Ex.lrimental BrainResearch Exp Brain Res (1988) 73:91-105 9 Springer-Verlag 1988 Eye movements induced by off-vertical axis rotation (OVAR) at small angles of tilt C. Darlot 1, P. Denise t, J. Droulez 1, B. Cohen 2, and A. Berthoz I 1 Laboratoire de PhysiologieNeurosensorielle du CNRS, 15, Rue de l'Ecole de M6decine, F-75270 Paris C6dex 06, France 2 Departments of Neurologyand Physiology,Mount Sinai School of Medicine, 1 East 100th Street, New York, NY 10029, USA Summary. Off-vertical rotation (OVAR) in darkness induced continuous horizontal nystagmus in humans at small tilts of the rotation axis (5 to 30 degrees). The horizontal slow eye velocity had two components: a mean velocity in the direction opposite to head rotation and a sinusoidal modulation around the mean. Mean velocity generally did not exceed 10 deg/s, and was less than or equal to the maximum velocity of optokinetic after-nystagmus (OKAN). Both the mean and modulation components of horizontal nystagmus increased with tilt angle and rotational velocity. Vertical slow eye velocity was also modulated sinusoidally, generally around zero. The amplitude of the vertical modulation increased with tilt angle, but not with rotational velocity. In addition to modulations in eye velocity, there were also modulations in horizontal and vertical eye positions. These would partially compensate for head position changes in the yaw and pitch planes during each cycle of OVAR. Modulations in vertical eye position were regular, increased with increases in tilt angle and were separated from eye velocity by 90 deg. These results are compatible with the interpretation that, during OVAR, mean slow velocity of horizontal nystagmus is produced by the velocity storage mechanism in the vestibular system. In addition, they indicate that the otolith organs induce compensatory eye position changes with regard to gravity for tilts in the pitch, yaw and probably also the roll planes. Such compensatory changes could be utilized to study the function of the otolith organs. A functional interpretation of these results is that nystagmus attempts to stabilize the image on the retina of one point of the surrounding world. Mean horizontal velocity would then be opposite to the estimate of head rotational velocity provided by the output of the velocity Offprint requests to: C. Darlot (address see above) storage mechanism, as charged by an otolithic input during OVAR. In spite of the lack of actual translation, an estimate of head translational velocity could, in this condition, be constructed from the otolithic signal. The modulation in horizontal eye position would then be compensatory for the perceived head translation. Modulation of vertical eye velocity would compensate for actual changes in head orientation with respect to gravity. Key words: Otolith - Nystagmus - Labyrinth - Eye movements - Velocity storage mechanism Introduction Rotation of the head at constant velocity about a vertical axis in darkness induces nystagmus and a sense of motion. These disappear within 40-60 s. However, if the axis of rotation is tilted with respect to gravity (Off-Vertical Axis Rotation, OVAR), nystagmus and the feeling of body movement last as long as rotation continues (Guedry 1965; Benson and Bodin 1966). Neuronal activity responsible for the continuous response arises primarily from excitation of the otolith organs by a component of gravity orthogonal to the rotation axis (Guedry 1965; Correia and Money 1970; Stockwell et al. 1970; Raphan et al. 1981; Goldberg and Fernfindez 1981, 1982; Cohen et al. 1983; see Raphan and Cohen 1985 for review). The gravitY vector can be described in a stereotaxic set of orthogonal axes (Fig. 1). The X axis is the intersection of sagittal and horizontal planes; the line of sight coincides with this axis when the subject looks ahead. The Y axis is the interaural axis at the intersection of the horizontal and frontal planes, and 92 Fig. 1. Representation of the gravity vector in stereotaxic axes. The origin of the three axes X, Y and Z is located between the two labyrinths, at the intersectionof the frontal, sagittal and horizontal planes. The gravity vector G is decomposedinto its components along the axes: Gx, Gy and Gz. During OVAR, this latter component does not vary with time, while the first two ones vary sinusoidallyand in quadrature. Their sum, the gravitycomponent GC, is fixedin space. When the head rotates about the Z axistilted with respect to gravity (the rounded arrow indicates a clockwise rotation), GC rotates, in head coordinates, in the opposite direction (counterclockwise) the Z axis is the intersection of frontal and sagittal planes. To describe geometrically the otolithic stimulation due to O V A R , it is convenient to decompose the gravity vector into three components, each along one stereotaxic axis. The excitation level of each sensory cell in the maculae of the utricle and saccule is proportional to the scalar product of its polarization vector and linear acceleration. The polarization vectors of most utricular cells are contained within the horizontal stereotaxic plane and those of the saccular cells within the sagittal plane (Goldberg and Fernandez 1982). During O V A R , subjects are rotated around the Z axis. Therefore, the component of gravity along this axis does not vary with time, and thus does not induce any otolithic stimulation. The gravity components along the X and Y axes vary sinusoidaUy and in quadrature during each turn. The extremes of the X axis component occur when the head is in nose-down and nose-up positions, and those of the Y axis component when the head is laterally tilted. The sum of the components along X and Z axes lies within the sagittal plane. This vector sweeps the maculae, alternately rotating forward and backward during each half of one cycle. It particularly stimulates the saccular maculae and probably elicits vertical eye position changes and vertical nystagmus. The sum of the components along Y and Z axes lies within the frontal plane. This vector also alternately rotates but sideways, and probably induces ocular counter-rolling (Markham and Diamond 1983, 1985). The sum of the components along the X and Y axes is a vector located in the horizontal stereotaxic plane, orthogonal to the rotation axis. Its magnitude is proportional to the sine of the tilt angle of this axis. This vector is fixed in space, but in head coordinates it rotates at a velocity opposite to that of the head in space. This is an equivalent way of saying that the components along the two axes vary sinusoidally and in quadrature. This "rotating vector" sequentially excites the sensory cells of the maculae according to the orientation of their polarization vector (Guedry 1966; Raphan et al. 1981a). The sequential activation enables the Central Nervous System (CNS) to elaborate an estimate of rotational head velocity to elaborate an estimate of rotational head velocity (Raphan and Schnalbok 1988). It is postulated that this estimate is sent to the Velocity Storage Mechanism (VSM) to activate neurons in the vestibulo-ocular pathway (Raphan et al. 1981a; see Raphan and Cohen 1985 for review). The resultant horizontal slow phase eye velocity has two components (Guedry 1965; Benson and Bodin 1966; Harris 1987): 1. A mean eye velocity, which is compensatory to the direction of head rotation. This velocity is probably the output of the velocity storage mechanism (Cohen et al. 1983). 