Self-Correcting Exams: Making the Summative Formative Melanie Wong Dodge, Diane Ryan This paper was completed and submitted in partial fulfillment of the Master Teacher Program, a 2-year faculty professional development program conducted by the Center for Teaching Excellence, United States Military Academy, West Point, NY, 2009. Abstract As instructors of an introductory psychology course at the United States Military Academy (USMA), we are interested in helping our students understand, explain, and influence human behavior. Likewise, as developing leaders of character, it is paramount they discover the value of and strategies to becoming effective lifelong learners for themselves and as future leaders of adult professionals. We assume that our freshmen are psychologically ready to transition to and reap the benefits of an andragogical model of education. Our focus is on investigating the merits of one particular student-centered teaching and learning strategy: self-correcting exams. We are interested in how summative evaluations may serve as formative assessments if designed and deployed as such. Our exploratory research investigates whether self-correcting exams help students learn the subject matter better as measured by improved test scores as well as if the strategy helps develop enhanced self-regulated learning behaviors. Our preliminary findings did not support our hypothesis that students who self-corrected their exams performed better on subsequent testing than those who didn’t self-correct, however that may be due to the convenience sample utilized and other variables not controlled for in our pilot study. With respect to our second line of inquiry, in a follow-on survey, students reported overwhelmingly Self-Correcting Exams positive attitudes on the self-correcting activity and indicated improved self-regulated learning behaviors and insights. Further investigation is ongoing to examine and clarify what factors influence the development and reinforcement of mastery learning goals as well as of selfregulated learning behaviors. 2 Self-Correcting Exams 3 Two common goals of most educators are to help their students gain understanding about a specific subject and develop their students’ ability to learn. As instructors of a mandatory introductory psychology course at the United States Military Academy (USMA), we are particularly interested in helping our students understand, explain, and influence human behavior which will be critical to their future success as leaders. Likewise, it is paramount they discover the value of and strategies to becoming effective lifelong learners for themselves as well as to help facilitate organizational learning in their future units. Most freshmen enter college with over twelve years experience with the pedagogical model of education. We define this model as one in which the teacher has full responsibility for the learning, including making all the decisions about what, when, and how a subject will be learned as well as if it has been learned (Knowles, Holton III, & Swanson, 2005). One of our tasks is to move the needle to where the student assumes more responsibility for their own learning which is consistent with the andragogical model of education (Knowles et al., 2005). The first step is to create a climate that goes beyond just learning about specific subject areas to also encouraging greater metacognition. This awareness requires students to understand their own cognitive processes in learning as well as beliefs about them and the subject (Ellis Ormrod, 2008). As students develop an awareness and appreciation for metacognition, they may be interested in exploring more effective learning strategies. At this point, we can assist our students by introducing them to the benefits and components of self-regulated learning theory. For example, we can explain how self-efficacy influences self-regulation, metacognition, and Self-Correcting Exams 4 resilience, whereas different cognitive and self-regulated strategies may directly affect performance (Pintrich & De Groot, 1990). A particular strategy of interest in this area is students’ cognitions about test taking. Many students may view tests simply as summative evaluations designed to make a judgment about how much learning has occurred (Boston, 2002). However, tests can also be formative; that is, tests may also serve to improve learning not only provide a grade (Angelo & Cross, 1993; Jaffe, 2008). Moreover, while a cramming strategy may have been successful in high school, many students discover the technique ineffective in college for both mastery and performance goals. This new need is representative of freshmen’s transition from an adolescent learner to an adult learner. Here we begin to encounter a shift from subject-centered learning to a problemcentered orientation where students become motivated to learn in order to help them meet their self-set goals (Knowles et al., 2005). One promising teaching and learning strategy that offers summative evaluations formative outcomes is the self-correcting approach. This is because students may experience a conceptual change about tests and their performance as they experience a greater locus of control over their performance and take an active learning approach to evaluating and monitoring their progress (Montepare, 2005). To investigate the notion of whether or not self-correcting exams improve learning, we conducted a pilot study. Our first research question was whether self-correcting exams help students learn better. We hypothesized that students who self-corrected their interim exams Self-Correcting Exams 5 would perform better on their final exam than those who didn’t. Our second research question was whether self-correcting exams help facilitate self-regulated learning behaviors. Methods Participants For our pilot study, a convenience sample of predominately male, 17-24 year old freshman students enrolled in our introductory psychology class volunteered as participants. All but six our students volunteered for the experimental group (n=126). An equal number of students drawn randomly from other instructors comprised the control group. About 20% of the participants had a year of prep school and some had completed a high school AP psychology class, but these variables were not measured. Procedure Students enrolled in many introductory psychology courses are customarily offered extra credit opportunities, chiefly to serve as participants in student and faculty research projects. Our course policy allows students to earn up to thirty points, which generally depends on the time expended not effort, i.e., the equivalent of ten points for each hour spent. Our students were asked if they wanted to participate in the pilot or other extra credit opportunities throughout the semester and if so, they signed an informed consent. Nearly all of our students volunteered to participate in our study despite the fact that the extra credit