Exp Brain Res (2000) 130:362–374 Digital Object Identifier (DOI) 10.1007/s002219900262 © Springer-Verlag 2000 R E S E A R C H A RT I C L E M.R. Velicki · C.J. Winstein · P.S. Pohl Impaired direction and extent specification of aimed arm movements in humans with stroke-related brain damage Received: 14 December 1998 / Accepted: 28 September 1999 / Published online: 10 December 1999 Abstract The role of sensorimotor (S-M) areas in the specification of kinematic parameters for aiming movements was studied by comparing the performance of six subjects with unilateral stroke to that of matched control subjects. Rapid arm movements were made to one of four targets by rotating the forearm in a short (20°) or long (45°) arc of motion. Thus, the four targets represented two directions (flexion or extension) and two extents (short or long). Subjects with stroke used the arm ipsilateral to the side of the lesion. A timed-response paradigm was used to dissociate response initiation and specification. Subjects initiated movements in concert with the last of four regularly timed tones. A visual cue of the designated target was presented during the preparation interval (400–0 ms) before the last tone. Targets were presented in a fixed sequence (predictable condition) or a random sequence (unpredictable condition). No significant differences in performance were found between stroke and control groups in the predictable condition. In the unpredictable condition, subjects with stroke produced more direction errors and were less accurate in extent than the control subjects. As specification time increased to 400 ms, the frequency of direction errors attenuated less for stroke than for control groups, but the reduction in magnitude of extent errors was similar for the two groups. When specification was minimal (i.e., <100 ms), default responses were distributed equally between directions and clustered around the short extent. Further, wrong direction responses did not converge on the designated extent as specification time increased. This pattern of findings is consistent with a view of parameterization of planning and executing movements, in M.R. Velicki · C.J. Winstein (✉) Department of Biokinesiology and Physical Therapy, University of Southern California, 1540 East Alcazar St., CHP 155, Los Angeles, CA 90033, USA e-mail: Winstein@hsc.usc.edu Tel.: +1-323-442-2903, Fax: +1-323-442-1515 P.S. Pohl Center on Aging and Department of Physical Therapy Education, University of Kansas Medical Center, Kansas City, KA, USA which direction and extent can be specified in parallel. Our results suggest that ipsilateral S-M areas contribute to the specification of an optimal motor program, particularly when imperative programming of unimanual goaldirected aiming movements is required. Key words Human · Motor control · Arm movements · Stroke · Ipsilateral motor pathways Introduction The purpose of this investigation was to evaluate the role of sensorimotor (S-M) areas in the specification of movement parameters for visually cued goal-directed aiming movements. Specifically, the ipsilateral arm movements of subjects with unilateral cerebral damage were compared with those of healthy matched control subjects to provide insight into the role of ipsilateral S-M areas in the specification of movement direction and extent. Several neurophysiological studies have implicated S-M areas in the specification of direction and extent movement parameters. Georgopoulos et al. (1989) and others have shown that cells in M1 are broadly tuned to movement direction. Posterior parietal area-5 cells have also been shown to be direction sensitive (Kalaska et al. 1983). Neurons in the basal ganglia (globus pallidus) were modulated with movement extent (Georgopoulos et al. 1983). More recently, Kurata (1993) recorded from neurons in the premotor cortex that varied with changes in both extent and direction. Finally, Fu and colleagues (1993) showed that cells in M1 and the superior precentral premotor area modulate activity with extent and direction during arm-reaching movements (see also Fu et al. 1995). Single-cell recordings of cell activity associated with the planning and execution of arm-aiming movements are almost always made from areas in the contralateral hemisphere. However, recent neural imaging studies have shown that, for both healthy subjects and those status-post unilateral stroke, there is increased met- 363 abolic activity in bilateral S-M areas during the performance of unimanual preplanned motor sequences or complex sensory-motor transformations (Cheyne et al. 1995; Chollet et al. 1991; Deiber et al. 1991; Roland et al. 1980; Winstein et al. 1997). Subjects with unilateral cerebral hemisphere damage involving S-M areas have demonstrated deficits in the preparation and execution of programmed actions involving the ipsilateral limb, as evidenced by increased RT (Dee and Van Allen 1973) and decreased accuracy of ballistic or programmed aiming movements (Carmon 1971; Haaland et al. 1987; Winstein et al. 1999; Wyke 1967, 1971) or both (Haaland and Harrington 1989; Tartaglione et al. 1991). These deficits do not simply reflect impaired response execution because performance of the limb ipsilateral to the lesion is relatively normal in conditions that do not require imperative programming. Imperative programming involves the construction of a motor plan under an imposed temporal