Size and Structure Matter to Mobile Users: An Empirical

advertisement
Size and Structure Matter to Mobile Users: An Empirical Study of the
Effects of Screen Size, Information Structure, and Task Complexity on User
Activities with Standard Web Phones
Citation: Chae, M . and Kim, J. (2004), Size and Structure Matter to Mobile Users:
An Empirical Study of the Effects of Screen Size, Information Structure, and Task
Complexity on User Activities with Standard Web Phones, Behaviour & Information
Technology, forthcoming
Minhee Chae and Jinwoo Kim
Human Computer Interaction Lab
Yonsei University
Seoul Korea
minheechae@yahoo.co.kr , jinwoo@base.yonsei.ac.kr
Primary contact person:
Jinwoo Kim, Professor
School of Business
Yonsei University
Seoul, 120-749, Korea
Tel) +822 2123 2528 Fax) +822 313 5331
1
Abstract
The small screens of mobile Internet devices, combined with the increasing complexity of
mobile tasks, create a serious obstacle to usability in the mobile Internet.
One way to
circumvent the obstacle is to organize an information structure with efficient depth/ breadth
tradeoffs.
A controlled lab experiment was conducted to investigate how screen size and
information structure affect user behaviors and perceptions.
The moderating effects of task
complexity on the relationship between screen size/information structure and user
navigation/perceptions were also investigated.
Study results indicate that both information
structure and screen size significantly affect the navigation behavior and perceptions of mobile
Internet users.
Task complexity was also found to heighten the influence of information
structure on user behavior and perceptions.
The paper ends with a discussion of theoretical and
practical implications, among them a key implication for mobile Internet businesses:
for
corporate intranet systems as well as m-commerce transaction systems, the horizontal depth of
information structures should be adapted to task complexity and anticipated screen size.
Keywords:
Mobile Internet, task complexity, screen size, breadth/depth tradeoffs,
menu/information structure
2
Size and Structure Matter to Mobile Users:
An Empirical Study of the Effects of Screen Size, Information Structure, and Task
Complexity on User Activities with Standard Web Phones
1. Introduction
The mobile Internet offers wireless access via handheld devices to the digitized contents of the
Internet (Francis 1997). A survey study has projected that the number of mobile Internet users
in the world will grow eighteen-fold between 2000 and 2005, to about 729 million (Intermarket
Group 2002).
The number of people in Japan using the mobile Internet already exceeds the
number using the traditional stationary Internet. Many forecasters, basing their predictions on
the increasing prevalence of standard mobile Internet phones, suggest that in the near future most
Internet access will take place by means of small wireless devices, equipped with a browser and
a wireless connection, that provide “anywhere and anytime” access (Buyukkoken and GarciaMolina 2000, Buyukkoken et al. 2000).
The popularity of the mobile Internet is not surprising
when one considers the considerable benefits it offers Internet users, for it enables them to access
Internet information at the moment of need, whether or not a desktop PC is available
(Buyukkoken and Garcia-Molina 2000).
The growth of various mobile technologies also holds
the promise of increasingly effective tools for wireless access, such as new protocols and
3
browsers adapted to the mobile interface (Intermarket Group 2002).
However, despite the industry’s conviction that the mobile Internet is the next “killer
application,” the reactions of actual users are quite negative in terms of usability (Nielsen and
Ramsay 2000).
Their disappointing experiences with the mobile Internet result from the
limitations that distinguish mobile devices from conventional desktop PCs (Chae and Kim 2003).
Mobile Internet devices, especially Internet-enabled phones, have resources vastly inferior to
those of the desktop computers that access the traditional stationary Internet.
Most current
mobile Internet devices suffer from small screens, low bandwidths, limited storage, a short
battery life, slow CPU speed, and cumbersome input facilities (Kamba et al. 1996, Albers and
Kim 2000, Buchanan et al. 2000). In short, usability is the greatest barrier between what the
mobile Internet could be and what it currently is (Venkatesh et al. 2003).
Though mobile devices will, in future generations, gradually redress many of their
present limitations, the display is not likely to become much larger, for the need for portability
will continue to constrain the size of the screen. Because text size cannot be reduced below a
threshold of legibility, only a small amount of information can be shown on the screen at a time;
on the typical mobile Internet phone, there are fewer than 15 lines vertically and fewer than 12
characters per line.
Therefore, most of the time users cannot be shown complete lists of
available options within the screen’s display area. Instead they have to scroll through the menu
4
list, select an option, scroll through a sub-menu, select an option, and so on, repeatedly.
They
are required to perform multiple key presses and may commit numerous navigation errors
(Albers and Kim 2000).
The usability problems faced by mobile Internet devices are exacerbated by the nature
of the tasks that users characteristically perform with them.
Users frequently face situations in
which they must access complex information at the point of need, as, for instance, when a user
wishes to find the gas station nearest his or her current location. In such a situation, unless
users can retrieve the exact information they want immediately, they cannot reap the benefits of
the mobile Internet, namely, portability and instant accessibility.
Moreover, mobile users
involved in complex tasks are more likely to be under time pressure than traditional Internet
users are, and thus may be more prone to errors when they try to accomplish a task.
One way to address these usability problems would be to develop an efficient menu
structure, one that took into consideration the small screen and the complex nature of the tasks
users can perform on the mobile Internet.
Currently, information on a mobile screen is
presented to users in the form of a strict hierarchy.
Compared to conventional menus in
stationary Internet systems, which provide users with multiple paths to a given target page, most
mobile content menus only allow one-path navigation.
Moreover, the limited screen size forces
most mobile browsers to support only a line-based navigation, with a few soft keys such as OK,
5
Clear, Previous, and Next.
The keys, when pressed, display a page on the next level down, the
page directly above, the next page on the same level, or the previous page on the same level,
respectively. Moreover, mobile users are unable to jump directly from page to page, and are
required instead to follow paths or links sequentially.
Such a stepwise, line-based navigation
has been known to lead users to unexpected, undesired outcomes (Wallace et al. 1995).
Several
studies have indicated that mobile Internet users suffer more severely from the problem of
undesired outcomes than stationary Internet users do (Kim et al. 2002, Nielsen and Ramsay
2000).
In sum, the portability of the mobile Internet poses a formidable design challenge:
how can the information necessary for complex tasks be presented effectively on small screens
that offer only limited navigation facilities? The main goal of this study is to examine how
information structure and screen size affect a mobile user’s navigation activities and perceptions.
A secondary goal is to identify the moderating effect of task complexity on the relation between
information structure/screen size and behavior/ activities.
Thus the main research question is:
how may mobile Internet sites be structured to facilitate the complex tasks users will perform on
them, given that they will be displayed on small screens with limited navigation facilities?
The
results may provide a starting point for the design of optimal menu structures for the mobile
Internet.
6
This paper consists of five parts.
background.
The following section presents the study’s research
The next section explains the research model and hypotheses, as well as the
research method – a controlled laboratory experiment.
The results of the experiment follow,
and the paper closes with a discussion of the study’s implications and limitations.
2. Research Background
This section reviews three research areas closely related to our study:
hierarchical menu
structure, the usability of small screens, and task complexity.
2.1 Hierarchical Menu Structure
In most Internet systems, menus use a complex mixture of various structures, but the overall
architecture of a website is generally hierarchical.
In a purely hierarchal structure, each node
(that is, menu panel) in the hierarchy can be reached only from a single superordinate node that
lies directly above it in the hierarchy. The two key characteristics to be considered in the design
of a hierarchical structure are the depth and the breadth of the menu (Henneman and Rouse
1984).
Depth (d) is usually defined as the number of levels in the hierarchy, breadth (b) as the
number of options per menu panel (Paap and Cooke 1997).
When there are equal numbers of
options on each panel, the number of terminal nodes is a function of breadth raised to the power
7
of depth: N= bd.
Navigation problems (e.g., getting lost, or choosing an incorrect pathway to a goal)
become more severe as the hierarchy grows deeper.
A hierarchical structure with several levels
requires a user either to recall or to discover a pathway from the present location to the target
location.
In fact, a prior study (Snowberry et al. 1983) showed that error rates increased from
4.0% to 34.0% as depth increased from one to six levels.