points for our study were not assigned as a result of quantity of time expended, but rather the number of initially wrong answers that were successfully corrected which required effort. Self-Correcting Exams Participants were permitted to self-correct the first three interim exams. To execute the self-correcting exams, we scored all three exams without comments and returned them to our students within 2 to 4 days depending on the availability of the majority of the subject pool. To receive credit, students were required to specify why they chose the wrong answer, identify the correct answer, and provide a justification for their corrected choice. Students were allowed to use their homework (course guide) as a resource in their self-correction endeavors. Points awarded were based on the following guidelines: Points awarded: % Items Corrected 100 70 50 30 Points awarded 10 7 5 3 Table 1 Upon completion of the final self-correcting exam, students also completed a 17-item online survey to assess their learning and study behaviors. Measures Final exam. Students were given a cumulative final exam comprised of 75 multiple choice questions (two points each) and five essay questions (10 points each). The entire exam was worth 250 course points, however for the purposes of this study were converted to the standard 100 point scale. Three and a half hours were allotted to complete the exam. Online survey. Following the three self-correction opportunities, students were asked to complete a 17-item online survey designed to capture their subjective appraisals of the exercise (Appendix A). The first section asked the participants how many exams they had self corrected 6 Self-Correcting Exams and their assessment of the importance of factors that may have influenced their decision to partake in the endeavor, as well as their appraisal of their effort level. Section Two dealt with individual study habits and expectations of what the student hoped to gain from self-correcting. The final section requested feedback for improving future self-correcting opportunities. Students were given an incentive to complete the survey in lieu of a daily quiz worth five points. Results Research question 1. An independent samples t-test revealed no significant difference between the experimental and control groups on final exam results. Hence the hypothesis was not supported. Research question 2. Participants were asked to assess the importance of three major factors that influenced their decision to self-correct their exams (Figure 1). Almost 90 percent felt that the ability to improve their learning was an important factor with more than one-third considering it to be very important. However, the chance to earn bonus points was the highest rated factor with a nearly unanimous rating of very important. Finally, participants were asked how much the opportunity to influence their instructor’s opinion of them mattered. About three quarters considered this to be very to somewhat important, while 16% felt it was just slightly important. The remaining 14% reported that this factor did not influence their decision to participate at all. 7 Self-Correcting Exams 8 Figure 1. Factors Influencing Decision to SelfCorrect 100% 90% 23.1 34.2 80% 70% 60% 93.2 50% 40% 46.2 54.7 Very Important Somewhat Important Slightly Important Not at All Important 30% 16.2 20% 10% 10.3 0% Chance to Improve Learning 6 14.5 Chance to Earn Extra Instructor's Opinion of Credit Me Figure 1. Subjects were asked to estimate how much they believed self-corrections contributed to a discrete number of learning tasks and outcomes (Figure 2). Results were evenly divided, and participants overwhelmingly agreed that self-corrections significantly assisted them in preparation for exams, performance on exams, and the necessity to prepare homework. In each case, more than half of the participants strongly agreed that self-corrections had greatly aided their efforts in these endeavors. Self-Correcting Exams 9 Figure 2. To What Extent Did Self-Corrections Help You? 100% 90% 80% 70% 60.7 53.8 53 60% Greatly 50% Somewhat 40% Slightly 30% 32.5 35.9 5.1 1.7 10.3 3.4 9.4 1.7 Prepare for Exams Perform on Exams Increase Homework Effort 32.5 20% 10% 0% Not at All Figure 2. Discussion The results from the final exam scores did not support the hypothesis, yet, that may be due to the use of the convenience sample. On the other hand, the qualitative results support research question two and encourage further investigation. The participants reported overwhelmingly positive attitudes toward self-correction and indicated that they learned valuable study habits and insights as a result of their participation, e.g., that they should complete their homework as completely and accurately as possible during the semester and not just cram. And thought they stated that earning extra credit points was a major factor in self-selecting to participate, they had a choice to do traditional extra credit that guaranteed full points for time as opposed to this riskier option. Moreover, while they indicated that the opportunity to influence Self-Correcting Exams 10 their instructor’s opinion was a factor in participating, there was no tangible incentive in terms of earning instructor points towards their overall grade in the course. Further investigation is currently ongoing to pilot alternative measures and procedures. In the future we will employ a random sample of students from the entire course. Some variables such as prior academic experience may also be controlled for in follow-on studies. Moreover, collaboration and inquiry with other departments on their experience with similar strategies will be explored to further augment our self-correcting study to investigate if the activity can develop and enhance adult learning behavior and favorable mastery outcomes. Self-Correcting Exams Appendix A: Self-Correcting Exam Student Survey 11 Self-Correcting Exams 12 Self-Correcting Exams 13 Self-Correcting Exams 14 Self-Correcting Exams 15 Self-Correcting Exams 16 Self-Correcting Exams 17 Self-Correcting Exams 18 References Angelo, T. A., & Cross, K. P. (1993). Classroom assessment techniques (2nd ed.). San Francisco, CA, USA: Jossey-Bass. Boston, C. (2002, August 6). The concept of formative assessment. Retrieved 2008, from Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation: http://pareonline.net/getvn.asp?v=8&n=9 Ellis Ormrod, J. (2008). Educational Psychology (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River: Pearson. Jaffe, E. (2008, November). Will that be on the test? Observer , 21 (10), pp. 18-21. Knowles, M. S., Holton III, E. F., & Swanson, R. A. (2005). The adult learner: The definitive classic in adult education and human resource development (6th ed.). London, UK: Elsevier. Montepare, J. M. (2005, October). A self-correcting approach to multiple choice tests. Observer , 18 (10). Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology , 82 (1), 33-40.