Educational Studies, Vol. 30, No. 2, June 2004 Why boys achieve less at school than girls: the difference between boys’ and girls’ academic culture Mieke Van Houtte* M.Van HoutteDepartment of SociologyGhent UniversityUniversiteitstraat 4Ghent9000BelgiumMieke.VanHoutte@UGhent.be Ghent University, Belgium Recently, research into gender differences in achievement has mainly concentrated on the underperformance of boys in comparison with girls. Qualitative research in particular points to the importance of the gender-specific cultures adolescents experience. The purpose of this article is to test quantitatively the explanatory value of academic culture with respect to the stated gender differences in achievement. Use is made of data of 3760 pupils in the third and the fourth year of secondary education in a sample of 34 schools in Flanders (Belgium). A distinction is made between general schools preparing students for higher education and schools offering technical and vocational education. It is demonstrated that boys’ culture is less study oriented than girls’ culture and that this difference can be held responsible for the gender differences in achievement, at least in general schools. In technical/vocational schools, boys seem to oppose the study culture. Introduction In recent years, in many countries, increasing attention has been paid to the underachievement of boys in comparison with girls. Actually, the last few decades have displayed a shift in attention: whereas in the mid-1970s it was girls’ underperformance that was identified as problematic, in the 1990s boys’ underachievement became the focal point (Epstein et al., 1998; Frosh et al., 2002). The question of whether this increasing attention is pertinent or not (see different contributions in Epstein et al., 1998; Frosh et al., 2002) will not be under discussion in this article. It has been demonstrated repeatedly that in general girls outperform boys (e.g. Jackson, 1998). Starting from this plain fact, this article focuses on secondary education and aims to advance and to test quantitatively the study orientation of girls’ and boys’ adolescent culture as an explanation for the differing achievement levels of boys and girls. Girls’ and boys’ study culture Researchers and other persons involved have suggested various possible explanations for the underperformance of boys. A common explanation points at the difference *Corresponding author: Department of Sociology. Ghent University. Universiteitstraat 4, 9000 Gent, Belgium. Email: Mieke.VanHoutte@UGent.be ISSN 0305-5698 (print)/ISSN 1465-3500 (online)/04/020159-15 2004 Taylor & Francis Ltd DOI: 10/1080.0305569032000159804 160 M. Van Houtte in school attitudes between boys and girls. Various studies have shown that boys are less motivated than girls and have less positive attitudes toward school (Davies, 1984; Darom & Rich, 1988; Cox, 2000; Francis, 2000; Warrington et al., 2000), although the difference is not always that big (Keys & Fernandes, 1993; Blatchford, 1996). In general, it is recorded that girls spend more time doing homework, display less disturbing behaviour in the classroom and play truant less often. Girls have higher expectations of themselves and are more enthusiastic about continuing their studies. Boys take it easier, work less hard and are distracted more quickly (Barber, 1996; Warrington et al., 2000). Obviously, these kinds of explanations present the stated differences in achievement as something bio-psychological, thus a personal given. By nature, girls would be more persevering, while boys would need more encouragement. Boys would be more inclined to risky behaviour, with as a consequence more whiz-kids but, equally, more failures (Barber, 1996). Other, more sociologically inspired explanations focus on girls and boys as members of a group and reason in terms of popularity. In this argument, boys more than girls consider educational achievement as not ‘cool’ (Francis, 2000; Warrington et al., 2000; Whitelaw et al., 2000). Here attention is paid to the effect of the group one belongs to, independently of the individual’s beliefs. A boy possibly could be personally convinced of the importance of studying and achieving but not act accordingly for fear of becoming a pariah (Warrington et al., 2000; Whitelaw et al., 2000). As far back as the early 1960s, James Coleman (1961) demonstrated that educational achievement does not make adolescents more popular with peers. The numerous replications of Coleman’s research supported this finding time and again (e.g. Sebald, 1981; Williams & White, 1983; Thirer & Wright, 1985; Suitor & Reavis, 1995; Landsheer et al., 1998). This does not mean that adolescents consider educational achievement as not important at all, but in comparison to other things, such as sports (boys) and physical appearance (girls), educational achievement comes off worst (Coleman, 1961; Suitor & Reavis, 1995). For both boys and girls, being popular is exactly what counts in adolescence, but with respect to boys it can be stated that achievement does not at all suit their image. Girls, on the other hand, seem permitted to combine two images: as long as they come across as ‘cool’ outside school, it is acceptable that they work hard at school (Francis, 2000; Warrington et al., 2000). There seems to be an incongruity between the manifestation of masculine behaviour on the one hand and educational effort and achievement on the other hand. Educational achievement or displaying other school attitudes is antithetical to typical masculine—that is, macho—behaviour, which is a condition of popularity for boys. Educational effort and achievement is typified as feminine behaviour (Epstein, 1998; Power et al., 1998; Frosh et al., 2002). All this can be placed within the broader frame of the existence of gender-specific youth cultures. In adolescence peers become more and more the main source of approval, admiration and respect. In alliance with this evolution, adolescents define their own status criteria. They develop their own youth culture with a particular language, particular symbols and a particular value system (Coleman, 1961). What is more, girls and boys tend to seek the company of youngsters of the same gender, Why boys achieve less at school than girls 161 even in a gender-mixed environment. Girls and boys do associate with each other, but they identify chiefly with people of the same gender and prefer friends of the same gender (Kandel, 1978; Karweit & Hansell, 1983; Dornbush, 1989; Warrington et al., 2000; Gurian, 2001). As such, one can expect a distinction between girls’ culture and boys’ culture, and given the difference in school attitudes between boys and girls, one can expect the boys’ culture to be less study oriented than the girls’ culture. A first hypothesis that can be derived, then, is that boys’ culture is less study oriented than girls’ culture. Study culture and achievement There are two ways in which culture may influence behaviour, in casu achievement. In the first instance, there is of course the process of socialization, or the process by which an individual acquires the habits, values and norms of a group in order to be able to function within that group (Schein, 1984; Owens & Steinhoff, 1989; Stockard & Mayberry, 1992). Secondly, groups tend to exert pressure on their members to adapt, signalling that nonconformity is not tolerated and would be penalized by, for example, exclusion (Owens, 1987; Owens & Steinhoff, 1989; Stockard & Mayberry, 1992). Of course, individuals will not blindly accept the normative pressure and social control of a group. This happens only when the individual attaches importance to the acceptance and approval of the members of the group—in other words, when the group is a reference group (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Owens, 1987). The reference group theory teaches that usually individuals attach the greatest importance to those groups they belong to themselves (Shibutani, 1955). As for adolescents, it is a fact that acceptance by peers is primary (Coleman, 1961). Peers in general and companions in particular—for example, fellow pupils— form the main reference group for adolescents (Sherif & Sherif, 1964). As such, there is a big chance that fellow pupils at school will function as a normative reference group. Coleman (1961; Coleman et al., 1966) showed that the prominent pupils at school determine the academic aspirations of youngsters in the first place. The relationship between pupils exerts more influence on pupils’ achievement than any other factor (Johnson, 1980). It can be expected that boys will be directed by their male schoolmates, whereas girls will look to their female schoolmates, because, as said before, adolescents identify mainly with peers of their own gender (Warrington et al., 2000; Gurian, 2001). Furthermore, boys seem to attach more importance to how they are seen by the group, while girls consider interpersonal, intimate relationships more important (Davies, 1984; Francis, 2000; Warrington et al., 2000). Accordingly, boys are more concerned about their image than girls are: boys want to look ‘cool’ and want to behave in a ‘cool’ manner. Demonstrating good school attitudes has no part in that. On the basis of ethnographic research Francis (2000) concludes that this difference in what concerns boys and girls could be the source of the difference in achievement between boys and girls. On the basis of all this, a second hypothesis can be derived, namely that the 162 M. Van Houtte gender-specific study cultures influence the academic achievement of both boys and girls and that this influence is stronger with respect to boys. Ultimately, given the two derived hypotheses, the final question is then whether the boys’ and girls’ study cultures might be responsible for the differential achievement of boys and girls. It is this question that the present article seeks to answer. Research design As a preamble to the actual aim of this article, we need to examine first whether the boys and girls under research display a difference in academic achievement. We will concentrate on the second grade of secondary education—that is, the third and the fourth year (see ‘Data’). All analyses (performed using SPSS11, unless stated otherwise) will be carried out separately for two types of schools, namely schools offering general