Violence in dating relationships. New York: Praeger. Marcus, R.F., & Swett, B. (2002). Violence and intimacy in close relationships. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 17, 570-586. Marshall, L. L. (1994). Psychological abuse of women: Six distinct clusters. Journal of Family Violence, 11, 379-409. McFarlane, J., Wilson, P., Malacha, A., & Lemmey, D. (2000). Intimate partner violence. A gender comparison. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 15, 158-169. McLaughlin, I. G., Leonard, K. E., & Senchak, M. (1992). Prevalence and distribution of premarital aggression among couples applying for a marriage license. Journal of Family Violence, 7, 309-319. Molidor, C. E. (1995). Gender differences in psychological abuse in high school dating relationships. Child and Adolescent Social Work, 12, 119-134. Noller, P. (1981). Gender and martial adjustment level differences in decoding message from spouses and strangers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 272-278. Okun, L. (1986). Women abuse: Facts replacing myths. Albany State University of New York Press. O’Leary, K. D. (1999). Psychological abuse: A variable deserving critical attention in domestic violence. Violence and Victims, 14, 3-23. O’Leary, K. D., & Jouriles, E. N. (1994). Psychological abuse between adult partners: Prevalence and impact on partners and children. In L. L’Abate (Ed.), Handbook of developmental family psychology and psychopathology (pp.330-349). New York: John Wiley & Sons. Pipes, R. B., & LeBov-Keeler, K. (1997). Psychological abuse among college women in exclusive heterosexual dating relationships. Sex Roles, 36, 585-603. Prager, K.J. (1995). The psychology of intimacy. New York: Guilford Press. Raymond, B., & Bruschi, I. G. (1989). Psychological abuse among college women in dating relationships. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 69, 1283-1297. Rosenfeld, L. B. (1979). Self-disclosure avoidance: Why am I afraid to tell you who I am? Communication Monographs, 46, 63-74. Rubin, Z., Hill, C. T., Peplau, L. A., Dunkel-Schetter, C. (1980). Self-disclosure in dating couples: Sex roles in the ethic of openness. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 42, 305318. Sabourin, T. C. (1991). Perceptions of verbal aggression in interspousal violence, In D. Knudsen & J. L. Miller (Eds.). Abused and battered: Social and legal responses to family violence. New York: Aldine de Gruyter. Sharp, D., & Taylor, J. K. (1999). An examination of variables from a social-developmental model to explain physical and psychological dating violence. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 31, 165-175. Abusive Relationships 312 Stets, J. E.,& Straus, M. A. (1990). Gender differences in reporting marital violence and its medical and psychological consequences. In M. A. Straus & R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical violence in American families: Risk factors and adaptations to violence in 8,145 families (pp. 151-165). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction. Straus, M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The Conflict Tactics (CT) Scales. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 75-88. Straus, M. A., Sweet, S., & Vissing, Y. M. (1989). Verbal aggression against spouses and children in a nationally representative sample of American families. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Speech Communication Association, San Francisco, CA. Sugarman, D. B., & Hotaling, G. T. (1989). Dating violence: Prevalence, context, and risk markers. In M. A. Pirog-Good & J. E. Stets (Eds.), Violence in dating relationships:Emerging social issues (pp.3-32). New York: Praeger. Tolman, R. M. (1989). The development of a measure of psychological maltreatment of women by their male partners. Violence and Victims, 4, 159-177. Tolman, R. M., & Bhosley, G. (1991). The outcome of participation in a shelter sponsored program for men who batter. In D. Knudsen & J. Miller (Eds.), Abused and battered: Social and legal responses to family violence. New York: Aldine de Gruyter. Tolman, R. M. (1992). Psychological abuse of women. In R.T. Ammerman & M. Hersen (Eds.), Assessment of family violence: A clinical and a legal source book.(pp. 291-310). New York: Wiley. Walker, L. E., (1979). The battered woman. New York: Harper and Row. Walker, L. E. (1984). The battered woman syndrome. New York: Springer. Wheeless, L. R., & Grotz, J. (1976). Conceptualization and measurement of reported selfdisclosure. Human Communication Research, 2, 338-346. Wheeless, L. R., & Grotz, J. (1977). The measurement of trust and its relationship to selfdisclosure, Human Communication Research, 3, 250-257. Wheeless, L. R. (1978). A follow-up study of the relationships among trust, disclosure, and interpersonal solidarity. Human Communication Research, 4, 143-157. White, J. W., & Koss, M. P. (1991). Courtship violence: Incidence in a national sample of higher education students. Violence and Victims, 6, 247-256. Human Communication. A Publication of the Pacific and Asian Communication Association. Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 313 – 322. Extroversion versus Similarity: An Exploration of Factors Influencing Communication Accuracy of Social Cognitions David Dugas Texas Christian University William G. Powers Texas Christian University Chris Sawyer Texas Christian University David Dugas (B.S., Texas Christian University, 2001), graduated as an honors seminar student in the Department of Communication Studies, Texas Christian University, Fort Worth, TX 76129. William G. Powers (Ph.D., University of Oklahoma, 1973) is a Professor and Chris R. Sawyer (Ph.D., University of North Texas, 1993) is an Associate Professor in the same department. Direct all correspondence to: w.powers@tcu.edu Sawyer Basic Communication Fidelity: Extroversion 314 Abstract Although extroversion has been linked to effectiveness in numerous communicative situations, it is unclear whether this dimension of personality promotes communication competence because of enhanced levels of expressiveness or through other processes, such as perceived similarities. Expanding on this theoretical divide, two contrary predictions were advanced representing both perspectives, i.e., amount of information versus similarity. In the current study, the view that source extroversion promotes interpersonal accuracy based upon amount of information was supported rather than the similarity prediction. Implications for future research are discussed. 315 David Dugas. William G. Powers, and Chris Introduction Misunderstandings are experienced at every level of human interaction and in every context, in part, due to the quality of the communication presented by the source (Brandt & Powers, 1980, Powers and Lowry, 1984a,b). Although many misunderstandings are inconsequential, some ultimately have serious impact upon the quality of life. Identifying and understanding the factors that contribute to misunderstandings, or conversely communication accuracy, is essential to increase the potential for satisfying social relationships. Powers and Lowry (1984a) defined basic communication fidelity (BCF) as the “degree of congruence between the cognitions of two or more individuals following a communication event” (p. 58). The degree of congruence establishes the level of communication accuracy. Measures of interpersonal communication accuracy, such as BCF (Powers & Spitzberg, 1986), represent one of the foundational elements of communication competence (McCroskey, 1984). Early BCF studies involved the communication of tangible cognitions, such as geometric shapes (Brandt & Powers, 1980, Powers and Lowry, 1984a,b) as the test bed for this construct. However, the attention of BCF researchers soon turned to social cognitions. This expanded focus on social cognitions established the reliability and validity of measuring the communication accuracy of such images in social and professional contexts (Powers & Love, 1989; Powers & Spitzberg, 1986). Later, Kopecky and Powers (2002) found both gender and relationship level to be factors impacting the communication accuracy of social cognitions. The current study seeks to enhance understanding of the communication accuracy of social cognitions by examining two competing explanations related to the ability to acquire higher levels of self-identity communication accuracy (personality and similarity) in the social cognition context. Theoretical Perspective Extroversion and Interpersonal Accuracy Since Eysenck's (1967) seminal work, extroversion has been viewed an integral part of personality and is said to have many manifestations in social life including assertiveness, gregariousness, cheerfulness, and energy (John & Srivastava, 1999). Extroversion measures an individual’s tendency to be gregarious, assertive, and sociable while introversion, its bipolar opposite, describes the tendency to be reserved, timid, and quiet (Antonioni, 1998). Headey and Wearing (1989) suggest that extroversion predisposes individuals to have favorable life events in friendship and work, and that these traits lead to a positive sense of wellbeing. McCrae and Costa (1986) argue that extroverts adapt to stressful events more efficiently than introverts do. Extroverts experience more positive emotions and less negative sentiments than do introverts (Moberg, 1999). In addition, extroverts are seen as more socially oriented people. Brown and Hendrick (1971) found that extroverts are generally more popular due to the fact that they are seen as more interesting, warm, and more influential than do introverts. These studies point to the idea that extroverts may communicate more effectively with others simply because they send out more interpretable cues and information than introverts. Extroverts tend to have strong social skills and a desire to work with others (Antonioni, 1998; McCrae & Costa, 1987). These individuals exude more confidence and assertiveness. They tend to seek social support from friends when faced with different forms of stress Sawyer Basic Communication Fidelity: Extroversion 316 (Amirkhan, Risinger, & Swickert, 1995). They draw energy from other people and things outside themselves. They think by talking out loud and need the company of others to maintain emotional balance (Kurcinka, 1994). Introverts draw energy from within and need time to reflect on their experiences to understand them. They need opportunities to watch or listen before carrying out an act and need time alone before they are ready to share their experiences. This information supports a proposal stating that extroverts convey more information in a communication event. Numerous studies point to specific communication outcomes or behaviors directly related to differences along the extroversion-introversion continuum. Extroverts talk more than do introverts (Campbell & Rushton, 1978; John & Srivastava, 1999). Thorne (1987) notes that introverts and extroverts have distinctive communication styles and tendencies. Extroverts spend less time pausing before speaking in a conversation. They tend to speak more than introverts, although they do not self-disclose any more than introverts. People who speak more quickly are often extroverts while those who speak at a slower than average rate are usually introverts. Siegman (1956) argues that the greater amount of pausing and less speaking among introverts results from a higher cognitive activity and less impulsivity. Other research indicates that extroverts are more expressive than introverts (Giles & Street, 1994). In either case, more evidence is presented that different levels of the extroversion trait denote specific communication behaviors. In regards to communicative events, extroverts display a wide range of topics and claims of common ground. In contrast, introverts engage in fewer, more focused conversations (Thorne, 1987). Previous studies of interpersonal perception and extroversion (Kenny, 1994; Kenny, Horner, Kashy, & Chou, 1992; Malloy & Albright, 1990) suggest that extroverts maintain higher levels of interpersonal accuracy than do introverts. Perceived-Similarity and Communication Accuracy A contrary explanation for communicative accuracy stems from the concept of similarity as proposed in Byrne's (1997) law of interpersonal attraction. According to this position, humans are attracted to others with whom they share similar characteristics such as attitudes, beliefs, values, and personality traits. Because such similarities are self-confirming and rewarding in nature, people recognize and communicate these similarities in an on-going manner. Furthermore, Kenny and Kashy (1994) suggest that as a function of interpersonal communication, such as self-disclosure, humans discover the shared attitudes and perspectives held with their partners. These, in turn, strengthen their social bonds leading to increased interpersonal accuracy. It would stand to reason that conversation partners communicate more accurately with one another when they possess similar personality traits. Consequently, partners of similar extroversion levels would communicate more accurately than would partners with dissimilar levels. Thus, there are two clear lines of thought that are in competition with each other. On the one hand, literature clearly supports the notion that extroverts should produce greater communication accuracy than introverts due to their increased communication cues. On the other hand, a significant body of literature supports the notion that people of similar levels of extroversion/introversion should produce greater communication accuracy of their extroversion or introversion regardless of that level due to the ease of recognition. 317 David Dugas. William G. Powers, and Chris Hypotheses Based on the preceding theoretical perspective, the following contrasting hypotheses were advanced: H1: Individual sources in pairs with similar levels of extroversion will have significantly higher BCF regarding self-identity social cognitions than will individual sources in pairs having dissimilar levels of extroversion. H2: Individual sources with high extroversion will have significantly higher BCF than will individual sources with low extroversion regardless of the partner’s level of extroversion. Method Participants Participants in this study were 111 (40 male, 71 female) undergraduates enrolled in an introductory-level college speech communication courses. All participants were volunteers for a study of communication effectiveness and were given credit toward completion of the course in exchange for their involvement. Procedures Participants first completed a questionnaire containing demographic questions and a measure of extroversion (Francis, Brown, & Philipchalk, 1992) (Alpha = .91). Each person then selected one of three identical research sessions to attend approximately one week later. A median split of extroversion scores classified participants as either high or low extroversion. Pairs of participants were randomly assigned to one of three group categories based on their classification as demonstrating either High or Low Extroversion: 25% of the pairs were placed into the High-High category, 50% were placed into High-Low category, and 25% were placed into the Low-Low category. At each of the three sessions, partners were brought together and given a sheet of general conversation topics and instructed to get to know each other better over the next 10 minutes. After the communication event had concluded, partners were separated and seated on opposite sides of the room to complete the final questionnaire. The first section of the questionnaire used the same extroversion measurement form as previously completed a week earlier but now asked the participants to indicate how they portrayed themselves in the communication event (Alpha = .94). The second section was distributed upon completion of the first. Each participant then reported his/her perception of the extroversion displayed by his/her partner using the same extroversion form (Alpha = .92). Thus for each pair, the cognition of extroversion that was intended to be communicated to the partner could be compared to the cognition developed by the partner. The absolute difference score was computed and treated as representative of each person’s communication accuracy (BCF) in this context with this partner. Sawyer Basic Communication Fidelity: Extroversion 318 Results The mean and standard deviation for self-reported extroversion was 29.61 (6.11) with a median of 31. Means and standard deviations for the extroversion and BCF by treatment groups appear in Table 1. A 2 x 2 ANOVA in which actor extroversion (high v. low) and partner Table 1: Means and Standard Deviations for BCF Treatment Groups. Actor x Partner Extroversion Low Actor, Low Partner Low Actor, High Partner High Actor, Low Partner High Actor, High Partner Mean 6.88 7.14 4.14 3.82 Standard Deviation 3.91 3.23 2.46 2.36 Note: In this study, a lower BCF number represents greater accuracy while a higher BCF number indicates less accuracy. extroversion (high v. low) were predictors of dyad level BCF yielded one significant main effect (F1,1,120 = 30.34; p <.0001; eta2 = .203). Scheffé t-tests detected a significant difference in BCF for actor extroversion (p < .0001) and no significant difference in BCF for partner extroversion (p = .68). There were no interaction effects. Pairs in which the actor had high extroversion communicated with high accuracy regardless of the partner’s level of extroversion. Pairs in which the actor had low extroversion communicated with lower accuracy regardless of the partner’s extroversion level. Discussion After the results of the experiment were analyzed, only one of the hypotheses was supported. The first hypothesis, that paired individuals with similar extroversion levels will communicate more effectively than dissimilar personality pairs, was not confirmed. This result does not support the claim that individuals will communicate more effectively with others who have similar extroversion scores. It is important to note that the results of this test do not rule out the possibility that other similar social cognitions and characteristics do directly relate to communication fidelity. The second hypothesis, that regardless of their partner extroversion level, high extroversion speakers will communicate their personality traits more effectively than low extroversion speakers, was unequivocally sustained. Test results show overwhelming support for the hypothesis that high extroverts are more effective communicators than low extroverts. This finding is consistent with previous studies of interpersonal perception and extroversion (Kenny, 1994; Kenny, et al, 1992; Malloy & Albright, 1990) and provides support for the second of the two competing hypotheses. These results support the idea that extroverts reveal more social cognition related cues than introverts and therefore produce higher BCF, irrespective of their partner's extroversion. Given this finding, scholars should recognize that encoding more information when 319 David Dugas. William G. Powers, and Chris communicating can lead to more effective communication of at least one element of self-identity. This result is significant because it allows researchers to analyze communication events more efficiently and will lead to a better understanding of improved communication methods. By discovering one of the keys to more effective communication, scholars can take another step towards understanding how and why some communication events result in success while others leave the receiver with a drastically flawed interpretation of the sender’s original message. These findings lead researchers to new questions and concerns. Questions arise quickly about a maximally beneficial level of information that avoids over/under loads of information and the role of personality and communication accuracy. If extroversion can affect communication effectiveness, what other personality traits might have the same effect? There is enough previous evidence in other experiments and research to warrant the extended study of how similar characteristics between individuals in a communication event are related to communication outcomes. Many areas involving personality and its connection with BCF remain unexplored. The concept that more contextual cues encoded in an event leads to a better understanding between sender and receiver is significant in that it clearly identifies a major factor related to the potential for misunderstanding in human relationships. Sawyer Basic Communication Fidelity: Extroversion 320 References Amirkhan, J. H., Risinger, R. T., & Swickert, R. J. (1995). Extraversion: A 'hidden' personality factor in coping? Journal of Personality, 63, 189-212. Antonioni, D. (1998). Relationship between the big five personality factors and conflict management styles. International Journal of Conflict Management, 9, 336-355. Brandt, D. R., & Powers, W. G. (1980). An approach to developing communicative competence in scientific and technical communicators. Technical Writing and Communication, 10, 213-221. Brown, S. R. & Hendrick, C. (1971). Introversion, extraversion and social perception. British Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology. 10, pp. 313-319. Byrne, D. (1997). An overview (and underview) of research and theory within the attraction paradigm. Journal of Social & Personal Relationships, 14, 417-431. Campbell, A. & Rushton, J. (1978). Bodily communication and personality. British Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology. 17, pp. 31-36. Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, Ill.: Thomas. Francis, L. J., Brown, L. B., & Philipchalk, R. (1992). The development of an abbreviated form of the Revised Eysenck Personality Questionnaire: Its use among students in England, Canada, the USA, and Australia. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 442-449. Giles, H., & Street, R. L., Jr. (1994). Communicator characteristics and behavior. In M. L. Knapp, & G. R. Miller (Ed.), Handbook of interpersonal communication (2), (pp. 103161). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Headley, B., & Wearing, A. (1989). Personality, life events, and subjective well-being: Toward a dynamic equilibrium model. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 57, pp. 731739. John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The big five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research. New York: Guilford. Kenny, D. A. (1994). Interpersonal perception: A social relations analysis. New York: Guilford Press Kenny, D. A., Horner, C., Kashy, D. A., & Chou, L. (1992). Consensus at zero acquaintance: Replication, behavioral cues, and stability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 88-97. Kenny, D. A., & Kashy, D. A. (1994). Enhanced co-orientation in the perception of friends: A social relations analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1024-1033. Kopecky, C. C. & Powers, W. G. (2002). Relational Development and Self-Image Communication Accuracy. Communication Research Reports. 19, 283-290. Kurcinka, M. S. (1994, January). Introverts and extroverts. Parents Magazine, 69, 76-78. Malloy, T. E., & Albright, L. (1990). Interpersonal perception in a social context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 419-428. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1989). Different points of view: Self-reports and ratings in the assessment of personality. In J.P. Forgas & M.J. Innes (Eds.), Recent advances in social psychology (pp 429—439). Amsterdam: North-Holland. McCroskey, J. C. (1984). Communication competence: The elusive construct. In R. N. Bostrom (Ed.), Competence in communication (pp. 259-268). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. 321 David Dugas. William G. Powers, and Chris Moberg, D. J. (1999). The big five and organizational virtue. Business Ethics Quarterly, 9, 245272. Powers, W. G., & Love, D. (1985). Basic communication fidelity: An extension. Communication Research Reports, 6, 79-82. Powers, W. G. & Lowry, D. N. (1984b). Basic communication fidelity and nationality: Group interaction perceptions. Communication Research Reports, 1, 48-53. Powers, W. G., & Lowry, D.N. (1984a). Basic communication fidelity: A fundamental approach. In R.N. Bostrom (Ed.), Competence in communication (pp. 57-71). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Powers, W. G., & Spitzberg, B. (1986). Basic communication fidelity and image management. Communication Research Reports, 3, 60-63. Siegman, A. W. (1956). Cognitive, affective, and psychopathological correlates of the Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20, pp. 137-141. Thorne, A. (1887). The press of personality: A study of conversations between introverts and extraverts. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 53, pp. 718-726. Human Communication. A Publication of the Pacific and Asian Communication Association. Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 321 – 338. Communicating with College Students about STIs: Assessing Message Effectiveness and Preferred Source and Channel Marian L. Houser Texas State University-San Marcos Michael E. Burns Texas State University-San Marcos Nicholas R. Driver Texas State University-San Marcos Marian L. Houser (Ph.D., University of Tennessee) is an Assistant Professor, and Michael E. Burns and Nicholas R. Driver (MA, Texas State University-San Marcos) are graduates in the Communication Studies Department at Texas State University-San Marcos. Direct all future correspondence to Marian L. Houser, Department of Communication Studies, Texas State University-San Marcos, 601 University Drive, San Marcos, TX 78666, Email: mh53@txstate.edu, Phone, 512-245-3137, Fax: 512-245-3138). Communicating About STIs 322 Abstract This quasi-experiment explored effectiveness of message types (fearful, informative, or humorous) used to educate students about Sexually Transmitted Infections (STI) and preferred sources and channels for receiving them. STIs are a top health risk for 18 to 25 year olds (Synovitz, Herber, Kelly & Carlson, 2002) with numbers increasing across college campuses (Summerfield & Steinhoff, 1996). One hundred fifty undergraduate students responded to one of the three message types. Utilizing protection motivation theory (Rogers, 1975, 1983) as a theoretical lens, results revealed significant differences in effectiveness of message type with fearful being the most effective. Effectiveness also varied according to channel and source. 323 Marian L. Houser, Michael E. Burns, and Nicholas R. Driver Introduction Sexually Transmitted Infections (STIs) are considered one of the top health risks for college students between the ages of 18 and 25 (Synovitz, Herber, Kelley & Carlson, 2002). The Center for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) reported (2000) that more than 65 million Americans have incurable STIs such as HIV, human papillomavirus (HPV) and gonorrhea, with 15 million infected every year. According to the Bacchus Gamma Peer Health Education Network (2006) two-thirds of all STI cases in the United States occur in people 25 years of age or younger, the age of a traditional college student. In fact, the rise in STIs has become a major concern on college campuses (Summerfield & Steinhoff, 1996). Afifi and Weiner (2006) described an astounding rate of STIs on college campuses with 40% of sexually active students infected with HPV, the most prevalent STI. One of the primary reasons for this high susceptibility is young adult college students tend to engage in riskier sexual behavior leading to increased risk of STIs (Abdullah, Fielding, & Hedley, 2003; Meekers & Klein, 2002). The persistent threat calls into question whether they are receiving messages regarding the prevalence and threat of STIs and if so, whether they understand them and feel capable of responding. Protection motivation theory (PMT) has been widely used to study the role of perceived efficacy in specific health communication issues (Rogers, 1975, 1983) and the motivations for handling a possible risk. Communication efficacy reflects an individual’s perceived ability to communicate and obtain needed information (Afifi & Weiner, 2004). PMT contends if individuals understand the degree of risk involved, they will be motivated to protect themselves from that risk (Youn, 2005). With STI messages, therefore, it becomes important to determine the most effective means to reach college students. According to Cheah (2006), research aimed at young adults and focusing on message development for sexual health campaigns has been relatively nonexistent. In line with this, Bull, Cohen, Ortiz and Evans (2002) suggested the majority of STD campaign materials are based on untested assumptions. Therefore, investigating STI messages along with the sources and channels college students view as most effective and with which they are most responsive could help universities develop long term health campaigns aimed at educating them about STIs and protection. Many students are simply not educated on the topic because they do not understand its importance or personal relevance, especially if they abstain from sexual activity (Fields, 2002). Whether they are sexually active, however, is not the primary issue; being aware of the information will help them make educated decisions in the future and perhaps assist them in helping a friend locate needed help. Active education is the key to lowering the high STI rates on college campuses (Van Haveren, Blank, & Bentley, 2001) therefore this study’s overarching goal was aimed at helping health educators on college campuses understand what communication tactics are seen as most effective by their primary audience: college students. The purpose of the present quasi-experiment was to determine the most effective and comfortable message, source, and channel for educating students about STIs. This could provide the first step to discovering a new way to relate the importance of protection and available services. Review of Literature Communicating to educate U.S. college students about sexually transmitted infections and prevention is becoming more important as it has become a critical health issue (Synovitz et Communicating About STIs 324 al., 2002). The American Social Health Association (ASHA) suggests social stigma and lack of STI awareness are the primary discussion inhibitors (ASHA, 1998). Unfortunately, the CDC continues to report a rise in STIs. The incidence of genital herpes infections, for example, has increased by 30 percent since the late 1970’s (CDC, 2006) and there was a 6.5% increase in cases of Chlamydia between 2001 and 2002 (CDC, 2002). According to the Guttmacher Institute (1994) each year one in four U.S. teens contracts an STI with about half of all STIs in 2000 reported in young people between the ages of 15 and 24 (Weinstock, Berman, & Cates, 2004). These numbers translate, unfortunately, to a rising incidence of STIs among college students, prompting universities to consider different message tactics, sources, and channels for communicating the importance of protection and prevention rather than abstinence (Summerfield & Steinhoff, 1996). Studies have, in fact, shown college students lack knowledge about STIs (Synovitz et al., 2002) and believe they are invulnerable (Ku et al., 2002). This view has, unfortunately led to the rise in STI incidence among young adults in the U.S. (Cheah, 2006), creating an increased need for the development of effective sexual heath education. Television has typically been the major source of effective health message campaigns, issuing warnings about topics such as AIDS, drug abuse, drinking and driving, and smoking (Block & Keller, 1995). However, it has been reported that today’s college students spend less time watching television and more time on computers (Davis, 2006; Story, 2007). A majority of students according to one study spend less than three hours per week watching television (Davis, 2006). If