In addition, as the depth increases, so
does the number of page transactions, that is, the number of movements from one page to
another (Paap and Cooke 1997). Each page transaction requires an action from the user (e.g., a
keystroke or a mouse selection) and a response from the computer (e.g., a change of display).
Obviously, each transaction adds to the cumulative response time (Paap and Cooke 1997).
In
sum, depth in an information structure increases the likelihood of navigational errors, and also
decreases execution speed.
Nonetheless, there are good reasons to consider a system with greater depth.
Certainly,
when the amount of information exceeds the available space, at least some depth must be
introduced – and, in fact, a structure that favors depth can avoid the crowding brought about by
excessive breadth. Crowding – the presence on a single menu of more options than a user can
process quickly – increases the time it takes a user to make his or her selection.
Thus, though
computer execution time is reduced, there is no net gain in the time it takes to accomplish a task
8
– and user frustration is likely greater. Papp and Cooke (1997) have found that a structure that
favors depth over breadth can avoid crowding by allowing funneling – that is a reduction in the
total number of options a user must choose among. Funneling can generate efficiency gains,
particularly in situations where more cognitive processing is required of users (Kiger 1984).
Thus it is generally agreed that developers should use depth to avoid crowding and to encourage
funneling.
In sum, the advantage of depth is that it encourages funneling, the disadvantage that it
induces errors and increases the number of page transactions. The advantage of breadth is that
it reduces navigation errors and the number of page transactions, the disadvantage that it leads to
crowding.
D. Miller (1981) examined the tradeoff between depth and breadth in menu structure
design.
His study tested four different structures, all with 64 nodes at the bottom level:
82, and 641.
26, 43,
The results indicated that increased breadth decreased the number of page
transaction but came at the expense of display crowding.
The study results suggested that the
82 structure allowed the best ratio of performance speed to navigation errors among the four
structures. This level of breadth fits comfortably within the range of G. Miller’s (1956) finding
that short-term memory can typically retain 7 +/- 2 items.
Kiger (1984) extended D. Miller’s
research, investigating which of his several menu structures users preferred. It turned out that
9
users were found to prefer the 82 structure.
preference was consistent with performance:
Clearly, the balance of depth and breadth in hierarchical menu systems affects both user
navigation behaviors and user preferences – but whether the optimal balance is affected by
screen size remains to be seen.
This is the topic of the next sub-section.
2.2 Usability of Small Screens
Considerable research has been done on the usability of small screens (Duchnicky and Kolers
1983, Dillon et al. 1990, Han and Kwahk 1994, Jones et al. 1999, Kim and Albers 2001).
particular, researchers have been interested in the question of information presentation:
In
how
does one display information effectively on screens far smaller than conventional computer
screens?
Studies investigating the effects of small displays have indicated that reduced screen
size is closely related to various user behaviors, including navigation, searching, and browsing
(Duchnicky and Kolers 1983, Dillon et al. 1990).
However, the results of these studies are somewhat inconsistent.
Some found that while
user performance, measured in terms of the time taken to select an option, worsened as the
display size decreased, the effect was not dramatic (Jones et al. 1999). However, a study of the
effect of display size on web interaction found that small screen size reduced user effectiveness
by up to 50% for the tasks being observed (Han and Kwahk 1994, Telstra 2001).
10
There are at least three possible causes for the inconsistency.
First, typical displays
explored in studies such as Jones et al. (1999) ranged from one-quarter to one-half the size of
typical VGA displays (1024x768), whereas mobile displays are generally much smaller, ranging
from 128x128 (mobile phone) to 320x240 (PDA).
It may be that real problems only occur
when a display is so small that only a few options can be displayed at one time, and that results
from prior studies like Jones et al. (1999) are not directly applicable to the much smaller displays
of mobile Internet devices.
Second, many previous studies were interested in displays on
devices like typewriters and photocopiers (Buchanan 2001).
Such early office-automation
devices allowed users to choose functions from a brief list presented on a small LCD screen.
Information structures encased in these devices were much simpler than mobile Internet
information structures are; the latter generally provide a great deal of information and arrange it
in complex structures. It is likely that mobile Internet users would not struggle if only a simple
list of choices were presented to them.
Thus it may be that the findings of Han and Kwahk
(1994) are no more applicable to the mobile Internet than those of Jones et al (1999).
Third,
though several studies, acknowledging that menu structure exerts an influence on user behavior
and cognitive process, have attempted to design effective mobile menu systems, they have done
little to consider the relation between the screen size of mobile devices and the hierarchical menu
structure of mobile Internet services.
11
In sum, prior studies have not directly treated the present research questions. Those
that considered a complex information structure considered too large a screen; those that
considered an appropriately small screen considered too simple an information structure; those
that have specifically taken up mobile Internet menus have failed to consider the relation of
menu structure to screen size.
Thus the very small screens of mobile devices should be studied
anew, in light of the complex information structures of mobile Internet services.
Further, the effect of reduced screen size on user behavior may depend on task
complexity – the topic of the next sub-section.
2.3 Task Complexity and Menu Structure
The relation of task complexity to menu depth has been discussed extensively by Jacko and
Salvendy (1996).
Their study demonstrated that the perceived complexity of a computerized
task increased as the depth of the hierarchical menu increased.
The study takes as its theoretical
basis Campbell’s framework of the complexity of tasks (Campbell 1988) and Frese’s definition
of task complexity in a hypertext menu structure (Frese 1987).
characteristics of a complex task:
Campbell identified four
multiple paths, multiple outcomes, conflicting
interdependence among paths, and uncertain linkages (Campbell 1988). Frese proposed that
complexity is determined by the number of decisions that have to be made and by the relations
12
among those decisions (Frese 1987).
In Jacko and Salvendy (1996), the central questions were,
first, which menu depths would yield the fastest responses and the fewest errors, and, second,
how these optimal designs would be perceived by users in terms of complexity. They found
that perceived complexity increased as menu depth increased, and also that perceived complexity
lengthened response time and reduced response accuracy.
Thus minimizing perceived
complexity by creating a shallow menu structure might improve user response time.
3. Research Model and Hypotheses: A Theory of Information Structure for the Mobile
Internet
This section discusses three concepts critical to the present study: horizontal depth, screen size,
and task complexity. The study takes horizontal depth and screen size as independent variables
and task complexity as a moderating variable. The section ends with a discussion of dependent
variables, sub-hypotheses, and the research model, which is summarized in Figure 3 later.
3.1 Horizontal Depth
In terms of information structure, there is a subtle but important difference between the mobile
Internet and the conventional desktop-based Internet.
Consider, for instance, a user finding
local traffic information (target, in Figure 1) through a mobile Internet–enabled phone.
13
The
user starts searching at the top level (x in Figure 1) and moves down to the sub-level (y), where
he or she encounters a list of street names.
If there are several street names on the list, there
may be several ways to present the whole list on the screen, such as providing the list on one
page (y - y’:
1-B).
Figure 1-A), or presenting the list on several pages (y1 - y5’:
five pages in Figure
In the former case, the user searches for the street (list k) by scrolling down the long list,
while in the latter case, the user must move from the first page to the third page, which contains
list k (y1 y2
y3 ).
After choosing list k, he or she continues, scrolling down to z and to the
target information.
In both cases, then, the user moves through three levels of vertical depth
(x y z target).
But the cases are different in terms of what may be called horizontal depth.
To reach the page where list k is, the user in Figure 1-A only has to scroll down to level y, while
the user in Figure 1-B must move through two more levels of horizontal depth (y1 y2 y3).
We refer to the depth that exists between pages within a single level of the menu hierarchy as
“horizontal depth” (y - y1 - y2 - y3 - y4 - y’ in Figure 1-B), distinguishing it from the “vertical
depth” that exists between levels of the hierarchy (x y z target in both 1-A and 1-B).
Horizontal depth divides a unit of content into multiple sequential links, rather than
leaving a single larger unit on one page. “Horizontal depth” is horizontal because the content is
divided into multiple pages that sit on the same level of the information hierarchy (e.g., Main
14
Street and Sunset Boulevard in downtown LA), but it has depth because users must take more
steps in their search (e.g., from the page of “M” street names to the page of “S” street names).
