Accepted Manuscript Simply adding the word “fruit” makes sugar healthier: The misleading effect of symbolic information on the perceived healthiness of food Bernadette Sütterlin, Michael Siegrist PII: S0195-6663(15)00328-1 DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.07.011 Reference: APPET 2633 To appear in: Appetite Received Date: 20 August 2014 Revised Date: 2 July 2015 Accepted Date: 9 July 2015 Please cite this article as: Sütterlin B. & Siegrist M., Simply adding the word “fruit” makes sugar healthier: The misleading effect of symbolic information on the perceived healthiness of food, Appetite (2015), doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.07.011. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain. ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 1 2 Simply adding the word “fruit” makes sugar healthier: The misleading effect of 3 symbolic information on the perceived healthiness of food 4 Bernadette Sütterlin and Michael Siegrist 6 ETH Zurich, Institute for Environmental Decisions (IED), Consumer Behavior, Switzerland RI PT 5 7 Address for correspondence: 9 Michael Siegrist SC 8 ETH Zurich 11 Institute for Environmental Decisions (IED), Consumer Behavior 12 Universitaetstrasse 22, CHN J76.3 13 CH-8092 Zurich 14 Switzerland 15 E-mail: msiegrist@ethz.ch 16 Telephone: +41 (0)44 632 63 21 AC C EP TE D M AN U 10 1 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Abstract 18 People may use simple heuristics to assess the healthiness of food products. For instance, the 19 information that a product contains “fruit sugar” (in German, “fruit sugar” is the colloquial 20 term for fructose) could be interpreted as a cue that the product is relatively healthy, since the 21 term “fruit” symbolizes healthiness. This can have a misleading effect on the perceived 22 healthiness of a product. In Experiment 1, participants (N = 164) were asked to evaluate the 23 healthiness of one of two breakfast cereals based on the information provided in a nutrition 24 table. For one group, the label “fruit sugar” was used; for the other, the label “sugar” was 25 used. Results suggest that the phrase “fruit sugar” listed as an ingredient of the breakfast 26 cereal resulted in a more positive perception of the healthiness of the cereal compared with 27 the ingredient labeled “sugar.” In Experiment 2 (N = 202), the results of Experiment 1 were 28 replicated with a within-subjects design in which participants evaluated the two products 29 simultaneously. Experiment 3 (N = 251) ruled out the alternative explanation that the effect 30 could be due to differing inferences about the product’s ingredients based on the label used, 31 that is, that the product labeled with “fruit sugar” contains fruit. Finally, in Experiment 4 (N = 32 162), the results show that the perceived healthiness associated with the labeling of the 33 ingredient “sugar” (“fruit sugar” vs. “sugar”) mediates the observed effect. Results of the four 34 experiments indicate that symbolic information is an important factor that can influence 35 people’s health perceptions of food. These findings have implications for marketing and 36 public health. SC M AN U TE D EP AC C 37 RI PT 17 38 Keywords: Health perception, fructose, fruit sugar, heuristics, health halo effect, symbolic 39 information 2 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 40 Simply adding the word “fruit” makes sugar healthier: The misleading effect of 41 symbolic information on the perceived healthiness of food 42 1. Introduction 43 Most consumers seem to have limited nutrition knowledge (Parmenter & Wardle, 1999). For instance, a Swiss study showed that two thirds of the study participants 45 erroneously believed that brown sugar is much healthier than white sugar (Dickson- 46 Spillmann, Siegrist, & Keller, 2011). If people lack the necessary knowledge to make 47 informed decisions, they have to rely on substitutes for knowledge. In such situations, people 48 may rely on simple heuristics to make decisions (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). A possible 49 side effect related to the use of heuristics is that wrong conclusions are drawn. In the present 50 research, we examined whether a product labeled as containing “fruit sugar” 1 (in German, 51 “fruit sugar” is the colloquial term for fructose) is perceived to be healthier than a product 52 labeled as containing sugar, since the term “fruit” symbolizes healthiness. Such a result 53 would suggest that consumers rely on symbolic information (e.g., the word “fruit” in “fruit 54 sugar”) to judge non-observable food properties, such as the healthiness of a specific food 55 product. Furthermore, we examined the underlying mechanisms and whether the effect also 56 extends to other product expectations, such as taste. EP TE D M AN U SC RI PT 44 A general feature of heuristic judgment is attribute substitution (Kahneman & 58 Frederick, 2005). A judgment is mediated by a heuristic when the attribute of the object a 59 person wants to judge (target attribute) is not readily accessible and a person assesses this 60 target attribute by substituting it with a seemingly semantically or associatively related 61 property that comes more easily to mind (heuristic attribute). Informational attributes with 62 high symbolic significance can serve as such heuristic attributes. Since the substituted 63 heuristic attribute differs from the target attribute, the use of heuristics may result in biased 64 decisions. AC C 57 3 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 65 The influence of the symbolic significance of information on people’s information processing and evaluation of behaviors, attitudinal objects, and incidents has already been 67 demonstrated in previous studies. Recent research, for example, showed that due to the 68 tendency to focus on information with a strong symbolic meaning, people underestimate the 69 energy consumption of consumers engaging in behaviors with a strong symbolic meaning of 70 energy-friendliness, while they overestimate the energy consumption of consumers who show 71 behaviors with a strong symbolic meaning of energy-unfriendliness (Sütterlin & Siegrist, 72 2014). In line with the assumption that people rely mainly on symbolically meaningful 73 information, another study found that people are more concerned about negative outcomes of 74 hazards, and evaluate them more negatively when they are caused by humans, compared with 75 the identical negative outcomes caused by nature (Siegrist & Sütterlin, 2014). Due to the 76 symbolic nature, the judgments were mainly based on the information about the cause, 77 resulting in different evaluations of the very same outcome. In judgments of the healthiness of 78 food, the word “fruit” has high symbolic significance, since “fruit” is commonly perceived as 79 symbolizing healthiness (Briz et al., 2008). Therefore, if the phrase “fruit sugar” evokes 80 healthy associations because it includes the word “fruit,” this may positively influence the 81 perceived healthiness of a product that contains “fruit sugar,” compared with a product that is 82 described as containing “sugar.” EP TE D M AN U SC RI PT 66 What type of symbolic information is relevant when it comes to the perceived 84 healthiness of food? Characteristics such as synthetic and natural seem to be important for 85 evaluating food products (Evans, de Challemaison, & Cox, 2010; Rozin, 2005). Consumers in 86 the Western world have a clear preference for natural products and seem to perceive synthetic 87 products in a negative way. Consequently, symbolic information that indicates the naturalness 88 of a food or reminds people that the food contains artificial ingredients may also influence the 89 perceived healthiness of a product and its sensory evaluation. A symbolic meaning attributed 90 to an aspect of a product or to a term used in the description of the ingredients (e.g., “fruit AC C 83 4 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT sugar”) is based on people’s interpretation of this aspect (e.g., natural ingredient that is 92 relatively healthy). The meaning is symbolic because it transcends the available facts, and 93 stereotypical information associated with the food shapes perception. In line with this 94 reasoning, findings of a recent qualitative study, for example, indicated that children tend to 95 perceive honey to be healthier than white sugar, because they associate it with naturalness 96 (Brierley & Elliott, 2015). 