television is not a prime message outlet, other more effective means must be discovered to disseminate STI information. Sheer and Cline (1995) suggested prevention education programs and active awareness on college campuses are important methods of educating students on STIs. In addition, they advocated these programs over persuasive appeals aimed at changing risky behavior. Fields (2002) reported a need for formalized programs on campuses as the majority of sex education outreach is based on unwanted pregnancy and assault with significantly less time spent on STI education. Though the federal government’s mandate that sexual assault prevention efforts be conducted on all college campuses is vital, it is primarily targeted for women and fails to incorporate STI education (Anderson & Wiston, 2005). A reduced focus on STI education creates a need for colleges to discover the most viable means to spread the word if it is to reach students at all. According to Van Haveren et al. (2001) active education is the most successful means to inform students that fall in the age range of 18 to 25. Choosing an effective message and means of communication is the key to convincing a college student they may be at risk yet have control over protecting themselves from becoming infected with an STI (Van Haveren et al., 2001). This study utilized Rogers’s (1975, 1983) protection motivation theory (PMT) as a theoretical lens to help explain college students’ perceptions of STI messages, sources, and channels. PMT contends individuals must perceive something to be risky or harmful to be motivated to protect oneself. Rogers asserted that this motivation to protect is behavior inducing. This theory creates a logical link to STI communication and education due to the connection between message understanding and value, self-efficacy, and the outcome behaviors college students have following exposure to STI messages. It is important for them to understand the STI risk as well as their ability to take control of their sexual behavior and protect themselves (Van Haveren et al., 2001). Weinstock et al. (2004) reported half of all new STI’s in 2000 occurred among young people between the ages of 15 and 24 and the practice of unprotected sex on 325 Marian L. Houser, Michael E. Burns, and Nicholas R. Driver college campuses has resulted in a high percentage of students infected with STIs. Therefore, effective health campaigns or messages are invaluable for educating and inducing behavior. Past research connecting the use of fear and self-efficacy in health campaigns offers a direct link to the current study as the use of fear alone has not been shown to adequately persuade people to change their attitudes (Witte, 1994). It is through feelings of self-efficacy that individuals are motivated to change their attitudes or behaviors (Witte, 1994). Threatening messages combined with alternative solutions allows individuals to feel in control (e.g., efficacy to promote condom use). Witte (1994) utilized this method in an experiment investigating fear in AIDS-related messages and confirmed the combined effectiveness of fear and message selfefficacy. In an investigation of fear appeals in the college classroom, Sprinkle, Hunt, Simonds, and Comadena (2006) reported positive effects (learning) of fear and efficacy together compared to the use of fear alone. Other studies, however, have reported negative effects of fear (Hastings, Stead, & Webb, 2004), recommending the use of positive reinforcement appeals in social marketing campaigns. Beaudoin (2002) explored antismoking ads and reported enhanced effectiveness with humor in youth-oriented advertising and fear for adult-oriented advertising. Earlier research by Booker (1981), however, found mixed effects for fearful messages in advertising with highly correlated, positive effects for humor and straightforward information messages. Focus group research by Cheah (2006) suggested college students have preferences for message type, channel, and source and that STI information should be gradually relayed to them through a variety of means and communication channels. While informative, this finding fails to create an understanding of what actually works best. With STI information, therefore, the initial step must be to determine the most effective messages. Mixed findings regarding STI messages for college students stimulated the development of three message variations: humor, informative, and fearful. Rogers’s (1975, 1983) protection motivation theory was incorporated as a lens to explain the perceived effectiveness of the three messages according to their ability to induce motivation for handling the risk associated with the STI messages (e.g., campus specific statistics illustrating the prevalence of STIs), self-efficacy (e.g., information on how and where to get protection) and response efficacy (e.g., I will go to the health center for purchase of latex and testing). Atkin and Freimuth (1989) explain that formative evaluation research, as in the current study, is important when creating a health campaign aimed at prevention to develop messages that will not only appeal to the target audience, but will also create a realistic connection between the audience and the health risk. As past research has reported differential impacts of message type, the following hypothesis was created: H1: Fearful, humorous, and informative message types will differ in level of perceived effectiveness. In addition to message type, source and channel may also impact student responsiveness and overall message effectiveness (Rimal, Flora, & Schooler, 1999). Marin and Marin (1991) suggested the value of investigating source and channel credibility in AIDS health messages. Their findings indicated manipulation of sources and channels used in a campaign can affect message effectiveness. Marshall, Smith, and McKeon (1995) also supported the value of source and channel with their investigation of health messages regarding cervical and breast cancer. They discovered within group commonalities for persuasive strategies, sources, and channels along with overall differential group preferences (Marshall et al., 1995). Cheah’s qualitative analysis (2006) suggested a broad range of channels for receiving STI messages was preferred by Communicating About STIs 326 college students. Channels such as mandatory classes, the web, public service announcements, posters, exhibitions, campus sexual awareness events, and pamphlets were suggested (Cheah, 2006). Credibility of the source disseminating this information, however, was also a strong point of consideration. Previous research has consistently shown that credibility enhances message acceptance (Chebat, Filiatrault, & Perrien, 1990) and has consistently reported the persuasive effects of highly credible sources (Greenberg & Miller, 1966; Greenberg & Tannenbaum, 1961). Gaining a clearer overall understanding of the source from which students prefer to receive STI information as well as how or where they receive it will allow active communication and, ultimately, education to begin. This will enable campuses to offer students numerous opportunities for message exposure. With this past research in mind, the key to reaching college students may be discovering source and channel preferences for receiving different types of STI messages (fearful, informative, and humorous). Therefore, the following hypothesis was created: H2: Preferred source and channel will differ according to message type (fearful, informative, and humorous). To assist university health programs in developing a more complete understanding of channel and source effectiveness in regard to message type, it was also important to discover why students found specific sources and channels more effective or comforting. In an effort to accomplish this, an open-ended section of survey questions was provided to participants. Keyton (2006) posited that the use of qualitative data could provide considerable support for quantitative findings. Therefore, the following research question was created to develop a more complete understanding of participants’ preferred sources and channels: RQ1: What reasons do students provide for rankings of their most and least preferred source and channel? Method Participants Full-time undergraduate students at a large southern university were asked to complete the study’s survey instrument. After obtaining IRB approval, instructors of the basic communication course (a required university course) allowed surveys to be completed in their classes. All participants were debriefed on the study and signed consent forms indicating their agreement to participate. Three separate surveys measuring the effects of message type (fearful, informative, and humorous) source, and channel were created. One hundred-fifty total surveys were completed with 50 surveys for each message type. Surveys were randomly distributed with even numbers of each message type distributed to each class. Each student was only presented one message type. All 150 surveys were completed to measure message type with nine surveys eliminated from the source and channel analyses due to missing information. Participants consisted of 89 females and 61 males and 39 freshman, 42 sophomores, 30 juniors and 39 seniors. Variables In order to measure the effectiveness (self-efficacy, and response efficacy) of STI messages and the subsequent preference for source and channel, several variables were 327 Marian L. Houser, Michael E. Burns, and Nicholas R. Driver incorporated. The independent variable representing the study’s health campaign approach was the type of message provided to participants (fearful, informative, and humorous). These messages were written to reflect their respective communicative tone or approach to STI warnings. Preferred source and channel for receiving the message and message effectiveness represented the dependent variables as impacted by message type. To measure effectiveness, students were asked to reveal how persuasive they perceived the messages to be along with their ability (self-efficacy) and likelihood of following the STI message recommendations (response efficacy). Source and channel variables were also examined with a follow-up qualitative section of the survey instrument to justify and explain participants’ first and last choices for source and channel. Specific sources and channels were chosen based on past study results where college students reported in focus groups the most preferred source and channel for receiving STD information (Cheah, 2006). Sources incorporated in the present survey were as follows: medical doctor, health center official, certified peer educator, guest lecturer, instructor/professor, resident advisor, hall director, campus posters and orientation leader. Channels included in the survey were as follows: large lecture, small class, dorm lobby, orientation, voluntary information session, health fair and one-on-one discussion. Message Development The fearful and humorous messages utilized in the current study were adapted from existing messages developed and used by Ohio University’s Department of Health Education and Wellness. According to Kopchick, head of this department, “there has been a significant increase in the number of students who have come to the health center to be tested for STIs since using these messages” (C. Kopchick, personal communication, October 15, 2005). Due to the reported success of these messages, the authors chose to use them as a basic framework for the present study. This information along with research supporting the use of a variety of message types for developing health campaigns (Beaudoin, 2002; Booker, 1981; Cheah, 2006), led to the creation of three new messages (fearful, neutral—strictly informative, and humorous) developed and specifically adapted to the university where the research was conducted. Manipulation Check To determine the successful development of message types, a manipulation check was conducted. Researchers designed three messages (fearful, informative, and humorous) and enlisted 285 undergraduate students, unfamiliar with the study, to indicate, on three 5-point Likert-type scales, the extent to which they which they thought a message was fearful, informative, humorous (see Appendix). Each student was provided only one message and asked to indicate their perceptions of its fear, information provided, or humor. The informative or neutral message was created by stating the information about STIs and the campus health center in a very straightforward manner without using any obvious message tone. The humorous message used rhyming phrases with slang terms that encouraged the use of latex protection while also providing ways to get more information about STIs from the campus health center. The final message used fear by applying the campus’ STI statistics. A one-way ANOVA was computed comparing the perceptions of one of each of the STI message manipulations (fear, informative, humor). A significant difference in message perception was found among the three STI messages: Fearful (F(2,282) = 55.95, p<.001), Communicating About STIs 328 Informative (F(2,282) = 11.88, p<.001), and Humorous (F(2,282) = 220.92, p<.001). Tukey’s HSD was used to determine the nature of the different message perceptions. This analysis revealed that students who viewed the fearful message perceived it to be significantly more fearful (M=4.10, SD=.93, p<.001) than either informative (M=3.08, SD=1.22) or humorous (M=2.36, SD=2.37). Students who viewed the informative STI message perceived it to be significantly more informative (M=4.18, SD=.80, p<.001) than either fearful (M=3.69, SD=1.21) or humorous (M=3.42, SD=1.19). Finally, students who viewed the humorous message found it to be significantly more humorous (M=4.16, SD=1.03, p<.001) than either fearful (M=1.60, SD=.88) or informative (M=1.68, SD=.94). With the significance of these results, it appears the tone of each message was properly manipulated. Survey Instrument The entire survey was separated into three sections. The first section measured student perceptions of the effectiveness (persuasiveness, self-efficacy, and response efficacy) of one of the three message types. The second section asked students to indicate their rankings of preferred source and channel for the respective message they received in the first survey section (fearful, informative, or humorous). The final qualitative section asked students to describe their reasoning for the rank order of their most and least preferred source and channel selections. Message effectiveness. The first section of the survey in this quasi-experiment incorporated an instrument to measure message effectiveness (persuasiveness, self-efficacy, and response efficacy) developed from a series of studies by Keller and Block (1997). Their general purpose was to measure the persuasiveness of a health campaign message to determine how able and likely subjects would be to use the information shown in health campaign brochures. They reported alphas ranging from .78 to .84 (Keller & Block, 1997). In the 12-item scale items one through four were Likert-type items ranging from one (strongly disagree) to five (strongly agree). These items focused on whether or not the subjects found the information presented to be beneficial in their future efforts to protect themselves from becoming infected with an STI. Items five through nine were Likert-type items with scores ranging from one (very unlikely) to five (very likely). These items were used to measure the likelihood that subjects would feel capable of engaging in the immediate behaviors recommended by the message (self-efficacy) and their intent to actually engage in this behavior (response efficacy). Items ten through twelve utilized a semantic differential scale (useful-useless, helpful-not helpful, persuasive-not persuasive) ranging from one to seven. These three items were used to measure whether subjects found the information presented as useful, helpful, and persuasive. Possible scores on the entire instrument ranged from a low score of 12 to a high score of 66. Higher scores on the instrument indicated greater message effectiveness due to feelings of being persuaded, motivated, and the likelihood of responding to the message recommendations. Minor adjustments to the scales’ items were made by adapting them to an STI and protection content. For example, items that previously stated, “I believe the pamphlet is persuasive” and “I am likely to follow the recommendations in the brochure” were altered to read, “I believe the STI message provided is persuasive” and “I am likely to follow the recommendations in the STI message provided.” The adapted scale for this study produced an alpha coefficient of .81. Source and channel. The second section of the survey focused on specific sources and channels for health campaign messages. Two separate lists of channels and sources were created to represent an exhaustive possibility of options for the specific campus represented in the study. 329 Marian L. Houser, Michael E. Burns, and Nicholas R. Driver Based on previous studies’ procedures (Rimal et al., 1999; Witte, 1994) and information provided by Ohio University’s Department of Health Education and Wellness, participants were asked to rank order the nine source options and seven channel options. To assist college and university health campaigns hoping to educate the greatest number of students on STI risk and prevention, the third survey section incorporated open-ended questions. Following each list of rank-ordered source and channel, students were asked to justify their first and last choices. Results Hypothesis one predicted differential levels in student perceptions of effectiveness for fearful, informative, and humorous STI messages. A one-way ANOVA was conducted and significant differences were discovered in STI message effectiveness for the three messages (F(2,147) =19.83, p<.001). Follow-up post-hoc analysis (LSD) revealed the fearful message (M=52.70, SD=6.84) was found to be significantly more effective (p<.001) than the humorous message (M=44.18, SD=7.52) and the informative (p<.05) message (M=49.68, SD = 6.14). In addition, the informative STI message (M=49.68, SD=6.14) was significantly more effective (p<.001) than the humorous message (M=44.18, SD=7.52). Students appear to perceive differences in the effectiveness of STI messages with fearful having the greatest effect. Hypothesis two predicted differential source and channel preferences based on message type. After students rated the effectiveness of their particular STI message (fearful, informative, or humorous), they were asked to rank order the preferred source and channel for receiving this message. Sources were ranked from one to nine with one being the most preferred and nine the least preferred; channels were ranked from one to seven, one being the most preferred and seven the least preferred. Frequency distributions were obtained to discover preferred sources (1=most preferred; 9=least preferred) and channels (1=most preferred; 7=least preferred). Examining mean scores (see Table 1) for each enabled researchers to determine which sources and channels were preferred by the participants for each message type. A Kruskal-Wallis test was conducted on this ordinal data to compare the preferred channel, or location, for receiving STI information when presented with fearful, informative, or humorous message types. Significant results were found for the dorm lobby, large lecture class, and health fair (see Table 1). Preferred message types significantly differed for the dorm lobby (H(2)=7.45, p<.05) with the informative message receiving the lowest mean ranking of 60.78 or most preferred message, the humorous message averaging 69.13 and the fearful message receiving the highest mean ranking or least preferred message type with M=83.40. In other words, students did not want to hear a fearful message about STIs in their dorm lobby. Preferred message type also significantly differed in the large lecture class (H(2)=6.35, p<.05) with the fearful message receiving the lowest mean ranking of 62.39 or most preferred message, the humorous message averaging 67.94 and the informative message receiving the highest mean ranking of 82.55. Students perceived the fearful message to be more effective in the large lecture class. Finally, preferred message type significantly differed in the health fair channel (H(2)=5.87, p<.05) with the informative message receiving the lowest mean ranking of 61.43 or most preferred message type, the fearful message averaging 69.93 and the humorous message receiving the highest mean ranking or least preferred message type with M=81.40. Students prefer straightforward information in the health fair setting. It appears, therefore, that students have differential preferences for the type of STI message received in at least three locations: dorm lobby-informative, large lecture-fearful, and health fair-informative. Communicating About STIs 330 Table 1: Kruskal-Wallis Results for Preferred Channel and Source of Fearful, Informative, and Humorous STI Messages ______________________________________________________________________________ Fearful Informative Humorous ______________________________________________________________________________ CHANNEL M SD Mean M SD Mean M SD Mean Rank Rank Rank ______________________________________________________________________________ Dorm Lobby** 3.89 1.92 83.40* 4.61 1.98 60.78 3.49 2.13 69.13 One-On-One 4.81 2.51 64.37 4.63 2.27 78.16* 5.29 2.38 70.34 Orientation 3.92 2.20 70.76 3.96 2.17 72.27* 4.02 1.91 69.99 Large Lecture** 3.27 1.36 62.39 3.13 1.44 82.55* 3.89 1.63 67.94 Small Class 3.52 1.65 62.42 3.17 1.66 80.19* 3.98 2.01 70.22 Health Fair** 4.54 1.99 69.93 4.00 1.96 61.43 3.58 1.91 81.40* Voluntary Info. 4.15 1.87 79.73* 4.48 1.88 61.52 3.79 1.46 71.92 Session SOURCE Medical Doctor 2.02 2.44 69.88 1.76 1.69 74.51* 2.53 2.89 68.64 Peer Educator 3.38 1.77 63.96 3.11 1.61 73.04* 4.11 2.34 69.40 Health Center 3.56 1.61 62.38 3.15 1.58 76.74* 3.91 1.97 73.64 Guest Lecturer 4.63 1.89 77.63* 5.11 2.05 66.77 4.62 1.70 68.79 Professor 4.92 2.21 74.63* 5.37 2.10 73.04 5.28 1.89 65.52 Hall Director 6.38 1.77 72.41* 6.54 1.55 71.34 6.45 1.80 69.31 Posters 6.63 2.32 76.49 6.55 2.39 58.82 5.29 2.87 77.67* Orientation Leader 6.65 1.83 74.84* 6.91 2.23 71.73 6.55 2.58 66.60 Resident Advisor** 6.79 2.10 68.87 6.54 1.55 65.69 6.26 2.05 78.24* ______________________________________________________________________________ * Indicates highest mean ranking within message type; ** Significant at p< .05 A Kruskal-Wallis test was also conducted on the ordinal data to compare the preferred source for receiving fearful, informative, or humorous messages about STIs. Only one significant result (see Table 1) was found for posters around campus (H(2)=6.49, p<.05), with the informative message type receiving the lowest mean ranking or most preferred message type with M=58.82, the fearful message receiving an average ranking of 76.49 and the humorous message type receiving M=77.67. As message type only significantly varied with the posters around campus source, it appears fearful, humorous, or informative messages are perceived acceptable to be delivered by most all sources provided. Research question one produced qualitative data based on the respondents’ justifications for their most and least preferred sources and channels according to message type received. A thematic analysis (Strauss & Corbin, 1998) was undertaken in order to code common themes students provided for the two source and channel rankings. The three authors in the current study separately coded themes emerging from participant responses using a consistent comparison method. As the themes emerged from the data, labels were assigned (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). Once separate themes were designated, a comparative analysis was conducted and a .86 intercoder reliability for themes of source and channel was achieved. Qualitative results revealed similar justifications for all three messages presented in the experiment. Three common themes 331 Marian L. Houser, Michael E. Burns, and Nicholas R. Driver appeared when justifying source: Comfort Level (“I hate my dorm’s resident assistant and I don’t want to talk to her about sex.”), Credibility Concerns (“Doctors or someone else certified are educated and more credible sources.”), and Exposure (“Posters will catch people’s attention and will reach a lot more people.”). Four common themes resulted when evaluating channels: Comfort Level with Location Size (less embarrassment in large vs. small classroom groups), Comfort Level with Others (being with familiar individuals decreases involvement), Familiarity of Channel (greater comfort with information provided in familiar surroundings), and Exposure Capabilities (information presented in environments requiring attendance). Discussion Protection motivation theory (Rogers, 1975, 1983) was used as a lens to help explain college students’ perceptions of STI messages, sources, and channels. PMT contends individuals must perceive something as risky or harmful to be motivated to protect oneself. Rogers asserted that this motivation to protect is behavior inducing. In this case, college students exhibited a preference for receiving fearful, humorous, or informative STI messages via specific sources and channels. Their expressed preferences were based on perceptions that they could successfully engage in communication (e.g., talking to a friend or peer counselor) or the behavior (e.g., visit the health center, attend a campus voluntary information session) to get the required information (self-efficacy) and the likelihood they would follow the message recommendations from the preferred source and channel (response efficacy). Recognizing the rising incidences of STIs for students age 25 and younger (Bacchus Network, 2006; Summerfield & Steinhoff, 1996) it is imperative they are not only alerted to STI issues and concerns, but also offered possible attitudinal and behavioral options. The first hypothesis investigated the message type college students would see as the most effective (fearful, informative, or humorous) for receiving information about STIs and with which they would feel most capable following recommendations. Results indicated that the fearful message was seen as the most effective. The informative message scored a close second in level of effectiveness and the humorous message was perceived as least effective. This appears to reflect the findings of Witte (1994) and Sprinkle et al., (2006) who reported threatening messages combined with alternative solutions allows individuals to feel in control. In this case students who read the fearful message were also told where to go to get assistance or more information and when asked whether they would do so, responded they felt persuaded and it was likely. As PMT suggests (Roger, 1975, 1983) perceived risk is a motivating factor especially when delivered via a fearful message combined with message self-efficacy. What was less expected was for the informative message to be perceived as more effective in relaying STI information than the humorous message. This result may be supported by Beaudoin’s research (2002) with humor and fear in antismoking ads. He reported enhanced effectiveness with humor in youth-oriented advertising and fear working best for adult-oriented advertising. With the topic of STIs, college students may be considered young adults, as they are typically sexually active and engage in more risky sexual behaviors (Abdullah, et al., 2003; Cheah, 2006; Meekers & Klein, 2002). In addition, this result is in line with Booker’s (1981) study reporting more positive effects for straightforward information and humor messages. Ultimately, these results appear to indicate that although fearful messages were perceived as most effective it may be prudent to blend the use of fearful with informative STI messages. A mix of channels increases the Communicating About STIs 332 exposure of the messages by giving students options on how they would like to receive the information (Metzler, Weiskotten, & Morgen, 2000). The results of hypothesis one are also supported by the findings for hypothesis two where, in most cases, little mean differences were discovered for preferred message type with specific sources and channels. The effectiveness of humorous messages, however, was consistently perceived to be less effective, indicating it should not be considered the sole message type to use in conveying STI warnings to college students. Prevention educators should consider this finding when developing STI campaigns. The second hypothesis predicted differential source and channel preference for the three STI messages. Students reported differential message preferences for some locations or channels. In the two larger locations, health fair and dorm lobby, students preferred to receive STI education in a straightforward, informative manner. However, in the large lecture classroom, they perceived fearful messages would be effective. Though this result is somewhat curious, the humorous message type received a very similar mean ranking, indicating, perhaps, that students desire or expect professors in their large lecture courses (100-300 students) to offer a more dramatic presentational style by incorporating humor or even threats. These results also appear to confirm Cheah’s (2006) focus group findings where students indicated an overall desire for a variety of channels to receive STI information. With student responses in the present study, it appears they visualize some benefits of each message type depending on the particular location. In regard to specific sources, however, students did not appear to have a strong preference for message type presented. This is essentially good news for anyone selected to present STI information on campuses as students have no clear expectations of individuals offering the message. Campus posters were the only source students appeared to perceive needing a more informative approach. Within this source, the significant ranking difference between informative, and humorous and fearful is important as campus health officials frequently place signs around campus to warn students about STIs. The preference for posters disseminating informative messages lends support to Cheah’s (2006) findings that students preferred to anonymously receive the information on campus via posters and pamphlets in “community” bags. Ultimately, however, Cheah reported they expressed an overall desire to avoid STI discussions. With the desire for anonymity, it is important to note the value placed on credibility as discovered in the qualitative portion of the study. This student sample does not want to see jokes made or threats delivered about various sexually transmitted infections. When they read it on a poster they want the facts. They view this as more credible. This could be related to their desire to feel comfortable when receiving STI messages (Cheah, 2006). According to these results they prefer a straightforward presentation combined with some fear tactics. Earlier research by Booker (1981) reported more positive effects for the straightforward message in specific and public contexts. In this environment, humor may simply seem inappropriate and messages less credible when used to communicate STI prevention and risk messages. More specifically, in regard to credibility of valued sources in the open-ended responses, participants revealed they would believe someone who had specific education or certification in this area, such as a doctor, certified peer health educator or health center official. These results suggest campuses should consider highly credible sources such as these when communicating with students about STIs because the messages will be perceived as more effective with students more likely to seek out information and assistance. Comfort level with source was also a concern for participants. Many