constraint (Haaland and Harrington 1989; see also Pohl et al. 1997 for a review of ipsilateral limb deficits). Indeed, the neural activation pattern associated with the planning of unimanual actions can involve S-M areas in both hemispheres, while the pattern associated with motor execution may only involve S-M areas in the contralateral hemisphere (Cheyne et al. 1995). It has been suggested that the ipsilateral M1 is not directly involved in activating spinal motoneurons (at least those required to execute distal hand muscles), but is more involved in the planning and higher-order organization of movements (Chen et al. 1997b). This idea is further supported by the greater involvement of the ipsilateral motor areas in complex compared with simple movement sequences, as revealed by positron emission tomography (Dieber et al. 1991; Winstein et al. 1997), functional imaging (Rao et al. 1993; Shibasaki et al. 1993), repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation [rTMS, (Chen et al. 1997a)], and movement-related cortical potential studies (Kitamura et al. 1993). For example, Deiber and colleagues (1991) found that the pattern of regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) differed between a simple conditional response and one where imperative response programming was required. In the imperative programming condition, subjects were required to move a joystick to one of four targets in response to an unpredictable auditory cue. Thus, the movement planning and response preparation had to be completed during the interval between the presentation of the auditory cue and movement onset. Regional cerebral blood flow increased bilaterally in parietal and frontal areas. In contrast, a primarily contralateral neural activation pattern was seen when subjects moved a joystick to one target in response to a predictable auditory cue. In neurologically normal subjects, a timed-response paradigm (TRP) has been used to evaluate the specification of direction and extent of goal-directed actions (Favilla and De Cecco 1996; Favilla et al. 1989, 1990; Ghez et al. 1989; Hening et al. 1988a, 1988b). With this paradigm, RT is manipulated as the independent variable rather than measured as the dependent variable. This allows the specification process to be reflected solely by the accuracy of rapid, goal-directed movement responses. The characteristics of responses produced following various imposed RTs throughout the preparation interval also provide insight into the nature of the parameterspecification process. In these previous studies using a TRP, when targets appeared in a fixed sequence (predictable condition), healthy subjects prepared each response ahead of the movement cue. Response accuracy was high and independent of preparation time. When targets appeared in a random sequence (unpredictable condition), response accuracy was dependent on preparation time and responses gradually became more accurate as preparation time increased. When preparation time was less than 100 ms (forcing specification to be minimal), subjects produced a default response. Using these methods, Favilla and colleagues (1989) demonstrated gradual and parallel specification of direction and extent for aimed force responses. There is some evidence that healthy subjects may not be confined only to a parallel specification strategy. Megaw (1972) noted that healthy subjects performing a RT task appeared to use serial processing to transform direction and extent information. Damage to ipsilateral S-M areas may likely impair the neural activity associated with the planning and higherorder organization of movement (Chen et al. 1997b). In our experiment, a TRP procedure was used to evaluate the role of ipsilateral S-M areas in the parameter-specification process by comparing the responses of subjects with unilateral cerebral-hemisphere damage with those of healthy control subjects. Specifically, we evaluated how adults with chronic stroke, using the limb ipsilateral to the lesion, specified the parameters of extent and direction during the preparation interval prior to the initiation of a rapid, visually cued, goal-directed arm movement. The use of an unpredictable and predictable condition allowed us to distinguish deficits in response specification from deficits in performance of the task itself, which required visual cue identification and response execution. Thus, we investigated whether subjects with stroke would perform differently from controls only in the unpredictable condition, which would indicate a deficit in imperative motor planning, or whether they would also perform differently in the predictable condition, a result that would indicate a difficulty in simply performing the movement task adequately. The use of the TRP also allowed us to evaluate the nature of the specification process. Three possible alterations in the specification process following ipsilateral S-M area damage were considered. First, would subjects with a S-M lesion demonstrate a greater magnitude of extent errors or a greater frequency of direction errors? Second, would the stroke group specify extent and direction in series rather than in parallel? Third, would the stroke group require more time to attain response accuracy than would matched control subjects, thereby altering the time-course for parameter specification? Preliminary results of this work 364 have been