97 RI PT 91 A closely related phenomenon is the health halo effect, which may lead consumers to make incorrect inferences (Andrews, Burton, & Netemeyer, 2000; Roe, Levy, & Derby, 99 1999). A health halo effect occurs when the perception of an attribute of a product influences SC 98 the health evaluation of an unrelated product attribute. Research suggests that health claims 101 can cause the perception of inappropriate health benefits (Roe et al., 1999). Perceiving food as 102 healthy may result in underestimating their calories. For instance, the calories in food at fast- 103 food restaurants that are perceived to be healthy were underestimated (Chandon & Wansink, 104 2007). This health halo effect has resulted in higher calorie consumption when food was 105 provided by “healthy” restaurants rather than “unhealthy” restaurants. Health claims also have 106 an impact on product evaluation and the perceived naturalness (Lähteenmäki et al., 2010). 107 Furthermore, health labels have been shown to influence people’s expectations of a product’s 108 taste (Liem, Toraman Aydin, & Zandstra, 2012). However, in studies demonstrating health 109 halo effects, participants did not receive numerical information about the products. 110 Participants had to evaluate food properties (e.g., calories) in a state of high ambiguity. 111 Studies need to demonstrate that symbolic information also influences the interpretation of 112 concrete numerical information, without providing health claims that are explicitly targeted to 113 push consumers’ thoughts in a certain direction. 114 AC C EP TE D M AN U 100 The goal of the present study was to test whether information interpretation and, 115 consequently, perception of a food product differ depending on whether symbolic information 116 is included, even if identical information about the nutrients and calories of the product is 5 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT provided. We hypothesized that due to the positive symbolic meaning of the term “fruit,” the 118 ingredient “fruit sugar” would be perceived as healthier than (the unspecific term) “sugar.” 119 Thus, products labeled as containing “fruit sugar” may be perceived as healthier than products 120 labeled as containing sugar. We expected such a result even if the nutrition profiles of the 121 products are otherwise identical. 122 2. Experiment 1 123 RI PT 117 In the first experiment, we used a between-subjects design to examine whether participants in fact evaluate the identical nutrition information of breakfast cereals differently 125 with regard to healthiness when the labels “sugar” versus “fruit sugar” are used in the 126 nutrition table for the ingredient sugar. We postulated that, due to the symbolic meaning of 127 healthiness associated with the term “fruit,” the ingredient sugar is perceived to be healthier 128 when labeled as “fruit sugar” than when labeled with the umbrella term “sugar,” and that, as a 129 result, cereals presented as containing “fruit sugar” are assessed as healthier than cereals 130 presented as containing “sugar.” 131 2.1. Methods 132 2.1.1. Participants M AN U TE D Experiment 1 involved 164 participants. Participants represented a convenience sample EP 133 SC 124 of people living in the German-speaking part of Switzerland who agreed to participate in 135 online studies conducted by our group. The sample consisted of 61 (37%) women and 103 136 (63%) men. The mean age was 55 years (SD = 14). 137 2.1.2. Materials and Procedure 138 AC C 134 Participants received an e-mail asking them to fill out a short online questionnaire. In 139 addition to the experiment presented here, participants answered questions not related to the 140 research question of this study. 141 Participants received the following information: “Miss Meier wants to buy breakfast 142 cereals for her children. The following information is displayed on the package.” After this 6 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT introductory text, a nutrition table containing the nutrition information presented in Table 1 144 was shown. Participants were presented with either the nutrition table with the label “fruit 145 sugar” for the ingredient sugar (see Table 1, left side) or the one with the unspecific term 146 “sugar” (see Table 1, right side). They were then asked to answer the following question 147 using a slider: “How healthy are these breakfast cereals in your opinion?” The possible 148 responses ranged from “not healthy at all” (0) to “very healthy” (100). Study participants were randomly assigned to the “fruit sugar” condition or the “sugar” 150 condition. 151 2.2. Results and Discussion Experiment 1 showed that participants who received the nutrition table with the label M AN U 152 SC 149 RI PT 143 153 “sugar” assessed the cereal as less healthy compared with participants who received the 154 nutrition table with the label “fruit sugar.” The difference between the two groups was 155 significant, t(162) = 2.16, p = .032. Means and standard deviations are shown in Table 2. In studies examining the health halo effect, the information thought to influence TE D 156 participants’ perception was in the foreground and stood out from the other information (e.g., 158 a health claim or a brand); no further information about the food product was provided 159 (Gravel et al., 2012; Schuldt & Hannahan, 2013; Schuldt, Muller, & Schwarz, 2012). Table 1 160 shows that we used a very subtle manipulation, and provided the same concrete nutrition 161 information about the food product. Nevertheless, the symbolic information resulted in a 162 different perception of the product. Furthermore, in contrast to the studies on the health halo 163 effect, no specific product attribute (e.g., ingredient) was especially emphasized, only a 164 different labeling of the same attribute in the nutrition table was used. 165 3. Experiment 2 166 AC C EP 157 In Experiment 1, we used a between-subjects design; participants evaluated either the 167 product with the nutrition table with the label “sugar” or the one with the label “fruit sugar.” 168 This result could also be explained by the evaluability hypothesis described by Hsee (1998). 7 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT For example, he showed that in a separate evaluation condition, in which the participants were 170 presented with the information about only one object of evaluation (in this case, a cup of ice 171 cream), the participants’ willingness to pay for the ice cream was influenced by the irrelevant 172 but easier to evaluate dimension of cup size. That is, they based their decisions on whether the 173 cup was overfilled (5 oz cup with 7 oz of ice cream) or underfilled (10 oz cup with 8 oz of ice 174 cream). In a joint evaluation condition, however, participants’ willingness to pay for the ice 175 cream was influenced by the relevant dimension, that is, the actual amount of ice cream. The 176 study results suggest that people generally use the dimension that is easiest to evaluate as a 177 criterion for making a decision. It is therefore possible that people do not have any idea 178 whether a given amount of sugar or calories is “a lot” when presented with a product. The 179 information “fruit sugar” may be easier to evaluate due to its symbolic meaning, and may be 180 interpreted as a signal that the product does not contain a lot of sugar or calories. Therefore, 181 the numerical information was probably not taken into account, or to a lesser extent. To rule 182 out this alternative explanation, we conducted an experiment with a within-subjects design in 183 which participants could compare and evaluate two breakfast cereals: one with the label 184 “sugar” and one with the label “fruit sugar.” This is a realistic scenario because consumers 185 often compare products. Therefore, examining whether the same results can be observed in a 186 joint evaluation (i.e., within-subjects design) and in separate evaluations (i.e., between- 187 subjects design) is important. 188 3.1. Methods 189 3.1.1. Participants 190 AC C EP TE D M AN U SC RI PT 169 Experiment 2 involved 202 participants. Participants represented a convenience sample 191 of people living in the German-speaking part of Switzerland who agreed to participate in 192 online studies conducted by our group. The sample for Experiment 2 consisted of 95 (47%) 193 women and 107 (53%) men. The mean age was 54 years (SD = 15). 194 3.1.2. Materials and Procedure 8 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 195 Participants received an e-mail asking them to fill out a short online questionnaire. In 196 addition to the experiment presented here, participants answered questions not related to the 197 research question of this study. 