explained that people like orientation advisors are sources they barely 333 Marian L. Houser, Michael E. Burns, and Nicholas R. Driver know and they need more time to get to know someone before talking about such personal information. As previous studies have indicated (Cheah, 2006) college students preferred not to discuss their sexual health, but a direct link exists between information seeking and safer-sex behavior (Afifi & Weiner, 2006). PMT presents the pivotal role of efficacy, making it imperative for students to understand the risk and realize their behavioral options. Colleges and universities, therefore, need to determine the appropriate time for a source to discuss these types of messages. A relationship needs to be developed first before using what students feel might be a less credible source. The final qualitative theme, exposure, was primarily linked to posters as a source. Many students thought posters would work if they were linked to other sources like peer educators or utilized to advertise not only the message but an event where more information would be provided—again, linking the message to perceived self-efficacy and response efficacy. Student rankings for preferred channel also produced interesting results as students provided open-ended responses to justify selections. In regard to a concern for their own comfort level, students made statements like, In a large group it wouldn’t be embarrassing to hear, but in a small classroom I wouldn’t feel comfortable with just a few people. You see these people you live with everyday and would be more reluctant to ask questions. With channel familiarity and exposure capabilities, typical responses were statements such as, I would feel most comfortable in a surrounding I am most at home in like my dorm lobby. Every student is expected to go to orientation therefore every student will have to hear STI information. The qualitative results concurred with those in previous studies (Metzler et al., 2000; Perreira et al., 2002) that, overall, students prefer a variety of channels. There was great variety in locations students perceived they would feel comfortable, thus using many channels to increase exposure could result in student attentiveness and understanding. Limitations and Conclusions The importance of STI education is supported by the growing number of students at risk and the abundance of studies regarding the issue (Summerfield & Steinhoff, 1996; Synovitz et al., 2002). Focusing on education and prevention can be very beneficial to anyone whether or not they are sexually active. However, though the importance of education is recognized by the research literature, the current study did not ask participants if they felt STIs were a problem on their campus or if they felt a need for more education about STIs and prevention. Students were asked to evaluate a hypothetical message in a particular context. It is possible, if they have no concern for STIs, that imagining such a scenario was difficult or unrealistic. In addition, several messages for each message type could have been used in order to avoid any confusion students may have had based simply on the message content rather than type. A final limitation, as with many studies, was sample size. More participants will only make a study more generalizable and expanding the number of participants above 150 could produce stronger conclusions. The results of this study open the door to future endeavors that would help researchers understand what students see as the most effective modes of education about STIs. Universities and colleges could use these findings as a pilot study for the internal development of campus health campaigns. Future research could test the effectiveness of mixed message campaigns as well as the effectiveness of the sources and channels used to send them. A pre-test / post-test measuring students’ STI knowledge and responses toward STI messages might also be a Communicating About STIs 334 profitable future investigation to better determine if the campaigns are successfully educating them. Though this quasi-experiment was specific to the university in the study, it is important to note that the survey instrument was designed to be easily adapted to any campus and their available resources. Overall, it can provide a starting point for university officials when trying to develop educational programs or health campaigns about STIs on campus. The conclusions revealed significant differences in levels of effectiveness with each message type; students felt more comfortable and capable of taking action with messages from specific sources and in particular contexts. Using informative and fearful message types in a blended manner could prove most effective as well as educational. The humorous message may be most helpful in a secondary, supportive role with them. The qualitative data may, itself, serve as an educational tool as it revealed participants saw some importance in receiving an STI education via specific channels and sources. Protection motivation theory (Rogers, 1975, 1983) provides a lens to help university sexual health educators understand how to frame and present STI education and prevention materials to appeal to the greatest number of college students. 335 Marian L. Houser, Michael E. Burns, and Nicholas R. Driver References Abdullah, A. S. M., Fielding, R., & Hedley, A. J. (2003). Understanding sexual risk taking behaviour in Hong Kong university students: A health promotion perspective. Preventive Medicine, 37, 311-318. Afifi, W. A., & Weiner, J. L. (2004). Toward a theory of motivated information management. Communication Theory, 14, 167-190. Afifi, W. A., & Weiner, J. L. (2006). Seeking information about sexual health: Applying the theory of motivated information management. Human Communication Research, 32, 35-57. Anderson, L. A., & Wiston, S. C. (2005). Sexual assault education programs: A metaanalytic examination of their effectiveness. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29, 374388. ASHA (1998, December). Sexually transmitted diseases in America: How many cases and at what cost. Retrieved December 6, 2006 from the American Social Health Association Web site: http://www.ashastd.org/learn/learn_statistics.cfm. Atkin, C. K., & Freimuth, V. (1989). Formative research in campaign design. In R. E. Rice & C. K. Atkin (Eds.), Public communication campaigns (pp. 131-150). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Bacchus Network. (2002). STI fast stats. Retrieved December 5, 2006 from http://www.smartersex.org/stis/sti_stats.asp Beaudoin, C. E. (2002). Exploring antismoking ads: Appeals, themes, and consequences. Journal of Health Communication, 7, 123-137. Block, L. G., & Keller, P. A. (1995). When to accentuate the negative: The effects of perceived efficacy and message framing on intentions to perform a health-related behavior. Journal of Marketing Research, 32(2), 192-203. Booker, G. W. (1981). A comparison of the persuasive effects of mild humor and mild fear appeals. Journal of Advertising, 10, 29-40. Bull, S. S., Cohen, J., Ortiz, C., & Evans, T. (2002). The POWER campaign for promotion of female and male condoms: Audience research and campaign development. Health Communication, 14, 475-491. Center for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). (2000). Tracking the hidden epidemics: Trends in STDs in the United States. Atlanta, GA: CDC. Center for Disease Control and Prevention. (2002). 2002 National STD Surveillance Report. Retrieved December 5, 2006, from http://www.cdc.gov/std/stats02/natoverview.htm Center for Disease Control and Prevention. (2006). Sexually Transmitted Diseases: Genital Herpes Fact Sheet. Retrieved December 5, 2006, from http://www.cdc.gov/std/Herpes/STDFact-Herpes.htm#common Cheah, W. H. (2006). College students’ perspectives in sexually transmitted infections (STIs) in Malaysia, Singapore, England, and the US: Formative evaluation insights and implications. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research, 35, 219-233. Chebat, J. C., Filiatrault, P., & Perrien, J. (1990). Limits of credibility: The case of political persuasion. Journal of Social Psychology, 130(2), 157-167, Communicating About STIs 336 Davis, W. (2006, September 19). Students more likely webheads than TV addicts. MediaPostPublications. Retrieved March 23, 2007, from http://publications.mediapost.com/index.cfm?fuseaction=Articles.san&s=48322&Nid=23 494&p=344806 Fields, C. D. (2002). Sexual responsibility on campus. Black Issues in Higher Education, 18, 18-24. Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory; Strategies for qualitative inquiry. Chicago, IL: Aldine. Greenberg, B. S., & Miller, G. R. (1966). The effects of low-credible sources on message acceptance. Speech Monographs, 33, 127-136. Greenberg, B. S., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1961). The effects of bylines on attitude change. Journalism Quarterly, 38, 535-537. Guttmacher Institute. (1994). Sex and America’s Teenagers. New York: Alan Guttmacher Institute. Hastings, G., Stead, M., Webb, J. (2004). Fear appeals in social marketing: Strategic and ethical reasons for concern. Psychology & Marketing, 21(11), 961-986. Keller, P. A., & Block, L. G. (1997). Vividness effects: A resource-matching perspective. JCR, 24, 295-304. Keyton, J. (Ed.). (2006). Communication research: Asking questions, finding answers. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. Ku, L., St. Louis, M., Farshy, C., Aral, S., Turner, C. F., Lindberg, L. D., et al. (2002). Risk behaviors, medical care, and Chlamydial infection among young men in the United States. American Journal of Public Health, 92(7), 1140-1143. Marin, G., & Marin, B. V. (1991). Research with Hispanic populations. In Applied social research methods series (Vol. 23). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Marshall, A. A., Smith, S. W., & McKeon, J. K. (1995). Persuading low-income women to engage in mammography screening: Source message, and channel preferences. Health Communication, 7, 283-299. Meekers, D., & Klein, M. (2002). Understanding gender differences in condom use selfefficacy among youth in urban Cameroon. AIDS Education and Prevention, 14(1), 62-72. Metzler, A. E., Weiskotten, D., & Morgen, K. J. (2000). Adolescent HIV prevention: An application of the elaboration likelihood model. Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the American Psychological Association (108th, Washington, DC, August 4-8, 2000). Perreira, K. M., Bailey, P. E., de Bocaletti, E., Hurtado, E., de Villagran, S. R., & Jorge Matute, L. (2002). Increasing awareness of danger signs in pregnancy through community- and clinic-based education in Guatemala. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 6, 19-28. Rimal, R. N., Flora, J. A., & Schooler, C. (1999). Achieving improvements in overall health orientation: Effects of campaign exposure, information seeking, and heath media use. Communication Research, 26, 322-348. Rogers, R. W. (1975). A protection motivation theory of fear appeals and attitude change. Journal of Psychology, 91, 93-114. Rogers, R. W. (1983). Cognitive and physiological processes in fear appeals and attitude change: A revised theory of protection motivation. In J. Cacioppo & R. Petty (Eds.), Social psycho-physiology (pp. 153-176). New York: Guilford. 337 Marian L. Houser, Michael E. Burns, and Nicholas R. Driver Sheer, V. C., & Cline, R. W. (1995). Individual differences in sensation seeking and sexual behavior: Implications for communication intervention for HIV/AIDS prevention among college students. Health Communication, 7, 205-223. Sprinkle, R., Hunt, S., Simonds, C., & Comadena, M. (2006). Fear in the classroom: An examination of teachers’ use of fear appeals and students’ learning outcomes. Communication Education, 55, 389-402. Story, L. (2007, January 29). At last, television ratings go to college. The New York Times. Retrieved March 23, 2007, from http://www.nytimes.com/2007/01/29/business/media/29nielsen.html?ex=1327726800&e n=4592a793a2610aeb&ei=5088&partner=rssnyt&emc=rss Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basis of qualitative research: Techniques and procedures for developing grounded theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Summerfield, C. M., & Steinhoff, D. (1996). Sexuality education program. Wellness Perspectives, 7, 101-115. Synovitz, L., Herber, E., Kelley, R. M., & Carlson, G. (2002). Sexual knowledge of college students in a southern state: Relationship to sexuality education. American Journal of Health Studies, 17, 163-172. Van Haveren, R. A., Blank, W., Bentley, K. (2001). "Lafeneline": promoting sexual health through college radio. Journal of College Counseling, 4, 186-189. Weinstock, H., Berman, S., & Cates, W., Jr. (2004). Sexually transmitted diseases among American youth: Incidence and prevalence estimates, 2000. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 36, 6-10. Witte, K. (1994). Fear control and danger control: A test of extended parallel process model (EPPM). Communication Monographs, 61, 113-135. Youn, S. (2005). Teenagers’ perceptions of online privacy and coping behaviors: A riskbenefit appraisal approach. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 49, 86-110. Communicating About STIs 338 Appendix Fearful Message Since 1 in 4 students at _____________ (insert name of school) are infected with sexually transmitted infections, that means: • 8 students in your ________ (insert class name) are infected. • 12 students riding on the bus are infected. • 100 students in your large lecture are infected. • 7000 students at ________ (insert name of school) are infected. Scared? You should be. Are you protected? To get more information about Sexually Transmitted Infections (STI’s) contact the __________________ (insert name of student health center and phone number) or make a visit Monday, Wednesday, or Friday from 8 A.M-5P.M or Tuesday and Thursday between 9 A.M. and 6 P.M. Informative Message Sexually Transmitted Infections (STI’s) are one of the top health issues concerning people ages 18-25. STI’s can be transmitted from one individual to another through sexual activity such as vaginal, oral, or anal intercourse as well as digital manipulation. The most effective way to reduce transmission is the use of latex protection. The only protection that is 100 percent is abstinence. To get more information about Sexually Transmitted Infections (STI’s) contact the ____________________________ (insert name of student health center and phone number) or make a visit Monday, Wednesday, or Friday from 8 A.M-5P.M or Tuesday and Thursday between 9 A.M. and 6 P.M. Humorous Message Bag It Before You Shag It Wrap It Before You Tap It Don’t Be Silly, Protect Your Willy However you want to say it, protect yourself from Sexually Transmitted Infections (STI’s). But like they say, practice makes perfect; so next time your at the grocery store pick up an extra banana. To get more information about Sexually Transmitted Infections (STI’s) contact the ______________________ (insert name of student health center and phone number) or make a visit Monday, Wednesday, or Friday from 8 A.M-5P.M or Tuesday and Thursday between 9 A.M. and 6 P.M. Human Communication. A Publication of the Pacific and Asian Communication Association. Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 339 – 356. Audience Reactions to Negative Campaigns Spots in the 2005 German National Elections: The Case of Two Ads Called “The Ball” Jürgen Maier University of Kaiserslautern Michaela Maier University of Koblenz-Landau Jürgen Maier, Faculty of Social Sciences, Department of Methods of Empirical Social Research, University of Kaiserslautern; Michaela Maier, Faculty of Psychology, Institute of Communication Psychology, Media Pedagogy, and Speech, University of Koblenz-Landau. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jürgen Maier, Department of Methods of Empirical Social Research, University of Kaiserslautern, Pfaffenbergstrasse, Building 6, 67663 Kaiserslautern, Germany. E-mail: maier@sowi.uni-kl.de AUDIENCE REACTIONS TO NEGATIVE CAMPAIGNS SPOTS 340 Abstract For the 2005 German national elections the Social Democratic Party (SPD) and the Christian Democrats (CDU/CSU) each produced a TV-ad called “The Ball” (“Die Kugel”). Both ads were unusual for German campaigns for two reasons: they explicitly attacked the political opponent, and one ad referred to the other. The question this paper focuses on is: How did German voters react to these negative spots. In order to test the impact of the spots, an experiment was conducted with 51 citizens using written pretest- and posttestquestionnaires as well as real-time response measurement. The major finding of this paper is that both spots received negative evaluations; they were not able to damage the image of the political opponent but instead led to a negative backlash for the own party respectively the own candidate. 341 Jürgen Maier and Michaela Maier Introduction After the senior partner of the governing coalition in Berlin, the Social Democratic Party (SPD), had lost nine state elections between 2003 and 2005, Chancellor Gerhard Schröder announced on May 22nd, 2005, that he was seeking a re-election of the German national parliament (“Bundestag”) in order to receive a new electoral mandate for his politics. On July 1st, he asked the parliament for a vote of confidence (“Vertrauensfrage”) – the only (but a highly controversial) possibility provided by the German constitution to call for early elections ahead of schedule. As intended, Schröder lost the role-call vote; only 151 of 600 representatives voted for him. Three weeks later, Federal President Horst Köhler announced that the early election would be held on September 18th, 2005. As demanded by the constitution, a very short election campaign of a maximum of 60 days followed the dissolvement of the parliament. Although the parties already started to campaign on the day of Köhler’s decision on July 21st, last doubts about the scheduling of the election day were not resolved until the decision of the Federal Constitutional Court on August 25th (i.e. 25 days before election day), dismissing an action of two members of the parliament. But aside from the tight schedule and the fact that for the very first time a woman and an East German politician in the person of Angela Merkel was running for chancellor, the course of the campaign was quite normal and included the traditional spectrum of political advertisement (i.e. interpersonal communication with voters in public places or public events, posters, television advertising) as well as one televised debate. In order to promote their top candidates, the campaign teams of the two major parties, the SPD and the Christian Democrats (CDU/CSU), each produced an ad called “The Ball” (“Die Kugel”) in a television format. Both ads were exceptional for German election campaigns. On the one hand, the spots were remarkable because they referred to each other: The SPD spot was a parody of the CDU spot. Interaction between campaign spots is not common in Germany because of the specifics of the television system. In 1984, the broadcasting system was split up into public and private channels. According to a series of decisions by the Federal Constitutional Court, the two public TV channels (ARD and ZDF) have to fulfill the task to inform the public. To do so in the context of elections, public television does not only cover the campaign but also provides free airtime for the campaign spots of every political party allowed to participate in an election. In national and European elections, each channel provides air time for eight spots with a maximum length of 90 seconds for the two major parties CDU and SPD, four spots for all other parties represented in the national parliament, and two spots for small parties not represented in the national parliament (see Holtz-Bacha, 2000). Because the number of free spots is strictly limited, the number of spots produced for the campaigns is very small – usually, the parties produce only a single spot. As a consequence, televised campaign ads do not refer to each other. Parties are also allowed to air campaign spots on private TV channels, where they have to buy commercial time at regular market prices. Because this is very expensive, only the two major parties regularly make use of this opportunity. But in order to keep the costs within reasonable limits, they usually shorten the spots produced for public TV. On the other hand, the two spots were extraordinary for German campaigns because of their degree of negativity. German campaign spots generally have a positive tone. Longterm studies show that criticizing statements or attacks against the political opponent appear only in about three out of ten spots aired in the context of national elections (see HoltzBacha, 2000; Holtz-Bacha & Kaid, 1995). Critical statements are very seldom found in the spots of the two major parties. Between 1957 and 1998, the CDU and SPD expressed critique in only 15 respectively 23 percent of the sequences in their campaign spots. Of course, the number of critical statements was even lower at the times when the CDU or respectively SPD AUDIENCE REACTIONS TO NEGATIVE CAMPAIGNS SPOTS 342 were in government and above average when they were in opposition (see Holtz-Bacha, 2000). In contrast, the CDU version of “The Ball” contains 13 verbal statements, eight of which (62 percent) are critical of or attack the government. The rebuttal of the SPD consists of six verbal statements; five of them (83 percent) were a direct attack on Angela Merkel. The unusualness of both versions of “The Ball” raises the question how voters reacted to these commercials. This paper focuses on the perception, evaluation, and impact of these spots on attitudes towards parties and candidates which are analyzed on the basis of an experimental study conducted with a sample of German citizens. To do this, we start with a brief review of the research on the impact of televised ads in Germany. After a description of the data and the stimuli shown to the participants of our study, we analyze the perception and evaluation of the two spots. Finally, we focus on the impact of the campaign spots on party and candidate evaluation. Research on Televised Ads in German Elections For German voters, televised campaign spots are one of the sources of information used most frequently in the course of election campaigns (see, e.g., Holtz-Bacha, 2000; Kliment, 1994; Schmitt-Beck, 2002; Semetko & Schönbach, 1994). Because campaign spots are broadcasted directly before or after the major evening newscasts as well as in between entertainment programs they usually draw a large audience. Generally speaking, the reception of political television advertising is not related very strongly with voters, demographic or political characteristics (see Holtz-Bacha, 1990). Although there is a greater probability that people with a high level of political interest will watch campaign ads, a major part of the reached audiences consists of voters with low levels of political interest (see Holtz-Bacha, 2000). Müller (2002) shows that about two thirds of the electorate saw at least one spot aired at prime time on public TV during the last four weeks of the 2002 German national election campaign; the average number of spots seen by the voters was 4.4. Compared to other countries like, e.g., the United States, this number is rather small. In fact, it is only about one quarter to one half as many contacts per ad as recommended by American campaign managers in order to have effects on political attitudes (e.g., Filzmaier & Plasser, 2001). As a consequence, in Germany it is unrealistic to expect any great changes of opinions by watching campaigns spots. Maybe this is the reason why most of the German campaign managers think that televised ads are not a very important type of advertising (see Müller, 2002). The low expectations concerning the impact of campaign spots might be an explanation why only few studies have focused on this particular topic within the context of German election campaigns. Research on the effects of televised political advertising in Germany started with a study of Holtz-Bacha (1990) on the European Union parliamentary elections in 1984 and 1989. Based on survey data, her findings suggested that exposure to campaign ads had a substantial positive impact on the attitudes toward the EU as well as toward the European integration process. In addition, voters who frequently watched campaign spots had a more favorable opinion about the campaign than voters who were not exposed to that kind of advertising. Experimental research on the 2004 European Union parliamentary election conducted with a student sample showed that the reception of campaign spots negatively affected the evaluation of the political parties as well as the attitudes toward EU membership (see Esser, Holtz-Bacha, & Lessinger, 2005). In contrast, watching campaign spots had no impact on political involvement, on most of the attitudes toward the EU and the European integration process, as well as on voting intentions. Esser, Holtz-Bacha, and Lessinger (2005) concluded that campaign spots do not change but reinforce political attitudes. Based on an experiment with a citizen sample, Maier and Maier 343 Jürgen Maier and Michaela Maier (2005) observed that watching campaign spots increased the knowledge about the top candidates and changed the perception of the parties’ position toward the European integration process. Although no effects were found on the voters’ agenda of the most important problems, the reasons for voting for a particular party (but not the voting decision itself) changed after the respondents had watched the spots. In addition, Maier and Maier (2005) found that exposure to campaign ads tended to decrease political involvement. In another experiment with a student sample, they showed that especially for politically unaffiliated voters the impact of political ads on interest in the campaign depends on the evaluation of the spots (see Maier & Maier, 2006). In general, the studies on the impact of European election campaign spots made clear that televised political advertising can alter political attitudes but those effects are – all in all – rather small. The very first studies on the impact of televised ads aired during German national elections campaigns were conducted in 1990. Based on a panel study of West German voters, Semetko and Schönbach (1994) found only weak evidence that exposure to televised ads increased interest in the election. In addition, effects of TV spots on attitudes toward the political parties and their top candidates only occurred for the post-communists, the PDS (see Schönbach & Semetko, 1994; Semetko & Schönbach, 1994). In contrast to this, experimental studies with student samples showed that watching campaign ads caused only minor changes of the overall evaluation but had larger effects on the image of the chancellor candidates. The direction and the size of the effects depended on gender as well as on the fact if the students lived in East or West Germany (see Holtz-Bacha & Kaid, 1995; Kaid & Holtz-Bacha, 1993a, 1993b). In addition, Kaid and Holtz-Bacha (1993a) showed that watching televised ads evoked feelings about the spot. Those feelings moderated the impact of the party commercials: While positive feelings, like optimism, or excitement caused positive changes of the candidates’ images, negative feelings, like concernment, resulted in the deterioration of the images of the top candidates. In the context of the 2005 German national election, Kaid and Postelnicu (2006) realized an online experiment with German students who watched either the CDU or the SPD version of “The Ball” or both spots. They showed that the CDU spot had a significant, negative impact on the evaluation of Gerhard Schröder. In contrast, neither watching the SPD ad nor exposure to both spots significantly changed candidate orientations. Kaid and Postelnicu (2006) concluded that the SPD spot obviously cancelled out the negative effect of the CDU ad. In addition, they could not prove an impact of the spots on political cynicism but were able to show that the CDU spot substantially increased internal efficacy. In summary, research has generated some evidence that campaign ads aired in the context of German national elections can affect attitudes toward the top candidates. In addition, the only study published so far on the impact of the two versions of “The Ball” indicates that negative ads seem to work in the intended way, i.e. they damage the image of the political opponent. Data and Stimuli In order to test the impact of televised campaign spots on political attitudes and behavior, on August 31st (i.e. one week after the launch of the campaign ads and 19 days before the election for the German parliament) an experiment was conducted among 51 citizens from Landau, a small city in Rhineland-Palatinate.1 In addition, a control group of 20 1 Recruitement of subjects took place according to a previously set up sampling plan which controlled for sex, age, and education. Whereas men and women were represented almost equally among the participants (49 vs. 51 percent), there were some divergences in reference AUDIENCE REACTIONS TO NEGATIVE CAMPAIGNS SPOTS 344 citizens was recruited. 2 Six campaign spots from the parties represented in the national parliament were shown to the participants of the experiment. The second last spot shown was the CDU version, the last spot the SPD version of “The Ball”. After each spot, the subjects were asked to fill out a short questionnaire regarding their impressions of the particular ad, specific political attitudes, as well as their voting intentions. In addition, before the reception of the very first spot and after the last spot participants had to fill out an extensive questionnaire about their political involvement, their attitudes towards the political parties, the candidates, and the political issues relevant in this campaign, about their voting behavior, media diet, and demographics. Finally, 50 participants were able to place second-by-second responses to the spots during the reception (“real-time response measurement”). To do so, they were provided with 7-point dials with a scale from -3 (“very bad impression of the spot”) to +3 (“very good impression of the spot”) which transmitted the ratings by the respondents to a central computer every second. The content of the CDU spot shown to the participants of the study may be summarized as follows: A man drops a metal billiard ball on a conference table located in a dusky room. The ball rolls over the table, hits several objects like, e.g., pencils and a glass of water, and leaves them in disorder or broken. As the ball moves, a male voice from the off speaks about the poor economic performance of the government during the last seven years. Shortly before the ball falls from the table, Angela Merkel stops it with her hand, talks about leadership and her political goals, and asks for political support in the upcoming election. The spot of the SPD was only provided in the internet and not aired on television. 3 This spot was a rebuttal to the CDU spot and shows a woman juggling with a metal billiard ball similar to the one in the CDU spot. Although the face of the woman is not shown, it is obvious from the clothes that she wears that this woman is supposed to represent Angela to participants’ age (18-29: 43 percent; 30-44: 28 percent; 45-59: 22 percent; 60 and above: 8 percent) and education (elementary education: 10 percent; modern secondary school education: 22 percent; A levels/high school: 55 percent, still in school: 14 percent). In reference to party affiliation, which was not assessed beforehand, it was found that government supporters participated in the survey significantly more often than supporters of the opposition parties (CDU/CSU: 33 percent; SPD: 22 percent; FDP: 0 percent; Bündnis 90/Die Grünen: 26 percent; PDS: 2 percent; no party identification: 14 percent, no answer to this question: 4 percent). All participants applied for the study in response to ads published in the daily newspaper “Die Rheinpfalz” as well as in a free weekly local newspaper and received an incentive of 15 Euros. For further details about the design of the study see Maier, Maier, & Klietsch (2006). 