education in preparation for higher education and schools offering technical and vocational education. This separation is made to rule out possible effects of streaming. The effect of gender on achievement will be examined by means of a t-test and by means of a multiple regression analysis comprising several control variables, namely ability, socio-economic status (SES), parental involvement, times retained in primary education, and whether a pupil’s selected curriculum is the result of a positive choice or not. Only when we find a gender difference in achievement can we proceed with testing the previously generated hypotheses. To test the first hypothesis—that boys’ culture is less study oriented than girls’ culture—it is necessary to develop a measure of study culture first (see ‘Variables’). Since it is demonstrated that schoolmates of the same gender influence adolescents the most, we will measure boys’ and girls’ study cultures for each school. After this is accomplished, we will compare the cultures boys and girls experience by means of a t-test to discover whether they are different and which of the two is more study oriented. The first part of the second hypothesis—stating that the gender-specific study culture influences the academic achievement of both boys and girls—will be tested by means of a multiple regression analysis carried out separately for boys and girls, including the same control variables as the preceding regression analysis. To test the second part of the hypothesis—that the influence of study culture is stronger for boys—we will carry out additional tests of interaction effects. Only once it can be confirmed that boys’ and girls’ study cultures are different, and that boys’ and girls’ achievement is affected by their gender-specific study culture, does it make sense to examine if the gender-specific study culture can be held responsible for the difference in achievement between boys and girls. We will answer this question by means of a multiple regression analysis controlling the relation between gender and achievement for gender-specific study culture, holding constant the previously mentioned control variables (ability, SES, and so forth). If the relation between gender and achievement does not hold when controlling for gender-specific study culture, the role of study culture as an intermediating variable in the relation between gender and achievement will be demonstrated. Why boys achieve less at school than girls 163 Data The analyses in this article are based on retrospective data gathered in the school year 1999–2000 in a sample of 34 secondary schools—15 general schools preparing students for higher education and 19 technical/vocational schools—in the Flemishspeaking part of Belgium. A total of 3760 fifth-year pupils (generally speaking, 16–17 years old)—1591 in general schools and 2169 in technical/vocational schools—filled out a written questionnaire in class in the presence of a teacher and the researcher. Since the data are retrospective in nature, we know the achievement scores of the years preceding the fifth year but not of the fifth year. Under study here then is the second grade of secondary education—containing the third and the fourth year. Variables Academic achievement is represented by the grade point average at the end of the school year. In general schools we find a mean grade point average of 72.44% (st.dev. ⫽ 7.03, N ⫽ 1572) in the third year and a mean of 69.90% (st. dev. ⫽ 7.42, N ⫽ 1496) in the fourth year. In technical/vocational schools we find a mean grade point average of 68.89% (st. dev. 7.25, N ⫽ 1635) in the third year and a mean of 68.31% (st. dev. 7.15, N ⫽ 1811) in the fourth year. As for the main determinant gender, being a boy is coded 0 and being a girl is coded 1. Sixty-five per cent of the respondents are male. In the schools for general education 55.1% are male, in the technical/vocational schools 71.2%. The measurement of the explaining variable gender-specific study culture needs a more thorough description. In order to conceptualize the study culture, the study involvement of the pupils is questioned by means of a five-point scale consisting of 11 items (Brutsaert & Bracke, 1994; Brutsaert, 2001), assessing the general feeling of study involvement and learning motivation—for example, ‘I don’t like to study’ or ‘I don’t understand the importance of studying’. This instrument measures how concerned pupils are about going to school and studying in general. After substituting the missing values (for each item only one is missing, except for items three and four with two missing) by means of ‘item correlation substitution’ (ICS) (Huisman, 1999), a confirmatory factor analysis by LISREL 8.12 (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1993) produces for each item a significant loading higher than 0.40, so all items can be retained to form one scale (2 ⫽ 27.99; df ⫽ 21, p ⫽ 0.14, RMSEA ⫽ 0.0094, AGFI ⫽ 1.00; Cronbach’s alpha ⫽ 0.82). At this point, we have a reliable measure of study involvement at the individual pupil level, but since it is the purpose to measure culture, and since culture is a group feature usually defined as ‘shared assumptions’, ‘shared