2. A cyclic modulation in velocity at the frequency of the head rotation, added to the mean velocity. Several authors have described horizontal nystagmus and perceptual effects induced by O V A R at 60-90 degrees of axis tilt and various head velocities (Guedry 1965; Benson and Bodin 1966; Correia and Guedry 1966; Stockwell et al. 1970; Young and Henn 1975; see Guedry 1974 and Benson 1974 for review). Both components of horizontal eye velocity increase with tilt of the axis of rotation and with head velocity. The maximum of the modulation occurs about 30 degrees in advance of the nose-up position (Stockwell et al. 1970). However, relatively little information is available about the nystagmus induced by O V A R at small angles of tilt of the axis of rotation, especially in man. The present article reports observations and measurements of eye movements induced by O V A R at tilt angle up to 30 degrees. 93 OFF-VERTICAL AXIS ROTATION CW 45 Deg/s Tilt=15Degrees Horizontaleyevelocity Right o Deg/s o H ~ ~ ~i"~ Left Verticaleyevelocity/~ r ~,~P ~'~ ~r ~ q ~t' f~t / ~ ~ VerticaleyePosition Up Deg Down [~ Deg/s io a Dog (~ / ~) 1 Nosedown Noseup ~ ! ~ I 0 9'0 180 2"~0 3t~0Degrees 2 z~ ~ 8 Seconds Nosedown Noseup Nosedown 0 An accompanying article describes motion sensations experienced by the subjects (Denise et al. 1988). Methods Subjects. Results were obtained from 27 subjects, from 21 to 45 years old. They had no history of auditory or vestibular disorder, and they had neither spontaneous nystagmus nor a Romberg sign in darkness. The integrity of the horizontal canals was checked by post-rotatory nystagmus. Five subjects became nauseated so quickly that their oculomotor responses could not be measured. Nevertheless, their sensation of body motion was similar to that of other subjects (see Denise et al. 1988). Recording technique. Horizontal and vertical eye movements were recorded with DC electrooculography (EOG). Care was taken to avoid cross-coupling between the horizontal and vertical electrodes by placing them at right angles to each other. Subjects were adapted for 15 min in darkness before their eye movements were calibrated in dim red light. Pretesting. Per-rotatory nystagmus was induced by a step increase in head velocity from 0 to 45 deg/s. Maximum accelerations and decelerations at the onset and end of rotation were approximately 500 deg/sz. The dim red lights were then switched on, and OKN Fig. 2. Nystagmic pattern during OVAR. This nystagmus was induced by clockwise OVAR at 45~ about an axis tilted 15 degrees from the vertical. The traces are, from top to bottom: horizontal eye velocity, horizontal eye position, vertical eye velocity, vertical eye position, and head position about the yaw (Z) axis. Eye movements to the fight and up, and clockwise (rightward) head rotation, caused upward trace deflections. Head position was measured from chair position, by a potentiometer in a bridge circuit that resets after each turn. The sudden drop in this trace coincides with origin of phases (nose-down position). The position of the subject at various times during the rotational cycle is shown diagrammatically at bottom left, and the correspondence between the phase of rotation and time at bottom right was measured during two minutes of constant velocity rotation. The lights were switched off again, and at the end of the test, rotation was stopped, and post-rotatory nystagmus was recorded in darkness. Gains and time constants of these responses were within normal limits in all subjects. Stimulation. OVAR nystagmus was induced by rotating the subjects, in darkness, at constant rotational velocity, around an axis tilted with respect to gravity. The apparatus was a rotating chair driven by a torque motor (Toennies) on a platform that could be tilted by hydraulic jacks placed at one side. Tilting from zero to 30 degrees or vice versa was achieved in about 10 s. To avoid any influence of canal stimulation, two minutes were allowed to elapse after every change in tilt angle or rotational velocity, to let canal activity subside. Then nystagmus was recorded for two to five minutes, depending on the velocity. There was a slight sinusoidal modulation of rotational velocity~ but its amplitude did not exceed 0.1 deg/s at 45 deg/s and 30 degrees of tilt. The corresponding acceleration is below canal threshold; thus eye movements resulted only from otolith stimulation. Subjects were instructed to maintain their heads stationary upright, keep their eyes open in darkness, look straight ahead at an imaginary horizon, and not to try to follow imaginary points or noises in the room. Experimenters kept silent except when asking subjects to describe their feelings. The experiment was terminated whenever the subjects wished. Six angles of tilt were used: 5, 10, 15, 20, 25 and 30 degrees. The tilt angle was varied randomly in three subjects. This led to 94 Subject 3 HORIZONTAL Deg. 10- 45~ Velocity modulation Deg/s Right 10 Position Right ~ . 25 ~ 9 ~ ~176149 9 9149 5 l_e :, 0 :::t Left . :. % r t I 1 I I I' I I I 0 90 180 270 360 0 90 180 270 360 VERTICAL Deg/s 5 Up Deg. 10 Up 9 o9 .." . ; ; Down 0 ; 9t0 o9 I 180 99 I 270 -5 I 360 Down ~ 0 9t0 r 180 I 270 i 360 Phase in degrees. Fig. 3. Results of record processing. Cyclic modulations of horizontal (upper row) and vertical (lower row) eye position (left column) and velocity (right column) are shown during OVAR at 45~ and 25 degrees of axis tilt. 20 cycles were sampled and averaged, with 120 points per cycle. Each point represents the average value of eye position or velocity during the time required for the head to rotate by 3 degrees. The origin of the phase plots is the nose-down position. The phase scale is drawn at the bottom line. Sinusoidal curve fitted to each set of points is drawn with a solid line, and mean value of velocity with an interrupted line early development of nausea. Consequently, in the other subjects the angle of tilt was progressively increased. All subjects were tested at the standard velocity of 45 deg/s. Nine were successively rotated at 30, 45 and 60 deg/s, at one or several tilt angles from 5 to 20 degrees. Three subjects were also rotated at 90, 120 and 140 deg/s at 10 degrees of tilt. Sixteen subjects were rotated in only one direction. The other six subjects had successive clockwise and counterclockwise rotation. The results were not different in the two groups. Most subjects asked that the experiment be terminated before completion of all velocities and tilt angles. This was frequently not because of nausea, but because the whole experiment lasted for an hour and a half. Consequently, sets of measurements were satisfactorily large only for tilt angles from 5 to 20 degrees, and conclusions on effects of larger tilt angles are based on fewer observations. All subjects leaned their head on a concave head-rest, in similar positions with regard to the rotational axis. Thus, otolithic 95 A Deg/s 10 Subject 1 Mean velocity DegYs 10 8 HORIZONTAL Velocity modulation Analysis of eye movements. Signals representing horizontal and Position modulation [3eg 6 9 6 6 4 4 2 2 ~'0 3'0 1; i; ~legYs Deg/s 10 8 8 6 6 4 4 2'o o ~'o 1'o 2'~ 3'o O VERTICAL peg lo [] a 0 [] 6 [] 0 4 <> [3 [] 2 [] [] o 2 2 O [] 0 [ 2'0 10 6' 3'o 1'0 1'o 2'0 3'o 2'0 a'o A n g l e of tilt in d e g r e e s B Dog/s 10 Subject 2 Mean velocity Deg/s l 8 4 6 3 4 2 2 1 1'o a'o 210 o Deg/s 5 Deg/s 5 HORIZONTAL Velocity modulation Position modulation Deg i Results 9 9 1'o 2'o 0 ~'o VERTICAL 1'0 2'0 do 2'o 3'o Deg [] 4 4 3 3 2 2 o 1 ,o 10 o i o 21o 310 vertical eye position, chair position and velocity, sensation of body acceleration and velocity or position felt by the subject (see accompanying article) were displayed on an oscillograph and simultaneously recorded by a computer (HP 1000) for off-line processing. Eye position was digitally differentiated to obtain eye velocity. Saccades and quick phases were identified and eliminated from the record. Each cycle was