2 The control group was recruited from visitors of a movie theatre, while the experimental study was conducted at the University. The composition of this group is as follows: men: 50 percent; 18-29 years: 70 percent, 30-44 years: 25 percent, 45-59 years: 5 percent, 60 years and older: 0 percent; elementary education: 20 percent; modern secondary school education: 30 percent; A levels/high school: 45 percent, still in school: 5 percent; identification with CDU/CSU: 40 percent, SPD: 15 percent, FDP: 5 percent, Bündnis 90/Die Grünen: 20 percent, Die Linkspartei. PDS: 0 percent, no party identification: 20 percent. None of the differences between experimental and control group are statistically significant on the level of p<.05. All participants of the control group received an incentive of 10 Euros. 3 The SPD spot can still be downloaded (e.g., http://www.spd.de/prjspdwkportal05/mediabrowser/html/tv_spot03.html). 3 The SPD spot can still be downloaded (e.g., http://www.spd.de/prjspdwkportal05/mediabrowser/html/tv_spot03.html). 345 Jürgen Maier and Michaela Maier Merkel. The spot starts when the woman catches the ball falling from the table and begins to toss the ball back and forth between her hands. From the off, a male voice describes Merkel as a flip-flopper, who cannot decide about her political goals. Finally, the woman looses control over the ball, it falls down and the voice states that Merkel is not able to decide what to do, but the voters can. Perception of the Spots In the sample of West German citizens, both versions of “The Ball” left unfavorable impressions. 4 Immediately after having watched each spot, the participants of the study were asked how they liked the particular ad. Most people did not like them: 26 percent enjoyed the CDU spot and 38 percent disapproved of it. On average, the spot received a negative rating of -.20 on a 5-point scale from -2 (“did not like the spot at all”) to +2 (“liked the spot very much”). The evaluation of the SPD ad was, on average, even worse. 38 percent rated this spot positively, but 50 percent had negative feelings; the average evaluation was -.30. More detailed information about the impressions that both spots evoked was provided by the real-time response measurement applied in this study. As Figure 1 shows, the 1 0,5 0 -0,5 CDU SPD -1 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 Time in seconds Figure 1. Perception of the Spots: Data from Real-time response measurement (7-point scale from -3 to +3). 4 It is unlikely that a larger number of participants of the study had had the chance to develop an attitude toward the spots beforehand because the CDU as well as the SPD ad were largely unknown in the sample. Only 14 percent of the participants of the study had seen the CDU version of “The Ball”. Eight percent had taken a look at the parody of this ad provided on the web site of the SPD. Only one person had watched both spots. AUDIENCE REACTIONS TO NEGATIVE CAMPAIGNS SPOTS 346 evaluation of the CDU as well as the SPD version of “The Ball” continuously dropped during the course of the spot. At the end of the 30-second spot of the SPD, the average rating was .49. The CDU spot, which was twice as long as the SPD ad, showed an almost similar trend up to the moment when Angela Merkel stopped the ball (second 31) and started to talk about what would have to be done in Germany by the future government. After that, the spot evaluation went up and down. The most negative average rating the spot received was -.80. During the last 20 seconds, the spot rating dramatically improved. At its end, the average rating of the spot was -.15. Obviously, the participants of the experiment did not appreciate the attacking parts of the ads, but when the CDU spot started to talk about what would have to be done in the future and the Christian Democrats claimed that they could solve the problems, the average spot evaluation sharply increased. The explanation for this reaction is that there is no culture of negative or comparative advertising in Germany. This is not only true for political advertising but also for regular consumer products for which comparative advertising was forbidden by German law and regarded ethically condemnable until European law allowed it in 1997. 5 First attempts to introduce comparative and therewith negative advertising in Germany only caused negative reactions in the public and thus were abandoned. Having this in mind, it’s easy to understand why the participants in the study did not like this way of advertising. Even if the CDU spot created somewhat more favorable (but on average still negative) impressions toward its end, Table 1 shows that the mean score measured with help of the real-time response technique was almost the same for both spots (-.34 vs. -.31). This result supports the findings of the spot evaluation by the questionnaires. 6 Females perceived both spots more negatively than males. While in the case of the CDU spot, younger participants had the most negative impressions, the SPD received its worst evaluations from older persons. In both cases, subjects with a medium level of education gave the most Table 1: Mean Value of Real-time Response Measurement by Social and Political Groups CDU spot SPD spot N Total sample -.34 -.31 50 Male -.25 -.01 24 Female -.43 -.59 26 18-29 years -.45 .14* 21 30-44 years -.48 -.38 14 45-59 years -.01 -.98 11 60+ years -.25 -.66 4 Low education .12 -.68** 5 Medium education -.58 -1.06 11 High education -.34 -.02 34 Identification SPD or Green Party -1.12*** -.07* 24 Identification CDU/CSU or FDP .96 -.78 16 No/other party identification -.58 -.50 10 Note. Data from Real-time response measurement; 7-point scale from -3 to +3. Level of significance: * p<.1, ** p<.05, *** p<.01. 5 For the European law see EU-Richtlinie 97/55/EG from October 9th 1997. For the German implementation see UWG (2004). 6 The mean values of the real-time responses measurement were strongly correlated with the general spot evaluation reported in the questionnaires. For the CDU the correlation was .78 (p<.001), for the SPD .66 (p<.001). 347 Jürgen Maier and Michaela Maier negative ratings. Finally, spot evaluation heavily depended on party attachment in the expected direction (i.e. supporters of the governing parties had a positive impression of the SPD and a negative impression of the CDU spot, while the evaluations by supporters of the opposition were opposite). Persons without party identification perceived both ads as clearly negative. The all in all negative evaluations of the two ads are not only to be seen as a function of social or political characteristics of the respondents of the study but also a consequence of the strong deviation of the spots from the German notion of an “ideal” campaign spot (Table 2). Such an ideal spot, according to the statements of the test persons, should provide credible and convincing information presented in an interesting (but serious) way. In addition, it should cast a favourable light on politicians. The last thing what a campaign spot in Germany should do is to be provoking, aggressive, or to attack the political opponent. Both, the CDU and the SPD spot, differed significantly from the ideal televised ad on almost all dimensions. In the case of the CDU, the difference was especially large with respect to attacking the political opponent. For the SPD spot (which in sum showed a significantly stronger deviation from the ideal spot than the CDU ad), the most salient differences were the lack of credibility and the high amount of aggression. AUDIENCE REACTIONS TO NEGATIVE CAMPAIGNS SPOTS 348 Table 2:Characteristics of an ideal campaign spot, the CDU spot, and the SPD spot An ideal campaign spot…/ the CDU (SPD) spot…a Ideal spot CDU spot SPD spot .08*** .33*** .90*** .66*** .78*** .60*** .58*** …should be/was credible 1.70 …should be/was convincing 1.51 …should/did provide information 1.50 .02*** …should be/was interesting 1.22 .00*** …should be/was not boring 1.16 …should/did show responsible politicians .96 …should have/had style .88 …should/did show competent politicians .78 …should/did show energetic politicians .38 …should show/showed congenial politicians .36 …should show/showed no pessimism .34 …should/did show appealing pictures .32 .52*** .12*** .16*** .42*** .30*** .53*** .65 …should be/was imaginative …should be/was entertaining …should have/had good music .24 .04 -.04 .22*** -.02 -.14 - …should be/was not worrying -.13 -.42 …should be/was funny -.22 1.18*** …should be/was provoking -.76 .78*** …should be/was aggressive -1.04 .63*** …should/did attack the political opponent -1.14 Mean squared difference to ideal spot N 50 1.24** * 3.92** * 50 .86*** .30*** .84*** .04 .02 .86*** -.04 1.50** * 1.36** * 5.26 50 Note. a 5-point scales from -2 (“does not apply at all”) to +2 (“fully applies”). - = Item was not asked for the SPD spot. Levels of significance: * p<.1, ** p<.05, *** p<.01. 349 Jürgen Maier and Michaela Maier Party identification and the perceived difference of the ads from an ideal spot were the most powerful variables explaining the general spot evaluation (Table 3). In both cases, the impact of the discrepancy between the spots shown to the participants of the study and their notion of an ideal campaign spot was clearly stronger than the effect of their party attachment. All other variables (i.e. sex, age, education) had no statistically significant effects on spot evaluation. Table 3: Determinants of the evaluation of the CDU and the SPD spot CDU spot SPD spot 2 Adjusted R .39 .42 Sex .17 -.16 Age -.06 -.14 Education -.04 .10 Strength of party identification .32** .33*** Mean squared difference to ideal spot -.51*** -.41*** N 50 50 Note. Presented are the results of an OLS-regression analysis, standardized regression coefficients. Levels of significance: * p<.1, ** p<.05, *** p<.01. Impact of the Spots As the final aspect of this paper, the impact of the campaign spots on the attitudes towards parties and candidates will be analyzed. The impact of the spots on the evaluation of the two parties CDU/CSU and SPD was rather small and always insignificant (Table 4). Both the CDU and the SPD spot had slightly negative consequences for the evaluation of the Social Democrats. After having watched both versions of “The Ball”, the evaluation of the Table 4: Impact of the CDU and the SPD spot on party and candidate evaluation CDU spot SPD spot After both spots a SPD Pretest .30 .22 .30 Posttest .22 .12 .12 Difference -.08 -.10 -.18 CDU/CSUa Pretest .26 .18 .26 Posttest .18 .22 .22 Difference -.08 +.04 -.04 Schrödera Pretest .02 .10 .02 Posttest .10 .06 .06 Difference +.08 -.04 +.04 Merkela Pretest .10 -.16 .10 Posttest -.16 -.06 -.06 Difference -.26* +.10 -.16 N 50 50 50 Note. a Scales from -5 (“does not like [party, politician] at all”) to +5 (“like the [party, politician] very much”). Levels of significance: * p<.1, ** p<.05, *** p<.01. AUDIENCE REACTIONS TO NEGATIVE CAMPAIGNS SPOTS 350 SPD dropped about -.18 scale points. In the case of the CDU, the original spot had a small negative impact, while the rebuttal had a small positive effect. After the reception of both spots, the participants had only a slightly more positive feeling about the CDU (+.04). Looking at the evaluations of the two chancellor candidates, the picture of the limited impact of the two spots does not change very much. In the case of Schröder, the CDU spot had a small positive effect, while the SPD spot had a minor negative influence. After subjects had watched both ads, Schröder received slightly better evaluations from the participants of the experiment (+.04). The CDU spot had a significantly negative impact on Merkel, while the SPD had a small positive effect. After having watched both spots, Merkel’s image had suffered (-.16) instead of improved. All in all, it appears that the CDU spot damaged the image of the Christian Democrats and their top candidate Angela Merkel instead of improving her standing before the elections. At the same time, the SPD spot had a negative impact on the Social Democrats as well as on Schröder. Considering this backlash effect, the main intention of both spots – namely to damage the image of the political opponent – did not work very well. Only in one case (the impact of the CDU spot on the evaluation of the SPD), an ad was successful in this respect. Reactions to the campaign spots, of course, were not uniform, but the responses of the participants depended on their spot evaluation. At the same time, the reactions did not always follow a common, clearly visible pattern. Again, there were only few significant effects of the two campaign spots (Table 5): First, for those respondents who liked the CDU spot an Table 5: Impact of the CDU and the SPD spot on party and candidate evaluation by spot evaluation CDU spot SPD spot SPDa Total sample -.08 -.10 Negative evaluation -.09 -.15 Neutral evaluation -.17 -.50 Positive evaluation .00 +.11 CDU/CSUa Total sample -.08 +.04 Negative evaluation -.11 +.03 Neutral evaluation -.11 -.17 Positive evaluation +.23* -.11 a Schröder Total sample +.08 -.04 Negative evaluation +.09 -.05 Neutral evaluation +.06 -.50 Positive evaluation -.08 +.05 Merkela Total sample -.26* +.10 Negative evaluation -.31* .00 Neutral evaluation -.06 +.33 Positive evaluation +.08 +.16 N 50 50 Note. a Scales from -5 (“does not like [party, politician] at all”) to +5 (“like the [party, politician] very much”). Levels of significance: * p<.1, ** p<.05, *** p<.01. increase of +.23 for the sympathy rating for the CDU/CSU occurred. Second, for those who had negative impressions from the CDU ad, a strong backlash effect for Angela Merkel can 351 Jürgen Maier and Michaela Maier be observed (-.31). In addition, there are two interesting relationships between spot evaluation and the direction of their effects: On the one hand, attacking the political opponent generally did not have the intended consequences. In three out of four cases, a positive evaluation of a spot did not lead to poorer evaluation of the attacked party or person. On the other hand, there is a tendency that unfavorably rated televised ads damaged the image of parties and politicians – of those who were target of the attack and of those who produced the spot. Some studies in the field of political communication argue that negative campaign ads increase the voter’s skepticism about the political system and its representatives. In the context of this analysis, this would mean that the acceptance of both parties, respectively both candidates, should suffer from the reception of the spots. In order to measure those effects, an composite measure based on the score of the best-rated party (and in analogy to this, an index based on the score of the best-rated candidate) was created (see, e.g., Rattinger, 1993; Maier, 2000). This indicator is very common in Germany to measure dissatisfaction with political parties. The logic behind this index is that the evaluation of political parties largely depends on individual party attachments. Persons who have a specific party identification usually evaluate “their” party more favorably than other parties. Although those people have negative feelings toward many other parties, they are not dissatisfied with the political parties because they have a clear-cut picture of the political world. Because of the selective perception and processing of information, it would be no surprise if watching campaign spots would increase the positive image of the party a voter is attached to and at the same time lead to more negative evaluations of other parties. If, in contrast to this, the reception of negative campaign spots would be followed by a decline of the created composite measures that would be a strong indicator for political cynicism caused by negative campaign spots. After watching the two versions of “The Ball”, none of the six possible effects analyzed here were statistically significant (Table 6). Nevertheless, after the exposure to the Table 6: Impact of the CDU and the SPD spot on best party/best candidate evaluation CDU spot SPD spot After both spots a Best party evaluation Pretest 2.56 2.44 2.56 Posttest 2.44 2.46 2.46 Difference -.12 +.02 -.10 Best candidate evaluationb Pretest 2.12 2.16 2.12 Posttest 2.16 2.20 2.20 Difference +.04 +.04 +.08 N 50 50 50 Note: a Index based on the best evaluation for CDU/CSU and SPD; index ranges from -5 (“does not like both parties at all”) to +5 (“likes at least one of the parties very much”). b Index based on the best evaluation for Schröder and Merkel; index ranges from -5 (“does not like both candidates at all”) to +5 (“likes at least one of the candidates very much”). Levels of significance: * p<.1, ** p<.05, *** p<.01. CDU spot, the index score dropped somewhat (-.12) ad rose negligibly after the reception of the rebuttal (+.02). In sum, both spots slightly weakened the positive feelings toward the two parties (-.10). In the case of candidate evaluation, the opposite effect occurred. Both spots caused a small, but uniform increase of candidate evaluations (each +.04). In sum, watching the two negative campaign ads strengthened the feelings toward the candidates (+.08). AUDIENCE REACTIONS TO NEGATIVE CAMPAIGNS SPOTS 352 When we, once again, put aside the assumption that all participants of the experiment reacted in the same way to the campaign spots and analyze the effects of the ads by spot evaluation, the effects of the campaign spots on citizens’ satisfaction with the major political parties and the chancellor candidates became stronger. Interestingly, the effects on the index of best party evaluation for the single spots are not very strong and not consistent (Table 7). Table 7: Impact of the CDU and the SPD spot on best party/best candidate evaluation by spot evaluation CDU spot SPD spot After both spotsc a Best party evaluation Total sample -.12 +.02 -.10 Negative evaluation -.37 +.04 -.89 Neutral evaluation -.11 -.17 -.07 Positive evaluation +.23* +.05 +.15* Best candidate evaluationb Total sample +.04 +.04 +.08 Negative evaluation +.05 -.08 -.44 Neutral evaluation .00 +.50 +.29 Positive evaluation +.08 +.05 +.15 N 50 50 50 Note. aIndex includes the best evaluation for CDU/CSU and SPD; index ranges from -5 (“does not like both parties at all”) to +5 (“likes at least one of the parties very much”). b Index includes the best evaluation for Schröder and Merkel; index ranges from -5 (“does not like both candidates at all”) to +5 (“likes at least one of the candidates very much”). cIndex of best spot evaluation, includes the best evaluation for the CDU and the SPD spot; index ranges from -2 (“does not like both spots at all”) to +2 (“likes at least one of the spots very much”). Levels of significance: * p<.1, ** p<.05, *** p<.01. The only exception from this is the impact of the CDU spot: After watching this ad, persons with a positive spot evaluation showed a significant increase of the best party evaluation. In contrast to this, participants who passed a negative sentence about the spot evaluated the party more negatively after than before watching the ad. However, this picture changes considerably if we analyze the impact of the campaign ads after the reception of both spots. For participants who had a positive impression about at least one of the spots, the image of the two major parties as well as of the chancellor candidates somewhat improved (+.15). In the case of the parties, this increase is statistically significant. Those subjects who evaluated both spots negatively also rated parties and candidates more negatively after watching the ads (-.89 and -.44, respectively). Although both effects are not statistically significant, this seems to be a clear indication that negative campaign spots can foster political dissatisfaction. Summary and Conclusion The two campaign spots examined in this paper were exceptional for Germany because of their explicit attacks on the political opponent and the fact that one referred to the other. German campaign spots primarily have a positive valence and due to the small numbers of spots produced and aired in the course of a campaign, rebuttals are unfamiliar to German voters. Thus, the question in focus of this paper was how German voters reacted to these spots. The short answer is: not very favorably. Both spots received, on average, negative evaluations by a sample of German citizens who watched the ads in an experimental 353 Jürgen Maier and Michaela Maier setting. In addition, real-time response measurement showed that negative feelings toward the spots occurred as long as the political opponent was being directly attacked. Although the average ratings of the two spots were negative, the impact of the ads on party and candidate orientations was quite limited and only in few cases statistically significant. Nevertheless, it was obvious that both, the CDU and the SPD spot, were not able to damage the image of the particular political opponent but rather caused a negative backlash on the evaluation of the own party respectively the own candidate. This pattern is, of course, the opposite of the results reported by Kaid and Postelnicu (2006) for young German voters. A differentiation of the impact of the two ads by positive, neutral, and negative spot evaluations made clear that even in the case of positive assessments of the ads, negative campaigning did not work in the intended way (i.e. damaging the image of the political opponent). Moreover, in the case of negative evaluation the ads tended to belittle the reputation of both parties and both chancellor candidates. This observation leads to the question whether negative campaign spots foster dissatisfaction with parties and politicians in general. The results of the simple pretestposttest comparison yielded no effects. Again, after analyzing the effects for those participants who liked the spots and for those who did not, a much clearer pattern emerged – especially if we take a look at the changes of party and candidate evaluations after the reception of both spots. For those persons who liked at least one of the ads, the image of the parties as well as of their top candidates slightly improved. In contrast to this, for those participants who rejected both spots, dissatisfaction with parties and politicians increased. This result also differs from the findings of Kaid and Postelnicu (2006) reporting that the two negative ads did not create political cynicism among young voters. This study (like most of the other studies in this field, too) has, of course, its limitations – limitations which are typical of experimental research. In addition, we have to admit that the subsequent viewing of both versions of “The Ball” was highly unlikely for German voters, as the SPD spot was only available in the internet. Although the reception situation created in this experiment was not very realistic, the results presented in this paper confirm other analyses reporting that campaign spots are able to influence German voters. As the findings of previous studies on the impact of televised advertising in the course of German election campaigns are very mixed, further research on this topic is strongly recommended. AUDIENCE REACTIONS TO NEGATIVE CAMPAIGNS SPOTS 354 References Esser, F., Holtz-Bacha, C., & Lessinger, E.-M. (2005). Sparsam in jeder Hinsicht: Die Fernsehwerbung der Parteien im Europawahlkampf 2004. In C. Holtz-Bacha (Ed.), Europawahl 2004: Die Massenmedien im Europawahlkampf (pp. 65-89). Wiesbaden, Germany: Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften. Filzmaier, P., & Plasser, F. (2001). Wahlkampf um das Weiße Haus: Presidential Elections in den USA. Opladen, Germany: Leske + Budrich. Holtz-Bacha, C. (1990). Nur bei den Wasserwerken Effekte? Eine Studie zur parteipolitischen Spot-Werbung vor Europawahlen. Medium, 3, 50-53. Holtz-Bacha, C. (2000). Wahlwerbung als politische Kultur: Parteienspots im Fernsehen 1957-1998. Wiesbaden, Germany: Westdeutscher Verlag. Kaid, L. L., & Holtz-Bacha, C. (1993a). Audience Reactions to Televised Political Programs: An Experimental Study of the 1990 German National Election. European Journal of Communication, 8, 77-99. Kaid, L. L., & Holtz-Bacha, C. (1993b). Die Beurteilung von Wahlspots im Fernsehen: Ein Experiment mit Teilnehmern in den alten und neuen Bundesländern. In C. HoltzBacha (Ed.), Die Massenmedien im Wahlkampf: Untersuchungen aus dem Wahljahr 1990 (pp. 185-207). Opladen, Germany: Westdeutscher Verlag. Kaid, L. L., & Holtz-Bacha, C. (1995). Television Spots in German National Elections. In L. L. Kaid, & C. Holtz-Bacha (Eds.), Political Advertising in Western Democracies: Parties and Candidates on Television (pp. 61-88). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Kaid, L. L., & Postelnicu, M. (2006). The Impact of Television Advertising in the 2005 German National Elections. Paper presented at the Midwest Political Science Association Convention, Chicago, IL. Kliment, T. (1994). Orientierung im Wahlkampf oder nur Propaganda? Wahlwerbespots im Urteil der Bevölkerung – eine Repräsentativumfrage in Hessen. Media Perspektiven, 8, 419-427. Maier, J. (2000). Politikverdrossenheit in der Bundesrepublik Deutschland: Dimensionen, Determinanten, Konsequenzen. Opladen, Germany: Leske + Budrich. Maier, J., & Maier, M. (in press). Does the Reception of European Election Campaign Spots Foster Political Involvement? Results of an Experimental Study with Young German Voters. In L. L. Kaid (Ed.), The Expansion Election: Communicating Shared Sovereignty in the 2004 European Parliamentary Elections. New York: Peter Lang. Maier, J., Maier, M., & Klietsch, K. (2006). Inhalt und Wahrnehmung von Wahlwerbesendungen der im Bundestag vertretenen Parteien zur Bundestagswahl 2005 – eine Dokumentation (Kaiserslauterer Beiträge zur Sozialforschung, Nr. 2). Kaiserslautern, Germany: University of Kaiserslautern, Department of Methods of Empirical Social Research. Maier, M., & Maier, J. (2005). Nebensache Europa: Parteienspots zur Europawahl 2004 und ihre Wirkung – Ergebnisse einer Experimentalstudie. In J. Tenscher (Ed.), WahlKampf um Europa: Analysen aus Anlass der Wahlen zum Europäischen Parlament 2004 (pp. 118-135). Wiesbaden, Germany: Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften. Müller, D. K. (2002). ARD und ZDF als Werbeträger nach 20.00 Uhr: Wahlwerbung im Fernsehen. Media Perspektiven, 12, 623-628. Müller, M. G. (2002). Parteienwerbung im Bundestagswahlkampf 2002: Eine qualitative Analyse politischer Werbung und PR. Media Perspektiven, 12, 629-638. Rattinger, H. (1993). Abkehr von den Parteien? Dimensionen der Parteiverdrossenheit. Aus Politik und Zeitgeschichte, B 11, 24-35. 355 Jürgen Maier and Michaela Maier Schönbach, K., & Semetko, H. A. (1994). Medienberichterstattung und Parteienwerbung im Bundestagswahlkampf 1990. Media Perspektiven, 7, 328-340. Semetko, H. A., & Schönbach, K. (1994). Germany’s “Unity Election“: Voters and the Media. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. UWG (2004). Gesetz gegen den unlauteren Wettbewerb. Bundesgesetzblatt Jg. 2004, Teil I, Nr. 32, Bonn, 7.7.2004. Human Communication. A Publication of the Pacific and Asian Communication Association. Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 357 – 386. A Comparison of Role Models Among Japanese, Korean and Chinese University Students Masao Nakabayashi Former Kansai Gaidai College Yumi Hasegawa Daejin University Yang Yi Kansai Gaidai University A Comparison of Role Models 358 Abstract The purpose of this paper is to clarify the common and different views concerning role models respected by university students in Japan, Korea and China, based on the survey of questionnaires. This paper reports the analysis of one question regarding the respected role models, “Write the names of three people you respect in the order that you respect.” The results were compared among the countries and derived some interesting tendencies. Japanese students respected “present Japanese (e.g., Ichiro),” while Korean and Chinese students respected “historical people (e.g., Lee, Sun-Shin).” From the viewpoint of Confucianism, many different factors were found and some suggestions were described based on the detailed analysis. 359 Masao Nakabayashi, Yumi Hasegawa, and Yang Yi Introduction The aim of this paper is to clarify the common and different views concerning role models (public figures) respected by university students in three Asian countries (Japan, Korea and China), based on the survey of questionnaires. A modern society is said to have multiple and diversified values. As this term suggests, it is pointed out that the variation in living environments has weakened human traditional values and world-view and also the bondage with life styles. On the other hand, there is a weak point that this variation may make people destroy the conviction toward their own value as well as life styles and useful life. In short, in a society putting much emphasis on individualization and diversification, there is a fear that people might lack common ideal, values and norms. How do those three countries once having had a common cultural background evaluate the feature of multiple values? Generally speaking, when the values of the three countries are compared as an attitude toward seniors, Chinese and Korean people put more emphasis on duty to their parents while the Japanese highly value loyalty. In other words, from the senior author’s studies, it is a common view that “owing to the effect of Confucianism, Chinese and Koreans continue to have dutiful spirits, while the Japanese have maintained a loyalty spirit since the feudal period, both of which fundamentally have never changed.” The respondents were 162 Japanese, 102 Korean and 100 Chinese university students, totaling 364 students. Two questions were asked: write the names of three people you respect in the order that you respect the most and is either of your parents included among them? Tell the reasons why. If the result of this survey concerning respected people by the students in the three countries is reviewed, is this common view appropriate? The category is explained at the beginning, respected people by students of the three countries are broken down, and the features of every country are compared. Finally, the reasons for the common and different views and their background will be reviewed and analyzed. This paper is the analysis of two questions concerning the respected role models to them. All the data obtained were carefully analyzed and examined from the viewpoints of their values. Finally, the results were compared among the three countries and derived some interesting tendencies. For instance, Japanese students respected “present Japanese,” while Korean and Chinese students respected “historical people.” From the viewpoint of Confucianism, many different factors were found and some suggestions were described based on the detailed analysis. Method The questionnaires consist of fifteen questions, which were translated into Korean and Chinese from a Japanese text (Appendix 1). The questions adopted for this paper are only No. A Comparison of Role Models 360 6 as follow. 