198 In Experiment 2, a within-subjects design was used; participants evaluated breakfast cereals with a nutrition table with the label “fruit sugar” (“fruit sugar” condition) as well as 200 cereals with a nutrition table with the label “sugar” (“sugar” condition). Participants received 201 the following information: “Miss Meier wants to buy breakfast cereals for her children. The 202 following information is displayed on the package.” After this introductory text, the 203 description of the first product with the respective nutrition information was shown, followed 204 by the second product with the respective nutrition information. The nutrition information 205 depicted in Table 1 was used for the two products (i.e., conditions). The two product 206 descriptions with the respective nutrition tables that differed only in the labeling of the 207 ingredient sugar (“fruit sugar” vs. “sugar”) were presented simultaneously on the same screen, 208 one above the other. This allowed for a direct comparison of the described attributes, and 209 ensured the evaluability of the information about the amount of sugar and calories, which was 210 identical in both nutrition tables. SC M AN U TE D To correct for possible order effects, for half of the participants, the nutrition table for EP 211 RI PT 199 the product labeled with “fruit sugar” was presented above the nutrition table for the product 213 labeled with “sugar.” For the other half of the participants, the order was reversed. The 214 presentation order to which the participants were assigned was randomly determined. After 215 each product description, the participants were asked to answer the following question using a 216 slider: “How healthy are these breakfast cereals in your opinion?” The possible responses 217 ranged from “not healthy at all” (0) to “very healthy” (100). 218 3.2. Results and Discussion 219 220 AC C 212 All participants evaluated both products. The order of the product descriptions (i.e., nutrition tables) did not affect participants’ evaluations. Therefore, pooled data were 9 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 221 analyzed. As shown in Table 2, participants perceived the cereals with the label “fruit sugar” 222 as healthier than the cereals with the label “sugar.” Results of a paired t-test show that 223 participants evaluated the two products significantly differently, t(201) = 5.28, p < .001. 224 Although we used a joint evaluation design in Experiment 2, allowing the participants to directly compare the description attributes and the (identical) information on the amount of 226 sugar and calories, the results of Experiment 1 were successfully replicated. Therefore, we 227 can rule out evaluability as an alternative explanation for our results. The same effect can be 228 observed in the case of separate (Experiment 1) and joint (Experiment 2) evaluations. 229 4. Experiment 3 SC RI PT 225 Based on the information given to the participants in Experiments 1 and 2, we cannot 231 rule out that the participants in the “fruit sugar” condition assumed that the cereal contained 232 some fruit and, therefore, perceived them to be healthier. Consequently, the ingredient 233 “fruits,” not the ingredient “fruit sugar,” may have influenced participants’ responses. To rule 234 out this alternative explanation of the findings, in Experiment 3, a package of the cereals was 235 shown to inform participants that the cereals did not contain any fruits (see Figure 1). TE D M AN U 230 One goal of Experiment 3 was to replicate the findings of Experiments 1 and 2, this 237 time making it clear to the participants that the cereal did not contain any fruits. To test for the 238 robustness of the misleading effect of the symbolic meaning of the term “fruit sugar” on the 239 perceived healthiness of the products, we used somewhat different numerical information 240 about the nutrients of the cereals. A higher sugar content was indicated. Increasing the amount 241 of sugar could make this information more prevalent, and possibly shift participants’ focus 242 toward the indicated amount of sugar. This could result in greater consideration of the amount 243 of sugar when evaluating a product’s healthiness and, consequently, in better judgments. 244 4.1. Method 245 4.1.1. Participants AC C EP 236 10 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 246 Experiment 3 involved 251 participants. Participants represented a convenience sample of people living in the German-speaking part of Switzerland who agreed to participate in 248 online studies conducted by our group. The sample consisted of 104 (41%) women and 150 249 (59%) men. The mean age was 58 years (SD = 13). 250 4.1.2. Materials and Procedure 251 RI PT 247 Participants received an e-mail asking them to fill out a short online questionnaire. In addition to the experiment presented here, participants answered questions not related to this 253 research question. 254 SC 252 In this experiment, after the introductory text, a corn flakes package was shown and, subsequently, the nutrition table was presented. Figure 1 shows the text and information 256 provided to the participants. The design and color of the corn flakes package did not have any 257 similarities to existing packages on the Swiss market. The picture on the package indicated 258 that the cereal did not contain any fruits. Furthermore, to make the information about the 259 amount of sugar more prevalent, in the nutrition tables a higher amount of sugar (double digit 260 number) was indicated. As in the two previous experiments, the nutrition information 261 presented in the “fruit sugar” and the “sugar” conditions was identical, except for the labeling 262 of the ingredient sugar. TE D EP 263 M AN U 255 After the product description, participants were asked to answer the following question using a slider: “How healthy are these breakfast cereals in your opinion?” The possible 265 responses ranged from “not healthy at all” (0) to “very healthy” (100). 266 AC C 264 Participants were randomly assigned to the “fruit sugar” (n = 127) or the “sugar” 267 condition (n = 124). 268 4.2. Results and Discussion 269 In line with the previous experiments, in the “fruit sugar” condition, the cereals were 270 perceived to be significantly healthier (M = 39.61, SD = 21.52), compared with the “sugar” 271 condition (M = 32.51, SD = 19.32), t(249) = 2.75, p = .006. 11 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 272 Again, the same nutrition information was evaluated as less healthy when the label “sugar” was used in the nutrition table, compared with the label “fruit sugar.” In this 274 experiment, a picture of the package made it clear to the participants that the cereals did not 275 contain fruits. Therefore, we can rule out the alternative explanation that in the “fruit sugar” 276 condition, the cereals were perceived to be healthier because participants believed that the 277 cereals contained some fruit. Furthermore, pointing to the robustness of the effect, increasing 278 the amount of sugar to make this information more prevalent did not result in a greater 279 consideration of the amount of sugar. Consequently, the misleading effect of the symbolic 280 meaning of “fruit sugar” on the perceived healthiness of a product persisted, and was equally 281 pronounced. 282 5. Experiment 4 SC M AN U 283 RI PT 273 To this point, we have provided strong evidence for the existence and robustness of the misleading effect of the label “fruit sugar” on the perceived healthiness of products. However, 285 that this misleading effect is in fact attributable to the symbolic meaning of healthiness 286 associated with the term “fruit,” respectively, the label “fruit sugar,” has yet to be 287 demonstrated. Consequently, a primary goal of Experiment 4 was to provide evidence for the 288 assumption that the healthiness evaluation of the cereals was influenced, that is, mediated by 289 the symbolic meaning of the information provided to the participants (i.e., the labeling of the 290 ingredient sugar). EP AC C 291 TE D 284 With regard to the underlying mechanisms, the question about possible enforcing 292 factors that could increase people’s susceptibility to the fallacy arises. Previous research 293 showed that health-conscious consumers and consumers with high self-control tend to engage 294 in more elaborate decision-making behavior, and are less subject to judgment biases that 295 could arise from a reliance on heuristic cues, when it comes to explicit beliefs and cognitively 296 shaped evaluations regarding health-related issues (Mai & Hoffmann, 2015). As a result, they 297 profit less from information presentation measures aimed at facilitating the evaluation of 12 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT concrete nutrition information, such as the amount of calories (Koenigstorfer, Groeppel- 299 Klein, & Kamm, 2014). Given these findings, it would be of interest to see whether this 300 moderating influence of health-consciousness holds true for people’s susceptibility to the 301 misleading effect of the “fruit sugar” labeling. Attempting to shed more light on the 302 mechanisms underlying the postulated misleading effect of the symbolic meaning of the 303 labels used for ingredients, Experiment 4 was additionally aimed to investigate whether more 304 health-conscious consumers are less prone to this fallacy than less health-conscious 305 consumers, or whether it is equally prevalent. SC 306 RI PT 298 Summing up, the first goal of Experiment 4 was to provide more insights into the mechanisms underlying the misleading effect of the label “fruit sugar.” For this purpose, it 308 was aimed at more profoundly analyzing the differential perceptions associated with the terms 309 “fruit sugar” versus “sugar,” substantiating our claim that the effect is due to the symbolic 310 meaning assigned to the label “fruit sugar,” and identifying factors that exert a moderating 311 impact on the pronunciation of the fallacy. TE D 312 M AN U 307 Front-of-package food claims are generally associated with favorable impressions about food products bearing such a claim, and they exert a substantial impact on consumers’ 314 perceptions of a product’s healthiness (Roe et al., 1999). The presence of front-of-package 315 food claims can have a misleading effect on consumers’ product evaluations, and result in 316 suboptimal decision-making, because consumers tend to attribute inappropriate nutritional 317 characteristics and health benefits to products (Labiner-Wolfe, Jordan Lin, & Verrill, 2010). 