divided into 120 bins, each 3 degrees wide, and 20 cycles were averaged. Position and velocity data were fitted with least square sinusoids. An example is shown in Fig. 3. From this, mean horizontal and vertical eye position and velocity, together with amplitude of their modulations (half of peak to peak modulation) and the phase with respect to chair position were determined. The nose-down position was taken as the origin of the phase plots (Stockwell et al. 1970). Estimates of the means and amplitudes of modulation around the mean were not independent, so that a sudden change of one could reduce the accuracy of measurement of the other. Results obtained by computer processing were compared to measurements made by hand on the paper records. There were only minimal differences. Phase measurements were accurate to no more than + 25 degrees, due to the inherent irregularity in the eye movement patterns. This was determined by recording the same subject several times on the same and on different days. [] 0 [] 110 O [3 0 210 3tO o ~'0 A n g l e of tilt in d e g r e e s Fig. 4A, B. Effects of tilt angle on nystgmus. Abscissa: axis tilt angle in degrees. Ordinates, from left to right: mean value (first column, circles), modulation amplitude of slow phase eye velocity (second column, squares), and modulation amplitude of eye position (third column, diamonds) of horizontal (upper row, filled symbols) and vertical (lower row, empty symbols) eye movements. Results are shown for two subjects. Subject 2 is the same as in Fig. 3, but the record was made on another day Horizontal and vertical nystagmus were induced by OVAR (Fig. 2). It was necessary to keep the eyes open in darkness to maintain the nystagmus. When they were closed, the nystagmus became altered and soon disappeared. There was considerable variation in the responses, both among and between individuals. This made it difficult to tabulate group averages. Among the 22 subjects studied, 15 had regular eye movements with similar characteristics. Sample values from this group are shown in Fig. 4 and pooled values are presented in Fig. 5. The other 7 subjects had irregular movements, with sudden changes unrelated to stimulus, that were analyzed separately. Eye velocity Horizontal eye velocity stimulation should not have been different from subject to subject. Slight transient head tilts could have occurred, but the resulting canal stimulation would have been weak and could not have modified the low frequency ocular responses. In two subjects the head was fixed to the head rest with a strip of cloth. This soon became painful and was abandoned. In two other subjects the head was stabilized with a bite-board. The results in these four subjects were similar to those observed in subjects whose heads were simply leaning on the head-rest. The chair was a bucket seat with arm and foot rests and a security harness. Since important tactile cues were felt during OVAR, for two subjects the seat was fitted with a depression mattress that was firmly molded to the body. This did not change the results. Horizontal slow phase velocity had two components (Figs. 2 and 3), a sinusoidal modulation at the frequency of rotation, superimposed on a mean eye velocity. This was considered compensatory in that it was in a direction opposite to head rotation. Both components generally ranged up to 8 deg/s. However, mean velocities up to 20 deg/s and amplitudes of modulation of 25 deg/s were encountered occasionally. Effect of tilt of the rotation axis on horizontal eye velocity. Weak nystagmic movements could be distin- 96 HORIZONTAL Meanvelocity Velocity modulation 8I 68 I Deg/s 4 0 PHASE1 6 ~'X ~ Rotation 6 4 TTT l 4 t 2 ll0 ~ 210 i 180 NU 270/ LED~ ~ 9 0 Deg/s Position modulation = 20 I 8 0 Phasebead ND T 110 I 210 I Deg/sVERTICAL , T 648 I Deg 1 RED2 T 0 l Ti i 1~0 2~0 i Angleof tilt indegrees T * 0 i 1~0 i 2t0 Fig. 5. Summary of the results on 15 subjects with regular eye movements. Same presentation as in Fig. 4, except that the polar plot on the bottom left, is similar to that of Fig. 5. Each point in the diagram stands for the average of the measurements made on the 15 subjects. Standard deviations are represented by vertical segments. The polar plot is divided into sectors identified by numbers. Each sector represents the range of the maximum of the modulation of one nystagmic variable. 1: Vertical upwardmost eye position - 2: Horizontal compensatory eye position - 3: Vertical upwardmost eye velocity - 4: Horizontal compensatory eye velocity. The compensatory direction is toward the left in this figure, as rotation has been conventionally figured clockwise guished from baseline for tilts of the axis of rotation of 5 degrees and above. The relationship between eye velocity and tilt angle is illustrated in Fig. 4 for 2 subjects, and in Fig. 5 for the 15 subjects with regular eye movements. In most subjects, mean velocity and modulation amplitude increased with tilt angle. However, for the group as a whole, mean velocity varied around 3 deg/s for tilts of the axis of rotation up to 20 degrees, and increased thereafter. This is due to the large spread of the responses. In some subjects, at a given tilt angle, mean eye velocity diminished with time, and was sometimes smaller at the end of the experiment than at the beginning, even if the tilt angle was greater. Exceptionally, amplitude of eye velocity modulation was also reduced, as the tilt angle was increased. Generally, malaise or a drop of attention occurred simultaneously. Effects of rotation velocity. For all subjects, both components of horizontal eye velocity increased with rotation velocity. At a tilt angle of 10 degrees, in the 10 subjects with regular eye movements that were tested at several velocities, average values of mean velocity and velocity modulation increased respectively from 2 to 8 deg/s and from 2 to 6 deg/s when head rotational velocity increased from 30 to 60 deg/s. In the three subjects tested at higher velocities, mean eye velocity showed sudden variations within the range 4-8 deg/s, and decreased when rotation velocity was further increased. In the two subjects that were rotated at 120 and 140 dens, mean eye velocity decreased to 0 after about two minutes, and only a cyclic modulation of slow phase eye velocity remained. For one of these subjects, whose eye movements were especially fast, amplitude of modulation increased linearly from 5 to 20 deg/s when rotational velocity was increased from 30 to 140 deg/s (R-sq = 0.8). Vertical eye velocity OVAR also induced a cyclical modulation of vertical eye velocity (Figs. 2 and 3). The amplitude of modulation varied from 0 to 25 deg/s, depending on the subject and the tilt angle, but most often ranged between 0 and 12 deg/s. For the 15 subjects with regular eye movements, the average amplitude of modulation of vertical eye velocity increased proportionally with tilt angle, from 2.3 deg/s at 10 degrees to 7.4 deg/s at 25 degrees of tilt. This held true both for individual subjects (Fig. 4) and for the group as a whole (Fig. 5, R-sq = 0.9). The mean vertical eye velocity was equal to zero in most subjects. In some individuals it could be directed either upward or downward. It never exceeded 4 deg/s, and was the same at all tilt angles and velocities for the same subject on the same day. In one subject with downward beating nystagmus, mean vertical slow eye velocity increased from 3 to 16 deg/s when the velocity of rotation was increased from 30 to 120 deg/s. The amplitude of modulation of the velocity did not change, however. Another subject, recorded twice at an interval of several months, had a downward beating nystagmus on one occasion and upward beating nystagmus the