beliefs’ or ‘shared cognitions’ (e.g. Schein, 1984; Rousseau, 1990), the aggregation of the obtained measure is a necessary next step. A customary aggregation strategy is to calculate the mean of the scores of the individual members of the group or organization (e.g. Stern, 1970; Trickett et al., 1982; Hofstede et al., 1990). In doing this, one has to be sure this aggregation is permitted. In other words, one must examine whether the 164 M. Van Houtte aggregated measure is reliable and represents something actually shared at the group or organization level (Glick, 1985). A useful measure is the ‘mean rater reliability’ (Glick, 1985), calculated by means of the Spearman-Brown formula based on the intraclass correlation of a one-way analysis of variance: ICC(1, k) ⫽ (BMS-WMS)/ BMS (with k ⫽ number of raters in each group or organization) (Shrout & Fleiss, 1979; Glick, 1985). The result must be at minimum 0.60 to permit an aggregation at the group or organization level (Shrout & Fleiss, 1979; Glick, 1985). Concerning study involvement, aggregation is permitted at the school level per gender category by calculating the mean per school for boys and girls separately (for boys ICC ⫽ 0.68 and for girls ICC ⫽ 0.67). Study involvement is clearly something shared by the pupils of the same gender in the same school, so it is legitimate to speak of a ‘study culture’. The oneway analyses of variance show that the genderspecific mean study involvement of the pupils differs significantly between the schools (p ⫽ 0.000). Given these differences between schools, one can say that the gender-specific pupil culture is more study oriented in some schools than in others. A pertinent question here is whether the culture of the fifth-year pupils of a school forms a good representation of the pupil culture of the whole school. There are good reasons to think it does. It is known that organizational culture—and, specifically, pupil culture—is passed on from one generation to another (Bidwell, 1965; Hughes et al., 1968). In each group or organization, newcomers are socialized. In other words, newcomers adopt the dominant beliefs and assumptions. These and other factors make culture a very stable organizational feature that almost never changes, except very slowly (Schein, 1984). So it should be acceptable to use the pupil culture of the fifth-year pupils as a representation of the pupil culture of the whole school. Finally, for reasons of practicability, the obtained measure of gender-specific study culture is disaggregated to the individual pupil level: each respondent receives the value of his or her gender group at school. As such, the measure can be interpreted as the study culture a pupil deals with. Since culture is a supra-individual feature, it would be more correct not to disaggregate the measure and to perform a multilevel analysis, the proper method when dealing with cases that are not independent from each other. But given the reasoning followed, highlighting gender-specific culture, girls and boys are subdivided into different groups, which precludes a comparison by means of multilevel analysis. The gender-specific study culture, as expected, correlates with the study involvement of the pupil (Pearsons r ⫽ 0.279, p ⫽ 0.000), but there is no question of exchangeable measures. ‘Study culture’ definitely measures something different from ‘study involvement’. The gender-specific study culture strongly correlates with gender (Pearsons r ⫽ 0.650, p ⫽ 0.000). The pupil’s ability is measured by means of the grade point average (GPA) at the end of primary education. Neither in general schools nor in technical/vocational schools can a difference be discerned in the mean ability between boys and girls (general schools: mean boys ⫽ 83.65, mean girls ⫽ 83.36, t ⫽ 0.951, p ⫽ 0.342; technical/vocational schools: mean boys ⫽ 70.85, mean girls ⫽ 70.39, t ⫽ 1.134, p ⫽ 0.257). Because pupils are grouped in streams by ability and because the dependent variable is achievement, we calculate and work with the pupil’s deviance Why boys achieve less at school than girls 165 from the mean of his or her stream in the year being considered. After all, we must take into account the possibility that a pupil with high ability achieves less in a higher stream than a pupil with low ability in a lower stream. As such, the relative ability is more relevant than the absolute ability. The pupil’s socio-economic background (SES) is measured by means of the occupational prestige of father and mother (Erikson et al., 1979)—the highest of both is used as an indicator of the SES of the family (cf. Forehand et al., 1987). On average, in schools for general education boys (mean ⫽ 6.27) have a significantly (t ⫽ 4.699, p ⫽ 0.000) higher SES than girls (mean ⫽ 5.86). In technical/vocational schools no difference can be found between the SES of boys (mean ⫽ 4.46) and girls (mean ⫽ 4.35, t ⫽ 1.235, p ⫽ 0.217). The parental school involvement is measured by means of an index consisting of nine questions (inspired by Muller, 1995, 1998; and Rumberger, 1995), like ‘Do your parents take part in certain school activities?’ or ‘Do your parents keep an eye on your homework?’, which the respondents could answer with five answering categories from ‘never’ to ‘always’ (range 0–4). The total score on these questions defines the general school involvement of the parents. On average, with respect to parental involvement there is no difference between boys (general schools: mean ⫽ 19.71, technical/vocational schools: mean ⫽ 19.26) and girls (general schools: mean ⫽ 20.11, t ⫽ ⫺ 1.49, p ⫽ 0.136; technical/vocational schools: mean ⫽ 18.93, t ⫽ 1.177, p ⫽ 0.239). On average, there is no significant difference in the number of times a year was repeated in primary education between boys and girls in general schools (t ⫽ ⫺ 1.69, p ⫽ 0.091) and in technical/vocational schools (t ⫽ 1.61, p ⫽ 0.107). Finally, for the third year—the year in which pupils must make an explicit stream choice—the pupils were asked why they chose their stream. A distinction is made between ‘positive’ reasons (code 1), such as ‘It is interesting’ or ‘I have the capacity to do it’, and rather ‘negative’ reasons (code 0), such as ‘I follow the choice of my friends’ or ‘I want to aim as high as possible’. In general schools 83.2% of the girls gave a positive reason, as did 76.4% of the boys, and in technical/vocational schools 80.9% of the girls gave a positive reason, as did 77.5% of the boys. Analysis To start with, a t-test is performed comparing the achievement scores of boys and girls in the third and the fourth year of secondary education in general schools and technical/vocational schools. On average, in each year boys obtain a significantly (see Table 1) lower achievement score than girls. A regression analysis relating achievement to gender confirms this finding. Furthermore, the significant relation between gender and achievement holds when controlling for the pupil characteristics of ability, SES, parental involvement, times retained in primary school and, for the third year, reason of stream choice (see Table 2). To test the first hypothesis—that girls’ culture is more study oriented than boys’ culture—a t-test compares the study cultures boys and girls deal with. On average, 166 M. Van Houtte Table 1. Achievement by gender in the third and fourth year of secondary education, general schools and technical/vocational schools. Means (with standard deviations) and results of the t-test General schools Achievement third year Achievement fourth year Boys 71.71 (7.22) N ⫽ 881 69.01 (7.55) N ⫽ 841 Girls 73.37 (6.67) N ⫽ 691 71.05 (7.09) N ⫽ 655 Technical/vocational schools t ⫺ 4.70*** ⫺ 5.33*** Boys 68.09 (7.20) N ⫽ 1177 67.47 (7.15) N ⫽ 1291 Girls 70.96 (6.98) N ⫽ 458 70.38 (6.72) N ⫽ 520 t ⫺ 7.31*** ⫺ 7.98*** ***p ⬍ 0.001 the culture girls experience is significantly more study oriented than the culture boys experience, in general schools as well as in technical/vocational schools (see Table 3). This finding permits a confirmation of the first hypothesis. The second hypothesis—that the achievement of boys and girls is influenced by the gender-specific study culture and that this influence is stronger for boys—is tested by means of a multiple regression analysis carried out separately for boys and girls with controls for several pupil characteristics. The hypothesis cannot be confirmed entirely (see Table 4). In general schools the study culture a pupil experiences obviously exerts an influence on the pupil’s achievement, and this is true for boys and girls equally. In technical/vocational schools, on the other hand, the achievement of boys is not influenced significantly by study culture in the third year. The effect of study culture on achievement for girls in the third year can be accepted as borderline significant (p ⫽ 0.052). In the fourth year a significant effect is found for boys and girls, but the direction of the effect for boys is very surprising. The negative coefficient indicates that the more study oriented the culture is, the less the boys achieve. This could mean that boys in technical/vocational education tend to react against the study culture rather than comply with it. This does not apply for girls, and the interaction effect measuring the difference between girls and boys in this respect is significant (p ⫽ 0.013). Otherwise no other significant interaction effects are found, so the second part of the hypothesis—stating that the influence of study culture is stronger for boys—cannot be confirmed. Finally, the central question of this article, namely whether the gender-specific study culture may be responsible for the differential achievement of boys and girls, can be answered. On the basis of the multiple regression analysis, we can tell that in general schools in the third and fourth year the effect of gender disappears when holding study culture constant (see Table 5), indicating that study culture may be held responsible for the gender differences in achievement. In technical/vocational schools when controlling for study culture, the effect of gender on achievement slightly diminishes but remains highly significant in the third year. In the fourth year, controlling for gender-specific study culture engenders an increase of the effect (see Table 5). Obviously this has everything to do with the fact that study