318 Consumers with high product involvement were found to be less susceptible to the misleading 319 effect of product claims (Lee, Shimizu, Kniffin, & Wansink, 2013). Their higher motivation 320 to deal with this topic in depth leads to a more comprehensive and critical evaluation of the 321 information provided (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). In light of these findings, a second goal of 322 Experiment 4 was to investigate whether the additional presence of a front-of-package claim AC C EP 313 13 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 323 that the cereal contained “fruit sugar” would have a reinforcing effect on the fallacy of the 324 symbolic meaning, especially when it comes to consumers with low health consciousness. 325 When judging food products, criteria other than healthiness are also evaluated. The evaluation of these criteria may be derived based on different product characteristics, such as 327 package design, front-of-package claims and labels, as well as based on information on 328 nutritional components (cf. Orquin & Scholderer, 2015). For example, consumers 329 automatically draw inferences from claims about a food’s healthiness on the tastiness. They 330 tend to associate “unhealthy” foods with being tasty (Raghunathan, Naylor, & Hoyer, 2006). 331 Consequently, a final aim of Experiment 4 was to find out whether the fallacy related to the 332 symbolic meaning of the label “fruit sugar” also extends to the other evaluation criteria of 333 food, or whether it is specifically related to perceived healthiness. 334 5.1. Method 335 5.1.1. Participants SC M AN U As in the previous experiments, the data were collected using a convenience sample of TE D 336 RI PT 326 residents of the German-speaking part of Switzerland who agreed to take part in online 338 studies of our group on a regular basis. A total of 162 people participated in the study. The 339 sample consisted of 53 women (33%) and 109 (67%) men, with an average age of 59 years 340 (SD = 13). 341 5.1.2. Materials and Procedure AC C 342 EP 337 An invitation email was sent to the participants, asking them to take part in a short 343 online study. In addition to the experiment presented here, participants also answered 344 questions not related to these research questions. 345 The experiment consisted of three conditions. For the “fruit sugar” and the “sugar” 346 conditions, scenario descriptions and information (including the picture of the package of 347 cereal) identical to Experiment 3 were used. The text and information provided in the third 348 condition, the “fruit sugar & claim” condition, were the same as in the “fruit sugar” condition, 14 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT except for the fact that in the “fruit sugar & claim” condition, on the corn flakes package, the 350 food claim “contains 100% fruit sugar” was depicted on the front. Figure 2 shows the picture 351 of the corn flakes package presented to the participants in the “fruit sugar & claim” condition. 352 Participants were randomly assigned to one out of the three experimental conditions. 353 Following the product description, participants were asked to indicate their perceived 354 healthiness of the corn flakes using a slider ranging from “not healthy at all” (0) to “very 355 healthy” (100). The question read as follows: “How healthy are these breakfast cereals in your 356 opinion?” Moreover, they were asked to indicate how they judged the cereals with regard to 357 naturalness, from “not natural at all” (0) to “very natural” (100); how they judged the 358 nutritional value of these cereals, from “very low” (0) to “very high” (100); and how they 359 would judged the taste of these cereals, from “very bad” (0) to “very good” (100). SC M AN U 360 RI PT 349 To provide evidence for the differential perceptions associated with the terms “fruit sugar” and “sugar” related to their symbolic meaning of healthiness, previous to the 362 experimental part with the breakfast cereals, participants were asked the following questions: 363 “How healthy is fruit sugar [sugar] in your opinion?” (“not healthy at all” [0] to “very 364 healthy” [100]); “How high do you judge the nutritional value of fruit sugar [sugar]? (“very 365 low” [0] to “very high” [100]); and “How strongly does the consumption of food products 366 containing fruit sugar [sugar] symbolize health consciousness in your opinion? (“very weak 367 symbol” [0] to “very strong symbol” [100]). The participants assigned to the conditions “fruit 368 sugar” or “fruit sugar & claim” answered these questions with regard to “fruit sugar”. In the 369 questions provided to the participants assigned to the condition “sugar,” the term “fruit sugar” 370 was replaced by “sugar.” AC C EP TE D 361 371 Health consciousness, which was assumed to represent a moderator, was measured by 372 three items formulated in the form of statements: “I am interested in nutritional issues.”; “A 373 healthy diet is important to me.”; and “I am not interested in the topic of nutrition.” 374 Participants had to rate, on a 6-point scale ranging from “applies not at all” (1) to “completely 15 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 375 applies” (6), how much the statement applied to them. The reliability of the health 376 consciousness measurement was satisfying, with a Cronbach’s alpha of .74. 377 To control for a possible social desirability bias that might occur, especially for topics that are strongly subject to social norms, such as health and environmental friendliness, 379 socially desirable response patterns were assessed. For this purpose, we utilized the short 380 form of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale M-C 1, proposed by Strahan and 381 Gerbasi (1972). This short form was shown to be most appropriate for online studies, for 382 which, due to time constraints, it is essential to draw on short forms of the scale that are of 383 sufficient psychometric quality (Vésteinsdóttir, Reips, Joinson, & Thorsdottir, 2015). The 384 internal consistency of the short social desirability scale was α = .56, which corresponds to the 385 reliability scores found in other studies (Strahan & Gerbasi, 1972; Vésteinsdóttir et al., 2015). 386 5.2. Results and Discussion 387 5.2.1. The misleading effect of the label “fruit sugar” M AN U SC RI PT 378 The data were analyzed for each evaluation criterion (healthiness, naturalness, 389 nutritional value, taste) of the cereals separately using one-way analysis of variance 390 (ANOVA) with the factor experimental condition (“fruit sugar” vs. “sugar” vs. “fruit sugar & 391 claim”). Analyses revealed a significant effect of the factor experimental condition on the 392 perceived healthiness of the cereals, F(2, 159) = 3.91, p = .022. The cereals were judged 393 significantly healthier when the label “fruit sugar” compared to “sugar” was used. The same 394 was true for the condition with the additional “fruit sugar” claim. The conditions “fruit sugar” 395 and “fruit sugar & claim” did not significantly differ in the perceived healthiness of the 396 cereals. Detailed results are displayed in Table 3. The three experimental conditions did not 397 differ with regard to the other evaluation criteria: naturalness (F[2, 159] = 0.71, p = .493), 398 nutritional value (F[2, 159] = 2.31, p = .103), and taste (F[2, 159] = 0.18, p = .837). No 399 biasing influence of socially desirable response patterns was detected. 