It should be noted that horizontal depth differs from the “paging” process in traditional stationary
Internet systems.
Users with mobile Internet phones must scroll to the last entry of the current
page (y1) in order to proceed to the first entry of the next page (y2), whereas traditional Internet
users can jump from one page to another directly (y - y1 - y2 - y3 - y4 - y5’).
This difference is
caused by the limitation of most mobile Internet browsers to a line-based interface (rather than a
graphic user interface), as will be explained in more detail later.
Therefore, mobile Internet
users may perceive horizontal depth as a form of depth rather than as a form of breadth.
[INSERT FIGURE 1 ABOUT HERE]
Because less information per page (breadth) leads to more pages, which leads to more
horizontal depth, there may be a tradeoff in a menu system between breadth and horizontal depth
similar to the well-known tradeoff between breadth and vertical depth (Geiser and Schumacher
1976, Henneman and Rouse 1984).
Previous studies on paging have shown that paging results in more errors (Geiser and
Schumacher 1976) and that error rates may rise as horizontal depth increases (Henneman and
Rouse 1984).
Moreover, increasing horizontal depth may also increase perceived complexity
for users: encountering horizontal depth within one level of a hierarchy, they may mistake it for
15
vertical depth, and believe they are exploring a deeper structure than they actually are.
As
perceived complexity increases, users may have more difficulty forming clear mental models or
structural frameworks (Schwarz 1983) – and, unless users have a correct mental model, they will
have difficulty sorting and structuring the information they receive as they progress through a
site (Albers and Kim 2000).
The problem is especially pertinent to the mobile Internet, where
relatively deep menu structures are to some degree unavoidable, given the large amount of
information involved and the limited screen space available.
However, the negative effects of greater breadth in mobile Internet systems should also
be considered. The method the typical mobile device has for presenting options on a page
differs from that of the conventional Internet system.
In the latter system, users just need to
scan a list of options in order to encode those options and decide whether to terminate their
search or continue (Miller 1981, Redish 1994).
But in a mobile menu system, though many
options can reside on a single page, only a few are visible at a time, because only a few text lines
are available to the display screens.
must scroll line by line.
To view the options beyond the visible display, the user
When he or she presses the scroll-down button, for instance, all the
information on the screen moves up one line. On a four-line screen, three lines of the displayed
information will move up one line, the top line will disappear, and a new line will appear at the
bottom.
After moving down the list by scrolling, the user must refocus on the correct part of
16
the content (Mechior 2001), the changes to which may increase task complexity: refocusing
becomes more difficult as more information changes.
Thus processing time for mobile users
includes not just the time it takes to scan options displayed in the visible area, but also the time it
takes to scroll line by line to view as much of the list as necessary and to refocus on it.
In a
mobile Internet system, reducing breadth by creating more horizontal depth might reduce
perceived complexity in serial information searches (Jacko and Salvendy 1996).
What is clear is that there are tradeoffs between horizontal depth and breadth, just as
there are tradeoffs between vertical depth and breadth.
The study’s first main hypothesis is as
follows:
H1:
Horizontal depth affects users’ navigation activities and perceptions.
3.2 Screen Size and Horizontal Depth
One aspect of screen size, as it relates to horizontal depth, is the rate at which information
changes as users scroll line by line through the menu structure.
Consider two mobile devices
with different screen sizes, as shown in Figure 2.
[INSERT FIGURE 2 ABOUT HERE]
One screen has six lines, and the other has nine. Each screen devotes two lines to icons and
pictures (the top line showing a phone icon, the bottom line “UP” and “OK”) and one line to the
17
heading (“Best Melodies”).
None of these lines can be used to display content. In other
words, the nine-line screen has only six lines of content area (Figure 2-A), the six-line screen
only three (Figure 2-B). The difference between the two screen sizes lies in the rate at which
information changes when users scroll up or down. If, for example, a user scrolls down line by
line on the six-line mobile screen (with three lines of content), 1/3 of the information changes per
scroll. However, if he or she scrolls down on the nine-line screen (with six lines of content),
only 1/6 of the information changes per scroll.
The smaller the screen, the more radical the information change users experience – and
the higher their cognitive load when they attempt to understand their current location relative to a
reference point.
According to prior research (Kahneman et al. 1982), people tend to anchor a
reference point when they start to solve a problem, and to keep adjusting their current point on
the basis of their reference point.
This result can be applied to the present case, namely,
navigation through an information space.
If users scroll down one line on a standard PC
monitor, they can easily identify their reference point, because most of the screen content has not
changed. However, if they scroll down one line on a six-line screen, they may not be able to
relocate their reference point easily, because a large portion of the screen content has changed.
The higher the relative change, the more drastically users must adjust their anchoring points –
which will likely require greater cognitive effort of them.
18
Thus our second main hypothesis is
as follows:
H2:
Screen size affects users’ navigation activities and perceptions.
3.3 Task Complexity and Horizontal Depth
The mobile Internet has come to play an important role in numerous everyday tasks (Albers and
Kim 2000).
Some tasks are relatively simple to perform; for example, mobile Internet users
access the Internet to learn the current local temperature (Kim et al. 2002).
Others are
considerably more complex, as when users download popular animations or melodies through
their cellular phones, or bid on items in an on-line auction.
In such cases, users have to
compare several alternative items and then decide on one, a procedure more complex than a
simple search.
In fact, according to Frese’s (1987) theoretical background, this kind of task is
far more complex, because of the greater number of decisions to be made and the more
numerous increased inter-relations among these decisions (Frese 1987).
Users employ different navigation strategies for different sorts of task. In other words, a
user may have conceptual models for complex tasks different from his or her models for simple
tasks.
It may be that performing complex tasks through the mobile Internet is made more
difficult by the smaller screen size and the greater horizontal depth.
Since there is not enough
space on the screen to provide usable navigation cues or aids, such as a shopping cart or wish list,
19
the comparison process involved in selecting animations or melodies, for instance, can burden
the user with a heavier cognitive load (Albers and Kim 2000, Jacko et al. 1995).
Therefore, the
effect of horizontal depth and screen size on user navigation activities and perceptions may vary
with the level of task complexity. Our third main hypothesis is as follows:
H3: The relation between horizontal depth/screen size and navigation
activities/perceptions will be affected by task complexity.
3.4 Dependent Variables and Sub-Hypotheses
Dependent variables for objectively measurable navigation activities are Between-Page
Navigation (BPN) and Within-Page Navigation (WPN).
BPN represents the frequency of
“paging backwards and forwards.” BPN in the mobile Internet is different from traditional
paging (i.e., moving from one web page to another by clicking a button) because it includes
movement not just between vertical-depth pages, but also between horizontal-depth pages.
In
addition to the page transactions necessary to complete a task, users may perform BPN in an
attempt to orient themselves, or to provide context as they progress through the text (Dillon
1990). BPN is expected to be closely related to screen size, because a smaller screen reduces
contextual information, leading users to rely on BPN as a source of contextual information.
BPN is also expected to be closely related to horizontal depth, because with greater horizontal
20
depth users become lost more easily, leading them to perform more BPN in an effort to get their
bearings. Therefore, two sub-hypotheses regarding BPN are as follows:
H1a:
Greater horizontal depth will increase BPN.
H2a:
Reduced screen size will increase BPN.
Second, WPN refers to scrolling activities within a single page.
It is different from
traditional scrolling (scrolling up or down by moving the vertical scroll bar) in that most mobile
Internet users need to scroll line by line to the last item in order to proceed to the next page.
According to Jones et al. (1999), additional scrolling compromised users’ ability to accomplish a
task.
Increased scrolling might result in a more pronounced “lost-in-space” effect or in greater
user frustration and fatigue (Nielsen and Ramsay 2000). The amount of scrolling reflects the
cognitive load a user experiences, and therefore WPN is also expected to be closely related to
screen size and horizontal depth.
Since there is not enough space to provide contextual cues or
navigational aids, on a smaller screen users will scroll up and down more to make sense of the
page (Dillon 1990).