second time. On both occasions, mean horizontal eye velocity was opposite to head rotation. Slow phase eye velocity and nausea Subjects suffering from nausea often made smaller and slower eye movements than those without symptoms. Mean eye velocity was strongly reduced. The modulation in eye velocity was usually, but not necessarily, reduced. One subject, who performed the whole experiment, suffered from malaise from 97 OFF-VERTICAL AXIS ROTATION CW 45 Deg/s 41' Position left In Velocity left Subject.1 Nose up 180 () Position up [ ] Velocity up Subject 2 Nose up 180 210 7 Fo / 150 .I 25 20 9^ \o.oo oeo,, Deg or Deg/s 300 \ I , 25 25\ j 6o o2s .ot~t~o~~~ ~ o Nose down Degrees Nose down ~Ph~selead Degrees Fig. 6. Polar plots showing the phases and modulation amplitudes of the oculomotor responses represented in Fig. 4, when axis tilt angle was increased, at 45~ of clockwise head rotational velocity. Same symbols as in Fig. 4: squares for velocity and diamonds for position; open symbols for horizontal movements, filled symbols for vertical movements. Values of axis tilt angles are written in small numbers beside the points. The scale in degrees or degrees per second is drawn on the radius. Rotation progressed from nose-down position (0 deg) to left ear down position (270 deg) and so on, as indicated by the thin rounded arrow below 330 degrees. Phase leads are counted positively in the trigonometric (counterclockwise) direction, as indicated by the thin rounded arrow below 30 degrees. Each point is the tip of a vector whose origin is the center of the diagram, whose magnitude is proportional to the amplitude of modulation of the variable, and whose phase is that of the maximum of the variable. Subject 1 had regular and Subject 2 irregular eye movements. Note that vertical position and velocity (open symbols) were about 90 degrees apart, but that the phase angle between horizontal position and velocity (filled symbols) was more variable, but always greater than 90 degrees the beginning. He had no modulation in eye velocity, but nystagmus with mean velocities between 2.3 and 5.3 deg/s was present for rotational velocities between 30 and 90 deg/s. At 120 deg/s, a modulation in eye velocity of 24 deg/s suddenly appeared. At that time the mean eye velocity was 46 deg/s. He was considered to be in the irregular group. Eye position changes Absolute eye position could not be accurately measured, particularly in the vertical direction, due to slow drift of the E O G signal. However, :there were clear shifts of average eye position in the direction of the slow phases of horizontal nystagmus, as welt as shifts of vertical eye position (Figs. 2 and 3). The amplitude of the cyclical shift in horizontal eye position could vary from 0 to 12 degrees at any tilt angle, depending on the subject (Figs. 2 and 3). In individual subjects, amplitude of eye position modulation increased with tilt angle (Fig. 4B). Group averages were about 2 degrees for tilts of the rotational axis of 5 degrees, increasing to about 4 degrees for tilts of 25 degrees (Fig. 5). Changes in the beating field of nystagmus were particularly large in two subjects, varying up to 16 degrees. The relationship between head velocity and change in the beating field varied from one subject to another, but remained regular for each subject. The amplitude of the shift in vertical eye position ranged from 0 to 16 degrees at any tilt angle, with considerable intersubject variability (Figs. 2 and 3). For the subjects shown in Fig. 4, it increased monotonically with tilt angle. For the group as whole, there was also an increase in the amplitude of the modulation in eye position, going from 1 degree at 5 degrees of tilt of the rotation axis to 4.5 degrees at 25 degrees of tilt (Fig. 5). When rotation velocity was increased, whatever the initial value and at any tilt angle, the cyclical shifts in horizontal and vertical position increased by about 20% during the next 10 s. Then, they slowly dropped back to the previous steady state value, which was stable for each subject. A change in head rotation velocity, therefore, had no long-lasting effect on eye position, which was a gravity-dependent response. 98 Subject2 ~ ~ N=6 45~'s Amplitudeof saccades L l N=IO 45~ Amplitudeof saccades 1(~ig15. ~ 0 90 180 270 360 [- ' - 7 ~ ~ ] ~ N u mofbsctccades er - ~ ] 010 0 Subject 3 90 180 270 Cumulativepositionshift 0 560 go 180 , 270 Numberof saccades 360 go 360 180 i 270 100 g eg 0 90 180 Phase in degrees 270 560 ~ 0 90 180 I ~ 270 J 360 Phase in degrees Fig. 7. Average distributionof horizontal components of nystagmicfast phases within a cycle,for two subjects. Abscissa: Phase, from 0 to 360 degrees. Ordinates, from top to bottom: - Amplitudesof saccades, each vertical segmentrepresentingthe amplitude of one saccade, at its phase of occurrencewithinthe cycle- Number of saccades. Average number of saccadesoccurringin each time interval during whichthe head has rotated by 30 degrees- Cumulativeeye position. Sum of the amplitudesof the saccades occurringduring each time interval. For each subject, peaks in beating-frequency, amplitude and velocity of horizontal components of nystagmic fast phases occurred simultaneouslywith maximumslow eye velocity.Subject 2 was representative of the most commonlyobserved pattern. Fast phases were unevenlydistributed in all subjects. Averages of 6 cyclesfor Subject 2 and 10 cyclesfor Subject 3, are shown on these graphs Phase relations Phases of eye velocity and eye position modulations in horizontal and vertical directions are plotted in Fig. 6, in polar coordinates, for two subjects rotated clockwise at 45 deg/s. Each point on the diagram represents the maximum positive value of a sinusoid fitted to horizontal or vertical eye velocity or position. The number beside the point shows the tilt angle of the axis of rotation. Upward and leftward movements (i.e., in the direction opposite to head rotation) are denoted as positive values. The distance from the center of the diagram represents the amplitude of the response. Its angular position with respect to the origin represents the phase with respect to the nose-down position (Stockwell et al. 1970). Phase angles were incremented positively in the trigonometric (counterclockwise) direction. For instance, a point whose phase is 60 deg represents a response whose positive maximum leads the nosedown position by 60 deg. The polar diagram at the bottom left of Fig. 5 shows the range of the phases of velocity and position for the group with regular eye movements. The phase of the maximum compensatory horizontal eye velocity, i.e., in the direction opposite to that of head rotation, ranged between 150 and 260 deg, depending on the subject (Fig. 6). Most commonly, it ranged within 180-240 deg and was about 210 deg for subjects with regular eye movements (Fig. 5). All subjects did not behave in the same way. There were large variations of phase, that increased with increasing tilt angles in many subjects with irregular eye movements (e.g. Fig. 6, Subject 2). The phase of maximum compensatory horizontal eye position, in the direction opposite to lateral head tilt, was regularly about 70 deg in advance of the nose-down position (range 60-100 deg) in subjects with regular eye movements (Fig. 6, Subject 1). Occasionally, other subjects showed phases out of this range (Fig. 6, Subject 2). The modulation in horizontal eye position was measured for the 3 subjects rotated at 30, 45, 60 and 90 deg/s at 10 degrees of tilt. At each rotation velocity, the amplitude was multiplied by the pulsation. This gave the amplitudes of the derivative of the position modulation. These were always smaller than the amplitude of modulation in