culture does 0.013*** 0.115*** *p ⬍ 0.05 **p ⬍ 0.01 ***p ⬍ 0.001 Gender Ability SES Parental Involvement Retention Prim. Educ. Choice Stream R2 Third year Third year Fourth year Technical/vocational schools 0.115*** 0.019*** ⫺ 0.002 ⫺ 0.053* 0.092*** 0.033*** / ⫺ 0.030 0.081*** 0.036*** 0.007 0.061* 0.068*** / 0.054* 0.127*** 0.137*** 0.150*** 0.182*** 0.186*** 0.189*** 0.194*** 0.279*** 0.230*** 0.211*** 0.167*** 0.151*** 0.149*** ⫺ 0.015 ⫺ 0.055* ⫺ 0.040 ⫺ 0.012 0.036 0.002 Fourth year General schools Table 2. The relation between gender and achievement in the third and fourth year of secondary education in general and technical/vocational schools, without and with controls for several pupil characteristics. Results of the regression analysis (beta coefficients and explained variance R2) Why boys achieve less at school than girls 167 168 M. Van Houtte Table 3. Study culture by gender. Means (and standard deviations) and results of the t-test General schools Study culture third year Study culture fourth year Technical/vocational schools Boys Girls t Boys Girls t 21.91 (1.05) N ⫽ 883 21.82 (0.99) N ⫽ 842 24.17 (1.48) N ⫽ 692 23.92 (1.23) N ⫽ 662 –33.88*** 20.19 (1.01) N ⫽ 1213 20.20 (1.06) N ⫽ 1310 22.45 (1.25) N ⫽ 482 22.50 (1.23) N ⫽ 538 –35.29*** –35.74*** –37.89*** ***p ⬍ 0.001 not influence the achievement of boys in the third year and negatively influences their achievement in the fourth year. Discussion and conclusion The international, consistent finding that boys achieve less than girls at school calls for an explanation. In this article an attempt has been made to deliver a part of this explanation by focusing on the study culture boys and girls experience at school. It has been argued that boys and girls deal with their own gender-specific study cultures. Step by step it has been shown then that boys’ culture is less study oriented than girls’ culture and that this study culture influences boys’ and girls’ academic achievement. It should be noted, though, that the analyses make a distinction between schools offering general education and schools offering technical/vocational education, in order to exclude streaming effects. This distinction offers some interesting findings. As such it can be stated that in technical/vocational schools study culture does not exert any significant influence on the achievement of boys in the third year. In the fourth year the effect of study culture is significant but, contrary to what would be expected, negative! This means that the more study oriented their experienced culture is, the less the boys achieve. I suggested already that this could mean that boys in technical/vocational schools react against the existing study culture. An additional analysis shows that this negative effect of study culture occurs only in boys scoring below the mean, that is, in low-achieving boys, which can perhaps be seen as a confirmation that it is a matter of opposing. Chiefly poorly performing boys would go through a kind of identity crisis because of the growing importance of knowledge in society (Jackson, 1998), where stress is laid on cognitive ability, a criterion they do not and cannot fulfil. This stress especially affects boys in vocational curricula since these, more than general curricula, train the boys for specific, men’s work. Pupils in general education, even when performing poorly, feel quite certain of getting a job, but boys in vocational curricula are being trained for the very work whose existence is threatened, which explains their opposition toward study culture. Their former securities—in the first instance, getting a job—reassure then no longer, especially considering the feminization of society (Jackson, 1998). On the shop-floor, for example, automation means that 0.111*** 0.001 ⫺ 0.011 0.115*** ⫺ 0.085* ⫺ 0.026 0.072*** / ⫺ 0.026 0.105*** / ⫺ 0.043 0.240*** 0.183*** 0.011 0.306*** 0.249*** 0.234*** 0.140*** 0.169*** 0.120*** ⫺ 0.046 ⫺ 0.047 ⫺ 0.029 Girls 0.135*** 0.097** Boys Fourth year 0.092** 0.086** Girls °p ⫽ 0.052 *p ⬍ 0.05 **p ⬍ 0.01 ***p ⬍ 0.001 Study culture Ability SES Parental Involvement Retention prim. educ. Choice stream R2 Boys Third year General schools 0.089° Girls Girls 0.085* Boys ⫺ .062* Fourth year 0.050*** 0.045 0.082*** ⫺ 0.093 0.085** ⫺ 0.011 0.033*** / 0.075*** / 0.079** ⫺ 0.020 0.203*** 0.251*** 0.142*** 0.244*** 0.001 ⫺ 0.040 ⫺ 0.061 ⫺ 0.023 0.044 0.003 0.036 ⫺ 0.088* 0.018 Boys Third year Technical/vocational schools Table 4. The relation between study culture and achievement for boys and girls in the third and fourth year of secondary education in general and technical/vocational schools, with controls for several pupil characteristics. Results of the multiple regression analysis (beta coefficients and explained variance R2) Why boys achieve less at school than girls 169 Fourth year Third year Fourth year Technical/vocational schools 0.117*** 0.123*** ⫺ 0.006 ⫺ 0.002 0.115*** ⫺ 0.056* ⫺ 0.053* 0.092*** / ⫺ 0.030 0.150*** 0.104*** / ⫺ 0.034 0.081*** 0.007 0.061* 0.056° 0.082*** 0.008 0.060* 0.068*** / 0.054* ⫺ 0.026 0.068*** / 0.055* 0.127*** 0.050 0.150*** 0.047 0.186*** 0.147*** 0.194*** 0.212*** 0.279*** 0.279*** 0.230*** 0.235*** 0.211*** 