However, greater horizontal depth is expected to decrease WPN, because
relatively few items will be presented in a single page.
Therefore, two sub-hypotheses
regarding WPN are as follows:
H1b:
Greater horizontal depth will decrease WPN.
H2b:
Reduced screen size will increase WPN.
21
Dependent variables for the subjective features of navigation are perceived depth and
user satisfaction. Perceived depth was introduced to measure the user’s understanding of the
information structure (Jacko and Salvendy 1996).
A perceived depth shallower than actual
depth indicates that a user has a relatively simple mental model for a given Internet system.
A
perceived depth greater than actual depth suggests that a user is mistaking horizontal depth for
vertical depth.
Indeed, it is proposed here that horizontal depth may be perceived as “real,” i.e.
vertical depth, increasing the level of perceived complexity.
likely increase perceived depth:
Reduced screen sizes will also
getting lost more easily, as they do with a smaller screen, users
will experience a heavier cognitive load, and therefore perceive more depth.
Two sub-
hypotheses regarding perceived depth are as follows:
H1c:
Greater horizontal depth will increase perceived depth.
H2c:
Reduced screen size will increase perceived depth.
User satisfaction measures how satisfied users are with a given Internet system in
terms of navigation and structure.
Kiger (1984), who extended D. Miller’s research by
investigating user preferences among various menu structures, showed that users’ subjective
preferences were consistent with their satisfaction.
In the present context, it is expected that
user satisfaction with site navigation and structure will correspond with perceived depth.
Therefore, two sub-hypotheses regarding user satisfaction are as follows:
22
H1d:
Greater horizontal depth will decrease user satisfaction.
H2d:
Reduced screen size will decrease user satisfaction.
The overall research model for the study is summarized in Figure 3, with accompanying
research hypotheses. The research model consists of two independent variables (horizontal
depth and screen size), one moderating variable (task type), and two groups of dependent
variables, one objective (navigation activities) and one subjective (user perceptions).
[INSERT FIGURE 3 ABOUT HERE]
4. Experiment
4.1 Experimental Mobile Internet Site
How menus are categorized and labeled has been found to exert significant influence on user
behavior and perceptions (McDonald and Schvaneveldt 1988).
To control for the confounding
effect of categorization and labeling, a pilot study was conducted with 60 mobile Internet users.
Each participant was asked to sort 100 index cards, each of which bore the name of a mobile
Internet menu (Nimwegen 1999).
Participants were then asked to sort the cards into piles so
that cards representing similar concepts wound up in the same pile.
After a subject had
completed this task, he or she was asked to arrange the piles in larger groups that seemed to
belong together, and then to invent a name for each group.
23
The name, which offered further
insight into a user’s mental model of the information structure, was written on a Post-It note and
placed on the table next to the group.
On the basis of the pilot study results, the site for the main study was organized in a
3 3 3 3 structure (four vertical levels and three degrees of breadth). Each of the 81 (i.e. 34)
menu items at the bottom level was linked to a list of 60 selectable items.
For example, if a
subject were given the task of determining when a Britney Spears fan club meeting was to be
held, he or she would navigate along this path: For Fun (level 1)
Fan Clubs (level 3)
Communities (level 2)
Movie Stars (level 4). Upon selecting “Movie Stars,” he or she would
encounter a list of 60 movie stars, each linked to specific information.
A standard navigation system, based on industry guidelines for mobile Internet phones
(Nokia 2001, Openwave 2001, Telstra 2001), was developed for the experimental site, as
follows:
The following functions were assigned to four hard keys: UP, DOWN, OK, and
CLEAR.
UP was used to scroll up one line, DOWN to scroll down one line. Movement was
only possible one line at a time.
OK was used to submit requests, CLEAR to go back to the previous page.
Scrolling could only go up and down, not left or right.
24
A cursor was placed (to start) on the top line. NEXT and PREVIOUS were offered
on the last line of the menu, in case the information was divided into multiple pages.
To move to the next page, users had to scroll down to the bottom line and select the
NEXT or PREVIOUS button.
There was no short-cut button (such as a “Home” button), or direct selection through
a number key or search engine.
The experimental site was built on a Game Virtual Machine (GVM)1 programmed in
Mobile C, a language based on ANSI C and optimized to the mobile environment.
developed content structures were uploaded to the GVM server group.
Next, the
Finally, the
experimental site was downloaded and executed on a GVM-installed mobile Internet phone, as
shown in Figure 4.
The mobile Internet phone was an LG Cyon CX-300V featuring WAP and
256-color, 120x160 resolution LCD.2
[INSERT FIGURE 4 ABOUT HERE]
4.2 Experiment Participants
Participants were solicited for the main experiment through advertisements posted on several
1
The GVM is composed of a mobile application download platform that uses an OS independent black box. The GVM system consists of
three parts: The GVM Server Solution, the GVM Module, and the GVM SDK. For more details, visit ShinjiSoft Corp. at
http://www.shinjisoft.com
2
Detailed information about the LG Cyon CX-300V can be found at http://www.cyon.co.kr/job/product/feature.jsp?svci=10&pmd=
CX-300DV
25
popular mobile Internet sites. To qualify, candidates had to be over the age of 12 and to use
mobile Internet services at least 30 minutes a month.
Of the more than 300 people who applied,
90 were selected on the basis of demographic profile, average usage, and familiarity with the
mobile Internet.
Their self-reported demographic and usage information was verified with the
mobile Internet carriers to which they subscribed.
Out of a concern that behavior might differ
with gender, equal numbers of males and females were selected, although in fact gender was
found not to play a significant role in this study.
Participant ages ranged from teens to thirties,
matching the customer profile for the mobile Internet.
The average time they spent using
mobile Internet services was about 67 minutes a month.
Subjects were thus well-balanced in
their experience and demographic profiles.
They were compensated $40.00US for their
participation.
4.3 Experimental Design
A 2 x 3 x 2 factorial design was selected, as shown in Table 1.
There were two between-
subject independent variables (screen size and horizontal depth) and one within-subject
moderating variable (task type).
and large (nine-line screen).
Screen size had two possible values:
small (six-line screen)
Horizontal depth had three possible values:
26
deep (six levels),
medium (four levels), and shallow (one level).
Finally, task type had two possible values:
simple (single search) and complex (comparison search).
Each of these variables is explained
in detail below.
[INSERT TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE]
First, horizontal depth was operationalized by dividing a content list of 60 items into
one-, four-, and six-page versions. As shown in Figure 5, if 60 options (e.g., 60 movie stars or
60 titles of incoming emails) are divided into four pages, 15 items (lines of content) are
presented per page, and there are four levels of horizontal depth.
Thus the horizontal depth
(HD) is 4 and the breadth (B) per display is 15 (4HD/15B). Alternatively, if 60 options are
divided into six pages, each page has 10 items (6HD/10B).
The study fixed vertical depth at 4
and the total number of items at 60 across all treatments.
[INSERT FIGURE 5 ABOUT HERE]
Second, two screen sizes were selected: six lines (the size most typical of mobile
Internet phones) and nine lines (the largest mobile phone display available at the time of
research). As seen already in Figure 2, a screen must devote two lines to icons and one line to
the heading.
Thus the large screen had six content lines, the small screen three. Each line
could display eight Korean characters at most.
screens in terms of resolution or color.
27
There was no difference between the two
Finally, four tasks were devised: two single-search tasks and two comparison-search
tasks.
The single-search task was equivalent to a typical information retrieval on the Web.
Participants were asked to find the answer to a given question, such as “Where will the musical
The Lion King be performed in New York?” Comparison-search tasks involved comparing
several alternatives and then choosing only one.
Subjects were given a task such as “Find the
five most popular ring melodies and then decide which is most cheerful.”
A comparison-search
task is more complex than a single-search task, because a greater number of decisions have to be
made (Frese 1987), and because there are more paths and possible outcomes, with a higher
chance of conflicting interdependence among paths (Campbell 1988).
4.4 Experimental Procedure
To check the experimental system and operational validity, another pilot test was conducted with
18 subjects, after which some revisions were made to the experimental procedure.
The final
experiment included two sessions: a practice session and the main session.