velocity. This difference must be due to the variation in beating frequency and size of the saccades during each cycle. In contrast, the maxima of the modulations in upward eye velocity and eye position were tightly phase-locked. For all subjects, at any tilt angle, the 99 phase of the maximum upward eye velocity occurred regularly about 90 deg in advance of the nose-down position (range 80-110 deg). Moreover, the phase of the upwardmost position remained around zero deg, never leading the nose-down position by more than 10 deg nor lagging behind it by more than 30 deg. Vertical eye position shifts during O V A R were, therefore, compensatory for head tilt in the pitch plane. An estimate of the amplitude of the derivative of the modulation in vertical position was calculated and compared to the modulation in velocity. The actual modulation was greater than the estimated derivative. Distribution of the fast phases of nystagmus Fast phases were not evenly distributed throughout the cycle. As illustrated for two subjects in Fig. 7, the amplitude and number of saccades and the cumulative shift in eye position tended to occur at the same phase angle as the maximum slow eye velocity. As slow eye velocity increased with tilt angle or rotation velocity, these values also increased. Discussion The continuous nystagmus induced by yaw rotation about axes tilted at small angles from the vertical, consisted of changes in horizontal and vertical eye position and velocity, with broad variability between and among subjects. The different variables will be successively considered. Mean slow phase eye. velocity Mean horizontal slow phase eye velocities elicited by OVAR were invariably compensatory, i.e. in the direction opposite to that of head rotation. In most subjects, the threshold for the appearance of a nystagmic response was at a tilt angle between 10 and 15 degrees (Fig. 4), although it can be lower, at about 5 degrees for some individuals. The latency of appearance of the response could not be determined because of the slow dynamics of the hydraulic jacks that moved the axis of rotation. In the monkey it is about one second (Raphan et al. 1981). In monkeys, mean horizontal velocities of 40-50 deg/s are commonly achieved during O V A R at 60 deg/s and 30 degrees of tilt (Raphan et al. 1981). In contrast, mean compensatory velocities reached by our subjects never exceeded 20 deg/s and generally ranged between 0 and 8 deg/s at a rotational velocity of 45 deg/s (Fig. 5). Similar results were obtained by Stockwell et al. (1970) and Harris and Barnes (1986) during O V A R at higher tilt angles, and by Benson and Barnes (1973) during counterrotation: Thus, in humans, the gain of the eye velocity that compensates for head velocity during OVAR seems to never exceed 0.2 and be often lower. The interpretation that, velocity storage mechanism produces horizontal mean eye velocity, during OVAR, is supported by the observation that this velocity was always equal to or less than the peak velocity of OKAN induced by OKN at the same rotation velocity. VSM saturates at 10-15 deg/s in most humans (Cohen et al. 1981; Jell et al. 1985); since mean eye velocity varies in the range 2-8 deg/s, its measurement during O V A R could give an adequate index of functioning of VSM. A tilt angle of no more than 15 degrees would be enough for this test, as Young and Henn (1975) showed that mean eye velocity tended to saturate at tilt angles greater than 30 degrees. The magnitude of the rotating vector, which is the driving force activating the otolith organs, increases proportionally to the sine of the tilt angle. Therefore, a likely possibility would be that the components of horizontal eye velocity would follow the same rule. For the tilt angles that were used, the values of the angle in radians and its sine are very dose. As a result, the increase in mean velocity should have been close to linear. Despite a large spread, this was the case in many subjects. In other subjects, the slope of the increase was greater at 10 than at 30 degrees of tilt angle. This could be due to the decrease of VSM gain with increases in tilt angle (Raphan and Cohen 1985). During barbecue rotation, horizontal mean eye velocity increased when rotation velocity was increased from 0 to 100 deg/s, and then decreased to 0 when rotation velocity was further increased to 180 deg/s (Correia and Guedry 1966; Guedry 1970). During OVAR, as Stockwell et al. (1970) observed, mean velocity decreased to zero after two minutes of rotation at high velocities (120 and 140 deg/s). Thus, a neural circuit behaving as a low-pass filter should exist in the otolith-ocular pathway (Mayne 1974). Raphan and Schnalbok (1988) have proposed a mechanism for estimation of velocity during OVAR. They postulate that the CNS calculates a velocity by comparing a pattern of input from the otolith organs with a previous pattern. Their model outputs an estimate of velocity that falls to zero at high speeds of rotation. In addition, high rotational velocities shorten the dominant time constant of the velocity storage mechanism (Raphan et al. 1979). Thus, there could 100 either be a reduction in the ability to estimate the velocity of head rotation and a drop in the signal driving the VSM, or a reduction in the saturation velocity and output of the VSM itself, which, in turn, would cause lower mean slow phase velocities. Vertical mean eye velocity (i.e. mean velocity in the sagittal plane) was absent or very small during O V A R in humans and in monkeys (Raphan et al. 1981). This is consistent with the finding of Harris and Barnes (1986). Although velocity storage in the sagittal plane is present during forward-downward head movements in the cat (Darlot et al. 1981) and monkey (Matsuo and Cohen 1984). It supports the interpretation that, during O V A R , storage would require a sustained sweeping of the maculae by an acceleration vector rotating for at least one minute in the same direction. Indeed, polarization vectors of most saccular sensory ceils are aligned with the sagittal plane (Fernfindez et ai. 1972; Fernfindez and Goldberg 1976b; Goldberg and Fernfindez 1982). The vector which stimulates these cells is, therefore, the sum of the constant vector along the Z axis and the component along the X axis. It is this latter component which actually changes with time, and stimulates the sensory cells. It sweeps the saccular maculae from forward to backward during a half cycle (4 s at a rotation velocity of 45 deg/s, and in the reverse direction during the other half cycle. In terms of system analysis, assuming that a vertical VSM exists; having similar dynamics as horizontal VSM, it should behave as a first order low-pass filter, with a time constant of 15 s, its corner frequency should be 0.067 Hz. The neural activity induced in the sensory cells by a rotation at 45 deg/s corresponding to a frequency of 0.125 Hz, would therefore be completely filtered out, so that the output of the VSM would be zero. In contrast, Harris and Barnes (1986), recording with a search coil, observed in humans and cats a small vertical mean velocity in the upward direction, which might reflect the functioning of the direct pathway, by-passing the VSM, to the ocular motoneurons. Indeed, although a small vertical mean velocity recorded by E O G could just be the result of some minute artefactual coupling between horizontal and vertical traces, this should not occur with a search coil. Modulation in eye velocity and eye position Amplitudes of horizontal eye velocity modulation measured in this study were comparable to those observed during O V A R by Stockwell et al. (1970), and slightly greater than those observed by Harris and Barnes (1986). They are consistent with the observations of Benson and Barnes (1973) during counter-rotation, Correia and Guedry (1966) and Guedry (1970) during barbecue rotation, and Buizza et al. (1980) during lateral oscillations. They were twice greater than most L-nystagmus amplitudes (Young 1967, 1972). Maxima in horizontal slow eye velocity in the direction opposite to head rotation occurred in most subjects 30 deg in advance of the nose-up position. This phase angle matches with previous measurements (Stockwell et al. 1970), and also coincides with the phase of perception that the force vector is acting backward in the sagittal plane during O V A R (Benson et ah 1975) and counter-rotation (Benson and Barnes 1973). Velocity storage does not appear to be involved in the modulation of horizontal eye velocity during OVAR. Indeed, modulations persisted at high velocities of rotation, when mean eye velocity had dropped to zero. This is consistent with the observation by Niven et al. (1966) that during sinusoidal translational oscillations along the Y axis, the decay of the nystagmus was immediate after removal of the stimulus. It is neither likely that velocity storage be involved in producing modulations in vertical eye velocity. For rotation at 45 deg/s and 30 degrees of tilt, the peak angular velocity in the sagittal plane to which our subjects were submitted was 23.6 deg/s. There was little or no mean velocity, but the average modulation amplitude in vertical velocity was about 8 deg/s, or a gain of 0.34. This modulation did not change when rotation velocity was increased, indicating that the two were independent. The amplitude of vertical eye velocity modulation was found to be smaller by Harris and Barnes, in man: the gain was 0.06 at a rotation velocity of 60 deg/s and 30 degrees of tilt angle. The question arises whether the otolithic signals which produce eye velocity modulations are related to head position or to head velocity. During O V A R at 60, 45 and 30 deg/s, head position rotates with regard to gravity each 6, 9 or 12 s at frequencies of 0.17, 0.11 or 0.08 Hz, respectively. The output of the otolith organs would be mainly related to head position at these frequencies (Fernfindez and Goldberg 1976a-c). The vertical eye position changes were surprisingly regular, and compensated for changes in head position in the pitch plane. As their amplitude was dependent on the angle of tilt of the axis of rotation, but not on rotational velocity, they were probably driven by an eye position signal. The change in horizontal eye position was more complex. During rotation about a vertical axis, in the absence of an otolith induced change in eye position, 101 the beating field of nystagmus depends on the distribution of saccade frequency and amplitude, which, in turn, is prominently controlled by slow phase eye velocity (Chun and Robinson 1978; Guitton and Voile 1987). During OVAR, on the other hand, the modulation in slow phase velocity was not the derivative of the modulation of position (Fig. 5). A double control in velocity and position could therefore exist, and the adjustment be made by saccade amplitude and distribution. Positional changes in the yaw and pitch plane that compensate for head position with regard to gravity are well known in lateral eyed animals, but have not been noted before in humans to our knowledge. During OVAR, in the absence of vision or canal input, they could arise either from otolithic or somatosensory informations. There is little information about somatosensory effect on eye position in humans, but in the monkey, the weak eye position changes induced by tilt with respect to gravity are mainly due to otolithic inputs (Krejcova et al. 1971). Therefore, in humans, the horizontal and vertical positional changes induced by O V A R probably also arise mainly from the otolith organs. Ocular counter-rolling is not registered by EOG, and therefore was not measured in this study. Diamond and Markham (1983) showed that counterrolling is the same whatever way the ear-down position is reached, either by tilting from the upright to the side-down position, or by rotating, as during OVAR. In the present experiment it would be expected that tilts of the rotation axis would have induced 4 to 8 degrees of counter-rolling. By analogy with vertical eye position changes, counter-rolling would be independent of rotational velocity. Thus, compensatory eye position shifts are probably present during OVAR, with the eyes moving up and down, side to side and counter-rolling, as the head moves through each cycle of rotation. During ocular counter-rolling, the e y e rolls around the line of sight. Horizontal and vertical EOG traces could therefore have been affected by counter-rolling when the line of sight did not coincide with the X axis. Whether this actually occurred is not known, but contamination of E O G traces by counter-rolling could perhaps explain the difference in amplitude of modulation and in the phases between our results and those of Harris (1985) obtained with search coils. Causes of variation Several factors could be responsible for the variability of the obtained results. First, sensations of discomfort were induced in many subjects, even at small angles of tilt, after 10 to 20 min of OVAR. Malaise and a loss of attention could have affected slow phase velocities. Since testing proceeded from lower to higher velocities of rotation in most subjects, this could have reduced the mean and modulation values of the responses at higher stimulus velocities. Moreover, when the rotation axis is vertical, exposure to passive rotation reduces the dominant time constant and the saturation velocity of the velocity storage mechanism. If habituation were to occur over the course of testing, it could also have reduced mean eye velocities at the higher stimulus velocities, which were usually tested last. All these factors, and also the fact that canals were not stimulated while otoliths were, could explain why maximum values of mean eye velocities during OVAR were somewhat lower than peak OKAN values in the same subjects. In addition, the visual task given to the subject, which was not precisely controlled here, is probably also an important factor (see below). In summary, slow velocity of nystagmus induced in human subjects by rotation about an axis tilted at small angles from the vertical, show cyclical modulations around a mean value probably produced by the velocity storage mechanism. Cyclical modulations in vertical eye position and velocity probably compensate for changes in head position with regard to gravity. Their regularity and magnitude suggest that they could be used to check otolith function. Functional interpretation of nystagmus induced by OVAR We hereafter present a theoretical analysis that could account for the observed eye movements. As any movement can be, at any time, decomposed into the sum of a translation and a rotation, a head displacement generally stimulates simultaneously the semi-circular canals and the otoliths. In a re-examination of the gain of the vestibulo-ocular reflex, Viirre et al. (1986) established that perfect stabilization of a point image on the retina requires the gain of the V O R to follow the equation: E'R = - H'R q- H'T X P / p2 (1) E'R: angular eye velocity. H'R: angular head velocity. H'T: translational head velocity. P : vector having as origin the eye, and as tip the point of the surrounding world whose image is stabilized. x : cross product. 102 1) The CNS estimates H'R, via the VSM (Raphan and Cohen 1981, 1983, 1985; Hain 1986). 