2 AC C EP TE D 388 16 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 400 To test for a possible moderating effect of health consciousness, participants were grouped into low and high health consciousness using a median split. Subsequently, an 402 ANOVA with the factor experimental condition and the factor health consciousness group 403 (low vs. high health consciousness) was performed. The analysis yielded significant main 404 effects for the factors experimental condition, F(2, 154) = 3.61, p = .029, and health 405 consciousness group, F(1, 154) = 31.59, p < .001. However, there was no interaction effect, 406 F(2, 154) = 1.09, p = .337, indicating that the participants low and high in health 407 consciousness were equally susceptible to the misleading effect of the label “fruit sugar.” The 408 two health consciousness groups only differed in their healthiness judgments in general. The 409 health conscious participants over all of the experimental conditions judged the cereal’s 410 healthiness to be lower. 3 SC M AN U 411 RI PT 401 According to the findings, the misleading effect of the symbolic meaning of the label “fruit sugar” seems to appear specifically when it comes to judgments related to the product’s 413 healthiness. Additionally, adding an explicit front-of-package claim that the product contains 414 “fruit sugar” did not significantly reinforce the biasing effect. No matter whether the symbolic 415 information is supposedly unobtrusively displayed in the nutrition table, or prominently 416 positioned on the front of the package, once the symbolic information has been read, it exerts 417 an equally misleading effect on the perceived healthiness. Furthermore, health consciousness 418 was not found to have a moderating influence on the misleading effect of the labeling. Health- 419 conscious consumers are as equally susceptible to the misleading effect as consumers who are 420 less interested in health-related issues. Consequently, when confronted with a labeling of 421 ingredients that has a strong symbolic meaning, a more profound evaluation of the depicted 422 nutritional information (as occurs with health conscious consumers) does not guarantee an 423 accurate judgment of the product’s healthiness. Also, in this case, the misleading effect of the 424 symbolic meaning of the used labeling still prevails, and reduces judgment accuracy. 425 5.2.2. The mediating role of the perceived healthiness associated with symbolic information AC C EP TE D 412 17 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 426 In line with the assumption that the term “fruit sugar” symbolizes healthiness due to the use of the word “fruit,” which finally results in an increase in the ingredient’s perceived 428 healthiness and a more favorable judgment of other health-related aspects, significant 429 differences in the perceptions associated with the terms “fruit sugar” and “sugar” emerged 430 (see Table 4). Compared to the general term “sugar,” “fruit sugar” was perceived to be 431 healthier, to have a higher nutritional value, and the consumption of products containing “fruit 432 sugar” was considered to be a stronger symbol for healthiness. RI PT 427 We postulated that the perceived healthiness of cereal was driven by the symbolic 434 meaning of the provided information, that is, the symbolic meaning of healthiness associated 435 with the labeling of the ingredient sugar (“fruit sugar” vs. “sugar”). Based on this assumption, 436 we expected that the impact of the different labeling (i.e., the provision of information with 437 different symbolic meanings) on the perceived healthiness of the cereal would be mediated by 438 the perceived healthiness associated with the labeling, as a result of its symbolic meaning. 439 This simple mediation effect was tested using the method proposed by Preacher and Hayes 440 (2008). The cereals in the experimental conditions “fruit sugar” and “fruit sugar & claim” 441 were described using the label “fruit sugar.” Since these two conditions did not significantly 442 differ in the perceived healthiness of the cereals, the data from these conditions were pooled 443 for the mediation analysis. The variable “labeling of the ingredient sugar” was dummy coded 444 (0 = sugar and 1 = fruit sugar). The results of the mediation analysis are presented in Figure 2. 445 Results show that the labeling of the ingredient sugar (i.e., the symbolic information) had a 446 significant impact on the healthiness associated with the ingredient, which, in turn, 447 significantly influenced the perceived healthiness of the cereal. Controlling for the healthiness 448 associated with the ingredient sugar due to its labeling resulted in a non-significant impact of 449 the labeling on the perceived healthiness of the cereal. Therefore, a full mediation could be 450 observed. A bias-corrected 95% bootstrap confidence interval for the indirect effect based on 451 1,000 bootstrap samples was clearly above zero (3.90 to 12.47). AC C EP TE D M AN U SC 433 18 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 452 Results suggest that the misleading effect of the labeling on the product’s perceived healthiness can be explained by the difference in the perceived healthiness of the ingredient 454 sugar, when labeled “fruit sugar” compared to “sugar” (i.e., umbrella term), which is due to 455 the associated symbolic meaning of healthiness. The mediation analysis revealed that the 456 labeling of the ingredient sugar led to a different perception of the healthiness of the 457 ingredient sugar, and that the participants evaluated the healthiness of the breakfast cereals 458 based on the perceived healthiness associated with the labeling of the ingredient sugar. 459 6. General Discussion SC RI PT 453 The aim of the present research was to provide evidence that people rely on symbolic 461 information when evaluating food properties. More precisely, it aimed to examine whether 462 presenting consumers with the information that a product contains “fruit sugar” affects 463 product evaluation due to the symbolic nature of the term “fruit.” Results of the present study 464 indicate that the phrase “fruit sugar” listed as an ingredient of breakfast cereals results in a 465 more positive perception of the cereals’ healthiness. The mediation analysis conducted in 466 Experiment 4 revealed that the labeling of the ingredient sugar as “fruit sugar” leads to a 467 higher perceived healthiness of the ingredient, compared with the labeling as “sugar,” and that 468 the healthiness associated with the ingredient as a result of the labeling mediates the perceived 469 healthiness of the food product. The findings impressively show that the healthiness of the 470 very same product, with the very same nutrition information, is perceived differently, simply 471 by changing the labeling of the ingredient sugar. The labeling of the ingredients by making 472 use of symbolic information may, consequently, exert a misleading effect on a consumer’s 473 assessment of the product’s healthiness. The findings suggest that the effect is quite robust. A 474 more profound and comprehensive evaluation of the provided information (as occurs with 475 people with pronounced health consciousness) does not protect against the misleading effect 476 of symbolic information, and does not add to judgment accuracy. This indicates that relying 477 and drawing on the symbolic meaning of information is, to a certain extent, an automatic and AC C EP TE D M AN U 460 19 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 478 implicit process that cannot easily be correct by increasing people’s health consciousness. 479 Furthermore, evidence was provided that the perception of a subtle cue is sufficient to trigger 480 reliance on symbolic information, when evaluating the healthiness of food products. Why can we claim that the decisions in our experiments were biased? In general, it can 482 be stated that the term “sugar” represents an umbrella term that subsumes the different types 483 of sugar, such as fructose (“fruit sugar”). Bearing this in mind, no difference between the 484 ingredient sugar labeled as “fruit sugar” and labeled with the umbrella term “sugar” should be 485 observed. However, consumers could automatically associate the term “sugar” with sucrose, 486 commonly named “table sugar,” which is a combination of fructose and glucose. Therefore, 487 the question arises whether associating “fruit sugar” with higher healthiness than sucrose is in 488 fact inaccurate, and results in biased judgments. It has been shown that high fructose 489 consumption can induce, among other things, insulin resistance (Rizkalla, 2010), and that it is 490 a driver for pathological cardiac growth and dysfunction (Mirtschink et al., 2015). However, 491 in most of these studies, unrealistically high doses of fructose were used. In a comprehensive 492 review of the literature, no direct evidence was found for more negative effects of high 493 fructose compared with sucrose (Tappy & Le, 2010). Based on the scientific evidence, it can 494 be concluded that it does not matter whether the breakfast cereals contain added fructose or 495 sucrose (Rizkalla, 2010; Tappy & Le, 