The practice session began with a brief explanation of the study’s purpose, and a general
introduction to the main features of the experimental mobile Internet site. Basic instructions
were given on use of the key buttons, as well as guidelines about navigation (for example, how
to go forward and backward). At the end of the practice session, participants were required to
28
perform two practice tasks.
In the main session, each participant was assigned the four study tasks in a random
order.
Participants were asked to perform the tasks, one by one, taking up to 10 minutes per
task – a time period drawn from the pilot test.3
answers within 10 minutes.
All the subjects succeeded in finding correct
The navigation behaviors of each participant were recorded in
system log files, which were transferred through a serial cable, in real time, from the mobile
Internet phone to a desktop PC, as shown in Figure 6(a). In order to maximize data integrity, a
small camera attached to the mobile phone recorded all user activity on a videotape in a remote
observation room, as shown in Figure 6(b). BPN and WPN were measured using the data from
the system log file and videotapes. BPN was calculated as the total number of movements from
one page to another, WPN as the number of up/down scrolling movements.
[INSERT FIGURE 6 ABOUT HERE ]
After completing each task, subjects were asked to answer a question about perceived
depth:
“Please check ‘V’ where you think the information you have just found is located in
terms of depth.”
The question was followed by a vertical line divided into equal sections. The
topmost node (i.e. the starting-point) was marked “Home,” and subjects were asked to indicate
the node where, relative to “Home,” the retrieved information had been found.
3
In the pilot test, all subjects found correct answers within 10 minutes.
Most subjects in the main test also spent nearly 10 minutes per task,
29
In order to assess participants’ satisfaction in terms of navigational usability, a
questionnaire of seven items was administered at the end of each task. As shown in Table 2,
two sets of questions widely used in HCI studies were adapted to measure user satisfaction:
“Perceived Usefulness and Ease of Use” (PUEU) (Davis 1989), and “Questionnaire for User
Interface Satisfaction” (QUIS) (Chin et al. 1988).
Participants answered each question on a
seven-point scale that ranged from “strongly disagree” (1) to “strongly agree” (7).
[ INSERT TABLE 2 ABOUT HERE]
5. Study Results
This section describes the results of the study, treating, first, objectively measurable navigation
activities, and, second, user perceptions about navigation and structure. The former results are
drawn from the system log and the video log; the latter based on the questionnaire responses.
5.1 Objectively Measurable Navigation Activities
Between-Page Navigation (BPN)
Three-way repeated-measures ANOVA results revealed a significant interaction effect between
task complexity and horizontal depth on BPN (F(2, 82) = 10.414, p < 0.01), but no significant
making comparison between the groups in terms of time taken less meaningful.
30
interaction effect between screen size and task complexity on BPN (F(2, 82) = 0.044, ns).
Therefore, H3 holds for BPN only in terms of horizontal depth, not in terms of screen size.
For simple search tasks, neither of the two independent variables showed statistically
significant main effects on BPN, nor any significant interaction effects on BPN (screen size (F(1,
83) = 2.108, ns); horizontal depth (F(2, 83) = 0.873, ns); interaction effect (F(2, 83) = 0.39, ns.)).
Therefore, H1a and H2a do not hold for BPN with simple tasks.
In contrast, for comparative tasks, there was a significant interaction effect between
screen size and horizontal depth on BPN (F(2, 83) = 3.80, p < 0.05).
As shown in Figure 7, as
horizontal depth increases from one level to six, BPN increases more quickly with the small
screen than with the large screen. There were also statistically significant main effects of
screen size (F(1, 83) = 16.756, p < 0.01) and horizontal depth (F(2, 83) = 13.802, p < 0.01) on
BPN. Therefore, H1a and H2a hold for BPN with comparative tasks.
[ INSERT FIGURE 7 ABOUT HERE]
Within-Page Navigation (WPN)
Repeated-measures ANOVA showed a significant interaction effect between task type and
horizontal depth on WPN (F(2, 82) = 8.699, p < 0.01). However, no significant interaction
effect between task type and screen size on WPN was observed (F(2, 82) = 0.62, ns). Therefore,
31
H3 was partially supported, holding for WPN for horizontal depth but not for screen size.
In the case of simple search tasks, screen size showed a statistically significant main
effect on WPN.
Subjects in the small-screen group carried out more scrolling actions than their
large-screen counterparts (F(1, 82) = 16.441, p < 0.01).
No significant main effect was found
for horizontal depth on WPN (F(2, 82) = 1.836, ns), nor any significant interaction effect
between the two independent variables on WPN (F(2, 82) = 2.14, ns). Therefore, only H2b is
supported for WPN in the case of simple search tasks.
However, in the case of complex comparative tasks, significant main effects were
observed for both horizontal depth (F(2, 82) = 12.987, p < 0.01) and screen size (F(1, 82) =
43.183, p < 0.01) on WPN, as shown in Figure 8.
Subjects with the small screen again carried
out more scrolling actions than their large-screen counterparts.
performed less WPN as horizontal depth increased.
At the same time, subjects
No significant interaction effect was found
between the two independent variables on WPN (F(2, 82)= 1.07, ns) for the comparative search
task.
Therefore, H1b and H2b are supported in the case of WPN.
[INSERT FIGURE 8 ABOUT HERE]
5.2 Subjective Factors: Perceived Depth and User Satisfaction
Perceived Depth
32
Three-way repeated ANOVA results showed no significant interaction effect between task
complexity and horizontal depth (F(2, 84) = 0.62, ns) or between task complexity and screen size
(F(2, 84) = 0.57, ns) on perceived depth.
Therefore, H3 was not supported in terms of perceived
depth.
With simple search tasks, there was no significant main effect of screen size on perceived
depth (F(1, 84) = 2.11, ns), but there was a significant main effect of horizontal depth on
perceived depth (F(2, 84) = 3.38, p < 0.05).
As horizontal depth decreased, perceived depth
increased. No significant interaction effects were found between the two independent variables
on perceived depth (F(2, 84) = 0.65, ns).
Therefore, only H1c was supported in the case of
simple search tasks.
However, in the case of comparative search tasks, there was a significant interaction
effect between screen size and horizontal depth on perceived depth (F(2, 84) = 8.89, p < 0.01), as
shown in Figure 9. With a large screen, the depth subjects perceived decreased as horizontal
depth decreased.
With a small screen, however, perceived depth was highest with a shallow
horizontal depth (one level), lowest with a medium horizontal depth (four levels), and in the
middle when horizontal depth was greatest (six levels). There were also significant main effects
of both screen size (F(1, 84) = 11.42, p < 0.01) and horizontal depth (F(2, 84) = 6.28, p < 0.01)
on perceived depth.
Therefore, H1c and H2c are both supported in the case of comparative tasks.
33
[INSERT FIGURE 9 ABOUT HERE]
These complex-task results suggest how small-screen users experience depth in an
information hierarchy.
Perceived depth was shallowest for these users when performing
complex tasks at four levels of horizontal depth, suggesting a tradeoff between breadth and
horizontal depth.
Presenting information with greater horizontal depth may reduce, to a certain
degree, the complexity users experience, by decreasing both the amount of scrolling per page and
the number of alternatives available for decision-making.
However, greater horizontal depth
also increases the number of pages that have to be traversed.
The two countervailing effects of
horizontal depth may achieve an optimal balance at four levels.
User Satisfaction
Factor analysis was conducted to verify that the seven questions were actually measuring the
single construct of user satisfaction.
The analysis indicated that the seven questions converged
nicely into a single factor (eigenvalue, 4.44; percentage of variance explained, 63.46%;
Cronbach alpha, 0.81). Therefore, the seven questions could be safely averaged and then
analyzed with repeated-measures ANOVA.
There was no interaction effect between task type
and horizontal depth (F(2, 84) = 0.54, ns) or between task type and screen size (F(2, 84) = 1.18,
ns) on user satisfaction. Therefore, H3 was not supported in the case of user satisfaction.
34
For simple search tasks, there was no main effect of either screen size (F(1, 84) = 1.32,
ns) or horizontal depth (F(2, 84) = 1.02, ns) on user satisfaction.