0.214*** 0.167*** 0.166*** 0.151*** 0.151*** 0.149*** 0.147*** ⫺ 0.015 ⫺ 0.015 ⫺ 0.055* ⫺ 0.055* ⫺ 0.040 ⫺ 0.046 ⫺ 0.012 ⫺ 0.011 0.036 0.035 0.002 0.003 °p ⬍ 0.10 *p ⬍ 0.05 **p ⬍ 0.01 ***p ⬍ 0.001 Gender Ability SES Parental Involvement Retention prim. educ. Choice stream Study culture R2 Third year General schools Table 5. Intermediating role of gender-specific study culture in the relation between gender and achievement in the third and fourth year of secondary education, general and technical/vocational schools. Results of the multiple regression analysis (beta-coefficients and explained variance R2) 170 M. Van Houtte Why boys achieve less at school than girls 171 fewer jobs are reserved strictly for men, so women push out men more and more (Power et al., 1998). As a consequence, boys in this situation, feeling threatened, are inclined to overdo their masculinity (Jackson, 1998), for example by opposing school (cf. Willis, 1977). Finally, it is demonstrated statistically that the effect of gender on achievement can be explained by the gender-specific study cultures, at least in general schools. The research presented here, and especially the results obtained for general schools, indicates that an explanation in terms of the adolescent study culture and the difference between boys’ and girls’ study cultures might be a good direction to go in. Future research should take into account other measures of culture, for example measures that concretely assess different forms of opposition toward school, which could perhaps yield better results, especially for technical/vocational schools. If it can be shown that the source of boys’ underachievement must be sought in their group functioning, we would then know where to intervene if necessary. Acknowledgements A previous version of this article was presented at the 4th NSV/VVS Market Day Sociology in Nijmegen (the Netherlands) at the 22nd of May 2003. I would like to thank Peter Mascini and Rene Veenstra for their valuable comments. References Ajzen, I. & Fishbein, M. (1980) Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior (Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall). Barber, M. (1996) The learning game: arguments for an education revolution (London, Victor Gollancz). Bidwell, C. (1965) The school as a formal organization, in: J. March (Ed.) Handbook of organization (Chicago, IL, Rand McNally). Blatchford, P. (1996) Pupils’ views on school work and school from 7 to 16 years, Research Papers in Education, 11(3), 263–288. Brutsaert, H. (2001) Co-educatie. Studiekansen en kwaliteit van het schoolleven (Leuven/Apeldoorn, Garant). Brutsaert, H. & Bracke, P. (1994) Gender context of the elementary school: sex differences in affective outcomes, Educational Studies, 20(1), 3–11. Coleman, J. (1961) The adolescent society. The social life of the teenager and its impact on education (New York, Free Press). Coleman, J., Campbell, E., Hobson, C., McPartland, J., Mood, A., Weinfeld, F. & York, R. (1966) Equality of educational opportunity (Washington DC, U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Office of Education, United States Government Printing Office). Cox, T. (2000) Pupils’ perspectives on their education, in: T. Cox (Ed.) Combating educational disadvantage. Meeting the needs of vulnerable children (London & New York, Falmer Press). Darom, E. & Rich, Y. (1988) Sex-differences in attitudes toward school: student self-reports and teacher perceptions, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 58(3), 350–355. Davies, L. (1984) Pupil power. Deviance and gender in school (London, Falmer Press). Dornbusch, S. (1989) The sociology of adolescence, Annual Review of Sociology, 15, 233–259. Epstein, D. (1998) Real boys don’t work: ‘underachievement’, masculinity and the harassment of the ‘sissies’, in: D. Epstein, J. Elwood, V. Hey & J. Maw (Eds) Failing boys? Issues in gender and achievement (Buckingham/Philadelphia, PA, Open University Press). 172 M. Van Houtte Epstein, D., Elwood, J., Hey, V. & Maw, J. (Eds) (1998)Failing boys? Issues in gender and achievement (Buckingham/Philadelphia, PA, Open University Press). Erikson, R., Goldthorpe, J. & Portocarero, L. (1979) Intergenerational class mobility in three West European countries: England, France and Sweden, British Journal of Sociology, 10, 415–441. Fishbein, M. & Ajzen, I. (1975) Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: an introduction to theory and research (Reading, MA, Addison-Wesley). Forehand, R., Middleton, K. & Long, N. (1987) Adolescent functioning as a consequence of recent parental divorce and the parental-adolescent relationship, Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 8(3), 305–315. Francis, B. (2000) Boys, girls and achievement. Addressing the classroom issues (London & New York, Routledge/Falmer). Frosh, S., Phoenix, A. & Pattman, R. (2002) Young masculinities. Understanding boys in contemporary society (New York, Palgrave). Glick, W. (1985) Conceptualizing and measuring organizational and psychological climate: pitfalls in multilevel research, Academy of Management Review, 10(3), 601–616. Gurian, M. (2001) Boys and girls learn differently! A guide for teachers