6. Please write the names of three people you respect in the order that you respect the most. (1) First (2) Second (3) Third Examinees are listed below and the survey in Korea was carried out in March 2005, in Japan in April 2005 and finally in China in November 2005. Japanese students: Kansai Gaidai College = 49 Kwansei Gakuin University = 113 Total= 162 students Korean students: Daejin University = 102 students Total= 102 students Chinese students: Guanxi Normal University = 50 Nanjing University = 50 Total= 100 students Grand Total = 364 students Note: Most students are freshmen and sophomore. Junior and senior are a few. Categories and Results The detailed results of this survey are shown in the Appendix 2 as follows and the graphs of comparison are shown in the middle of this paper. Table 5: Summary of Role Models Respected by Students in the Three Countries (Appendix2) Table 6: Respected Role Models in Japan (Appendix 2) Table 7: Respected Role Models in Korea (Appendix 2) Table 8: Respected Role Models in China (Appendix 2) Based on the analysis, the outline of role modes for each country is described and a comparison among the three countries is made to highlight some different points. As to the question No.6, the students in the three countries were requested to write three people from first to third. However, some students except Korean students named more or less than three people. Actually Japanese students named 488 people, Korean students chose exactly 306 people and Chinese students named 301 people. As many as 1,095 people were named and classified by groups in order to understand which kinds of role models are respected by the students. 361 Masao Nakabayashi, Yumi Hasegawa, and Yang Yi Categories Concerning the category, the following procedure was carried out. First, the large group was divided into two groups; Group 1 in Table 1 and Group 2 in Table 2. The former is the group consisting of individuals, families, intimate friends, and no answers. The role models in this group (excluding unclear or no answer) are mainly those persons that students know very well through their experiences. Therefore, those persons become respected persons for students after having met and/or talked with them. Table 1: Category Group 1 family with blood relation = f former teacher or present teacher = t personal acquaintance = k oneself = m no answer = z Answered but unclear = x On the contrary, those role models in Table 2 are the group of people who are difficult or impossible for students to have a chance to directly meet and/or talk with. It can be said that those persons become respected figures after students come to know them as the result of seeing them through the media such as books, newspapers, televisions and/or the Internet, or being informed of them by other people. Further, the role models in Group 2 are divided into three sub-categories. The first category is alive or dead, in other words, contemporary or past persons. The next one is nationality. This category clarifies whether they are native people or non-native people (foreigner). Finally, the third group is the main occupation and contribution of those role models. However, there are some people that have multiple contributions, for example, a person that was once famous as an activist (active in politics) became a politician. Some may want to respect their growth itself. This time, however, the main contributions of respected people were categorized. For example, for Japanese, Ichiro, a Japanese baseball player, belongs to pr-na-4, because he is a contemporary baseball player who is very active in the Major League. In other words, he is a contemporary native (Japanese) baseball player. Further, in the case of Korea, Lee, Sun-Shin is a historical native person (Korean) classified in pa-na-9. Lee, Sun-Shin was a famous general who defeated Japanese navy in 1592 A Comparison of Role Models 362 Table 2 Category Group 2 First Category(alive) pa = historical people pr = present people px = unclear Second (Nationality) na = native nn = on-native nx = unclear Third Category (Occupation) 1= scholar 2= politicians 3= activist 4= sports player, athlete 5= national symbol, top-leader 6= religionist 7= celebrity, singer, star, comedian 7= artist, novelist, photographer etc. 9= serviceperson, general 10= financier, manager 11= philosopher, thinker, educator 0= others . 3.2 Results – Outline – The Graph 1 to 5 are showing the results of the survey (more details, Table 5-8 in Appendix 2). Graph 1 Ratio of Respected Role Models 40 30 % 20 10 0 Japan Korea China parents/ fam ily 32 33.3 35.5 acquaintance teacher oneself 6.5 2.3 0.7 5.6 2.9 6.3 0 1.6 0.7 363 Masao Nakabayashi, Yumi Hasegawa, and Yang Yi Graph 2 Historical People or Present People? 60 40 % 20 0 Japan Korea China historicalpeople present people 14.3 30.7 46.2 15 8.5 3.7 unclear 0.8 0 1 Graph 3 Native or Non-native? 60 40 % 20 0 Japan Korea China native 16.1 33.7 43.2 non-native 13.4 5.6 6.6 unclear 0.6 0 1 A Comparison of Role Models 364 Graph 4 Historical or Present Native or Non-native 40 30 % 20 10 0 Japan Korea China pa-na 6.5 26.1 40 pa-nn 7.8 4.6 6.3 pr-na 9.6 7.5 3.3 pr-nn 5.1 1 0.3 pa-na = historical people and native pa-nn= historical people and non-native pr-na= present people and native pr-nn= present people and non-native Graph 5 Occupation and Performance of Respected 35 30 25 % 20 15 10 5 0 Japan Korea China C.1 1.6 2 7 C.2 0.7 2.9 31.2 C.3 3.8 3.9 0.3 C.4 6.7 0 0 C.5 2.5 14.4 2.7 Japan C.6 0.4 1 0 Korea C.7 6.3 0.3 0.3 C.8 2.9 1.3 3.7 C.9 1.6 7.8 1.7 C.10 C.11 0.9 1.1 4.2 0.7 0 3.7 C.0 0.4 0.7 0.3 China 3.2.1 Role Models Japanese Students’ Respect – Outline – Those people whom Japanese students respect most are the people with family blood relations (father or mother and parents). These students who respect family blood relatives are nearly 32 % and almost all of them say they respect fathers or mothers and parents. 365 Masao Nakabayashi, Yumi Hasegawa, and Yang Yi Concerning other respected role models, historical people account for 14.3% and present people account for 15 %, which are very close in percentage. For nationality, native people (Japanese) are 16.1%, which is a little bit higher than non-native people 13.4%. Further if historical and present people are added to nationality, respected historical native people (Japanese) are 6.5% and respected historical non-native people are 7.8%, while respected present native people are 9.6% and respected present non-native people are 5.1%. After family blood relatives, the category of role models the Japanese most respect is “present Japanese” which are higher in number. If we take a look at occupations, regardless of the period or place in the world, sports players and athletes are most respected, followed by celebrities, singers, stars and comedians. It is characteristic that they are centered on those people whom students can freely watch their performances through media like television. The respected figures participants named the most except for people they know are “Japanese athletes (pr-na-4) now playing actively” which reached 16 players. For example, Ichiro and Matsui playing in American Major League are nominated because their performances are reported everyday by sports news and their every single batting is watched and paid attention to. It seems that Japanese students cheer and respect those young Japanese players now playing actively and beautifully in foreign countries. Role Models Korean Students’ Respect Those people whom Korean students respect most are also the people with family blood relations. These students comprise nearly 33 % and almost all of them say they respect a father or a mother and parents. Concerning other respected role models, historical people account for 30.7% and present people only 8.5 %, which is a very low percentage. In the case of nationality, native people (Korean) are 33.7%, that is much higher than non-native people 5.6%. Further if historical and present role models are added to nationality, respected historical native people (Korean) are 26.1% and respected historical non-native people are only 4.6%, while respected present native people are 7.5% and respected present non-native people are the lowest (1%). Except for family blood relatives, the object of Korean respect is “Historical Koreans,” and it is an overwhelmingly high number. If we take a look at occupations, the highest number is 14% for national symbols and top leaders, followed by 8% for servicepersons and generals. Other role models are those persons actively working and contributing to the mother country. Role Models Chinese Students’ Respect Those people whom Chinese students respect the most are also the people with family A Comparison of Role Models 366 blood relations, which are the same as Japanese and Korean students. However, the ratio is 35.5%, that is higher than Japanese and Korean. They raised some reasons; parents give lives to them, become fostering benefactors, foster with profound love, greatly influence their morality and value and so on. Concerning other respected role models, historical people are 46.2% and present people are simply 3.7%. As for historical people, China is the highest but concerning present people, China is the lowest among the three countries. Further, if historical and present people are added with nationality, respected historical native people (Chinese) are 40% and respected historical non-native people are only 6.3%, while respected present native people are 3.3% and respected present non-native people are the lowest 0.3%. The object of role models Chinese students respect is “Historical Chinese,” which shows the highest percentage among the three countries, even exceeding Korea. If we take a look at occupations, the highest number is 31.2% for national politicians such as Mao, See-dong, Thou, En-al, and Ding, Ciao-pin. It is noteworthy that sports players, athletes, religionists, leading financiers and managers are less respected than national politicians. Comparison Comparison of the Three Countries (outline) Those people whom all Japanese, Korean and Chinese students respect most are the people with family blood relations with each ratio different. Thirty-two percent of Japanese students respect the people with family blood relations, but 33% of Korean students say they respect them. Compared with the other two countries, Chinese students showed the highest ratio at 35.5%. This may indicate that the family style in the three countries might be different from each other. At this point, a focus is put on historical and present people, or native and non-native people (foreigners). A big difference was found in historical people and present people. In the case of Japanese, both numbers are fifty-fifty, but in the case of Koreans, historical people tremendously surpassed present one. And in the case of Chinese, the ratio exceeded two countries and overwhelmingly historical people are the object of respect. Also concerning native or non-native people, it was found that the object of respect is definitely native people for Korean and Chinese students exceeding Japanese students. In the case of Japanese students, the ratio of present native people (Japanese) and non-native people (foreigners) are higher than Korean and Chinese students. Particularly, respected present non-native people by Japanese students are 5.1%, compared with Chinese students 0.3%. From this data, Japanese students do not show any significant number and only present people seem to be the object of their respect. 367 Masao Nakabayashi, Yumi Hasegawa, and Yang Yi Next, taking a look at occupations and main fields regardless of historical or present people, the result is reviewed. The occupations and fields of role models Japanese students respect are mainly sports players and athletes, including baseball players, soccer players, fighters etc. Then, celebrities, singers, stars and comedians who are actively performing in the media like television, follow the respected people. This trend is the result of mass media, particularly television, through which people can watch their activities of the above two fields after the end of World War II. On the contrary, role models respected by Korean students are national symbols and top leaders, followed by servicepersons and generals. In this case, president and premier are included in top leaders. National symbol and top leaders have a common point to servicepersons and generals, that is related to “politics” and “nation.” We conclude that Korean students tend to respect those persons that have contributed to national politics. The responses of the Chinese students were quite different from the Japanese, but were similar in some respect to the Koreans. Unlike Japanese students, no sports players or athletes are named and only one star is nominated. On the other hands, there are some common aspects to Korean. In other words, national politics and top-leaders is the object of their respect. Besides national politicians, however, they raised scholars as the second figures. That is the reason why there remains a conventional Chinese recognition that those who become bureaucrats are to pursue scholarship and those who endeavor for scholarship are to become bureaucrats. Since old days, the Japanese have the spirit of Confucianism to some extent. Recently there are less Japanese with it compared with Chinese and Korean people. But the peculiar value “the spirit of wa (harmony)” is still alive in Japan. In the field of education, there is some conflict between the young and middle-aged generation; the Japanese should follow this concept. Concerning respect for parents, the old, and teachers, Japanese students pay less respect than Korean and Chinese students do. Recently, many atrocious crimes of the young people are seen in Japan and in this sense, the Japanese have to tackle this problem more seriously. Comparison of the Three Countries (in details) The most commonly respected people in the three counties are family blood relatives (=family members). The order of ratio ranking is China (No.1), Korea (No.2) and Japan (No.3). There were some Korean and Chinese students who said that they respected themselves, but no Japanese students did. Also, there is a significant difference in category k (personal acquaintance); friends, seniors, persons at a part-time job place, etc. The figure among Japanese students showed 6.5%, while among Korean students 2% and Chinese students only 0.7%. Further, it is a matter of surprise that 5.6% of Japanese students chose teachers (t) as A Comparison of Role Models 368 respected role models, which is higher than Korean students. They are teachers in cram schools, junior and senior high schools. The figure among Chinese students was 6.3 %, which is higher than among Japanese students, while among Korean students the figure is only 2.9%. It might be a tendency for Japanese students to respect people around them more than Korean and Chinese students. The following table is the detailed analysis of respected role models excluding people around, family members, teachers who have personal relationship with them. In the category of respected people, the top three categories are shown and the top ranking of historical and present people are also shown. Table 4: Comparison of the Three Countries Category of Respected Japanese Korean Role Models No. 1 pr-na-7 pa-na-5 No. 2 pr-nn-4 pa-na-9 No. 3 pr-na-4 pr-na-10 Historical people pa-nn-3 pa-na-5 Present people pr-na-7 pr-na-10 Chinese pa-na-2 pa-na-1 pa-na-5, pa-na-11 pa-na-2 pr-na-8 Starting from the Japanese side, all respected role models from category 1 to 3 are present people (pr), with No.1 Japanese (na) and No.2 foreigners (nn). No. 1 includes celebrities, singers, stars and comedians (7) and those of No.2 and No.3 are sports players and athletes respectively (4). In summary, the persons respected by Japanese students range from sports players and athletes now playing actively to celebrities, singers, stars, comedians, and even foreigners are targets of their respect. Concerning historical people, activists (active persons in politics) seem to be respected (3), while there is no difference between native and non-native people. In the case of Koreans, the top groups of respected role models were historical people (pa) and Koreans (na). As to occupations (third category), No.1 was national symbols and top-leaders (5) and No.2 was servicepersons and generals (9). All of them contributed to the benefits of their own country. No.3 was present people (pr) and financiers and business managers were named. To be more precise, they are representatives of Korean financial groups. This result may be affected by the fact that those university students surveyed were influenced by the 1997 financial crisis while in junior high school, which led to Korea receiving monetary support from the IMF. During the crisis, for example, some students could not bring their lunch boxes, while others saw parents lose jobs. Therefore, they were probably forced to be sensitive to the economical success. As the feature of Korean students, respected role models from No. 1 to No.3 were all native people (Korean), reflecting those people who contributed to the nation of today and in the past. In the case of Chinese students, the targets of respected role models by them were all 369 Masao Nakabayashi, Yumi Hasegawa, and Yang Yi historical people (pa). The most popular category was politicians followed by scholars (No. 2) and No.3 includes top-leaders as well as philosophers, thinkers, and educators. It can be said that Chinese students typically respected those people who contributed to politics or culture and historically became famous. Unlike the result of Japanese students, people around them were not a target of their respect. Further, there was also some difference from Korean students. Although students of both countries named those people who contributed to the development of the nations, Korean students respected those economically successful persons, while Chinese students are apt to neglect any economical success focusing on both politics and thought. This tendency to respect leaders in politics (politicians) and thought (thinkers) can be said to be a traditional way of thinking in China and it seems that Chinese university students still have this traditional way of thinking. It is interesting to find a tendency that Japanese students respected “present Japanese,” while Korean and Chinese students respected “historical people.” Japanese students respected not only present Japanese but also present foreigners (=non-native) Media exposure may effect their choice and could account for them not putting much emphasis on nationality. Concerning their ages, those respected people are elder only by a generation or the same age as the students. It seems that those people of the same age now actively performing in the world become easily a target of their respect. In the case of Korean students, it is a strong tendency that historical people are a target of their respect. It is possible for them to know the activities of respected persons by TV programs, textbooks and books. Unfortunately, however, it is impossible to watch them live in action. Further, because of historical people, they cannot identify for themselves how true their activities were, making it very difficult to observe objectively. Those people named as respected persons coincide with those people taught in school education. This history education in schools seems to effect the result to some extent. Interpretation and Significance Description of Concept on “Respect” How is the concept of “respect” used and understood? From the results described above, it seems that students of the three countries have both common and different feeling toward the concept of “respect.” Literally, “respect” consists of son and kei, the meaning of which is almost the same such as to esteem or to look up to. From a social standing, it seems that the object of son is ranked higher than oneself and that of kei is ranked lower than oneself. Judging from the role models named, it is easily supposed that respect has a very important concept and vital meaning in Korea and China. People in the two countries tend to more formally use it in an official environment rather than in a private environment. In short, A Comparison of Role Models 370 it is firmly established in a value as a uniform and unified concept. On the contrary, although in Japan son and kei had the same vital meaning as in Korea and China, there is a drastic change among the younger generation and it is unofficially used as a light meaning judging from the role models nominated. They tend to lightly say “I respect” as a spoken language, which often shows a sense of closeness towards their opponents. In other words, there is no unified concept and the concept of “respect” seems to be determined by their way of living. It has become a concept used not in an official environment but in a private environment. What can be seen from the level of understanding the word “respect” in three countries? The cause is investigated based on the results of the survey. 4.2 Emphasis on Duty Spirits Based on Confucianism From the data obtained, it can be safely asserted that students of the three countries have a common spirit because many of them put down parents as people of respect, although there was some difference in the proportion of numbers. It seems that respecting one’s parents is influenced by duty spirit. From this point of view, an idea of duty to parents seems to be still alive in the daily life of the three countries. They raised such reasons as indebtedness for being brought up, giving birth to and fostering. This is the traditional Confucianism moral that has become a socially accepted idea. A favorable family with parents respected by children is an indispensable factor for a society to establish a stable base. However, concerning other family members besides parents, different numbers were found in the three countries regarding people in the community and acquaintances. In the case of Japanese students, more of them put down other people besides parents than the Korean and Chinese students did. This is derived from the concept of respect peculiar to the Japanese. In other words, there is no unified concept and the concept of “respect” seems to be an intimate feeling towards other people starting from interpersonal communication. Although the concept of respect itself is vague and loose compared with the two other countries, their respect towards others besides family members indicates that the concept of respects includes an intimate feeling. In the strict meaning of respect, they cannot respect sports players or celebrities, but they can do so from the viewpoint of intimate or close feeling. Therefore, a sense of closeness is very important for the Japanese when considering those whom they respect. It is worth while to investigate the reason why grandparents and seniors were less respected than parents. In China where nuclear families are increasing in number, children are communicating less and less with grandparents. As a result of the one-child policy carried out since 1979, “four-two-one” families have been increasing; four grandparents, two parents and one child. Six adults surround one child but the frequency of communication among them has not increased in proportion to the number of families. One of the reasons for this is the fact 371 Masao Nakabayashi, Yumi Hasegawa, and Yang Yi that their parents put more emphasis on child’s education than communication with grandparents. As a result, Chinese students in their 20’s did not write down grandparents as the role model of respect. Further, they did not put people around them like uncles or aunts either. It is thinkable that the traditional Chinese blood relations are at risk of destruction. In short, it cannot be said easily that many students respecting parents are following and succeeding the sound concept based on Chinese tradition of social common idea. In Confucianism, it has been a must for children to respect their parents in order to belong to the blood relations. Respecting parents meant to maintain mutual order among those seniors besides parents and brothers/sisters. Today in China, however, only parents are respected tremendously and other aged people like grandparents and the seniors in the communities are rarely respected. This trend in China is also true in Korea and Japan. Separation of Loyalty Spirits in Confucianism From the data obtained, another feature is the fact that the proportion of students who respect national leaders and politicians in Korea and China is much higher than in Japan. As mentioned above, the term “respect” in Korea and China has connotations of value and is not used as a general word. The role models of their respect are those people who have contributed to the benefits of the nation and society, performing a certain role and responsibility. As they don’t have any close feelings, sports players, athletes and celebrities do not command their respect. It can be said that there exists the duty spirit of Confucianism behind their high appreciation for the people who contributed to the nation. “Loyalty” is connected to the relationship between parents and children, which rules private relations, while “duty” prescribes official and political relations. In other words, there is no unified concept and the concept of “respect” seems to be determined by their way of living. It has become a concept used not in the official environment but in the private environment. What is the relation between voluntary “respect “ and heteronomous “duty”? If we look into this matter from an educational point of view, this relation will be understood easily. The role of politics should not be forgotten if we try to voluntarily plant a consciousness to respect politicians. It is through education that the attractiveness of individuals is taught to more people. Behind the education it is predictable that there is the purpose of duty to the nation. It is the purpose of education to foster such useful persons for nations and societies. Those students fostered or educated under this purpose are self-motivated and have a value in compliance with the educational purpose. In this sense, duty (political value concept) and respect (private voluntary consciousness) are connected to the process of education. As far as duty is concerned, there was some distance between Korea/China and Japan, and the data showed that Chinese and Korean students differed from Japanese students concerning the importance of duty. A Comparison of Role Models 372 Chinese students respected leaders who had contributed to Chinese political life. Mao, Zhe-Dong, Zhou, En-Lai, and Deng, Xiao-pin. This can be attributed to education but the ratio from the data is not satisfactory. Although Zhou, En-Lai gathered almost half numbers, Sun-bun, the pioneer of modernization, is respected less than more recent leaders such as Mao, Zhe-Dong, and Deng, Xiao-pin. This may be an indication of students’ apathy in political issues as well as in the nation. However, 79 out of 100 students named historical Chinese politicians, as role models shows that the duty to the nation and politicians is still alive in China, although enthusiastic admiration for the leaders that was evident 30 years ago has disappeared. On the contrary, in the case of Korea, politicians are respected in the same way as in China. Referring to the names of role models Korean students respect, they are Lee, Sun-Shin (pa-na-9), King Se-Jong (pa-na-5), Park, Jung-Hee (pa-na-5), Kim,Gu (pa-na-3), King Gwang-Ge-Do(pa-na-5) who were reported in The Chosun Ilbo. (2005, May 15th) as the country’s representative heroes. According to an article of the same paper, Lee, Sun-Shin is said to be very similar to Park, Jung-Hee, which means Lee, Sun-Shin must be the most respected figure as well as a hero. That is the reason why many Korean university students wrote those names. Although Park, Jung-Hee is the target of their respect, some dislike him because of his military and political power. Further, Park, Jung-Hee, former president, was appointed a chief executive officer as the most suitable person among successive presidents. A survey was carried out by Employment PORTAL, which asked, “Who is the most proper person to be a CEO among successive presidents?” to 4,264 female and male adults. It is interesting to know that 59.2% of the total respondents nominated him as a CEO (The Chosun Ilbo, 2005, May 3rd). Although Park, Jung-Hee former president is a politician, he was nominated as the most appropriate person, because he is considered to be good at management and economic issues. This fact would be common to the occupations of respected present people, who are in business and are managers. From the viewpoint of loyalty in Confucianism, what factors can be obtained? Today China is enjoying economic growth and, needless to say, those people who have succeeded in the business field are the admiration of every Chinese student. However, those people were not a target of their respect, although they are much admired. This concept seems to have been derived from a traditional idea of Confucianism concerning “politics” and “commerce.” This idea still remains alive in China today. Originally, Confucianism tended to make light of commerce, based on the reason that commerce is an individual work and does not have any connection with large-scale undertakings of the nation. On the other hands, those people engaging in politics are versed with history and classic studies and the destiny of the nation is rested on those people with Confucianism knowledge. In the Chinese society where public officials are highly ranked, the road to an officer is to succeed in imperial examinations. It is well known that this 373 Masao Nakabayashi, Yumi Hasegawa, and Yang Yi examination is based on classics and excludes economic knowledge. In short, a different dimension existing between politics and commerce has never disappeared from today’s China. The loyalty to the nation of Chinese students is shown, in a sense, as a form of their respect toward politicians. In the case of Korean students, however, those people who contributed to the nation is a target of their respect regardless of political and economic matters. Economics should not be separated from politics and considered an essential means to construct the nation. If economics is combined with politics, the goal of modernization will be achieved more effectively. This combination of economics and politics is a reformed concept of Confucianism. In this sense, it can be safely asserted that the current style of loyalty is not uniform, but transformed into a different style unique to the individual country. In Korea, commerce has been also looked down as a service job since the old days, but the way of thinking has changed recently. Particularly, many corruption cases of politicians were widely reported by newspapers and politicians are less respected people than before. In addition, those young people, who were born in the 1960s and university students in the 1980s, established continuously venture enterprises and succeeded in business with a single idea. Therefore, an old idea that only a big financial group succeeds is collapsing today. In short, like Chinese students, Korean students typically respected those people who contributed to the development of the nation whether it is in the political or economic field. Compared with the two countries, politicians and business managers commanded little respect from Japanese students. This indicates that Japanese students tend to neglect ideas that have contributed to the nation. As described above, they have their own respect with intimate feeling. It is worth considering the reason why there exists such a big difference. First of all, the Japanese educational system dramatically changed after World War II, following American system through the guidance of GHQ. It is true that Japan had been enjoying democratic politics but there were problems. That is the lack of moral education and appropriate education of Japanese history, which caused many social and political conflicts. Another aspect we should point out is the fact that Japan introduced so many products and cultures mainly from Europe in the Meiji era and from the USA after World War II. In a sense, Japan was Westernized and Americanized much earlier than Korea and China, and turns becoming an economically and technologically strong country. This trend might affect the spirit of the Japanese, changing even their values. In other words, the Japanese are more liberal than the other two countries. Conclusions The survey carried out in the three Asian countries covers only narrow areas and we should bear in mind that only one concept was surveyed. From the results of the questionnaires, the values and awareness of the issues among the students in the three A Comparison of Role Models 374 countries are carefully reviewed and found out to some extents. By comparing their respected role models, it seems that not only the common and different points were clarified, but also these background and causes were revealed to some extent. Although the true meaning of respect is a little bit different from each country, it can be safely said that Confucianism still remain as vital currents in the society of the three countries, whether it is recognized or not. Judging from the role models named, it was found that students of the three countries have both common and different feeling toward the concept of “respect.” People in Korea and China tend to more formally use it in an official environment rather than in a private environment. However, the younger generation people in Japan tend to more informally use it in a private environment. This trend may affect the results of the role models and should be further investigated in the future. We found also that role models named were different among three countries. The proportion of students who respect national leaders and politicians in Korea and China is much higher than in Japan. The role models of their respect are those people who have contributed to the benefits of the nation and society, performing a certain role and responsibility. However, Japanese students respected many sports players, athletes and celebrities rather than national leaders and politicians. This finding is very important to reconsider a proposition that the Japanese have maintained a loyalty spirit. Finally, students of the three countries have a common spirit because many of them put down parents as people of respect, although there was some difference in the proportion of numbers. It seems that respecting one’s parents is influenced by duty spirit in Confucianism. It is noteworthy that an idea of duty to parents seems to be still alive in the daily life of the three countries. We are sure that the traditional Confucianism is still followed and kept by the younger people in the three countries. It is a good subject to further investigate the background of this value. Confucianism was born in China and it spread to Korea and Japan later. It was said that before Confucianism was introduced to Japan, education in Japan was non-sense and unawake. But after it was brought to Japan, Japanese awoke education of morality and idealism. It affected politics, economy and culture and particularly effect to social ethics has continued until today. In this sense, we cannot neglect the important role of Confucianism and how it was influenced Korea and Japan. These three countries are called “the Confucianism cultural sphere”, from the view point that the three countries have a common base of spirit. From the viewpoint of duty and loyalty, which are basic to Confucianism spirit, it was found that the spirit has succeeded with modern sense created while its traditional aspects are being reserved. As time passes and develops, Confucianism cannot be fixed and universal truth and it is endowed with new meanings to comply with a new age. Recently, there are so many political and economical conflicts among the three countries. As the results of this survey indicate, people of the three countries have the 375 Masao Nakabayashi, Yumi Hasegawa, and Yang Yi common value and should talk more frankly to cooperate as the leaders in Asia based on revised Confucianism. A Comparison of Role Models 376 References Huang, J. , & Guo, J. , (2003) . Research on the Basic Problems in the Tradition and Modernization of China Education, Beijing Normal University Kawai, T. , (2004) English Education Policy of Korea, Kansai University Mizuno, S., (2003) Want to know the Korean Young, Iwanami Junior Shisho Nakabayashi, M., (2001) Exploring the Undercurrents of being the Japanese, Kansai Eigo Eibei Bungaku-kai Nishio, K., (2000) History of the Nation, Sankei Shimbunsha Numata, H.,(1995) A Comparative Study of the Aims of Education, Publication Dept. of Tamagawa University Samuel D. H., (1999) The Clash of Civilization. (S.Suzuki. Trans), Shueisha Shi, Z.,(1999) Cultural Character of Pedagogy, Shan Xi Education Publiction Takahashi, T., (1929) .Educational History of Japan, Educational Society Tanakam, K., (1999) .World Trend of Multi-culture Education, Nakanishiya Brezinka, W., (1992) Education in Value Diversification Age, Publication Department of Tamagawa University Yamazaki, T., ed.