No significant interaction
effect was observed on user satisfaction between the two independent variables either (F (2, 84)
=2.01, ns). Therefore, neither H1d nor H2d was supported in the case of simple tasks.
However, for comparative tasks, the interaction effect between screen size and
horizontal depth on user satisfaction (F(2, 84) = 3.76, p < 0.05) was significant, as shown in
Figure 10.
With the large screen, user satisfaction continues to increase as horizontal depth
increases, while with the small screen, the level of satisfaction is highest with medium horizontal
depth (four levels).
In other words, participants using small screens found the mobile Internet
system with four levels of horizontal depth more convenient for navigation than systems with
one or six levels.
This result is in line with the depth perceived by small-screen participants:
the mobile menu system appeared shallowest to them when information was divided into four
horizontal levels.
The main effect of screen size on user satisfaction was not statistically
significant (F(1, 84) = 1.66, ns), but the main effect of horizontal depth (F(2, 84) = 4.49, p <
0.05) was.
Therefore, H1d was supported in the case of complex tasks, but H2d was not.
[INSERT FIGURE 10 ABOUT HERE]
6. Conclusions and Discussion
35
The aim of this study was to examine the effects of screen size and horizontal depth on the
navigation activities and perceptions of mobile users.
As shown in Table 3, the study suggests
several conclusions.
[INSERT TABLE 3 ABOUT HERE]
One of the consistent results across different dependent variables was the moderating effect of
task complexity.
Only two of the eight possible effects were statistically significant with simple
tasks: the main effect of screen size on WPN, and the main effect of horizontal depth on
perceived depth.
In contrast, as shown in Table 3, seven effects were significant with complex
tasks – only the main effect of screen size on satisfaction failed to be significant. Thus the
effects of screen size and horizontal depth appear to be greater with complex comparative
searches than with simple search tasks.
Another consistent result, in the case of complex tasks, was the effect of screen size.
The small screen was found to increase BPN, WPN, and perceived depth when subjects were
performing complex tasks.
For example, subjects in the small-screen group carried out more
WPN than their large-screen counterparts.
difference:
Two aspects of the small screen may account for the
the visible area of the screen and the rate of information change.
First, to move
sequentially through pages and review the full contents, all users have to scroll considerably –
but because there is less viewable information on the small screen, small-screen users must scroll
36
more frequently.
Second, as screens become smaller, the information they display changes
more extensively with each scrolling action, making it more difficult to refocus on the page.
Small-screen users may compensate by carrying out a significant number of “two-way” actions
(scrolling up and down) in an attempt to get contextual cues for orientation (Dillon, Richardson
and McKnight 1990).
The study may also explain why prior research is inconsistent on the effect of reduced
screen size. Studies that failed to observe any small-screen effect may have used relatively
larger screens, or may have assigned tasks that were too simple to reveal the differences (Han
and Kwahk 1994).
a six-line screen.
The present study clearly shows a large difference between a three-line and
This result is consistent with prior studies suggesting that only a really small
screen can affect a user’s navigation activities (Duchnicky and Kolers 1983, Tombaugh and
Lickorish 1987).
Another consistent result, shown in Table 3, is the effect of horizontal depth when the
task is complex.
Specifically, the study clearly indicates a tradeoff between the effects of
horizontal depth on BPN and its effects on WPN.
horizontal depth.
On the one hand, BPN increases along with
With more pages at the same level of the hierarchy, the number of pages to be
traversed increases, requiring more use of the NEXT and PREVIOUS keys and more time
waiting for the results appear (Redish 1994). On the other hand, WPN decreases as horizontal
37
depth increases.
It may be that the greater depth is allowing funneling (Paap and Cooke 1997):
a user encountering greater depth has fewer options to process in single page, and thus has more
cognitive resources available, reducing the cognitive load he or she might experience with a
small screen.
The data on perceived depth and user satisfaction indicates that the two
countervailing effects achieve an optimal balance at four levels of horizontal depth.
It was at
this level that users found a given mobile Internet site least complex and most satisfying to use.
As screen size shrank, the effects of the tradeoff became more pronounced – that is, the influence
of horizontal depth on navigation activities grew more significant.
In sum, horizontal depth and screen size are important factors influencing a user’s
navigation activities and subjective perceptions, and their importance grows as the tasks
performed become more complex.
This study has both theoretical and practical implications.
On the theoretical side, it
explains differences observed in user navigation behavior, and in user perceptions of complexity
in mobile Internet hierarchical structures.
Existing research on the tradeoff between depth and
breadth has been extended and reinterpreted in the new context of the mobile Internet.
The distinction between horizontal and vertical depth, introduced here, allows the
study of user navigation patterns, as well as related issues of cognitive overload, in a new light.
Specifically, the concept of horizontal depth, and the observed tendency of users to mistake it for
38
vertical depth, show that small-scale design issues like scrolling and paging are actually
inseparable from large-scale issues of structural design.
These results can be extended to other
kinds of thin client devices, such as PDAs and e-books.
On the practical side, by identifying relationships among horizontal depth, screen size,
and task type, the study provides several design guidelines for mobile service providers.
First,
when search tasks are simple, horizontal depth and screen size do not affect user navigation
activities or perceptions very much.
When the tasks become complex, however, their influence
on navigation activities and perceptions becomes critical. The depth of information structures
and the complexity of tasks vary greatly from one mobile Internet domain to another.
For
example, the process of checking e-mail requires a fairly simple search, but buying a present in
an online shopping mall involves a relatively complex search operation. Clearly, information
structures on the mobile Internet should be adapted to the characteristics of different domains.
Second, screen size turned out to affect user navigation behavior and perceptions
significantly.
The more meaningful finding is that the screen size has a significant interaction
effect with horizontal depth on user perceptions.
For example, when using the small screen,
users expressed the highest satisfaction at four levels of horizontal depth, but with the large
screen their satisfaction was highest at six levels.
Current network protocols can provide
accurate information about the type and model of the mobile device through which a user is
39
accessing the mobile Internet.
Thus different hierarchical structures can be provided to suit the
screen size of the particular mobile device logging on.
Adjusting horizontal depth to task
complexity and screen size will help produce optimal information structures for mobile Internet
systems.
This study has several implications for Internet businesses.
According to prior
studies on Internet commercial transactions, the ways in which the mobile Internet differs from
the stationary Internet may have considerable influence on the preferences and buying behaviors
of customers (Chae and Kim 2003).
Compared to the stationary Internet, the mobile Internet is
limited in terms of available resources.
When purchasing products through mobile systems that
cannot provide enough product information, customers experience uncertainty, and are forced to
search for additional information.
Lowering the cost of searching by providing efficient
information structures may help persuade customers to participate more actively in mobile
commerce.
For example, the present study suggests that mobile news services, which usually
entail fairly simple problem-solving operations, should have a minimal horizontal depth,
regardless of screen size.
However, the horizontal depth of mobile games, which usually
require complex problem-solving operations, should be carefully gauged.
If a game is to be
played mostly on the very small screens typical of cellular phones, horizontal depth should be set
at around four levels. However, if the game is to be played on a relatively large screen, like that
40
of the Nintendo Gameboy, horizontal depth should be maximized.
Though studies of the mobile Internet have focused primarily on individual users, the
business implications of this study may be extendable to standard MIS issues. Jarvenpaa et al.
(2003) found in their field studies with mobile Internet users that one of the main reasons people
use the mobile Internet is to increase productivity.
In the future, mobile managers may use
web-enabled phones linked to a company intranet to make critical decisions, and the intranet
system for mobile phones should adjust the breadth and horizontal depth of its information
structures to optimize that process. In summary, the present study yields insights for providers
who want to develop more useful, more usable mobile services, whether for extranet services or
intranet systems.
This study has several limitations. First, because of the limited storage capacity of
the mobile Internet phone, the structure of the experimental mobile Internet site was simpler than
typically found in a natural environment.
The experimental site was implemented on a phone
rather than on a server, so as to avoid network delays and to record user navigation data in
system-log files.
Also, the overall information structure of the experimental site was a pure
hierarchy, whereas mobile Internet sites may in the future have semi-hierarchical or complex
network structures.