and parents (San Francisco, CA, Jossey-Bass). Hofstede, G., Neuijen, B., Ohayv, D. & Sanders, G. (1990) Measuring organizational cultures: a qualitative and quantitative study accross twenty cases, Administrative Science Quarterly, 35, 286–316. Hughes, E., Becker, H. & Geer, B. (1968) Student culture and academic effort, in: R. Bell & H. Stub (Eds) The sociology of education. A sourcebook (revised edition) (Homewood, IL, The Dorsey Press). Huisman, M. (1999) Imputation of missing item responses: some simple techniques, in: M. Huisman (Ed.) Item nonresponse: occurence, causes, and imputation of missing answers to test itmes (Leiden, DSWO Press, Leiden University). Jackson, D. (1998) Breaking out of the binary trap: boys’ underachievement, schooling and gender relations, in: D. Epstein, J. Elwood, V. Hey & J. Maw (Eds) Failing boys? Issues in gender and achievement (Buckingham/Philadelphia, PA, Open University Press). Johnson, D. (1980) Group processes: influences of student-student interaction on school outcomes, in: J. McMillan (Ed.) The social psychology of school learning (New York, Academic Press). Jöreskog, K. & Sörbom, D. (1993) Lisrel 8: structural equation modeling with the SIMPLIS command language (Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers). Kandel, D. (1978) Similarity in real-life adolescent friendship pairs, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(3), 306–312. Karweit, N. & Hansell, S. (1983) Sex differences in adolescent relationships: friendship and status, in: J. Epstein & N. Karweit (Eds) Friends in school. Patterns of selection and influence in secondary schools (New York, Academic Press). Keys, W. & Fernandes, C. (1993) What do students think about school? Research into the factors associated with positive and negative attitudes towards school and education (Slough, National Foundation for Educational Research). Landsheer, H., Maassen, G., Bisschop, P. & Adema, L. (1998) Can higher grades result in fewer friends? A reexamination of the relation between academic and social competence, Adolescence, 33(129), 185–191. Muller, C. (1995) Maternal employment, parent involvement, and mathematics achievement among adolescents, Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 85–100. Muller, C. (1998) Gender differences in parental involvement and adolescents’ mathematics achievement, Sociology of Education, 71, 336–356. Owens, R. (1987) Organizational behavior in education (Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall). Owens, R. & Steinhoff, C. (1989) Towards a theory of organisational culture, Journal of Educational Administration, 27(3), 6–16. Why boys achieve less at school than girls 173 Power, S., Whitty, G., Edwards, T. & Wigfall, V. (1998) Schoolboys and schoolwork: gender identification and academic achievement, International Journal of Inclusive Education, 2(2), 135–153. Rousseau, D. (1990) Assessing organizational culture: the case for multiple methods, in: B. Schneider (Ed.) Organizational climate and culture (San Francisco, CA, Jossey-Bass Publishers). Rumberger, R. (1995) Dropping out of middle school: a multilevel analysis of students and schools, American Educational Research Journal, 32(3), 583–625. Schein, E. (1984) Coming to a new awareness of organizational culture, Sloan Management Review, 3–16. Sebald, H. (1981) Adolescents’ concept of popularity and unpopularity, comparing 1960 with 1976, Adolescence, 16(61), 187–193. Sherif, M. & Sherif, C. (1964) Acceptable and unacceptable behavior defined by group norms, in: M. Sherif & C. Sherif (Eds) Reference groups. Exploration into conformity and deviation of adolescents (New York, Harper and Row). Shibutani, T. (1955) Reference groups as perspectives, American Journal of Sociology, 60, 562– 569. Shrout, P. & Fleiss, J. (1979) Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability, Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420–428. Stern, G. (1970) People in context: measuring person-environment congruence in education and industry (New York, Wiley). Stockard, J. & Mayberry, M. (1992) Effective educational environments (Newbury Park, CA, Corwin Press). Suitor, J. & Reavis, R. (1995) Football, fast cars, and cheerleading: adolescent gender norms, 1978–1989, Adolescence, 30(118), 265–272. Thirer, J. & Wright, S. (1985) Sport and social status for adolescent males and females, Sociology of Sport Journal, 2, 164–171. Trickett, E., Castro, J., Trickett, P. & Shaffner, P. (1982) The independent school experience: aspects of the normative environments of single-sex and coed secondary schools, Journal of Educational Psychology, 74(3), 374–381. Warrington, M., Younger, M. & Williams, J. (2000) Student attitudes, image and the gender gap, British Educational Research Journal, 26(3), 393–407. Whitelaw, S., Milosevic, L. & Daniels, S. (2000) Gender, behaviour and achievement: a preliminary study of pupil perceptions and attitudes, Gender and Education, 12(1), 87–113. Williams, J. & White, K. (1983) Adolescent status systems for males and females at three age levels, Adolescence, 18(70), 381–389. Willis, P. (1977) Learning to labour. How working class kids get working class jobs (Aldershot, Gower).