(2004) Invitation to Pedagogy, Bukkyo University Hoobler, T&D., (2003) . Confucianism, Seidosha URL Korean Fork Village Web Site. (2006). http://www.koreanfolk.co.kr/main.asp The Chosun Ilbo. (2005, May 5th)http://japanese.chosun.com/site/data/html_dir/2005/04/15/20050415000045.htmlT he Chosun Ilbo. (2005, May 3rd) http://japanese.chosun.com/site/data/html_dir/2005/05/03/20050503000014.html 377 Masao Nakabayashi, Yumi Hasegawa, and Yang Yi Appendix 1 QUESTIONNAIRES ON VALUES This is a survey on the values of Japanese, Korean and Chinese university students. Please frankly answer the following questions and circle the item you wish choose. Personal Information: Age: years Sex: male female Grade: 1. Do you respect your teachers? (1) most (2) more (3) average (4) less (5) least 2. Do you respect your parents? (1) most (2) more (3) average (4) less (5) least 3. Do you respect elderly people? (1) most (2) more (3) average (4) less (5) least 4. Do you think friendship is necessary? (1) very necessary (2) a little (3) average (4) less necessary (5) unnecessary 5. Are you proud of the culture of your country? (1) most (2) more (3) average (4) less (5) least 6. Please write the names of three people you respect in the order that you respect the most. (1) first (2) second (3) third 7. Among them, is either of your parents included? The reason why: Yes No 8. Will you care for your parents as they get older? (1) definitely yes (2) a little (3) average (4) less (5) definitely not 9. Suppose you are working or studying away from your own house. This morning, your mother calls and tells you your father fell down. What would you do? (1) go home immediately (2) go home later (3) undecided (4) stop working (5) continue working 10. Answer as if you were an office worker. What do you think about lifetime employment? (1) very agreeable (2) agreeable a little (3) average (4) disagreeable a little (5) very disagreeable 11. At the end of a fiscal year, you are ordered to transfer. What would you do ? (1) accept soon (2) accept later (3) undecided (4) try to refuse (5) definitely refuse 12. One of your co-workers cannot attend the funeral ceremony of his/her mother. Which idea do you support? (1) There is nothing you can do about it because of the important job at the company. A Comparison of Role Models 378 (2) How undutiful your co-worker is! The co-worker should attend the ceremony and forget about the job. (3) It is thoughtless of a company not to allow the co-worker to attend the ceremony. 13. If you are married, think about your spouse and if you are unmarried, think about your parents. Mainly who does the following housework? (1) cooking ( husband wife ) ( father mother ) (2) washing ( husband wife ) ( father mother ) (3) cleaning the toilet ( husband wife ) ( father mother ) (4) cleaning rooms ( husband wife ) ( father mother ) (5) taking out the garbage ( husband wife ) ( father mother ) 14. Who is in charge of the family budget? (not earning, but the right to manage) In case of a married male: (1) yourself (2) wife (3) both In case of a married female: (1) yourself (2) husband (3) both In case of single: (1) father (2) mother (3) both 15. What do you think about the saying “A wife should obey her husband”? (1) very agreeable (2) agreeable a little (3) average (4) disagreeable a little (5) very disagreeable Thank you for your cooperation. 379 Masao Nakabayashi, Yumi Hasegawa, and Yang Yi Appendix 2 Table 5 Summary of Role Models Respected by Students in the Three Countries Country(Total ) Category f k t m z x Japan total=448 Ans. No. 156 32.0 29 6.5 25 5.6 0 0 138 30.8 5 1.1 Korea total=306 Ans. No 102 33.3 7 2.3 9 2.9 5 1.6 57 18.6 6 2.0 China total=301 Ans. No. 107 35.5 2 0.7 19 6.3 2 0.7 15 5 3 1 pa pr px 64 67 4 14.3 15.0 0.8 94 26 0 30.7 8.5 0 139 11 3 46.2 3.7 1 na nn nx 72 60 3 16.1 13.4 0.6 103 17 0 33.7 5.6 0 130 20 3 43.2 6.6 1 pa-na pa-nn pr-na pr-nn pr-nx px-nn px-nx 1 pa-na-1 pa-nn-1 pr-na-1 29 35 43 23 1 2 2 7 3 3 1 6.5 7.8 9.6 5.1 0.2 0.4 0.4 1.6 0.7 0.7 0.2 80 14 23 3 0 0 0 6 1 3 2 26.1 4.6 7.5 1.0 0 0 0 2 0.3 1 0.7 120 19 10 1 3 0 0 21 17 3 0 40 6.3 3.3 0.3 1 0 0 7 0 3 0 0 0 0 6 9 2 0 0.7 0.7 9 0 0 0 0 3.8 1.3 12 2.0 0.4 0 3 2 0 3 1 10 1 1 0 2. 1 9 0.7 0 1 0.3 3. 3.3 9 0.3 0.3 1 79 8 1 6 0 1 0 0 12 17 1 3 6 6.7 2.7 0 3.8 0.2 2.5 0.6 44 1.3 0 0 0 41 3 0 2 3 4 5 px-nx-1 pa-na-2 pa-nn-2 px-nx-2 pr-na-2 pr-nn-2 pa-na-3 pa-nn-3 pr-na-3 pr-na-4 pr-nn-4 pr-nx-4 pa-na-5 pa-nn-5 3 17 30 12 0 0 0 1 13.4 4. 1 94 1 0 8 0 0 0 8 0 31.2 0.3 0 2.7 5.6 1 0 0.3 26.2 2.7 0.3 2 0 0.3 0 0 0 0 0 2.7 0 A Comparison of Role Models 380 pr-na-5 6 7 pr-na-6 pa-na-6 pa-nn-6 pa-na-7 pa-nn-7 10 pr-na-7 pr-nn-7 px-nx-7 pa-na-8 pa-nn-8 pr-na-8 pr-nn-8 pa-na-9 pa-nn-9 pa-na-10 11 pr-na-10 pr-nn-10 pa-na-11 8 9 0 x pa-nn-11 pr-na-11 pa-na-0 pa-nn-0 pr-na-0 px-nx-0 pa-na-x px-nn-x pa-nn-x 3 2 0.6 0 4 0 0 2 0 0 5 0.4 0 3 0.4 0 6.3 0 1 1.1 1 1 1 1 0 1 4 16 6 2 4 4 5 0 4 3 3 3.6 1.3 0.4 2.9 0.9 4 0.9 1.1 0 1.6 0.9 24 0.7 0.9 0.7 13 0 0 0 1 0 3 0 21 3 0 5 1 0 0 0.2 0.2 1.1 0 2 11 2 1 0 0 0 1. 0.3 3 0 1 0 7. 6.9 8 1 4. 0 2 3.6 3 2 0 1 1 0 1 2 1 0.7 0.4 0.4 0 2 0.2 0.2 0 0.9 0.2 0 0.4 0.2 1 0 0 0 2 0 0 0 0 29 13 7 2 4 0.3 0.3 0.3 0. 0.3 3 0 0.7 0. 0.3 7 0.3 0 0. 0 7 0 0.7 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 4 3 3 1 3 2 0 11 0 0 8 11 5 1 0 3 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0.3 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.3 0 0 1.3 1 1 0.3 1 0.7 0 3.7 0 0 2.7 3.7 1.7 0.3 0 1 0 0 0 0 0.3 0 0 0 Table 6 Respected Role Models in Japan Country(total Category f Japan total=448 Ans. 156 32.0 k 29 6.5 t m z x 25 0 138 5 5.6 0 30.8 1.1 pa pr px 64 67 4 14.3 15.0 0.8 Sample Figures Father, mother, parents, grand parents, elder sister, younger sister Friend, girl friend, senior, person at part-time job place Teacher 381 Masao Nakabayashi, Yumi Hasegawa, and Yang Yi na nn nx 72 60 3 16.1 13.4 0.6 pa-na pa-nn pr-na pr-nn pr-nx px-nn px-nx 1 pa-na-1 pa-nn-1 pr-na-1 px-nx-1 2 pa-na-2 pa-nn-2 px-nx-2 pr-na-2 pr-nn-2 3 pa-na-3 pa-nn-3 pr-na-3 4 pr-na-4 pr-nn-4 pr-nx-4 5 pa-na-5 pa-nn-5 pr-na-5 29 35 43 23 1 2 2 7 6.5 7.8 9.6 5.1 0.2 0.4 0.4 3 3 1 0 3 0 0 0 0 6 9 2 12 17 1 3 6 3 1.6 0.7 0.7 0.2 0 0.7 0.7 0 0 0 0 3.8 1.3 2.0 0.4 6.7 2.7 3.8 0.2 2.5 0.6 1.3 0.6 Ryuma Sakamoto Mother Teresa Sadako Ogata Ichiro, Tomokazu Miura Jidan, Beccam Sports Player same age Kakuei Tanaka President Kennedy Premier Koizumi, Emperor 6 2 0 2 0 0 5 0.4 0 0.4 0 6.3 0 1.1 John Lenon, Audrey Hepburn 7 8 9 pr-na-6 pa-na-6 pa-nn-6 pa-na-7 pa-nn-7 3 17 30 12 29 pr-na-7 16 pr-nn-7 px-nx-7 pa-na-8 pa-nn-8 pr-na-8 pr-nn-8 pa-na-9 pa-nn-9 6 2 4 4 5 0 4 3 13 7 3.6 1.3 0.4 3.1 0.9 0.9 1.1 0 1.6 0.9 0.7 Hideo Noguchi Yung, Descartes Hayao Kawai Shouzou Tanaka Takuya Kimura, MisterChildren, Hitoshi Matsumoto Eric Clampton Musician Ryoutaro Shiba Edgar Allan Poe Shokatsu Koumei Shingen Takaeda A Comparison of Role Models 382 10 11 0 X pa-na-10 4 3 0.9 0.7 pr-na-10 pr-nn-10 1 0 0.2 0.2 pa-na-11 5 pa-nn-11 pr-na-11 pa-na-0 2 pa-nn-0 pr-na-0 px-nx-0 pa-na-x 4 px-nn-x pa-nn-x 0 3 2 0 1 1 0 1 2 1 1.1 0 0.7 0.4 1.1 0 0.2 0.2 0 0 0.2 0.4 0.2 Kounosuke Matsushita Takafumi Horie Tom Ford Helen Keller Osamu Mizutani Note: Japanese figures’ name: First name and family name. Table 7 Respected Figures in Korea Country(total) Category f Korea (total=306) Ans. 102 33.3 k t m z x 7 9 5 57 6 2.3 2.9 1.6 18.6 2.0 pa pr px 94 26 0 30.7 8.5 0 na nn nx 103 17 0 33.7 5.6 0 pa-na pa-nn pr-na pr-nn pr-nx px-nn px-nx 1 pa-na-1 pa-nn-1 80 14 23 3 0 0 0 6 26.1 4.6 7.5 1.0 0 0 0 2 0.3 1 1 3 Parents, father, mother, elder sister, elder brother Friend Teacher Oneself Jung,Mong-Ju Nobel, Newton 383 Masao Nakabayashi, Yumi Hasegawa, and Yang Yi pr-na-1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 px-nx-1 pa-na-2 pa-nn-2 px-nx-2 pr-na-2 pr-nn-2 pa-na-3 pa-nn-3 pr-na-3 pr-na-4 pr-nn-4 pr-nx-4 pa-na-5 pa-nn-5 pr-na-5 pr-na-6 pa-na-6 pa-nn-6 pa-na-7 pa-nn-7 pr-na-7 pr-nn-7 px-nx-7 pa-na-8 pa-nn-8 pr-na-8 pr-nn-8 pa-na-9 pa-nn-9 pa-na-10 2 9 12 0 44 3 1 4 24 13 pr-na-10 pr-nn-10 11 0 x pa-na-11 pa-nn-11 pr-na-11 pa-na-0 pa-nn-0 pr-na-0 px-nx-0 pa-na-x px-nn-x pa-nn-x 2 2 0 0 3 2 0 3 0.7 2.9 0 1 0.7 0 1 Hwang,Wu-Suk Kim,Gu Lee,Myung-Bak 1 10 3.9 1 1 0.3 3.3 0.3 0.3 Condoleessa Rice An,Jung-Geon, Yu,Gwan-Sun Mother Teresa Han,Bi-Ya 0 0 0 0 41 14.4 3 0 1 1 1 0 0 0 13.4 1 0 0.3 0.3 King Gwang-Ge-Do, King De-Jong Lincoln, Qin Shi-Huang 1 1 0 0.3 0.3 0.3 0 0 0 0 1 1 0 3 0 21 7.8 3 0 4.2 0 0 0 0.3 0 1 0 6.9 1 0 11 2 3.6 0.7 Lee,Gun-Hee Jung,Ju-Young Bill Gates 0.7 0 0 0 0 0.3 0.3 0 0 0 Sin-Sa-Im-Dang 2 0 0 0 0 1 1 0 0 0 0.7 0.7 0 Beop-Jung (Buddhist Priest) Jesus Christ Chaplain Lee,Hyo-Lee Yoon,I-San Cho,Se-Hyun Shin,Yong-Bok Lee,Sun-Shin Chang,Bo-Go Shokatsu Koumei, Caesar Officer Volunteering people A Comparison of Role Models 384 Note: In the case of An, Jung-Geon, An is the surname and Jung is the first name and usually it is written An, Jung-Geon in English. A comma is put after the surname and the two words of the first name are connected by a hyphen. Historical figures are written with the surname first and the first name last, because it has become a proper noun. However, it is also possible to change the order to Jung-Geon An. Although the rule of Romanization is established, the standardization is not kept so that every body can write, as he/she likes. Table 8 Respected Role Models in China Country(total( Category f k t m z x China total=301 Ans. % 107 35.5 2 0.7 19 6.3 2 0.7 15 5 3 1 pa pr px 139 11 3 46.2 3.7 1 na nn nx 130 20 3 43.2 6.6 1 pa-na pa-nn pr-na pr-nn pr-nx px-nn px-nx 1 pa-na-1 pa-nn-1 pr-na-1 120 19 10 1 3 0 0 21 17 3 0 40 6.3 3.3 0.3 1 0 0 7 px-nx-1 pa-na-2 pa-nn-2 px-nx-2 pr-na-2 pr-nn-2 pa-na-3 pa-nn-3 1 79 8 1 6 0 1 0 2 3 94 1 31.2 0.3 parents, father, mother, grand-parents Acquaintance Teacher Oneself 5.6 1 0 Li Bai Du Pu Albert Einstein 0.3 26.2 2.7 0.3 2 0 0.3 0 Scholar Mao,Zhe-Dong Zhou,En-Lai Lincoln Napoleon Zhu,Rong-Ji Li,Zi-Cheng 385 Masao Nakabayashi, Yumi Hasegawa, and Yang Yi pr-na-3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 pr-na-4 pr-nn-4 pr-nx-4 pa-na-5 pa-nn-5 pr-na-5 pr-na-6 pa-na-6 pa-nn-6 pa-na-7 pa-nn-7 pr-na-7 pr-nn-7 px-nx-7 pa-na-8 pa-nn-8 pr-na-8 pr-nn-8 pa-na-9 pa-nn-9 pa-na-10 0 0 8 0 1 11 5 0 pr-na-10 pr-nn-10 11 0 X pa-na-11 pa-nn-11 pr-na-11 pa-na-0 pa-nn-0 pr-na-0 px-nx-0 pa-na-x px-nn-x pa-nn-x 0 0 0 8 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 4 3 3 1 3 2 0 0 0 2.7 0 0.3 3.7 1.7 0 0 0 11 1 0 8 3 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 2.7 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0.3 0 0 1.3 1 1 0.3 1 0.7 0 Liu Che Kang Xi Liu De-Huang Lu Xun Beethoven Yasunari Kawabata Shi,Tie-Sheng Takeshi Kitano Yue Fei Guan Yu Nobunaga Oda Maresuke Noki 0 0 3.7 0.3 0 2.7 Confucius 1 0 0 0 0 0.3 0 0 0 Kant Karl Marx Person with morality Note: The English spelling of Chinese people used in China sometimes differs from that of Chinese people used in other countries. Human Communication. A Publication of the Pacific and Asian Communication Association. Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 387 – 394. The Relationship Between Self-Deception and Extrinsic-Personal Religiosity Amanda J. Norris University of Alabama at Birmingham Larry Powell University of Alabama at Birmingham Mark Hickson, III University of Alabama at Birmingham Amanda J. Norris is a business manager in Birmingham, Alabama and a former undergraduate communication studies major at UAB. Larry Powell and Mark Hickson are professors of communication studies at UAB. They can be contacted at: Communication Studies Department, UAB, 1055 11th Street South, #117A, Birmingham, AL 35294. The Relationship Between Self-Deception and Extrinsic-Personal Religiosity 388 Abstract This study examined the relationship between self-deception and religiosity in two different samples: (1) fifty members of a non-denominational church, and (2) 54 students at a state university. The participants responded to Allport and Ross (1967) Religious Orientation Scale and the Paulhus (1984, 1998) Deception Scale. The results indicated that the nature of the relationship depends upon at least three factors: (1) the type of extrinsic religiosity being measured (personal or social), (2) the gender of the individual, and (3) whether the individual is affiliated with a religious institution. The form of self-deception identified as impression management was inversely associated with extrinsic-personal religiosity. Further, although this association was present in the total sample, it could only be verified for females and for those respondents who were affiliated with a church. This latter relationship might be described as the “Plain Mary” who establishes a personal image based on her religious activities rather than her appearance. 389 Amanda J. Norris, Larry Powell, and Mark Hickson, III Introduction Self-deception has been identified with such topics as alcoholism (Strom, & Barone, 1993), psychology (Johnson & Burroughs, 2000; Shapiro, 1996), religion (Leak & Fish 1989; Hall, 2000), science (Mele, 1997), and marital relationships (O’Rourke & Cappeliez, 2005). Some researchers have attempted to explain it as apparent ignorance about a belief that is formed by an individual’s motivations or an influence of an individual’s desire to protect themselves from various beliefs that may threaten their self (Statman, 1997). Some scholars view self-deception as a normal activity and a means of communicating with ourselves (Fingarette, 1998; Patten, 2003). Moomal and Henzi (2000), however, have noted a paradox in that concept because it is logically inconsistent to believe without believing at the same time. Similarly, Kirby (2003) noted that self-deception has a built-in paradox in that the individual being deceived is also the deceiver. The issue is further complicated by trying to distinguish instances of self-ignorance, or wishful thinking, from self-deception (Levine, 1998). Regardless, the hypothesis that typical self-deceivers do not intentionally deceive themselves has gained support (Mele, 1999). Verkasalo and Lindeman (1994) noted that self-deception is associated with an individual’s ingratiation and exemplification goals. People who deceive themselves cannot consider the situation in an objective manner because there is a suspension of the normal objective interest in reality. Thus the process of self-deception must include a way of hindering in advance the consciousness of adverse tendencies or feelings and doing so without self-awareness (Shapiro, 1996). Some authorities describe self-deception as a psychological coping device. Hagedorn (1996) linked it to happiness and life satisfaction. Lewis (2004) argued that self-deceivers distort the past so that they can live more comfortably in the present. That approach is similar to the view that selfdeception is a defensive mechanism that protects the individual’s ego (Norem, 2002). Others view self-deception as an ego-enhancement device (Tester & Gleaves, 2005), a concept that is supported by research that links self-deception to impression management (Pauls & Crost, 2004). Another research approach has identified a potential link between self-deception and moral behavior. Tenbrunsel and Messick (2004) identified self-deception as an enabling device that promotes unethical behavior. Paulhus and John (1998) argued that self-deception develops from both egoistic and moralistic motives. Von Hippel (2003) argued that self-deception is a component in self-serving biases. Johnson (1997) identified self deception as a source of fallible moral judgments. Levy (2004), however, argued that self-deceivers were not necessarily morally responsible for their deception since it “is simply a kind of mistake and has no more necessary connection to culpability than have other intellectual errors” (p. 294). Regardless, the potential link of self-deception and morality suggests a possible relationship between the concept and religiosity. Indeed, several sources have reported a potential link in selfdeception and religious beliefs (Saroglou & Galand, 2004), including beliefs about the soul (Johnson & Burroughs, 2000), salvation (Hall, 2000), religious conversion (Jones, 1998), and hypocrisy (Statman, 1997). Other studies have looked at the links of religiosity to self-esteem (Pradhan, 2001), depression (Wink, Larsen, & Dillon 2005), and well-being (Glenn, 1997; Krause, 2003), while The Relationship Between Self-Deception and Extrinsic-Personal Religiosity 390 Williams, Francis and Robbins (2006) found a positive relationship between low self-esteem and rejection of Christianity. Burris (1994) noted that intrinsic religiosity (the internalization of religious beliefs and values) and extrinsic religiosity (behavioral manifestations of religiosity) were inversely related and that both variables are related to self-deception and impression management. Burris and Navara (2002) looked directly at the relationship between religiosity and depression and identified a positive correlation between self-deception and intrinsic religiosity. Similarly, Leak and Fish (1989) found a link between intrinsic religiosity, impression management, and self-deception. They concluded that intrinsically religious individuals have “tendencies toward distortions in the way they see themselves and in the way they intentionally present themselves to others” (p. 355). What is missing from these findings is support for Burris’ (1994) contention that there is a relationship between extrinsic religiosity and self-deception. Given the strong relationship between self-deception and impression management, such a relationship would be anticipated. Extrinsic religiosity, after all, is a form of impression management within the religious context, but the only support for this contention is Burris’ reporting of an inverse relationship. Two factors may account for this negative finding. First, the results may be contaminated by testing extrinsic religiosity as a uni-dimensional concept. Paulhus’ (1984, 1998) version of extrinsic religiosity includes both a personal and social dimension, with the personal dimension closely related to impression management. This creates the possibility that self-deception could be related to extrinsic-personal religiosity even if the social dimension is unrelated. Further, the lack of association with one dimension could mask the relationship with the second, if extrinsic religiosity is measured only from a unidimensional perspective. Second, the results may be limited by samples that have not distinguished adequately in terms of varying degrees of religiosity. Past studies have typically used convenience samples from student populations or from religious institutions. Both approaches have the disadvantage of potentially constricting the range of responses. As such, the lack of a significant association based on high or low religiosity may be due to the fact that one sample is dominated by low religiosity scores (student samples) while the other may suffer from little variation among high religiosity scores (church samples). Either way, the lack of significant variation in the samples could result in little variation in participants’ responses, and thus little ability to measure the relationship between the variables. This study sought to re-test the relationship of religiosity to self-deception using samples from both a secular and a religious setting. In addition, previous studies have not differentiated between male and female respondents. Thus the following hypotheses were tested: 1. There will be a negative relationship between extrinsic-personal religiosity and selfdeception. 1a. The relationship will differ for male and female respondents 1b. The relationship will differ for secular and religious samples. 2. There will not be a relationship between extrinsic-social religiosity and self-deception. 2a. The relationship will not differ for male and female respondents 2b. The relationship will not differ for secular and religious samples. 3. There will not be a relationship between intrinsic religiosity and self-deception. 3a. The relationship will not differ for male and female respondents 3b. The relationship will not differ for secular and religious samples. 391 Amanda J. Norris, Larry Powell, and Mark Hickson, III Method The participants were 104 adults and students (46 males, 58 females). Fifty (50) were members of a non-denominational church in Alabama, and 54 were students at a state university. These participants responded to a question that included the Allport and Ross (1967) Religious Orientation Scale to test intrinsic religiosity, social extrinsic religiosity, and personal extrinsic religiosity. The participants also responded to the Paulhus (1984, 1998) Deception Scale (formerly the Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding, BIDR) to test self-deception and impression management. Pearson’s correlations between religiosity, self-deception, and impression management were used to test the hypotheses. Results Hypothesis 1 was supported. Self-deception was negatively associated with impression management (-.49, p < .01). Impression management, in turn, was negatively associated with extrinsic-personal religiosity (-.28, p < .01). Hypothesis 1a, which predicted a gender difference in the relationship, was supported. For females, self-deception was negatively associated with impression management (-.31, p < .05) and impression management was negatively associated with extrinsic-personal religiosity (-.35, p < .01). For males, self-deception was negatively associated with impression management (-.67, p < .01), but impression management was not associated significantly with extrinsic-personal religiosity (-.21). Hypothesis 1b, which predicted a difference in the secular/church subsamples, was also supported. Within the sample from the church group, self-deception was negatively associated with impression management (-.47, p < .01); impression management was also negatively associated with extrinsic-personal religiosity (-.39, p < .01). For the student sample, self-deception was also negatively associated with impression management (-.53, p < .01), but impression management was not associated with extrinsic-personal religiosity (-.10). Hypothesis 2. The null hypothesis of no relationship between self-deception and extrinsicsocial religiosity was verified. There was no significant correlation between the two variables (.10). The null hypothesis 2a, which predicted no difference between males and females on this relationship, was supported. There were no significant associations between self-deception, impression management and extrinsic-social religiosity. The null hypothesis 2b, which predicted no difference between the two samples on this relationship, was verified only for the student sample. There was a significant correlation between self-deception and extrinsic-social religiosity for the church sample (-.37, p < .01). Hypothesis 3. The null hypothesis of no relationship between self-deception and intrinsic religiosity was verified. There was no significant correlation between the two variables (.03). The null hypothesis 3a, which predicted no difference between males and females on this relationship, was supported. There were no significant associations between self-deception, impression management and intrinsic religiosity for either gender subsample. The null hypothesis 3b, which predicted no difference between the two samples on this The Relationship Between Self-Deception and Extrinsic-Personal Religiosity 392 relationship, was also verified. There were no significant associations between self-deception, impression management and intrinsic religiosity for either gender subsample. Discussion The results indicate that the relationship between self-deception and religiosity is somewhat more complex than previous studies have indicated, particularly as it applies to extrinsic religiosity. Specifically, the nature of the relationship depends upon at least three factors: (1) the type of extrinsic religiosity being measured (personal or social), (2) the gender of the individual, and (3) whether the individual is affiliated with a religious institution. The form of self-deception identified as impression management was inversely associated with extrinsic-personal religiosity. Further, although this association was present in the total sample, it could only be verified for females and for those respondents who were affiliated with a church. Thus individuals, particularly church-going women, who score higher on impression management will tend to have lower scores on extrinsic-personal religiosity. One possibility is that those who enter the religious environment with high impression management needs might view extrinsic religiosity behaviors as unnecessary. Conversely, those with low scores on impression management will have higher scores on extrinsic-personal religiosity. For these individuals, the personal religious behavior may fill an impression need that is not met by their own impression management behavior. This latter behavior might be described as the “Plain Mary” who establishes a personal image based on her religious activities rather than her appearance. One explanation may be that both high-impression management females and high extrinsic personal religiosity females are “presenting” themselves to an homophilous audience. Additional, membership in particular religions may affect both the demonstrated religiosity and the low impression management. Such a approach is likely to be taken by conservative religions with two values: (1) low tolerance for media images, especially as they relate to sexuality; and (2) what might be referred to as a “dress code” for its members. Thus, both groups may be controlling their impressions – just in different ways for different audiences. The results also indicated that higher scores on self-deception are associated with lower scores on impression management. This supports previous research and may indicate that people who are deceiving themselves make less effort to control the impression they make on others. Regardless, future research is needed to verify these concepts. Is the “church-going wallflower” an accurate description of this phenomenon are merely a convenient stereotype? To what extent is this effect gender-based and influenced by religious affiliation? More research in broader contexts and in different religious environments are needed. 393 Amanda J. Norris, Larry Powell, and Mark Hickson, III References Burris, C. T. (1994). Curvilinearity and religious types: A second look at intrinsic, extrinsic, and quest relations. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 4(4), 245-260. Burris, C. T., & Navara, G. S. (2002). Morality play or playing morality? Intrinsic religious orientation and socially desirable responding. Self & Identity, 1, 67-76. Fingarette, H. (1998). Self-deception needs no explaining. Philosophical Quarterly, 48(192), 291-303. Glenn, C.L. (1997). Relationship of mental health to religiosity. McGill Journal of Medicine, 3, 86-92. Hagedorn, J. W. (1996). Happiness and self-deception: An old question examined by a new measure of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 38(2), 139-160. Hall, A.L. (2000). Self-deception, confusion, and salvation in fear and trembling with works of love. Journal of Religious Ethics, 28(1), 37-61. Johnson, E. A. (1997). Real ascriptions of self-deception are fallible moral judgments. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 20, 117-118. Johnson, E.L., & Burroughs, C.S. (2000). Protecting one’s soul: A Christian inquiry into defensive activity. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 28(3), 175-189. Jones, W. E. (1998). Religious conversion, self-deception, and Pascal's wager. Journal of the History of Philosophy, 36, 167-188. Kirby, S. (2003). Telling lies? An exploration of self-deception and bad faith. European Journal of Psychotherapy, Counselling & Health, 6(2), 99-110. Krause, N. (2003). Religious meaning and subjective well-being in late life. The Journals of Geronology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58, S160-S170. Leak, G.K., & Fish, S. (1989). Religious orientation, impression management, and selfdeception: Toward a clarification of the link between religiosity and social desirability. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 28(3), 355-359. Levine, M.P. (1998). No-self, real-self, ignorance and self-deception: Does self-deception require a self? Asian Philosophy, 8(2), 103-110. Levy, N. (2004). Self-deception and moral responsibility. Ratio, 17(3), 294-311. Lewis, J. M. (2004). How much self-deception helps? Psychiatric Times, 21(7), 35-37. Mele, A.R. (1997). Real self-deception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20(1), 91-136. Mele, A.R. (1999). Twisted self-deception. Philosophical Psychology, 12(2), 117-137. Moomal, Z., & Henzi, S.P. (2000). The evolutionary psychology of deception and selfdeception. South African Journal of Psychology, 30(3), 45-51. Norem, J. K. (2002). Defensive self-deception and social adaptation among optimists. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 549-555. O'Rourke, N., & Cappeliez, P. (2005). Marital satisfaction and self-deception: Reconstruction of relationship histories among older adults. Social Behavior & Personality, 33, 273-282. Patten, D. (2003). How do we deceive ourselves? Philosophical Psychology, 16, 229-246. Pauls, C. A., & Crost, N. (2004). Effects of faking on self-deception and impression management The Relationship Between Self-Deception and Extrinsic-Personal Religiosity 394 scales. Personality & Individual Differences, 37, 1137-1151. Paulhus, D. L. (1984). Two component models of socially desirable responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 598-609. Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Manual for the Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding (BIDR-7). Toronto: Multi-Health Systems. Paulhus, D. L, & John, O. P. (1998). Egoistic and moralistic biases in self-perception: The interplay of self-deceptive styles with basic traits and motives. Journal of Personality, 66, 1025-1060. Shapiro, D. (1996). On the psychology of self-deception. Social Research, 63(3), 785-800. Statman, D. (1997). Hypocrisy and self-deception. Philosophical Psychology, 10(1), 57-75. Strom, J., & Barone, D.F. (1993). Self-deception, self-esteem, and control over drinking at different stages of alcohol involvement. Journal of Drug Issues, 23(4), 704-714. Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Messick, D. M. (2004). Ethical fading: The role of self-deception in unethical behavior. Social Justice Research, 17, 223-236. Tester, M. L., & Gleaves, D. H. (2005). Self-deceptive enhancement and family environment: Possible protective factors against internalization of the thin ideal. Eating Disorders, 13(2), 187-199. Verkasalo, M., & Lindeman, M. (1994). Personal ideals and socially desirable responding. European Journal of Personality, 8(5), 385-393. Von Hippel, W. (2003) Self-serving bias and self-deception. Australian Journal of Psychology, 55, 66. Williams, E., Francis, L. J., & Robbins, M. (2006). Rejection of Christianity and self-esteem. North American Journal of Psychology, 8, 193-195. Wink, P., Larsen, B., & Dillon, M. (2005). Religion as moderator of the depression-health connection. Research on Aging, 27, 197-220. Human Communication. A Publication of the Pacific and Asian Communication Association. Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 381 – 412. Laughing Before Takeoff: Humor, Sex, and the Preflight Safety Briefing Jason S. Wrench Ohio University – Eastern Brenda Millhouse Ohio University - Eastern David Sharp Ohio University – Chillicothe Jason S. Wrench (EdD - West Virginia University, 2002) is an assistant professor in the Communication & Media Department at SUNY New Paltz. Brenda Millhouse (BSS - Ohio University, 2006) is a master’s student in Applied Politics at University of Akron/Bliss Institute. David Sharp (BS in Communication - Ohio University, 2006) works for Federal Express. Correspondence to: Jason S. Wrench, CSB 51, New Paltz, NY 12561, USA. Email: wrenchj@newpaltz.edu Laughing Before Takeoff 396 Abstract The current study experimentally examined the effect of the use of humor in preflight safety briefings (standard script vs. humorous script) and the biological sex of the flight attendant who delivered the preflight safety briefing (female vs. male). Humorous preflight safety briefings were perceived as less rational; resulted in less content and flight attendant affect; resulted in lower levels of participant liking of the flight attendant; and resulted in lower levels of perceived flight attendant credibility. The participants in this study perceived male flight attendants to be more arousing, but viewed female flight attendants as more rational, more affective (both personally and in the preflight safety briefing content), more likeable, and more credible (competence, caring/goodwill, & trustworthiness). 397 Jason S. Wrench, Brenda Millhouse, and David Sharp Introduction On January 15th, 2005, the left nose wheel of Airbus 320 detached during takeoff, and after being placed in a holding pattern for two hours the airplane safely landed at London Gatwick Airport (AAIB Bulletin, 2005). While the flight crew was unharmed and there were only minor injuries reported by the 176 passengers on the airplane, the significance of this flight was less about the damaged airplane than about how the passengers responded during the emergency situation. According to a 2005 article in Flight Safety Australia covering the Airbus 320 event, most passengers did not know what to do when the oxygen masks were deployed as a result of a peaked cabin pressure during the emergency, “Recollection was [weak] – 20-30 percent – on topics of securing the mask by pulling the elastic band tight, fitting the mask first before helping others and breathing normally once the mask had been fitted” (“Most passengers need prompting,” (p. 40). In fact, in a survey after the incident, 95 percent of the passengers reported a lack of confidence in the operation of the emergency oxygen system. In essence, there was a considerable amount of passenger confusion on how to respond during an emergency situation. The confusion during the Airbus 320 flight is a clear indication of a larger problem currently seen in today’s commercial airline industry (“Most passengers need prompting,” 2005); the public does not know how to respond during airline emergencies. Studies in the airline industry have consistently shown that despite the inflated public perception that airline safety is “common sense,” most people do not know how to properly respond during an emergency (NTSB, 2000). In a widely cited study within the airline industry, the National Transportation Safety Board (2000) found that of passengers who had been involved in an airplane emergency landing only 48 percent of passengers said they watched at least 75 percent of the preflight safety briefing. Of those who had not watched the briefing completely, 54 percent reported that they had seen it before while 7 percent thought the briefing contained “basic knowledge.” In an airplane, the only other mode of safety instruction besides the preflight safety briefing is the safety card that resides in the seatback of every passenger seat. However, the NTSB (1985) found that 68 percent of flight passengers do not read the safety card while 44 percent reported that they did not read the safety card and did not listen to the preflight safety briefing. Furthermore, Silver, and Perlotto (1997) found that when people are actually quizzed over the information in safety cards, only 67 percent of the participants understood the meaning of half of the pictures on the safety card. In essence, the cards may be mandated by the FAA (2003), but they may not be the most useful way for providing information to airline passengers about how to handle themselves during a flight emergency. Therefore, the preflight safety briefings given by the flight attendants prior to take off serve as the number one source of information. One proposed method for handling the preflight safety briefing has been pioneered by Southwest Airlines who encourages the incorporation of humor into preflight safety briefings delivered by the flight attendants (“Creative Methods,” 2001; Freiberg & Freiberg, 1998; HofferGittell, 2005). Kathy Pettit, Southwest Airlines Director for Customer Service, was cited as saying, “We encourage [our flight attendants] to be casual and forthright. We are not afraid of song or humor during the presentation because when our flight attendants use their personalities, talents and sense of humour this results in a better relationship with customers” (“Creative Methods,” 2001, p. 37). While this method has proven fairly successful to Southwest Airlines qualitatively, “The airline does not have scientific data to support its policy on humour” (“Creative Methods,” 2001, p. 37). In fact, some argue that humor during the preflight safety Laughing Before Takeoff 398 briefing can actually be detrimental, “The technique is not without potential risks. Some passengers may respond negatively, perceiving this technique as unprofessional behaviour. Critics have also said that joking might diminish the authority of flight attendants” (“Creative Methods,” 2001, p. 37). Based on the need to scientifically study the use of humor as a way to draw attention to preflight briefings, the current study set out to experimentally test the use of humor during preflight safety briefings on passenger’s reported levels of arousal, perceptions of the preflight safety briefing’s rationality, affect towards the content of the preflight briefing and flight attendant, liking of the flight attendant, and perceptions of the flight attendant’s credibility. Rationale The goal of this section is to pose a series of hypotheses and research questions about the influence of humor in preflight safety briefings. First, this section is going to explore a series of dependent variables that have been chosen to examine the influence of humor in preflight safety briefings (arousal, rationality, affective learning, liking, & source credibility). This will be followed by a discussion of the possible influence of flight attendant biological sex in the preflight safety briefing. Dependent Variables Arousal. Arousal can be defined as the act of stirring up or making something exciting (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974). In the case of the current research, arousal will be utilized to measure the arousing effects of the preflight briefing. Consistently, research has shown that humorous artifacts lead to increased states of arousal (Chapman, 1973, 1974, 1996; Cooper, Fazio, & Rhodewalt, 1978). Furthermore, research in education has noted that infusion of humor into an otherwise traditional lecture makes the situation more exciting for students, which in turn leads to an increase in cognitive learning (Townsend & Mahoney, 1981; Wanzer & Frymier, 1999; Wrench & Richmond, 2004; Ziv, 1976, 1988). Based on this previous research, we can therefore predict: H1: Participants exposed to a humorous preflight safety briefing will experience a heightened state of arousal compared to those participants exposed to a standard preflight safety briefing. Rationality. Perceived rationality is the quality of being perceived as consistent with or based on reason (Hirschman, 1986). Hirschman found that when customers perceive an organization’s communication as rational, they are more likely to financially support that organization. However, humor is a double edge sword because some people may perceive the humor as fun while others see it as frivolous (Young & Frye, 1966). As a result, the impact of humor on passenger perceptions of the preflight safety briefings’ rationality is uncertain. Therefore, the following research question is posed: RQ1: What is the impact of humor on passenger perceptions of the preflight safety briefings’ rationality? 399 Jason S. Wrench, Brenda Millhouse, and David Sharp Affective Learning. Affective learning is “an increasing internalization of positive attitudes toward the content or subject matter” (Kearney, 1994, p. 81). While the preflight safety briefing is not education in its most traditional sense, the basic purpose of the briefing is to educate passengers on how to effectively manage themselves during an emergency situation (NTSB, 2000). Bloom, Englehart, Furst, Hill, and Krathwohl (1965) purport that there are three primary domains in which learning occurs (affective, behavioral, & cognitive), and Richmond, Wrench, and Gorham (2001) noted that affective learning is the basic path to the other two types. McCroskey (1994) believed that affective learning in the instructional process could be broken down into a series of different concepts. For the purpose of the current study, the two most important ones in the preflight safety briefing are affect toward the teacher (flight attendant) and affect toward the content of the course (the content of the preflight safety briefing). In a metaanalysis of the research examining humor and learning, Martin, Preiss, Gayle, and Allen (2006) found a moderate effect size for the relationship between student perceptions of humor and affective learning. If the preflight safety briefing functions in the same way as a traditional classroom lecture, then the following hypothesis can be posed: H2: Participants exposed to a humorous preflight safety briefing will report higher levels of both flight attendant and preflight safety briefing content affect compared to those participants exposed to a standard preflight safety briefing. Liking. There is a breadth of research examining the effect of humor on interpersonal relationships (Mettee, Hrelecm & Wilkens, 1971; O’Connell, 1969; O’Quin & Aronoff, 1981; Wanzer, Booth-Butterfield, Booth-Butterfield, 1996). As a whole, the research on humor in interpersonal relationships has consistently shown that humor in an interpersonal relationship leads to increased levels of liking and popularity (Wanzer, Booth-Butterfield, Booth-Butterfield, 1996). Furthermore, there is a clear relationship between affective learning in a classroom and liking of one’s teacher (Wanzer & McCroskey, 1998). Therefore, the following hypothesis is posed: H3: Participants exposed to a humorous preflight safety briefing will report higher levels of liking of the flight attendant who delivered the preflight safety briefing compared to those participants exposed to a standard preflight safety briefing. Source Credibility. McCroskey and Teven (1999) proposed that credibility is the combination of three factors: competence, trustworthiness, and goodwill. Competence is the extent that an individual truly knows what he or she is discussing. The second component of credibility is trustworthiness, which is the degree to which one individual perceives another person as being honest. The final component of credibility, goodwill, is the perceived caring that a receiver sees in a source. Out of all of these, goodwill may be the most important aspect of ethos (McCroskey, 1998). Research in the instructional setting has found that teachers who use humor in lectures are perceived as more credible (Wrench & Richmond, 2004), which was then replicated by Wrench and Punyanunt-Carter (2005) in the graduate advisor-advisee learning context. Since there is a clear relationship between use of humor and perceived credibility, the following hypothesis is posed: Laughing Before Takeoff 400 H4: Participants exposed to a humorous preflight safety briefing will report higher levels of perceived flight attendant credibility (competence, caring/goodwill, & trustworthiness) compared to those participants exposed to a standard preflight safety briefing. Flight Attendant Biological Sex The first reported flight attendants were actually men circa 1911 and were labeled “cabin boys” or “stewards.” The first female flight attendant, Ellen Church, was a nurse who was hired by United Airlines to provide both in-flight medical care and basic service to the passengers, and Church coined the position as the flight’s “stewardess” (Omelia & Walkdock, 2006). The need for flight attendants to be registered nurses was set aside during World War II since most nurses were needed for the war effort. By the 1950s, stewardesses were considered “wives-in-training,” and were expected to be equally adept at mixing drinks and taking care of children. Stewardesses really became highly sexualized during the 1960s and 1970s when they were actively used in airline advertisements as a way to gain businessmen’s travel business. Freiberg and Freiberg (1996) wrote that Southwest Airlines purposefully hired females who could exude sexuality to the airline’s predominantly male clientele, “Imagine this scene: Three long-legged Raquel Welch look-alikes in hot pants and white, high-heeled go-go boots serving Bloody Marys with a smile to a predominantly male group of wide-eyed, wide-awake commuters on Southwest’s 8:00 A.M. ‘Love Bird’ flight from Dallas to Houston” (p. 38). To make sure that Southwest Airlines flight attendants were adequately trained, one of the recruitment coordinators was the trainer for Hugh Hefner’s Playboy jet. In fact, Southwest’s whole motif was “Love” oriented. “Aboard the Love Bird, drinks were Love Potions peanuts were Love Bites, drink coupons were Love Stamps, and tickets came from Love Machines. In June 1971, the ‘Somebody Else Up There Loves You” airline began proudly promoting service within its Love Triangle” (Friedberg & Friendberg, 1996, p. 40). While early service of Southwest Airlines is more reminiscent of the short lived Hooter’s Air, Southwest Airlines eventually grew into one of the most powerful airlines in the United States. The success of Southwest Airlines is a culmination of many different factors, but clearly the sexualization of the Airline during its early years was very effective (Hoffer-Gittell, 2003). Early flight attendants were required to fit into a very specific mold including height, weight, age, biological sex, and marital status restrictions. The enactment of Title VII in 1965 of the Civil Rights Act of 1964 required these restrictions be forcibly lifted and male flight attendants started re-joining flight crews during the 1970s (McLaughlin, 1995; Omelia & Walkdock, 2006). Today the flight attendant industry is still over 80 percent female (Young & James, 2002). According to Gamble and Gamble (2003), any occupation where more than 75 percent of the workers belong to one specific biological sex is considered sex segregated. As a result of this sex segregation, people of the minority sex are perceived as “oddities” because they violate workplace norms. The norm within the airline industry still perceives women as flight attendants, and those flight attendants epitomize middle-class perceptions of heterosexual femininity (Hochschild, 1983). Where females are the epitome of heterosexual femininity, their counterparts, the pilots, represent the epitome of heterosexual masculinity (Mills, 1995). Male flight attendants then pose a break from the heterosexual fantasy created by traditional airlines (Butler, 2004), so people create the assumption that the violators of the heterosexual fantasy must be gay (Lock & Kleis, 1998; Patel & Long, 1995). While the exact number of gay flight attendants is not known, one study found that often heterosexual male flight attendants undergo 401 Jason S. Wrench, Brenda Millhouse, and David Sharp serious amounts of sexual harassment based purely on their perceived sexual orientation (Williams, 2003; Young & James, 2002). Since there is a bulk of research that supports the notion that violators of sex-role expectations are deemed as aberrant, the following hypothesis can be made: H5: There will be a significant difference between female and male flight attendants on the study dependent variables (arousal, rationality, affective learning, liking, & source credibility). Method Participants Participants in this study included 154 undergraduate students in a variety of majors who were approached in a variety of academic classes from two different campuses in a large midwestern university campus system. Students on these campuses tend to be older than your traditional college age sample, which is reflected in the mean found in the current study (M = 29.20, SD = 10.32). The sample consisted of 57 males (37%), 96 females (62.3%), and 1 person who did not respond to the biological sex question. Procedures The purpose of the study was to measure the impact of humorous preflight safety briefings on passenger perceptions of flight attendants. Using the requirements for preflight briefings issued by the Federal Aviation Administration (2003) in Advisory Circular 121-24C, the researchers created a preflight briefing that would sound very typical across airlines today (Appendix A). This preflight briefing was then given to two professional flight attendants to ascertain the realism of the script. Both professional flight attendants agreed that the script was realistic. Once the realism of the standard preflight safety briefing was determined, a second script was also created injecting humor throughout the entire script (Appendix B). Furthermore, the biological sex of the flight attendant was also controlled in the experiment. Participants were given one of four scripts (female standard, female humorous, male standard, or male humorous), and asked to read the script and then respond to a series of questions asking the participants to indicate their perceptions of the flight attendant who delivered the experimental script. Immediately after reading the script, the participants were asked to rate the script from “0” not humorous to “9” very humorous. An independent t-test was utilized to make sure the manipulation of the humorous script was effective. The independent ttest indicated that participants in the humorous script condition (M = 4.31, SD = 3.07) found the script more humorous than those in the standard script condition (M = 2.23, SD = 2.57); t (144) = 4.46, p < .0005, Cohen’s d = .69, which is considered a medium effect size (Green & Salkind, 2005). Overall, participants clearly indicated that the humorous script was more humorous than the standard preflight safety script. Laughing Before Takeoff 402 Instrumentation Arousal. The Arousal Scale was devised by Mehrabian and Russell (1974) and later validated by Holbrook et al. (1984) to examine how stimulated people feel during a specific situation or as a result of being exposed to a specific artifact. The scale consists of six oppositely worded adjective pairs (stimulated/relaxed, excited/calm, frenzied/sluggish, jittery/dull, wideawake/sleepy, & aroused/unaroused) measured with seven steps with the range existing from 6 to 42, which was seen in the current study. The alpha reliability of the Arousal Scale was .84 (M = 24.71, SD = 6.31). Items are coded so that higher scores equate to higher levels of reported arousal. Rationality. The Rationality Scale was devised by Elizabeth Hirschman (1986) to measure the utilitarian and rationality of a stimulus. The scale consists of five oppositely worded adjective pairs (logical/not logical, educational/not educational, informative/not informative, factual/not factual, & useful/not useful) measured with seven steps with the range existing from 5 to 35, which was seen in the current study. The alpha reliability of the Rationality Scale was .93 (M = 23.71, SD = 7.65). Items are coded so that higher scores equate to higher levels of perceived rationality. Affective Learning. The Affective Learning instrument was designed by McCroskey (1994) to mimic the objectives of the affective domain of learning original discussed by Krathwohl, Bloom, and Masia (1956). The measure examines the level of affect a student has for the course, subject matter, teacher, recommended behaviors, the desire for further courses in the area, and actually taking courses in the subject area. Each aspect of affective learning is measured by four 7-item bi-polar scales with a range existing from 4 to 28, which was seen in both of the subscales used in the current study. For the purposes of the current study, only those items for affect towards content in the preflight briefing were kept and affect towards flight attendant. The alpha reliability for affect towards content in the preflight briefing was .90 (M = 18.33, SD = 5.97), and the alpha reliability for affect towards flight attendant was .94 (M = 17.77, SD = 6.23). Items are coded so that higher scores equate to higher levels of passenger affect. Liking. The liking scale was devised by Bond, Chiu, and Wan (1984) and revised by Bresnahan, et al. (2002) to measure the degree to which a person likes another person. The scale consists of six oppositely worded adjective pairs (friendly/unfriendly, disagreeable/agreeable, pleasant/unpleasant, unkind/kind, unlikable/likeable, & cold/warm) measured with seven steps with the range existing from 6 to 42, which was seen in the current study. The alpha reliability of the Rationality Scale was .94 (M = 28.83, SD = 7.37). Items are coded so that higher scores equate to higher levels of liking. Credibility. The Credibility Measure was designed by McCroskey and Teven (1999) to test an individual’s perception of a communicator’s credibility (competence, trustworthiness, & caring/goodwill). Since the original measure was designed to be used in a number of situations to examine a communicator’s credibility, the measure used in this study did not have to be re-tooled to examine passenger perceptions of flight attendant credibility. The measure is a series of eighteen oppositely worded adjectives with a seven step bi-polar scale that is broken into three factors with 6 items on each factor. Each factor has a range from 6 to 42, which was seen in this study. The alpha reliability for competence in this study was .92 (M = 27.84, SD = 8.38); trustworthiness was .90 (M = 27.18; SD = 7.68); and caring/goodwill was .93 (M = 24.92; SD = 403 Jason S. Wrench, Brenda Millhouse, and David Sharp 8.21). Higher scores indicated that a communicator was perceived as highly credible by her or his receiver. Data Analysis To analyze the study’s data in the most parsimonious way, a series of factorial univariate ANOVAS and MANOVAs will be conducted. The two factors being analyzed will be flight attendant sex (male vs. female) and preflight safety briefing script (standard vs. humorous), which will be used as the independent variables, and passenger’s reported levels of arousal, perceptions of the preflight safety briefings rationality, affect towards the content of the preflight briefing and flight attendant, liking of the flight attendant, and perceptions of the flight attendant’s credibility as the dependent variables. Results A Univariate Factorial ANOVA was conducted using flight attendant sex (male vs. female) and preflight safety briefing script (standard vs. humorous) as the independent variables and arousal as the dependent variable. Main effect results revealed that the humorous (M = 25.19, SD = 5.74) and the standard (M = 24.26, SD = 6.83) preflight safety briefing scripts did not lead to differing levels of arousal among the participants, F (1, 149) = .72, p > .05. Main effect results did indicate a significant difference in participant arousal based on whether the flight attendant delivering the script was a female (M = 23.33, SD = 6.35) or male (M = 26.26, SD = 5.93), F (1, 149) = 8.42, p < .005, η2 = .05. Lastly, the interaction between preflight safety briefing script and flight attendant biological sex was not statistically significant, F (1, 149) = .24, p > .05. A Univariate Factorial ANOVA was conducted using flight attendant sex (male vs. female) and preflight safety briefing script (standard vs. humorous) as the independent variables and rationality as the dependent variable. Main effect results revealed that the humorous (M = 21.96, SD = 7.69) and the standard (M = 25.42, SD = 7.25) preflight safety briefing scripts led to differing perceptions of rationality by the participants, F (1, 150) = 8.11, p < .005, η2 = .05. Main effect results also indicated a significant difference in participant rationality based on whether the flight attendant delivering the script was a female (M = 26.05, SD = 7.43) or male (M = 21.12, SD = 7.07), F (1, 150) = 18.03, p < .0005, η2 = .11. Lastly, the interaction between preflight safety briefing script and flight attendant biological sex was not statistically significant, F (1, 149) = .24, p > .05. A two-way MANOVA was conducted to determine the effect of flight attendant sex (male vs. female) and preflight safety briefing script (standard vs. humorous) on content affect and flight attendant affect. MANOVA results indicated that that the humorous (M = 17.37, SD = 6.32) and the standard (M = 19.27, SD = 5.48) preflight safety briefing scripts did not significantly affect the combined DV of content effect and flight attendant affect, Wilks’ Λ = .97, F (2, 149) = 2.23, p > .05. However, MANOVA results indicated that the flight attendants’ biological sex significantly effect the combined DV of content affect and flight attendant affect, Wilks’ Λ = .90, F (2, 149) = 8.16, p < .0005; so univariate ANOVA post hoc tests were conducted. ANOVA results indicate that participants viewed their affect towards the content of the preflight safety briefing differently depending on whether the flight attendant was a female (M = 20.10, SD = 5.61) or male (M = 16.36, SD = 5.72), F (1, 150) = 16.33, p < .0005, η2 = .10. Laughing Before Takeoff 404 ANOVA results also indicated that participants viewed their affect towards the flight attendant differently depending on whether the flight attendant delivering the preflight safety briefing was a female (M = 19.33, SD = 5.95) or male (M = 16.03, SD = 6.10), F (1, 150) = 11.45, p < .005, η2 = .07. Lastly, an interaction effect between script and sex was not statistically significant, Wilks’ Λ = .99, F (2, 149) = 1.07, p > .05. A Univariate Factorial ANOVA was conducted using flight attendant sex (male vs. female) and preflight safety briefing script (standard vs. humorous) as the independent variables and liking as the dependent variable. Main effect results revealed that the humorous (M = 28.37, SD = 7.30) and the standard (M = 29.28, SD = 7.46) preflight safety briefing scripts did not lead to differing levels of flight attendant liking among the participants, F (1, 150) = .45, p > .05. Main effect results did indicate a significant difference in participant liking of the flight attendant based on whether the flight attendant delivering the script was a female (M = 30.15, SD = 7.11) or male (M = 27.37, SD = 7.43), F (1, 150) = 5.46, p < .05, η2 = .04. Lastly, the interaction between preflight safety briefing script and flight attendant biological sex was not statistically significant, F (1, 150) = 1.02, p > .05. A two-way MANOVA was conducted to determine the effect of flight attendant sex (male vs. female) and preflight safety briefing script (standard vs. humorous) on the three factors of perceived flight attendant credibility (competence, caring/goodwill, & trustworthiness). First, MANOVA results indicated that the