The simplicity of the structure may explain why all subjects found correct
answers within the allotted time, and why no differences were found in terms of task-completion
41
time.
As more memory space becomes available in mobile devices, deeper and more complex
information structures will become feasible, and future studies should adapt the present approach
to consider them.
Second, the selection of content for experimental sites and experimental tasks was
based on the researchers’ intuitions.
There were no accurate metrics for the domain
characteristics used in the study or for the tasks in each domain.
Therefore, it is difficult to
generalize the study results across all service domains of the mobile Internet.
In future studies,
the selection of content domains, as well as specific tasks assigned, should be based on accurate
and objective criteria that reflect a normative mobile Internet environment.
Third, the experimental site provided only line-based navigation capabilities.
For
example, users were not allowed to access information directly by means of number keys or a
“home” button.
The study used the common navigation facilities currently available on mobile
Internet phones; however, new means of navigation for the mobile Internet continue to arise as
new browsers and network protocols are developed.
A future study should include these
advanced navigation facilities, in order to test whether the present results hold in an environment
where more convenient navigation facilities are available.
It is expected, however, that
horizontal depth will remain important whether or not line-based scrolling is supplanted by other
methods like pen-based pointing. As long as users prefer devices with small screens, the
42
constrained display will make the role of horizontal depth in an information structure a key issue.
Fourth, the experimental design did not cover a wide variety of information structures
of mobile Internet phones.
For example, only 1, 4 and 6 levels of horizontal depth, as well as
only six- and nine-line screens were used.
Future research should extend the study to cover the
rapidly increasing variety of mobile Internet phones.
Finally, this study was conducted with a mobile Internet phone, firstly because among
the many mobile devices available, the mobile phone has the smallest screen size, and secondly
because it is the predominant platform for the mobile Internet in several countries where the
mobile Internet is popular, including Japan and Korea.
However, future studies should be
conducted on different platforms, such as PDAs, to test whether the study results hold with
different interaction styles and larger screens.
A follow-up study is currently underway to widen the scope of the experimental sites
and the assigned tasks, and to include other mobile devices, such as PDAs.
This research is
expected to provide a fuller background for designers considering screen size and horizontal
depth as they develop mobile services.
In conclusion, this study proposes a new concept of horizontal depth and investigates
how screen size and horizontal depth affect user navigation activities and perceptions.
We
believe that this kind of detailed experimental study may help the mobile Internet industry thrive.
43
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
The present research has been supported by a research grant from the Korea Research
Foundation [KRF-2003-042-B00045]. The authors are grateful to Dr. Joonah Park, Jeun Kim,
and Soyoung Kyung at the HCI Lab, to Enah Kim and Sujin Kim at Widerthan.com, and to the
editors and reviewers at AMCIS 2003 and the BIT special issue on HCI in MIS.
REFERENCES
Albers, M. J., and Kim, L., 2000, User Web browsing characteristics using palm handheld
for Information Retrieval, IEEE Technology & Teamwork, 125-135.
Buchanan, G., Farrant, S., Jones, M., Thimbleby, H., Marsden, G., and Pazzani, M., 2001,
Improving Mobile Internet Usability. The 10th International WWW Conference (WWW10), May
1-5, 2001, (Hong Kong).
Buyukkoken, O., Garcia-Molina, H., Paepcke, A., 2000, Focused Web searching with
PDAs, Computer Networks, 22, 213-230.
Buyukkoken, O., Garcia-Molina, H., Paepcke, A.,
and Winograd, T., 2000, Power
Browser: Efficient Web Browsing for PDAs. The 2000 ACM Conference on Human Factors and
Computing Systems (SIGCHI2000), April, 2000 (Hague, Netherlands) pp. 430-437.
44
Chae, M. and Kim, J, 2003, What’s so different about the Mobile Internet?,
Communications of the ACM, 46 (12ve), 240-247.
Campbell, D. J., 1988, Task complexity: A Review and Analysis, Academy of
Management Review, 13, 40-52.
Chin, J. P., Diehl, V. A., and Norman, K., 1988, Development of an Instrument Measuring
User Satisfaction with the Human Computer Interface, The 1998 ACM Conference on Human
Factors and Computing Systems (CHI ’88), May 15-19, 1988 (Washington DC, U.S.A.) pp. 213218.
Davis, F., 1989, Perceived Usefulness, Perceived Ease of Use, and User Acceptance of
Information Technology, MIS Quarterly, 13: 3, 319-340.
Dillon, A., Richardson, J., and McKnight, C., 1990, The effect of display size and text
splitting on reading lengthy text from the screen, Behavior and Information Technology, 9: 3,
215-227.
Duchnicky, R., and Kolers, P., Readability of text scrolled on visual display terminals as a
function of window size, Human Factors, 25, 683-692.
Francis, L., 1997, Mobile Computing, The 15th Annual International Conference on
Computer Documentation (SIGDOC’97), October 19-22, 1997 (Snowbird, UT) pp. 63-37.
Frese, M., 1987, A theory of control and complexity: implications for software design and
45
integration of computer systems into the work place, in: M. Frese, E. Ulich, & W. Dzida, (Ed.),
Psychological issues of human-computer interaction in the workplace, vol. Elsevier Science,
Amsterdam, pp.313-337.
Frey, P. R., Rouse, W., and Garris, R., 1992, Big Graphics and Little Screens: Designing
Graphical Displays for Maintenance Tasks, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man and Cybernetics,
22: 1, 10-20.
Geiser, G., and Schumacher, W., 1976, Parallel versus serial instrumentation for multivariable manual control in control rooms, in Monitoring behavior and supervisory control, in: T.
B. Sheridan and G. Johannsen, (Ed.), Plenum, New York, pp. 405-415.
Han S., and Kwahk, J., 1994, Design of a menu for small displays presenting a single
item at a time, in: Proc. Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 38th Annual Meeting, vol. 1, pp.
360-364.
Henneman, R. L., and Rouse, W. B., 1984, Human Performance in monitoring and
controlling hierarchical large-scale systems, IEEE Transactions on System, Man and Cybernetics
(SMC), 14, 184-191.
Intermarket Group, 2002, Wireless Web population to soar. January 16, 2002. Available at
http://www.intermarketgroup.com/.
Jacko, J., and Salvendy, G., 1996, Hierarchical Menu Design: Breadth, Depth and Task
46
Complexity. Perceptual and Motor Skill, 82, 1187-1201.
Jacko, J., Salvendy, G., and Koubek, R., 1995, Modeling of Menu Design in
Computerized Work, Interacting with Computers, 7: 3, 304-330.
Jarvenpaa, S., Lang, K., Takeda, Y., and Tuunainen, V., 2003, Mobile Commerce at
Crossroads, Communications of ACM, 46:12, 41-44.
Jones, M., Marsden, G., Mohd-Nasir, N., Boone, K., and Buchanan, G., 1999, Improving
Web Interaction on Small Displays, Computer Networks, 31, 1129-1137.
Kahneman, D., Solvic, P,.
and Tversky, A., 1982, Judgment under Uncertainty:
Heuristics and Biases, Cambridge University Press
Kamba, T., Elson, S., and Harpold, T., 1996, Using Small Screen Space More Efficiently,
The 1996 ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI ’96), Vancouver,
Canada, April 13-18, 1996 pp. 383-390.
Kiger, J., 1984, The depth/breadth tradeoff in the design of menu-driven interfaces,
International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 20, 201-213.
Kim, L., and Albers, M., 2001, Web Design Issues when Searching for Information in a
Small Screen Display, The 19th Annual International Conference on Computer Documentation
(SIGDOC’01), October 21-24, Santa Fe, New Mexico, 2001 pp. 193-200.
Kim, H., Kim, J., Lee, Y., Chae, M., and Choi, Y., 2002, An Empirical Study of the Use
47
Contexts and Usability Problems in the Mobile Internet, The 35th Hawaii International
Conference on Systems Sciences (HICSS’02), Hawaii, January, 2002.
McDonald, J., D., and Schvaneveldt, R., W., 1998, The application of user knowledge to
interface design, in: R. Guindon, (Ed.), Cognitive Science and its Applications for HumanComputer interaction. Hillsdale, Erlbaum, NJ, pp. 289-338.