2010). There is no scientific justification for assessing 496 the cereals containing “fruit sugar” as healthier than the cereals containing sugar. On the 497 contrary, a recent study provided evidence that the brain and behavioral response to fructose, 498 relative to glucose, may promote feeding behavior. It was shown that the ingestion of fructose 499 relative to glucose resulted in a greater brain reactivity to food cues and, in line with this, led 500 to greater hunger and desire for food, as well as greater willingness to give up long-term 501 monetary rewards to obtain immediate high-caloric food rewards (Luo, Monterosso, 502 Sarpelleh, & Page, 2015). It is safe to conclude, therefore, that we have observed biased AC C EP TE D M AN U SC RI PT 481 20 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 503 responses in our experiments, because participants were influenced by the symbolic 504 significance of information that is not relevant to the evaluation task. 505 In the present experimental design, we made sure that the participants had identical information about the two products, except for the manipulated variable (i.e., the labeling of 507 the ingredient sugar). The goal of our experiment was to test whether people perceive “fruit 508 sugar” as healthier than sugar and not whether people assume that the nutrition profile (e.g., 509 the number of calories or amount of fat) of a product containing “fruit sugar” is different from 510 that of a product containing sugar. Therefore, we used the identical realistic nutrition 511 information for the products and presented it in the form of a nutrition table. In nutrition 512 tables commonly used in Switzerland and other European countries, the unspecific term 513 “sugar,” not “fruit sugar,” is used. However, claims on food packages or in advertisements 514 may emphasize that the product is sweetened with “fruit sugar”. Had we only used the full 515 product packaging and the “fruit sugar” claim, the participants could have concluded that the 516 breakfast cereals labeled as containing “fruit sugar” had a different nutritional profile than the 517 breakfast cereals labeled as containing “sugar.” SC M AN U TE D 518 RI PT 506 In the present study, participants evaluated breakfast cereals. 4 This product category was chosen because in Switzerland, breakfast cereal manufacturers often emphasize that their 520 product does not contain additional sugar but is sweetened by honey and maple syrup or that 521 the product contains only “fruit sugar”. In various countries, soft drinks are sweetened with 522 high-fructose corn syrup. Sweetened soft drinks are most likely not perceived as healthy 523 products, which is why we used the more neutral food category of breakfast cereals. Future 524 studies could examine whether similar effects can also be observed for more hedonic products 525 such as soft drinks. 526 AC C EP 519 The present study is not the first to suggest that symbolic information influences the 527 perception of food (Chandon & Wansink, 2007; Gravel et al., 2012; Lähteenmäki et al., 2010; 528 Schuldt & Hannahan, 2013; Schuldt et al., 2012; Schuldt & Schwarz, 2010; Wansink & 21 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Chandon, 2006). However, our research differs from the existing studies in a number of 530 aspects. First, our manipulation was more subtle than the interventions in most past studies. 531 We did not make an explicit claim regarding the food products directly targeted at pushing 532 consumers’ product evaluation in a certain direction (Chandon & Wansink, 2007; Gravel et 533 al., 2012; Lähteenmäki et al., 2010; Schuldt & Hannahan, 2013; Schuldt et al., 2012; Schuldt 534 & Schwarz, 2010). In our experiments, we provided identical detailed information in the 535 nutrition tables with the same ingredients. Only a different labeling of the ingredient sugar 536 was used, which was effectuated by merely changing one word in the nutrition table. 537 Nevertheless, our manipulation had an impact. This further shows the powerful impact of 538 symbolic information on the perception of food. Second, in some past studies, the information 539 provided was ambiguous to participants (Gravel et al., 2012). It is very difficult to estimate 540 the calories of food without receiving concrete information about its ingredients. In the 541 present study, participants were fully informed about the nutrition profile of the food. Third, 542 past studies did not examine possible mechanisms of the observed effect. In Experiment 4, we 543 tested the perceived healthiness associated with ingredients’ labeling as a possible mechanism 544 underlying the effect of presenting symbolic information on the perception of food. Our 545 results point to the misleading effect of symbolic information, showing that simply by using 546 the symbolically positive label “fruit sugar,” consumers are misled to perceive the ingredient 547 sugar as healthier and, in the end, this results in the perception of the cereals as healthier. 548 Fourth, to the best of our knowledge, despite all the studies that have shown the possible 549 negative effects of high fructose intake (Luo et al., 2015; Tappy & Le, 2010), this is the first 550 study to suggest that the term “fruit sugar” is still perceived in a more positive way compared 551 with the unspecific term “sugar.” 552 AC C EP TE D M AN U SC RI PT 529 Several limitations of the present study must be addressed. In all three experiments, 553 convenience samples were used. It is, therefore, unclear to what extent the present findings 554 can be generalized. Furthermore, we focused on the effect of symbolic information on 22 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT information processing and the resulting perceived healthiness of products, and the perception 556 of other evaluation criteria (e.g., taste). Several response types can be used to evaluate the 557 impact of symbolic information. In the present study, we examined the effect of the label 558 “fruit sugar” on the perceived healthiness. Another type of response that could be assessed is 559 people’s willingness to buy the product utilizing real food packages. We did not choose this 560 second approach, because some participants may not buy cereal, and the perceived healthiness 561 may be only one factor that influences people’s willingness to buy food products. Given these 562 additional confounding variables, much larger samples would be needed to show the expected 563 effects. Therefore, the focus of the present study was on the perceived healthiness, and not on 564 the purchase decision. In our experiments, we provided evidence for the misleading effect of 565 the labeling of nutrition information. Of course, paying attention to the information presented 566 in the nutrition table does not guarantee for a healthier product choice. However, it is obvious 567 that consumers, first of all, need to correctly interpret and process the nutrition information, 568 before they can use it to make more healthy decisions. Therefore, insights into the processing 569 of nutrition facts and potentially biasing factors are crucial for producers and policy makers, 570 when it comes to the provision of health-related information. SC M AN U TE D Future research should examine how people judge the healthiness of food in more EP 571 RI PT 555 detail, and whether there exists other symbolic information that may exert an (misleading) 573 influence on a consumer’s perception of a product’s healthiness. Furthermore, exploring 574 whether the label “fruit sugar” also changes other product expectations (e.g., product quality, 575 environmental friendliness), or the perception of other nutritional values provided on the 576 package (e.g., fat) would be worthwhile. In the present research, we focused on “fruit sugar”; 577 however, examining how other sugars (e.g., honey) are perceived compared to “fruit sugar” 578 using an experimental approach would also be interesting. As mentioned above, the presented 579 experiments investigated the biasing effect of symbolic information on the perceived 580 healthiness of food products, that is, on the processing of nutrition information. Future AC C 572 23 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 581 research might wish to expand the focus to the analysis of the impact of symbolic information 582 on the final food choice. 