2) The CNS estimates translational head velocity from the otolithic signal, via an unknown neural circuit. This is likely, as translational head velocity can be estimated by subjects submitted to translational accelerations (Meiry 1965; Young and Meiry 1968). I I-i T / / / D Fig. 8. Valueof eyevelocitycompensatingfor headtranslation.To stabilize one point on the retina duringhead translation,the eye is to rotate at a velocitydepending,througha vectorialrelationship (see text) on head translationalvelocity(H'x), and distance (D) along the line of sight (LS) of the target (T). E is a unitaryvector along LS All these quantities are vectorial and expressed in head coordinates. This equation gives the angular velocity needed for a cyclopean eye having the same center of rotation as the head to stabilize the image of one point on the retina, not necessarily on the fovea. The rotation centers of the head and eye are considered to be coinciding. The transformation described by the second term of the right member is illustrated by Fig. 8. This term does not account for any counter-rolling, i.e. eye rotation around the direction of P, as the cross product of two vectors of which one is P itself has no component along P. This equation is valid instantaneously; to describe eye velocity during vestibular stimulation, one must also know how the central nervous system (CNS) estimates H'R and H'T. This equation simplifies to the classical VOR equation E' R = - H'R when the translational head velocity is zero, or when the distance of the stabilized point is infinite. Results obtained during simultaneous stimulation of canals and otoliths are quantitatively accounted for by this equation (Gresty and Bronstein 1986; Gresty et al. 1987). Assuming that it describes the actual functioning of the VOR, there remains the questions of the head coordinates used by the CNS (Raphan and Cohen 1985), and how cross products are performed. A differential equation gives the change of P which results from an actual head displacement: d P / d t = ' H ' T - H ' R X P. (2) To interpret the results of OVAR, we propose adding several hypotheses: 3) P is the gaze vector, given by the relation: P=E.D The direction of this vector is the line of sight, and its magnitude is the distance of a target, actual or imaginary. In this latter case, for instance in darkness, VOR gain can be modified by a mental resetting of the imaginary target (Barr et al. 1976; Baloh et al. 1984; Melvill-Jones et al. 1984). When no clearly located imaginary target can be evoked, D is set to a default value, which is neither zero nor infinite. An argument in favor of this hypothesis is that the gain of the eye velocity modulation decreases when the eyes are closed, due perhaps to cancellation of the second term of the right side of Eq. (3), or to the setting of D to infinite. Equation (1) then becomes Eq. (3), valid even in darkness, (and in which ocular counter-rolling is also given only by the first term of the right side). E'R = - H ' R + (H'TX E) / D (3) D: distance between the eye and the target. E: eye position vector i.e. vector of unit length, having the direction of the line of sight. Another differential equation links E'R and E: dE / dT = E'rt x E. (4) During OVAR, the components of the rotating vector on X and Y vary in quadrature, and so would H'T, the estimate of head translational velocity, with a lag % depending on stimulation frequency. Its three components on the stereotaxic axes would be: On X axis: On Y axis: On Z axis: [H'T] cos (cot + c9) ]H'a-[ sin (cot + q~) 0. The three components of the eye position vector are: On X axis: On Y axis: OnZaxis: Ex Ey E z. 103 As the subjects look forward, IExl is great with respect to IEyl and lEvi. The components of the cross product H'T x E are then: On X axis: IH'TI " Ez 9 sin (cot + q~) On Y axis: - [H'T[ 9 Ez 9 cos (cot + q)) On Z axis: IH'TI " (Ey 9 cos (cot + qg) - Ex 9 sin (cot + O r : - IH'T[ 9 ~/Ex2 + Ey2- sin (cot + qo - | With: @ = Arctg (By / Ex). O is therefore equal to the angle between the straight ahead direction and the gaze direction, in the horizontal plane, and is small if the subjects look ahead. The phase of the horizontal eye velocity modulation should therefore be very close to % Division of these three components by D gives respectively the velocity of rotation of the eye around the X, Y and Z axis. Rotation around X axis is not counter-roUing, which is rotation around P. Horizontal eye velocity modulation Rotation of the eye around the Z axis describes horizontal eye velocity modulation. Its gain is: IH'z[ 9 ~/Ex2 + Ey 2 / D. Each term is potential source of variation: 1) Estimate of translational head velocity may vary with time, especially if it derives from the combination of several sensory inputs (vestibular, tactile and proprioceptive). 2) Gaze orientation changes when it is reset to a new target. This could occur often in darkness, when the target is imaginary. The resulting gain variation should be small if the subject keeps looking ahead, i.e. if IEyl remains small with respect to [Ex[; but could be large if considerable gaze shifts would occur. 3) The distance of the target, D, may also be reset. To estimate the default value of D, one must know how the CNS estimates H'T from an acceleration signal. Assuming that translational head velocity is accurately estimated during lateral oscillations, and that the subjects were looking straight ahead, default value may be estimated at 27 + 9 m from the results of Niven et al. (1965), and at 18 + 9 m from those of Buizza et al. (1980). These values are so large, that they perhaps represent a resting position of the eye rather than the distance of an imaginary target. According to Eq. (3), the cross product of the estimate of H'T and E should be maximal when horizontal slow eye velocity is maximal. These two vectors should therefore be orthogonal at a phase angle of about 210 degrees. Provided the subjects were looking straight ahead, the estimate of H'ashould then be in the frontal plane. During barbecue nystagmus, maximum compensatory slow eye velocity occurs when the gravity vector is aligned with the Y axis (Benson and Bodin 1966; Correia and Guedry 1966; Saito et al. 1968; Wall and Black 1984). This leads the response during O V A R by 30 to 50 degrees. Hence, the phase lead could possibly increase with tilt angle (see also subject 2 in Fig. 5). Vertical eye velocity modulation Rotation of the eye around the Y axis describes vertical eye velocity modulation. Its expression, derived from the second term of the right side of Eq. (3), is: -IH'T] 9 E z . cos (cot + / D. The result of this expression is small, as Ez is far smaller than 1. Its sign depends on that of Ez, and therefore, its phase can change by any angle between 0 and n radians, depending whether the subject's gaze is upward or downward. These characteristics are not consistent with the experimental results. Therefore vertical eye velocity modulation cannot be accounted for by this expression. It can on the contrary be described by the first term of the right side of Eq. (3): - H'R. Head velocity in the sagittal plane could thus be sensed through the change in orientation of the gravity vector, and would elicit vertical eye movements, via the direct vestibuloocular pathway which has no storage, and would have a gain smaller than 1. Acknowledgements, The authors thank M. Ehrette for his technical help, Dr. Vidville for the acquisition program, and Dr. M. Smith for help with the English text. This study has been supported by the CNRS LP002, CNES 51003 and NS 00294. A preliminary report has been presented at the meeting of the Barany Society, Ann Arbor, Michigan, May 21-24, 1985. References Baloh RW, Lyerly K, Yee RD, Honrubia V (1984) Voluntary control of the human vestibulo-ocularreflex. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 97:1-6 104 Barr CC, Schulteis LW, Robinson DA (1976) Voluntary nonvisual control of the human vestibulo-ocular reflex. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 81:365-375 Benson AJ (1974) Modification of the response to angular accelerations by linear accelerations. In: Kornhuber H (ed) Handbook of sensory physiology, Vol VI/2. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 281-320 Benson AJ, Barnes GR (1973) Responses to rotating linear acceleration vectors considered in relation to a model of the otolith organs. In: 5th symposium on the role of the vestibular organs in space exploration. NASA SP 314 Benson AJ, Bodin MA (1966) Interaction of linear and angular accelerations on vestibular receptors in man. Aerosp Med 37: 144-154 Buizza A, Leger A, Droulez J, Berthoz A, Schmid R (1980) Influence of otolithic stimulation by horizontal linear acceleration on optokinetic nystagmus and visual motion perception. Exp Brain Res 39:509-522 Chun K-S, Robinson DA (1978) A model of quick phase generation in the vestibulo-oculr reflex. Biol Cybern 28: 209-231 Cohen B, Raphan T, Henn V, Dennett D (1981) Velocity storage nystagmus and visual vestibular interactions in humans. Ann NY Acad Sci 374:421-433 Cohen B, Suzuki J, Raphan T (1983) Role of the otolith organs in generation of horizontal nystagmus; effects of selective labyrinthine lesions. Brain Res 276:159-164 Correia MJ, Guedry FE (1966) Modification of vestibular responses as a function of rate of rotation about an earthhorizontal axis. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 62:297-308 Correia MJ, Money KE (1970) The effect of blockage of all six semi-circular canal ducts on nystagmus produced by linear acceleration in the cat. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 69:7-16 Darlot C, Lopez-Barneo J, Tracey D (1981) Asymmetry of vertical vestibular nystagmus in the cat. Exp Brain Res 41:420-426 Fernandez C, Goldberg JM (1976a) Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. I. Response to the static tilt and to long-duration centrifugal force. J Neurophysiol 39:970-984 Fern~indez C, Goldberg JM (1976b) Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. II. Directional selectivity and force-response relations. J Neurophysiol 39:985-995 Fern~indez C, Goldberg JM (1976c) Physiology of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. III. Response dynamics. J Neurophysiol 39:996-1008 Fern~indez C, Goldberg JM, Abend WK (1972) Response to static tilt of peripheral neurons innervating otolith organs of the squirrel monkey. J Neurophysiol 35:978-997 Goldberg JM, Fern~mdez C (1981) Physiological mechanisms of the nystagmus produced by rotations about an earth-horizontal axis. Ann NY Acad Sci 374:40-43 Goldberg JM, Fern~indez C (1982) Eye movements and vestibular nerve responses produced in the squirrel monkey by rotation about an earth-horizontal axis. Exp Brain Res 46:393-402 Gresty MA, Bronstein A (1986) Otolith stimulation evokes compensatory reflex eye movements of high velocity when linear motion of the head is combined with concurrent angular motion. Neurosci Lett 65:149-154 Gresty MA, Bronstein AM, Baratt H (1987) Eye movement responses to combined linear and angular head movement. Exp Brain Res 65:337-384 Guedry FE (1966) Influence of linear and angular accelerations on nystagmus. In: 2rid symposium on the role of the vestibular organs in space exploration. NASA SP 115:185-198 Guedry FE (1974) Psychophysics of the vestibular sensation. In: Kornhuber H (ed) Handbook of sensory physiology, Vol VI/ 2. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 3-154 Guedry FE (1965) Orientation of the rotation axis relative to gravity: its influence on nystagmus and the sense of rotation. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 60:30-48 Guitton D, Voile M (1987) Gaze control in humans: eye-head coordination during orienting movements to targets within and beyond the oculomotor range. J Neurophysiol 58: 427-459 Hain TC (1986) A model of the nystagmus induced by off vertical axis rotation. Biol Cybern 54:337-350 Harris LR (1987) Vestibular and optokinetic eye movements evoked in the cat by rotation about a tilted axis. Exp Brain Res 66:522-532 Harris LR, Barnes GB (1986) The orientation of vestibular nystagmus is modified by head tilt. Int. Meeting Barany Soc., Ann Arbor, Michigan Jell RM, Ireland D J, Lafortune S (1985) Human optokinetic afternystagmus. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 99:95-102 Krejcova H, Highstein S, Cohen B (1971) Labyrinthine and extralabyrinthine effects on ocular counter-rolling. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 72:165-171 Markham CH, Diamond SG (1983) Do the eyes counterroll during barbecue rotation? Soc Neurosci Abstr 9:868 Markham CH,-Diamond SG (1985) Dynamic ocular counterrolling. In: Igarashi, Black (eds) Vestibular and visual control on posture and locomotor equilibrium. 7th Int. Symp. Int. Soc. Posturography, Houston, Tex, 1983, pp 246-250 Matsuo V, Cohen B (1984) Vertical optokinetic nystagmus and vestibular nystagmus in the monkey: up-down as asymmetry and effects of gravity. Exp Brain Res 53:197-216 Mayne R (1974) A system concept of the vestibular organs. In: Kornhuber HH (ed) Handbook of sensory physiology, Vol VI/2. The vestibular system. Springer, New York, pp 493-580 Meiry JL (1965) The vestibular system and human dynamic space orientation. Thesis. M.I.T. Cambridge, Massachusetts Melvill-Jones G, Berthoz A, Segal B (1984) Adaptive modification of the vestibulo-ocular reflex by mental effort in darkness. Exp Brain Res 56:149-153 Niven JI, Hixson WC, Correia MJ (1965) Elicitation of horizontal nystagmus by periodic linear acceleration. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 62:429-441 Raphan T, Cohen B (1985) Velocity storage and the ocular response to multidimensional vestibular stimuli. Rev Oculomotor Res 1:123-143 Raphan T, Schnalbok C (1988) Modelling slow phase velocity generations during off vertical axis rotation (OVAR). In: Representation of three dimensional space in the vestibular, oculomotor and visual systems. Ann NY Acad Sci (in press) Raphan T, Cohen B, Henn V (1981a) Effects of gravity on rotatory nystagmus in monkeys. Ann NY Acad Sci 374: 337-346 Raphan T, Matuso V, Cohen B (1979) Velocity storage in the vestibulo-ocular reflex arc. Exp Brain Res 35:229-248 Raphan T, Waespe W, Cohen B (1981b) Vestibular nerve activity induced by off-vertical axis rotations. Soc Neurosci Abstr 7: 39 Saito I, Wada H, Yagura S, Kato N (1968) On the influences of gravitational acceleration to nystagmus. Jpn J Aerosp Med Psy Stockwell CW, Turnipseed GT, Guedry FE (1970) Nystagmus responses during rotation about a tilted axis. NAMRL 1129. U S A A R L 71-15, Pensacola, Fla., Naval Viirre E, Tweed D, Milner K, Vilis T (1986) A re-examination of the gain of the vestibulo-ocular reflex. J Neurophysiol 56: 439-450 105 Wall C, Black O (1984) The modulation component of nystagmus during earth-horizontal rotation: relationship with gaze angle. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 97:193--201 Young LR (1967) Effects of linear acceleration on vestibular nystagmus. 3rd symposium on the role of the vestibular organ in space exploration. NASA SP 152 Young L (1972) Cross coupling between effects of linear and angular acceleration on vestibular nystagmus. Bibl Ophthaltool 82:116-121 Young L, Henn V (1975) Nystagmus produced by pitch and yaw rotation of monkeys about non-vertical axes. Fortschr Zool 23:235-246 Young L, Meiry JL (1968) A revised dynamic otolith model. 3rd symposium on the role of the vestibular organ in space exploration. NASA SP 152 Received March 25, 1987 / Accepted April 22, 1988