preflight safety briefing script significantly affected the combined DV of source credibility, Wilks’ Λ = .93, F (3, 146) = 3.71, p < .05; so univariate ANOVA post hoc tests were conducted. ANOVA results indicate that participants viewed the flight attendant who delivered the preflight safety briefing’s level of competence, F (1, 148) = 9.93, p < .005, η2 = .06, and trustworthiness, F (1, 148) = 4.53, p < .05, η2 = .03, differently based on the script they read, but not the flight attendant’s level of caring/goodwill, F (1, 148) = 2.91, p > .05. Means and standard deviations are located in Table 1. Second, MANOVA results indicated that the biological sex of the flight attendant delivering the preflight safety briefing significantly affected the combined DV of source credibility, Wilks’ Λ = .84, F (3, 146) = 9.11, p < .0005. ANOVA post hoc results indicate that participants viewed the flight attendant who Table 1: Means for Source Credibility by Flight Attendant Sex and Preflight Safety Briefing Script Competence Caring/Goodwill Trustworthiness M SD M SD M SD Flight Attendant Biological Sex Female Male 30.49 24.77 6.95 8.93 27.81 21.46 6.77 8.40 29.81 24.05 6.26 8.01 Script Standard Humorous 29.88 25.71 8.06 8.28 26.01 23.65 8.11 8.15 28.43 25.77 7.45 7.72 delivered the preflight safety briefing differently based on the flight attendant’s biological sex on all three levels of credibility: competence, F (1, 148) = 20.09, p < .0005, η2 = .12; caring/goodwill, F (1, 148) = 26.45, p < .0005, η2 = .15; trustworthiness, F (1, 148) = 24.74, p < .0005, η2 = .14. Means and standard deviations are located in Table 1. Lastly, an interaction 405 Jason S. Wrench, Brenda Millhouse, and David Sharp effect between script and sex was not statistically significant, Wilks’ Λ = .99, F (3, 146) = .58, p > .05. Discussion The findings of this study are clearly intriguing and definitely need to be examined as to their actual significance for communication research. To examine these findings, it is most prudent to examine the use of humor in preflight safety briefings and flight attendant biological sex separately, which will be followed by a discussion of the limitations and directions for future research. Humor in Preflight Safety Briefings As a whole, the results for the preflight safety briefings are quite intriguing and contrary to previous research that has examined the use of humor in various contexts. Humorous preflight safety briefings were not found to be more arousing, but were perceived as less rational; resulted in less content and flight attendant affect; resulted in lower levels of participant liking of the flight attendant; and lower levels of perceived flight attendant credibility. These results are contrary to the predictions made in this study. One may think that the manipulation in the study possibly had not occurred, but the independent t-test used to test manipulation did find that the study participants found the humorous script significantly more humorous than the standard script. However, the effect size was only medium, but even at a medium effect people clearly found the humorous script funny. So why then would the participants find the script humorous, but react more favorably to the standard script? Two theoretical lines of thought will be discussed to explore methodologically why the results could occur, and one theoretical line of thought will be discussed to explore how these results could be generalized to the real world. First, Attardo (1994) in his text Linguistic Theories of Humor puts forth the notion of canned versus conversational joking. Canned jokes are jokes that are created jokes that can be used in a variety of contexts without regard of the actual situation where the joke is used; where as, conversational jokes are jokes that are inherently situationally based and do not transfer easily to other communicative contexts. The jokes utilized in this study were designed to be conversationally based, but presented in a canned, non-organic manner. A conversational “joke that hinges on a very situational-specific element will be (nearly) impossible to use outside of that situation. The only strategy available to the speaker is to try and reconstruct the situation verbally …, so as to make the situational joke exportable to another context” (Attardo, 1994, p. 297). While the humor used within the scripts in this study was designed as conversational, the research context is realistically more canned than situationally based. While the humorous script was intended to be read as conversational, reading humorous jokes is highly decontextualized. Furthermore, the humor used in the preflight safety briefing was designed to mimic conversational patterns, but Attardo (1994) notes that read jokes are read as singular instances of joking, which is more similar to canned jokes; whereas, conversational jokes “often build on previous jokes and seem to acquire an extra degree of funniness by doing so” (p. 299). In essence, it is theoretically plausible that the reaction to the scripts in this study has less to do with the humor used than it does with the manner in which the jokes were presented in this study through the read scripts. Laughing Before Takeoff 406 A second theoretical reason for the contrary results surrounding the humorous preflight safety briefing scripts in the current study could be due to the lack of a shared experience. Humor, as it exists in actual preflight safety briefings, like those seen on Southwest Airlines, is a shared experience had by many; where as, the scripts read in this study were individualized experiences. Previous research summarized by Sheehy-Skeffington (1977) noted the impact on humor caused by other people’s laughter. Basically, the sound of other people’s laughter has been shown to increase the mirth experience and the impact humor has on people. SheehySkeffington theorizes that “laughter serves as a situational cue, conditioning the listener to search for a humorous interpretation of the material” (p. 448). Furthermore, Chapman (1996) noted that humorous artifacts presented within a group situation are considerably more arousing than when presented to an individual in isolation. As Provine (2000) notes, “The ability of laughter to elicit contagious laughter raises the intriguing possibility that humans have an auditory laugh-detector – a neural circuit in our brain that responds exclusively to laughter” (p. 149). Not only do people respond to laughter in a clear fashion, the actual act of laughing also has neurological functions, specifically the release of various neuropeptides, including endorphins, and the neurotransmitter serotonin (McGhee, 1999). In essence, the actual act of laughing alters the neurological state, which could in turn alter how someone views the humorous artifact. Overall, the shared experience of humor is extremely important, and since this study relied on textual based scripts, there really was not a shared experience, which could be a causative factor of the results seen in this study. The last theoretical perspective that could explain the nature of the results seen in this study stems out of the notion of the preflight safety briefing context. The current study was conducted on post 9-11 air travel. As Sparks, Kreps, Botan, and Rowan (2005) note, “Terrorism, not surprisingly, has become a significant topic in crisis management, academe, industry, and in our communities” (p. 1). People in the United States feel the aftermath of 9-11 every time they fly because of the new heightened security standards at the nation’s airports. 9-11 could be an event that is seen as non-joking event, and humor in the preflight safety briefing could be seen as frivolous and inappropriate by many passengers. Lundgren and McMakin (2004) warn risk communicators to avoid using humor during risk communication because it is seen as insensitive. In the same way, flight attendants are essentially communicating the possibility of air travel risk and what actions to take if a crisis occurs. Based on this notion, it is theoretically plausible then that the preflight safety briefing is a context where passengers simply do not perceive the humor as appropriate. Flight Attendant Biological Sex The second category of results examined in this study relates to the impact that a flight attendant’s biological sex had on the study dependent variables. The results in this study indicated that passengers perceived male flight attendants to be more arousing, but participants viewed female flight attendants were more rational, more affective (both personally and in the preflight safety briefing content), more likeable, and more credible. Based on the understanding that eta-squared should be interpreted as either small (0.01), medium (0.06), and large (0.14) (Cohen, 1988), there was a minimal effect for arousal; a moderate effect for rationality, content affect, flight attendant affect, liking, and competence; and a large effect for caring/goodwill and trustworthiness. In essence, participants in this study clearly perceived the female and male flight attendants differently in this study based on nothing more than being given a biological sex and 407 Jason S. Wrench, Brenda Millhouse, and David Sharp the flight attendants name (Amy or Fredrick). In fact, there appears to be a very clear discriminatory position taken by the participants in this study that favors female flight attendants and devalues male flight attendants. These findings actually reinforce the notions from Gamble and Gamble (2003) that minority individuals in sex-segregated professions, or as Young and James (2002) labeled male flight attendants as the “token majority,” struggle to be taken seriously as fully participating workers. One possible reason for why male flight attendants are not valued as highly as female flight attendants could be because male flight attendants violate Hochschild’s (1983) notions of middle class heterosexuality. In essence, there is this myth in the general public that females should be flight attendants while males should be pilots (Mills, 1995), and as Judith Butler (2004) reasoned, people who violate the heterosexual myth are seen as deviants not to be trusted. Or in the case of male flight attendants, people generally reason that they must be gay (Lock & Kleis, 1998; Patel & Long, 1995). Based on research concerning people in the United States’ overarching homonegativity, finding that people would automatically view men who they perceive as violating heterosexual norms by being flight attendants would be subject to scorn and ridicule or at least be perceived as untrustworthy. These results are similar to the findings of Mottet (2000) who found that people predicted higher outcome values, higher levels of communication, and higher levels of liking when a target was explicitly labeled as heterosexual than when the target was explicitly labeled as homosexuality. Since there is this overarching stereotype that male flight attendants are gay, the current study’s findings may be viewed as an extension of the findings of Mottet (2000). Limitations This study has a number of limitations that need to be examined. First, the study relies on scripted scenarios for the experimental design employed. While the read scripts cannot be completely blamed for the lack of findings related to humor, as discussed earlier, the reading of humor is distinctly different than witnessing humor as a shared experience. The results in this study related to humor could be an outcome of the method, and not the actual impact of humor during preflight safety briefings. Second, the study relied on college students and not people sitting in a realistic airline environment. While the college students employed in this study are older than traditional age college students, this study still depended on students in an educational environment and not passengers on an actual airplane. Lastly, based on the questions asked in the current study, there really is no way to ascertain if people in the study actually viewed male flight attendants as gay men. While previous literature has suggested that passengers assume male flight attendants are gay (Williams, 2003; Young & James, 2002), without questioning the participants’ perceptions of male flight attendants as a group, we are basing our analysis of these findings not on concrete fact but on theoretical relationships that may not exist. Future Research First, future research in the area of humor in preflight safety briefings should compare read scripts, video taped enactments of scripts, and live enactments of scripts. While it is suspected that there is a difference between the three, the only way to know for sure would be to Laughing Before Takeoff 408 compare the delivery of the three methods. This information would be useful for future experimental designs examining humor. Second, ideally, this study should be replicated in an actual airline situation with real flight attendants not using fabricated scripts. While the humorous script was perceived as more humorous than the standard script in this study, the script was still considerably more canned and less conversational because of the nature of the study. Furthermore, future research should survey actual airline passengers. While this research would probably be more survey design than experimental, real survey data form actual passengers would be considerably more valid and generalizable than experimental designs in a contrived college classroom. Lastly, future research examining the differences between female and male flight attendants should examine people’s perceptions of both female and male flight attendants’ sexuality. These perceptions of sexuality could then be used to determine if it is the stereotyping that causes female flight attendants to be more valued than male flight attendants, or some other intervening variable. Furthermore, controlling for a participants’ level of homonegativity and ethnocentrism could also be very telling. Conclusion In 2005, 745.7 million passengers flew on both domestic and international flights from the United States, a 4.6 percent increase from the previous year (Smallen, 2006). As more and more passengers fly, the numbers of in-flight emergencies naturally increase (NTSB, 2000). As airlines, the NTSB, and the FAA look to find out how to increase passenger awareness of inflight safety procedures, two variables that appear to be relevant to this discussion based on the findings in this study are humor use during the preflight safety briefing and the biological sex of the flight attendant who delivers the preflight safety briefing. While the current project hopes to initiate a new line of research for communication scholars examining cabin crew-passenger communication, the results of the current project are mixed. If the preflight safety briefing had been more thoughtfully delivered on Airbus flight 320, maybe 95 percent of the passengers would not have expressed a lack in confidence about in-flight safety procedures. 409 Jason S. Wrench, Brenda Millhouse, and David Sharp References Air Accidents Investigation Branch Bulletin. (2005, February). Airbus A320-214m G-BKXD. Aldershot, Hampshire: Author. Attardo, S. (1994). Linguistic theories of humor. New York: Mouton De Gruyter. Bloom, B. S., Englehart, M. D., Furst, E. J., Hill, W. H., & Krathwohl, D. R. (1965). Taxonomy of educational objectives – The classification of educational goals, handbook I: Cognitive domain. New York: David McKay. Bond, M. H., Chiu, C. K., & Wan, K. C. (1984). When modesty fails: The social impact of group-effacing attributes following success or failure. European Journal of Social Psychology, 14, 335-338. Bresnahan, M. J., Shearman, S. M., Lee, S. Y., Park, C. Y., Mosher, D., & Ohashi, R. (2002). Responding to a friend: Perceptions of social appropriateness and liking in Japan, Korea, and the United States. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research, 31, 127-144. Butler, J. (2004). Undoing gender. New York: Taylor and Francis. Chapman, A. J. (1973). An electromyographic study of apprehension about evaluation. Psychological Reports, 33, 811-814. Chapman, A. J. (1974). An electromyographic study of social facilitation: A test of the ‘mere presence’ hypothesis. British Journal of Psychology, 65, 123-128. Chapman, A. J. (1996). Social aspects of humorous laughter. In. A. J. Chapman & H. C. Foot (Eds.), Humor and laughter: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 155-185). New York: John Wiley & Sons. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum. Cooper, J., Fazio, R. H., & Rhodewalt, F. (1978). Dissonance and humor: Evidence for the undifferentiated nature of dissonance arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 280-85. Creative methods may be the answer to improving passengers’ attention to safety briefings before take-off. (2001, July/August). Flight Safety Australia, 5 (4), 36-37. Federal Aviation Administration. (2003, July 23). Advisory circular: Passenger safety information briefing and briefing cards (AC No: 121-24C). Washington, DC: Author. Freiberg, K., & Freiberg, J. (1998). Nuts!: Southwest Airlines’ crazy recipe for business and personal success. New York: Bantam Doubleday Dell Publishing. Gamble, T. K., & Gamble, M. W. (2003). The gender communication connection. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Green, S. B., & Salkind, S. J. (2005). Using SPSS for Windows and Macintosh: Analyzing and understanding data (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson-Prentice Hall. Hirschman, E. C. (1986). The effect of verbal and pictorial advertising stimuli on aesthetic, utilitarian and family perceptions. Journal of Advertising, 15, 27-34. Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The managed heart: Commercialization of human feeling. Berkley, CA: University of California Press. Hoffer-Gittell, J. (2005). The Southwest Airlines way: Using the power of relationships to achieve high performance. New York: McGraw Hill. Holbrook, M. B., Chestnut, R. W., Oliva, T. A., & Greenleaf, E. A. (1984). Play as a consumption experience: The roles of emotion, performance, and personality in the enjoyment of games. Journal of Consumer Research, 11, 728-739. Laughing Before Takeoff 410 Kearney, P. (1994). Affective learning. In R. B. Rubin, P. Palmgreen, & H. E. Sypher (Eds.), Communication Research Measures: A Sourcebook (pp. 81-85). New York: The Guilford Press. Krathwohl, D. R., Bloom, B. S., & Masia, B. B. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objectives, Handbook II: Affective Domain. New York: David McKay. Lock, J., & Kleis, B. (1998). Origins of homophobia in males: Psychosexual vulnerabilities and defuse development. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 52, 425-437. Lundgren, R., & McMakin, A. (2004). Risk communication: A handbook for communicating environmental, safety, and health risks (3rd ed.). Columbus, OH: Battelle Press. Martin, D. M., Preiss, R. W., Gayle, B. M., & Allen, M. (2006). A meta-analytic assessment of the effect of humorous lectures on learning. In B. M. Gayle, R. W. Preiss, N. Burrell, & M. Allen (Eds.), Classroom communication and instructional processes: Advances through meta-analysis (pp. 295-313). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. McCroskey, J. C. (1994). Assessment of affect toward communication and affect toward instruction in communication. In S. Morreale & M. Brooks. (Eds.), 1994 SCA summer conference proceedings and prepared remarks: Assessing college student competence in speech communication. Annandale, VA: Speech Communication Association. McCroskey, J. C. (1998). An introduction to communication in the classroom (2nd Ed.). Acton, MA: Tapestry Press. McCroskey, J. C., & Teven, J. J. (1999). Goodwill: A reexamination of the construct and its measurement. Communication Monographs, 66, 90-103. McGhee, P. E. (1999). Health, healing, and the amuse system: Humor as survival training. Dubueque, IW: Kendall Hunt. McLaughlin, H. E. (1995). Footsteps in the sky: An informal review of U. S. Airlines inflight service, 1920-Present. Denver, CO: State of the Art, Ltd. Mettee, D. R., Hrelecm, E. S., & Wilkens, P. C. (1971). Humor as an interpersonal asset and liability. The Journal of Social Psychology, 85, 51-64. Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J. A. (1974). An approach to environmental psychology. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Mills, A. J. (1995). Cockpits, hangars, boys and galleys: Corporate masculinities and the development of British Airways. Gender, Work and Organization, 5, 172-188. Most passengers need prompting to focus their attention on safety information according to the Flight Safety Foundation. (2005, March/April). Flight Safety Australia, 9 (2), 40-41. Mottet, T. P. (2000). The role of sexual orientation in predicting outcome value and anticipated communication. Communication Quarterly, 48, 223-249. National Transportation Safety Board. (1985). Airline passenger safety education: A review of methods used to present safety information (NTSB No. SS-85/04). Washington, DC: Author. National Transportation Safety Board. (2000). Safety study: Emergency evacuation of commercial airplanes (NTIS No. PB2000-917002). Washington, DC: Author. O’Connell, W. E. (1969). The social aspects of wit and humor. Journal of Social Psychology, 79, 183-87. O’Quin, K., & Aronoff, J. (1981). Humor as a technique of social influence. Social Psychology Quarterly, 44, 349-357. Omelia, J., & Walkdock, M. (2006). Come fly with us! A global history of the airline hostess. Portland, OR: Collectors Press. 411 Jason S. Wrench, Brenda Millhouse, and David Sharp Patel, S., & Long, T. E. (1995). Personality and emotional correlates of self-reported antigay behaviors. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 10, 354-367. Provine, R. R. (2000). Laughter: A scientific investigation. New York: Viking. Richmond, V. P., Wrench, J. S., Gorham, J. (2001). Communication, affect, and learning in the classroom. Acton, MA: Tapestry Press. Sheehy-Skeffington, A. (1977). The measurement of humour appreciation. In A. J. Chapman & H. C. Foot (Eds.), It’s a funny thing, Humour (pp. 447-450). New York: Pergamon. Silver, N. C., & Perlotto, C. N. (1997, September). Comprehension of aviation safety pictograms: Gender and prior safety card reading influences. Paper presented at the 41st Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Albuquerque, NM. Smallen, D. (2006). 2005 airline system passengers traffic up 4.6% from 2004. U.S. Department of Transportation Bureau of Transportation Statistics, 20. Retrieved May 4, 2006 from http://www.bts.gov/press_releases/2006/bts020_06/pdf/bts020_06.pdf Sparks, L., Kreps, G. L., Botan, C., & Rowan, K. E. (2005). Responding to terrorism: Translating communication research into practice. Communication Research Reports, 22, 1-5. Townsend, M. A. R., & Mahoney, P. (1981). Humor and anxiety: Effects on class test performance. Psychology in the Schools, 18, 228-234. Wanzer, M. B., Booth-Butterfield, S., & Booth-Butterfield, M. (1996). Are funny people popular? An examination of humor orientation, loneliness, and social attraction. Communication Quarterly, 44, 42-52. Wanzer, M. B., & Frymier, A. B. (1999). The relationship between student perceptions of instructor humor and students’ reports of learning. Communication Education, 48, 205218. Wanzer, M B., & McCroskey, J. C. (1998). Teacher socio-communicative style as a correlate of student affect toward teach and course material. Communication Education, 47, 43-52. Williams, C. (2003). Sky service: The demands of emotional labour in the airline industry. Gender, Work, and Organization, 10, 513-550. Wrench, J. S., & Punyanunt, N. M. (2005). Advisor-advisee communication two: The influence of verbal aggression and humor assessment on advisee perceptions of advisor credibility and affective learning. Communication Research Reports, 22, 303-313. Wrench, J. S., & Richmond, V. P. (2004). Understanding the psychometric properties of the humor assessment instrument through an analysis of the relationships between teacher humor assessment and instructional communication variables in the college classroom. Communication Research Reports, 21, 92-103. Young, J. L., & James, E. H. (2001). Token majority: The work attitudes of male flight attendants. Sex Roles, 45 (5/6), 299-319. Young, R. D., & Frye, M. (1966). Some are laughing; Some are not – Why? Psychological Reports, 18, 747-754. Ziv, A. (1976). Facilitating effects of humor on creativity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 318-322. Ziv, A. (1988). Teaching and learning with humor: Experiment and replication. Journal of Experimental Education, 57, 5-15. Laughing Before Takeoff 412 Appendix A Preflight Safety Briefing (Standard) Instructions: Please read the following pre-flight briefing given by a female (male) flight attendant prior to the take-off of the aircraft. Hello and welcome aboard flight 475 with nonstop service to LAX, my name is Amy (Fredrick) and I would like to welcome you aboard Generic Airlines. Before we begin our flight today, Generic Airlines would like you to be aware of the following safety instructions: To fasten your seatbelt please press both ends of the buckle together until you hear the click, and pull on the strap to tighten. Your seatbelt should be snug against your waist while seated. To unfasten your safety belt, lift up on the seatbelt buckle. Once we reach our cruising altitude the captain will turn off the fasten seatbelt light and you are free move about the cabin. However, please keep your seatbelt fasten at all times while seating. In case of turbulence, the captain will turn on the fasten seatbelt light, and you will need to refrain from moving about the cabin. Please notice that the fasten seatbelt light is currently on and will stay lit through both takeoff and landings. During take offs and landings please refrain from using portable electronic devices. Once we are in the air, the captain will signal when it’s safe to use portable electronic devices. To see a list of approved electronic devices, please see page 76 of the in-flight magazine located in the seatback pocket in front of you. Our emergency exits are located in row one, sixteen, and twenty-three. During an emergency landing, guide lights will help you find the nearest emergency exit. If you are seated in an exit row, please review the safety card located in the seatback in front of you concerning the requirements and responsibilities of sitting in an exit row. If you do not meet the requirements or do not feel comfortable meeting the responsibilities, please inform a member of the flight crew to be moved to a non-exit row seat. In the event of a water landing, your seat cushions may be used as a flotation device. If our aircraft should loose cabin air pressure, oxygen masks will deploy from the ceiling. Place the oxygen mask firmly on your head, pull on the straps to tighten the mask, and then breathe naturally. If you are traveling with small children, please place your oxygen mask on first before trying to assist others. Once again, please review the safety card located in the seatback in front of you for further information about our emergency procedures. As per Federal Aviation Administration regulations, this is a non-smoking flight, so smoking is strictly prohibited aboard our aircraft. Tampering with, disabling, or destroying smoke detectors in the airplane or lavatories is punishable under Federal Law. Once you have exited our aircraft and entered into the airport terminal, you may smoke in designated smoking areas only. When it comes to flying, we know that you have a choice, and thank you for choosing to fly with Generic Airlines. We hope you enjoy your flight. If there is anything our flight crew can do to make you more comfortable please let them know. We will be departing shortly. 413 Jason S. Wrench, Brenda Millhouse, and David Sharp Appendix B Preflight Safety Briefing (Humorous) Instructions: Please read the following pre-flight briefing given by a male (female) flight attendant prior to the take-off of the aircraft. Hello and welcome aboard flight 574 with nonstop service to the arctic circle… no, sorry, wrong airplane… Welcome abroad flight 475 with nonstop service to LAX, my name is Fredrick (Amy) and on behalf of our flight crew and recent Microsoft Flight School grad captain Earhart, no relation so don’t worry, I would like to welcome you aboard Generic Airlines. Before our flight today, please be aware of the following safety instructions: If you were fortunate enough to be seated in a seat equipped with a safety belt please press both ends of the buckle together until you hear the click to fasten your seatbelt, and pull on the strap to tighten. Your seatbelt should be snug against your waist while seated. To unfasten your safety belt, lift up on the seatbelt buckle. Once we reach our cruising altitude the captain will turn off the fasten seatbelt light and you are free to move about the cabin. However, in the event of a midair collision, please keep your seatbelt fasten at all times while seated. In case of turbulence, the captain will turn on the fasten seatbelt light, and you will need to return to your seat and refrain from shouting expletives. Please notice that the fasten seatbelt light is currently on and will stay lit during takeoffs and landings. During takeoffs and landings please refrain from using portable electronic devices, especially devices that are necessary for your entertainment or business use. Once in flight, the captain will signal when it’s safe to use portable electronic. To see a list of approved electronic devices, please see page 10,076 of the abbreviated in-flight handbook located in the seatback pocket in front of you. Our emergency exits are located in row one, sixteen, and twenty-three. The exit in row twentynine was done away with after the plane’s accident and a new shorter tail section was attached. During an emergency landing, guide lights will help you find the nearest emergency exit. If you are seated in an exit row, please review the safety card located in the seatback in front of you concerning the requirements and responsibilities of sitting in an exit row. If you do not meet the requirements or do not feel comfortable meeting the responsibilities, please inform a member of the flight crew to be moved to a non-exit row seat. You should know the survival rate of passengers sitting in an exit row is just as good as any other passenger on the plane. In the event of a water landing, your seat cushions may be used as a flotation device. If our aircraft should loose cabin air pressure, oxygen masks will deploy from the ceiling. Place the oxygen mask firmly on your head, pull on the straps to tighten the mask, and then breathe naturally. If you are traveling with small child, or someone acting like a child, please place your oxygen mask on first and assist your favorite child next. Once again, please review the abbreviated in flight handbook located in the seatback in front of you for further information about our emergency procedures. As per California emission laws, this is a non-smoking airplane, so smoking is strictly prohibited aboard our aircraft. Tampering with, disabling, or destroying smoke detectors in the airplane or Laughing Before Takeoff 414 lavatories is punishable under Federal Law. Once you have exited our aircraft and entered into the airport terminal, you may smoke in designated smoking areas only, breathe un-recycled air, and/or pay ridiculous prices for any item you may have forgotten to pack. When it comes to flying, we know that you have a choice, and thank you for choosing to fly with Generic Airlines despite our safety record. We hope you enjoy your flight. If there is anything our flight crew can do to make you more comfortable please let them know. Please note we are presently out of pillows, blankets, and we will be unable to make any seating changes as it is a hassle. We will be departing shortly. Thank you.