583 In sum, results of the present study suggest that consumers perceive a product labeled as containing “fruit sugar” as healthier than one labeled with the unspecific term “sugar.” We 585 can rule out that the label “fruit sugar” made participants believe that the product has fewer 586 calories, less fat, or less sugar compared to the other product, because we provided identical 587 nutrition information for the products. Moreover, in the joint evaluation condition (i.e., 588 within-subjects design in Experiment 2), it was obvious to participants that the nutrition 589 profiles of the two products were identical. Therefore, our results are in line with our 590 hypothesis that symbolic information is an important factor in people’s evaluation of how 591 healthy a food product is, and may lead to biased judgments. People perceive fruits as healthy 592 food (Bucher, van der Horst, & Siegrist, 2013); therefore, since the word “fruit” symbolizes 593 healthiness, the term “fruit sugar” is associated with higher healthiness. As a result, the 594 ingredient “fruit sugar” and, consequently, the food product are perceived to be relatively 595 healthy. SC M AN U TE D 596 RI PT 584 The implications of these findings for marketing and public health are clear. From a marketing point of view, the emphasis on natural ingredients and ingredients with a positive 598 symbolic meaning may result in a better health perception of food. Not only direct health 599 claims (Lähteenmäki, 2013), but also ingredients may automatically evoke positive 600 associations that have an important impact on a consumer’s information processing and 601 decision-making. From a public health perspective, the challenge is how to educate the public 602 so that consumers are not influenced by misleading symbolic product information, when they 603 evaluate the healthiness of food products. Our findings suggest that consumers are highly 604 susceptible to the symbolic information that food marketers may specifically use in an attempt 605 of greenwashing or nutriwashing to promote their products. Such information may bias 606 consumers’ information processing and product evaluation, and finally, result in suboptimal AC C EP 597 24 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT purchase decisions. This is especially concerning, given that subtle symbolic cues are already 608 sufficient to induce biased product evaluation, and given that even high involvement 609 consumers, who generally engage in more profound information processing, are susceptible to 610 this fallacy. AC C EP TE D M AN U SC RI PT 607 25 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 611 References 612 Andrews, J. C., Burton, S., & Netemeyer, R. G. (2000). Are some comparative nutrition claims misleading? The role of nutrition knowledge, ad claim type and disclosure 614 conditions. Journal of Advertising, 29(3), 29-42. doi: 615 10.1080/00913367.2000.10673615 616 RI PT 613 Brierley, M., & Elliott, C. (2015). Nutritional components and children's interpretations of packaged food. International Journal of Health Promotion and Education, 1-14. doi: 618 10.1080/14635240.2015.1010654 619 SC 617 Briz, T., Sijtsema, S. J., Jasiulewicz, A., Kyriakidi, A., Dolors Guàrdia, M., van den Berg, I., & van der Lans, I. A. (2008). Barriers to fruit consumption: Driving forces behind 621 consumer behaviour. Scripta Horticulturae, 8, 7-18. 623 624 Bucher, T., van der Horst, K., & Siegrist, M. (2013). Fruit for dessert. How people compose healthier meals. Appetite, 60(0), 74-80. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.10.003 Chandon, P., & Wansink, B. (2007). The biasing health halos of fast-food restaurant health TE D 622 M AN U 620 625 claims: Lower calorie estimates and higher side-dish consumption intentions. Journal 626 of Consumer Research, 34(3), 301-314. doi: 10.1086/519499 Dickson-Spillmann, M., Siegrist, M., & Keller, C. (2011). Development and validation of a EP 627 short, consumer-oriented nutrition knowledge questionnaire. Appetite, 56(3), 617-620. 629 doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.01.034 630 631 632 633 AC C 628 Evans, G., de Challemaison, B., & Cox, D. N. (2010). Consumers’ ratings of the natural and unnatural qualities of foods. Appetite, 54(3), 557-563. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2010.02.014 Gravel, K., Doucet, É., Peter Herman, C., Pomerleau, S., Bourlaud, A.-S., & Provencher, V. 634 (2012). “Healthy,” “diet,” or “hedonic”. How nutrition claims affect food-related 635 perceptions and intake? Appetite, 59(3), 877-884. doi: 636 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.08.028 26 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 637 638 639 Hsee, C. K. (1998). Less is better: When low-value options are valued more highly than highvalue options. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 11(2), 107-121. Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2005). A model of heuristic judgment. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 267- 641 293). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. 642 RI PT 640 Koenigstorfer, J., Groeppel-Klein, A., & Kamm, F. (2014). Healthful food decision making in response to traffic light color-coded nutrition labeling. Journal of Public Policy & 644 Marketing, 33(1), 65-77. doi: http://journals.ama.org/doi/abs/10.1509/jppm.12.091 645 SC 643 Labiner-Wolfe, J., Jordan Lin, C.-T., & Verrill, L. (2010). Effect of low-carbohydrate claims on consumer perceptions about food products' healthfulness and helpfulness for 647 weight management. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 42(5), 315-320. 649 650 Lähteenmäki, L. (2013). Claiming health in food products. Food Quality and Preference, 27(2), 196-201. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.03.006 Lähteenmäki, L., Lampila, P., Grunert, K., Boztug, Y., Ueland, Ø., Åström, A., & TE D 648 M AN U 646 Martinsdóttir, E. (2010). Impact of health-related claims on the perception of other 652 product attributes. Food Policy, 35(3), 230-239. doi: 653 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodpol.2009.12.007 655 656 657 Lee, W.-c. J., Shimizu, M., Kniffin, K. M., & Wansink, B. (2013). You taste what you see: Do organic labels bias taste perceptions? Food Quality and Preference, 29(1), 33-39. AC C 654 EP 651 doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2013.01.010 Liem, D. G., Toraman Aydin, N., & Zandstra, E. H. (2012). Effects of health labels on 658 expected and actual taste perception of soup. Food Quality and Preference, 25(2), 659 192-197. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.02.015 660 661 Luo, S., Monterosso, J. R., Sarpelleh, K., & Page, K. A. (2015). Differential effects of fructose versus glucose on brain and appetitive responses to food cues and decisions 27 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 662 for food rewards. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(20), 6509- 663 6514. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1503358112 664 Mai, R., & Hoffmann, S. (2015). How to combat the unhealthy = tasty intuition: The influencing role of health consciousness. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 34(1), 666 63-83. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1509/jppm.14.006 667 RI PT 665 Mirtschink, P., Krishnan, J., Grimm, F., Sarre, A., Horl, M., Kayikci, M., . . . Krek, W. (2015). HIF-driven SF3B1 induces KHK-C to enforce fructolysis and heart disease. 669 Nature, advance online publication. doi: 10.1038/nature14508 670 SC 668 Orquin, J. L., & Scholderer, J. (2015). Consumer judgments of explicit and implied health claims on foods: Misguided but not misled. Food Policy, 51(0), 144-157. doi: 672 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodpol.2015.01.001 674 675 Parmenter, K., & Wardle, J. (1999). Development of a general nutrition knowledge questionnaire for adults. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 53(4), 298-308. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. TE D 673 M AN U 671 676 Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123-205). 677 New York: Academic Press. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing EP 678 and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research 680 Methods, 40(3), 879-891. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879 681 682 AC C 679 Raghunathan, R., Naylor, R. W., & Hoyer, W. D. (2006). The unhealthy = tasty intuition and its effects on taste inferences, enjoyment, and choice of food products. Journal of 683 Marketing, 70(4), 170-184. doi: 684 http://journals.ama.org/doi/abs/10.1509/jmkg.70.4.170 685 686 Rizkalla, S. W. (2010). Health implications of fructose consumption: A review of recent data. Nutrition and Metabolism, 7, 82. doi: 10.1186/1743-7075-7-82 28 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 687 Roe, B., Levy, A. S., & Derby, B. M. (1999). The impact of health claims on consumer search 688 and product evaluation outcomes: Results from FDA experimental data. Journal of 689 Public Policy & Marketing, 18(1), 89-105. doi: 10.2307/30000511 690 Rozin, P. (2005). The meaning of “natural”: Process more important than content. 692 Psychological Science, 16(8), 652-658. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01589.x RI PT 691 Schuldt, J. P., & Hannahan, M. (2013). When good deeds leave a bad taste. Negative inferences from ethical food claims. Appetite, 62(0), 76-83. doi: 694 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.11.004 695 SC 693 Schuldt, J. P., Muller, D., & Schwarz, N. (2012). The “fair trade” effect: Health halos from social ethics claims. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(5), 581-589. doi: 697 10.1177/1948550611431643 698 M AN U 696 Schuldt, J. P., & Schwarz, N. (2010). The "organic" path to obesity? Organic claims influence calorie judgments and exercise recommendations. Judgment and Decision Making, 700 5(3), 144-150. 701 702 TE D 699 Siegrist, M., & Sütterlin, B. (2014). Human and nature-caused hazards: The affect heuristic causes biased decisions. Risk Analysis, 34(8), 1482-1494. doi: 10.1111/risa.12179 Strahan, R., & Gerbasi, K. C. (1972). Short, homogeneous versions of the Marlowe-Crowne 704 Social Desirability Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 28(2), 191-193. doi: 705 10.1002/1097-4679(197204)28:2<191::AID-JCLP2270280220>3.0.CO;2-G 707 708 709 710 711 712 AC C 706 EP 703 Sütterlin, B., & Siegrist, M. (2014). The reliance on symbolically significant behavioral attributes when judging energy consumption behaviors. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 40(0), 259-272. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.07.005 Tappy, L., & Le, K. A. (2010). Metabolic effects of fructose and the worldwide increase in obesity. Physiological Reviews, 90(1), 23-46. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00019.2009 Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124-1131. doi: 10.1126/science.185.4157.1124 29 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 713 Vésteinsdóttir, V., Reips, U.-D., Joinson, A., & Thorsdottir, F. (2015). Psychometric 714 properties of measurements obtained with the Marlowe–Crowne Social Desirability 715 Scale in an Icelandic probability based Internet sample. Computers in Human 716 Behavior, 49(0), 608-614. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.03.044 718 Wansink, B., & Chandon, P. (2006). Can “low-fat” nutrition labels lead to obesity? Journal of RI PT 717 Marketing Research, 43(4), 605-617. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.43.4.605 AC C EP TE D M AN U SC 719 30 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 720 Footnotes 721 1 722 is used. In German, however, the word “Fruchtzucker,” which translated into English means 723 “fruit sugar,” is commonly used. This word has much closer associations with fruits and 724 naturalness compared with the word “fructose.” Therefore, we use the term “fruit sugar,” the 725 literal translation of the German word “Fruchtzucker,” in this paper. 726 2 727 possible social desirability effects, revealed no significant results for the covariate social 728 desirability, F(1, 154) = 0.38, p = .538. Consequently, the findings on the perceived 729 healthiness were not subject to a social desirability bias. The same was true for the perceived 730 naturalness (p = .957), estimated nutritional value (p = .856), and estimated taste (p = .221). 731 3 Regression analyses resulted in the same conclusion. 732 4 The German word “Frühstücksflocken” was used. This term refers to breakfast cereals and 733 muesli. RI PT EP TE D M AN U SC An analysis of variance, with participants’ social desirability score as a covariate to test for AC C 734 We are aware that in the English language, the word “fructose,” not the phrase “fruit sugar,” 31 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 735 Table 1 736 Nutrition information provided in the “fruit sugar” (nutrition table on the left) and “sugar” 737 (nutrition table on the right) conditions in Experiments 1 and 2. 738 Nutrition value for a portion (30 g) Nutrition value for a portion (30 g) Energy Energy 514.5 kJ 514.5 kJ 3.6 g Carbohydrates Protein 21.6 g - Fruit sugar - Starch Fat Carbohydrates 7.8 g - Sugar 13.8 g - Starch 2.1 g 3.6 g 21.6 g 7.8 g 13.8 g 2.1 g AC C EP TE D M AN U 739 Fat SC Protein 121.8 kcal RI PT 121.8 kcal 32 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 740 Table 2 741 Perceived healthiness of breakfast cereals using the label “fruit sugar” vs. “sugar” in the 742 nutrition table. Sugar M (SD) [N] M (SD) [N] 48.71 (23.82) [73] 40.16 (26.22) [91] 52.01 (20.37) [202] 45.40 (21.21) [202] (between-subjects design) Experiment 2 (within-subjects design) RI PT Experiment 1 Fruit sugar Note. The ratings of perceived healthiness could range from 0 (not healthy at all) to 100 (very 744 healthy). AC C EP TE D M AN U SC 743 33 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 745 Table 3 746 Perceived healthiness, perceived naturalness, estimated nutritional value, and estimated taste 747 of breakfast cereals in the conditions “fruit sugar” vs. “sugar” vs. “fruit sugar & claim” of 748 Experiment 4. Sugar Fruit sugar & claim M (SD) [N] M (SD) [N] M (SD) [N] Perceived healthiness 39.29a (21.50) [52] 29.25b (20.07) [55] 38.58a (21.07) [55] Perceived naturalness 32.60a (18.03) [52] 30.82a (23.47) [55] 35.60a (21.75) [55] Estimated nutritional value 48.15ab (21.57) [52] 41.78b (27.29) [55] 51.93a (25.68) [55] 50.52a (21.76) [52] 50.64a (27.96) [55] 52.93a (20.64) [55] SC Estimated taste RI PT Fruit sugar Note. Different letters indicate significant differences between particular experimental 750 conditions, p < 0.05, using planned comparisons. The ratings of perceived healthiness could 751 range from 0 (not healthy at all) to 100 (very healthy), ratings of perceived naturalness from 0 752 (not natural at all) to 100 (very natural), ratings of estimated nutritional value from 0 (very 753 low) to 100 (very high), and ratings of estimated taste from 0 (very bad) to 100 (very good). AC C EP TE D M AN U 749 34 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 754 Table 4 755 Health-related perceptions associated with the labels “fruit sugar” vs. “sugar” in Experiment 756 4. Fruit sugar Sugar t-test result M (SD) [N] M (SD) [N] 53.63 (19.24) [107] 32.55 (16.68) [55] t(160) = 6.90, p < .001 Estimated nutritional value 56.35 (21.88) [107] 49.47 (28.78) [55] t(160) = 1.70, p = .046 a Symbolic meaning of healthiness 47.33 (23.40) [107] 40.04 (29.44) [55] t(160) = 1.72, p = .044 a RI PT Perceived healthiness associated with consumption Note. a One-tailed p-value is indicated. Participants in the “fruit sugar” and the “fruit sugar & 758 claim” conditions answered the questions related to “fruit sugar,” while participants assigned 759 to the “sugar” condition answered the questions related to “sugar.” The ratings of perceived 760 healthiness could range from 0 (not healthy at all) to 100 (very healthy), ratings of estimated 761 nutritional value from 0 (very low) to 100 (very high), and ratings of the symbolic meaning of 762 healthiness associated with consumption from 0 (very weak symbol) to 100 (very strong 763 symbol). AC C EP TE D M AN U SC 757 35 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 764 Figure 1 765 Information provided in the “fruit sugar” condition in Experiment 3. In the “sugar” condition, 766 the same information was provided, but the label “fruit sugar” was replaced by the label 767 “sugar.” 768 770 Mrs. Meier wants to buy breakfast cereals for her children. M AN U SC 771 RI PT 769 772 773 Mrs. Meier finds the following information on the package: 775 TE D 774 Nutrition value for a portion (30 g) Energy 111.3 kcal Protein 1.4 g 26.1 g - Fruit sugar 11.1 g - Starch 15.0 g Fat 776 AC C Carbohydrates EP 466.0 kJ 0.2 g 36 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 777 Figure 2 778 Picture of the corn flakes package with the claim “contains 100% fruit sugar” (in German: 779 “enthält 100% Fruchtzucker”) on the front, as presented to the participants in the experimental 780 condition “fruit sugar & claim” of Experiment 4. M AN U SC RI PT 781 AC C EP TE D 782 37 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Figure 3 784 Results of the mediation analysis for the impact of the labeling of the ingredient sugar (“fruit 785 sugar” vs. “sugar”) on the perceived healthiness of cereals are shown. Direct effects are 786 presented in (a), and the model with the mediator variable “healthiness associated with 787 labeling” is shown in (b). Nonstandardized coefficients (and standard errors in parentheses) 788 are presented. Non-significant paths are depicted as dotted lines. * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < 789 .001. RI PT 783 AC C 791 EP TE D M AN U SC 790 38 ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Research Highlights • Symbolic information influences people’s health perceptions of foods • Using the label “fruit sugar” instead of “sugar” increases perceived healthiness of foods RI PT • Perceived healthiness associated with ingredients’ labeling affects health perception of foods AC C EP TE D M AN U SC • Health consciousness does not reduce the biasing effect of symbolic information