Melchior, M., 2001, Perceptually guided scrolling for reading continuous text on small
screen devices, HCI 2001, Lille, France, September 10-14.
Miller, D. P., 1981, The Depth/Breadth tradeoff in hierarchical computer menus, The
Human Factors Society 25th Annual Meeting, Miller, GA, 1981 pp.296-300.
Miller, G. A., 1956, The magical number seven plus or minus two: Some limits on our
capacity for processing information, Psychological Review, 63, 81-97.
Nielsen, J. and Ramsay, M., 2000, WAP Usability Report, 2000. Available at
http://www.nngroup.com/reports/wap/.
Nimwegen, C., Pouw, M., and Oostenorp, H., 1999, The Influence of structure and
reading-manipulation on usability of hypertexts, Interacting with computers, 12, 7-21.
NOKIA, 2001, Nokia Service development guideline, Available at www.nokia.com.
Openwave,
2001,
Openwave
GSM
http://www.openwave.com
48
Application
guideline,
Available
at
Otter, M., and Johnson, H., 2000, Lost in hyperspace: metrics and mental models.
Interacting with computers, 13, 1-40.
Paap, K., and Cooke, N., 1997, Design of Menus, in: M. Helander, T. K. Landauer, (2nd
Ed.), Handbook of Human-Computer Interaction, Elsevier Science B. V., Amsterdam,
Netherlands.
Redish, J., 1994, Understanding Readers, in: C. Barnum, and S. Carliner, (Ed.),
Techniques for Technical communicators. Macmillan, New York, pp. 15-41.
Schwarz, E., Beldie, I., and Pastoor, S., 1983, A Comparison of paging and scrolling for
changing screen contents by inexperienced user, Human Factors, 25, 279-282.
Snowberry, K., Parkinson, S., and Sisson, N. 1983, Computer display menus, Ergonomics,
26: 7, 699-712.
Swierenga, S., 1990, Menuing and Scrolling as alternative information access techniques
for computer systems: Interfacing with the user, The Human Factors Society the 34th Annual
Meeting, vol. 1, 1990 pp. 356-359.
Telstra, 2001, Telstra WAP Design Guideline for developers of WML Application,
available at http://www.telstra.com.au/brand/index.htm.
Tombaugh, J., Lickorish, A., and Wright, P., 1987, Multi-window displays for readers of
lengthy texts, International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 26, 597-616.
49
Venkatesh, V., Ramesh, V. and Massey, A., 2003, Understanding usability in mobile
commerce, Communications of ACM, 46 (12), 53-56.
Wallace, D., Anderson, N., and Shneiderman, B., 1995, Time stress effects on two menu
selection systems, in: G. Perlman, GK. Green, M. S. Wogalter, (Ed.), Human Factors
Perspectives on Human-Computer Interaction, Selections from Proc. of Human Factors and
Ergonomics Society Annual Meetings 1983-1994, pp. 105-109.
50
[Figure 1] Horizontal depth and Vertical depth
[Figure 1-B]
[Figure 1-A]
x
y
List1
List2
List3
.
.
.
y’
y
y1
y2
y3
y4
y’
Listk-1
ListK
z
ListK
.
.
target
: Horizontal Depth
: Vertical Depth
z
target
[Figure 2] Two sample screen size used in the experiment
(b) Small Screen / 1 page
(a) Large Screen / 1 page
57
58
59
60
51
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
item
item
item
item
57
58
59
60
Scroll Area
item
item
item
item
item
item
item
item
Item
item
item
item
.
.
.
Invisible area
8
9
10
11
Invisible area
Item
item
item
item
.
.
.
OK
Visible area
OK
UP
Scroll Area
UP
Best Melodies
Item 1
item 2
item 3
Visible area
Best Melodies
1
1. item
LG 카드
item 2
item 3
item 4
item 5
item 6
[Figure 3] Research Model
Task Complexity
Objective Navigation activities
Between-page Navigation
Within-page Navigation
Horizontal Depth
Screen Size
Subjective Navigation Satisfaction
Perceived Depth
User Satisfaction
[Figure 4] Configuration of GVM
Develop
Mobile
Experimental
Upload to GVM Server Group
Download Experimental Site
52
[Table 1] Experimental Design
Screen Size
Horizontal Depth
Small Screen
Large Screen
1
Simple / Comparative
Simple / Comparative
4
Simple / Comparative
Simple / Comparative
6
Simple / Comparative
Simple / Comparative
[Figure 5] Sample screen (4HD/15B-Small screen) used in this experiment
(a) 1st page / 4 pages
Best Melodies
1.item
LG1 카드
item 2
item 3
UP
item 4
item 5
item 6
.
.
item 13
item 14
item 15
N ext p ag e
OK
(b) 2nd page / 4 pages
Best Melodies
1.item
LG16카드
item 17
item 18
UP
OK
item 19
item 20
item 21
.
.
item 28
item 29
item 30
N ext
P revious
(d) 4th page / 4 pages
Best Melodies
1.item
LG46카드
item 47
item 48
UP
item
item
item
.
.
item
item
item
OK
49
50
51
58
59
60
P revious
53
[Figure 6] Experimental System
Small Camera was attached to the mobile
phone and the mobile screen was recorded
on a video tape.
Data cable was connected
to the desktop computer
(b) Observation Room
(a) Subject room
[Table 2] Questions of user satisfaction in terms of usability
No.
Post-Questionnaire Items
Source
Q1
It took longer to complete a given task than I had expected.
QUIS
Q2
It was easy to go back to the pages I visited previously.
QUIS
Q3
It was easy to get information about where I am now in the menu
QUIS
structure.
Q4
It was clear how to get an insight into the whole menu structure
QUIS
Q5
I just followed simple steps to reach target information.
PUEU
Q6
It was easy to navigate between pages I was interested in.
QUIS
Q7
It was easy to find target information.
PUEU
54
[Table 3] Summarizing Hypotheses and Results
HYPOTHESES
RESULTS
Simple Task
Comparative Task
H1: Horizontal depth affects users’ navigation activities and perception.
H1a: Deeper horizontal depth will increase BPN.
H1b: Deeper horizontal depth will decrease WPN.
H1c: Deeper horizontal depth will increase perceived depth.
H1d: Deeper horizontal depth will decrease user satisfaction.
Rejected
Rejected
Supported
Rejected
Supported
Supported
Supported
Supported
Rejected
Supported
Supported
Rejected
Rejected
Supported
Supported
Rejected
H2: Screen size affects users’ navigation activities and perception.
H2a: Smaller screen size will increase BPN.
H2b: Smaller screen size will increase WPN.
H2c: Smaller screen size will increase perceived depth.
H2d: Smaller screen size will decrease user satisfaction.
H3: The relation between horizontal depth/screen size and navigation activities/perception will be affected by task
complexity.
Three Way- ANOVA: Interaction Effects Between Task Complexity
and Horizontal Depth :
BPN, WPN
Three Way- ANOVA: Interaction Effects Between Task Complexity
and Screen Size :
None
55
[Figure 7] Between-Page Navigation Activities – Comparative Search
The number of BPN
Comparative Search
6
5
4
3
2
1
0
1 depth
4 depth
6 depth
Large
1.732
2.832
3.302
Small
1.482
4.271
5.357
Horizontal Depth
[Figure 8] Within-Page Navigation Activities –Comparative Search
The number of WPN
Comparative Search
10
8
6
4
2
0
1 depth
4 depth
6 depth
Large
6.372
4.253
4.303
Small
9.285
6.839
5.293
Horizontal Depth
56
[Figure 9] Perceived Depth
Perceived Depth Comparative Search
10.0
8.0
6.0
4.0
2.0
0.0
1 depth
4 depth
6 depth
Large
6.523
3.210
2.734
Small
8.636
4.394
6.032
Horizontal Depth
[Figure 10] Overall Satisfaction
Satisfaction
7.0
6.0
5.0
4.0
3.0
2.0
1.0
0.0
Comparative Search
1 depth
4 depth
6 depth
Large
4.399
5.390
6.420
Small
3.423
6.297
4.121
Horizontal Depth
57
Download