reported previously (Velicki et al. 1993; Fisher et al. 1996). Materials and methods Subjects Twelve adults participated in this study: six with a unilateral S-Marea lesion secondary to a stroke involving the anterior circulation system [age=62.7±8.9 years (mean±SD)], and six neurologically healthy control subjects (aged 62.5±8.0 years). Table 1 summarizes the demographic and lesion information for each subject. Each subject in the stroke group demonstrated some motor impairment that affected their functional mobility. They all used an assistive device to ambulate at a household or limited community level. Further, all but subject 10 demonstrated a contralateral hemiparesis and hemisensory loss of varying degrees. The average length of time from onset of the brain damage to participation in the study was (mean±SD) 4.2±2.9 years. Lesion location was obtained from a computer tomography (CT) or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan for each subject (Fig. 1). To determine lesion location, the lesion site was mapped onto the appropriate view in the functional atlas of Domasio and Domasio (1989). Lesion locations were identified for each subject in Table 1 using a classification scheme modified from Kunesch et al. (1995). Areas of central-nervous-system involvement were consistent with the distribution of the middle cerebral artery, including primary sensory and motor cortex (S1, M1), surrounding association areas, underlying white matter (internal capsule), and the subcortical basal ganglia in four cases (see Table 1). To obtain an index of lesion volume, the perimeter of each lesion site was outlined using a hand digitizer, and the area was measured. This area was multiplied by the slice thickness to obtain the volume of each lesion slice. These values were summed to obtain an index of lesion volume (Tartaglione et al. 1991). Each control subject was matched to a subject post-stroke by age, handdominance, and arm used for the task. All subjects passed screening tests for visual field, visual acuity, and hearing and completed an extensive practice session to criteria. Subjects were excluded from participation if they presented with evidence of posterior circulation involvement or if bilateral lesion areas were present on CT or MRI scan. Each subject received $60 for participation. Apparatus and task The experimental apparatus consisted of an instrumented manipulandum, IBM compatible CPU, and monitor. The manipulandum was a lightweight aluminum lever, which was mounted on the corner of a table with a near frictionless axle such that the lever was free to move in the horizontal plane above the table surface. A vertically oriented handgrip at the distal end of the lever was adjusted to accommodate the length of each subject’s forearm. A wooden cover was mounted over the table and lever to prevent the subject from visually monitoring the lever position. A computer monitor was positioned in front of the subject at eye level. The distance between the computer monitor and the subject ranged between 42 and 62 cm and was determined as the Table 1 Subject and lesion characteristics. CVA Cerebrovascular accident, M male, F female, R right, L left Subject ID Subject match Sex Age (years) Hand dominance Etiology Side CVA Lesion duration (years) Lesion size (cm3) Lesion location 7 8 9 10 11 12 F F M F M F M 54 61 67 56 61 76 51 R R R R L R R – – – – – – Infarct – – – – – – R – – – – – – 5.0 – – – – – – 11.5 8 F 53 R Infarct R 2.8 50.6 9 M 71 R Infarct R 5.1 83.7 10 11 F M 69 62 R L Hemorrhage Infarct L L 0.8 2.2 2.2 64.2 12 M 70 R Hemorrhage L 9.3 51.6 – – – – – – Primary motor cortex with subcortical frontal extensions Striatocapsular + cortex; primary sensorimotor cortical infarct with subcortical extensions; extensions into temporal association area Striatocapsular + cortex; primary sensorimotor cortical infarct with subcortical extensions; some extension into prefrontal and temporal association areas Posterior parietal cortex only Striatocapsular + cortex; primary sensorimotor cortical infarct with subcortical extensions; minor extensions into temporal and prefrontal association areas Striatocapsular + cortex; primary sensorimotor cortical areas with subcortical frontal/parietal extensions; extension into parietal and temporal association areas 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 365 Fig. 1 Lesion location for each stroke-group subject from magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) or computer tomography (CT) images. Each section shows the lesion-site area of greatest extent using the functional atlas of Domasio and Domasio (1989) distance where the subject could comfortably read messages displayed on the monitor. The lever was interfaced with an A-to-D board (Keithly/Metrabyte) installed in a 486-based, 50 MHz IBM-compatible computer. Custom ASYST software (Vanman 1992) was used for the control of data acquisition, data analysis, and storage. The analog signal from the lever potentiometer was sampled at 250 Hz and converted to digital without filtering. Each trial was 1000 ms in duration and consisted of A-D sampling of the lever potentiometer beginning 100 ms before the movement cue. Movement time (MT) was determined from movement onset to offset. Movement onset (start of movement) was defined as the onset of a 20 ms interval (moving window) in which the A-D values were changed from baseline (baseline break) and were consistently > or <3–5× the calibrated resting SD, or approximately ±0.5° change. Movement endpoint was defined as the onset of a 20-ms interval in which the A-D values did not change greater than 5× the calibrated resting SD. Procedure Each subject read and signed an Institutional Review Board-approved informed consent form. Subjects performing with the right arm sat to the left of the lever axis and those performing with the left arm sat to the right of the lever axis. Subjects post-stroke performed with the arm ipsilateral to the lesion. When the lever was placed in the home (or start) position, the subject was positioned in approximately 45° shoulder flexion and abduction and 70° elbow flexion. The task involved moving the lever from this home position to one of four target locations produced by mixing two directions and two extents. Subjects flexed or extended the elbow in either a short arc of motion (20° from the home position) or a long arc of motion (45° from the home position). The TRP is summarized in Fig. 2A. The onset of lever movement was to be timed with the last in a series of four tones. The computer presented a series of four audible tones, each with a progressively higher pitch, increasing from 625 to 1300 Hz. The tones each had 50 ms duration and were spaced by an inter-tone interval of 500 ms. Prior to the fourth tone, a yellow arrow appeared and pointed to the designated target. This movement cue was randomly presented before the fourth tone within one of the four preparation time bins (1=0–99 ms, 2=100–199 ms, 3=200–299 ms, and 4=300–400 ms). Preparation time bins were used to ensure that an adequate number of acceptable trials was collected throughout the preparation interval for subsequent analysis (see Data analysis). The first bin was selected because the default response was produced following these latencies in previous TRP studies (Favilla and De Cecco 1996; Favilla et al. 1989, 1990; Ghez et al. 1989; Hening et al. 1988a, 1988b). The remaining time was divided into three bins to allow for an assessment of change over the full preparation interval and to ensure that each bin had an adequate number of trials to be representative of the time period (see Data analysis). Prior to data collection, all subjects completed a three-phase practice session. Subjects were trained to: (1) accurately move to each target location without time constraints, (2) initiate responses within a timing window surrounding the fourth tone, and (3) move the lever quickly without final adjustments. Subjects practiced until 70% of the trials to each target were performed within preset criteria. The criteria for accuracy, initiation time, and MT were gradually narrowed over the practice phase to challenge the subject’s capability. At the completion of the practice phase, all subjects were able to: (1) move to within 5° of each target, (2) move within a timing window of at most 95 ms before and 95 ms after the tone, and (3) perform a rapid arm movement to each target with a MT of less than 500 ms. To complete the practice period, subjects required between 1 and 6 days and between 1.4 and 11.2 h of practice. The number of practice trials was greater for the stroke group, but this difference was not reliable between stroke (2282±873) and control groups (data available for only five of the six control subjects: 1301±832), Student’s t-test (t=–1.9, P=0.09). The trial sequence depicted in Fig. 2A was used throughout the third phase of practice and the subsequent experimental test phase. At the beginning of a trial, the subject viewed the instructions: “Return to home” displayed on the monitor. The subject used the real-time position of a red pointer on the monitor as feedback to move the lever to the home position. When the subject held a steady arm position within 2.5° of the home position for 1 s, the red pointer disappeared and a “Ready” cue appeared on the monitor. Then, the series of four tones began. Sometime between the third and fourth tone, a yellow arrow (movement cue) appeared and pointed to one of the four targets. Throughout the movement, the yellow arrow continued to point to the correct target. Following the movement, the red pointer reappeared and was fixed at the end position of the movement 366 Fig. 2 A The modified timedresponse paradigm. Movement initiation was to be synchronized with the last in a series of four tones (1–4). The movement cue was presented at variable times between 400 and 0 ms before the fourth tone. The time-course of movement cue and feedback presentation is shown below. The stimulustone (S-T) interval, the stimulus-response (S-R) interval, and the timing error [(S-R interval) – (S-T interval)] are shown above. B The written feedback displayed when movement was initiated at various times throughout the trial sequence. This example shows a timing window of 95 ms before and 95 ms after the fourth tone response. Visual feedback was provided regarding movement initiation (“too early”, “too late”) and MT (“too slow”) in relation to pre-established criteria (see below for explanation). Following this feedback, the cue, “Return to home” was displayed on the monitor, and the next trial sequence began. Subjects controlled the inter-trial interval by the rate at which they returned to the home position. Because it is difficult to initiate movement exactly with the fourth tone, acceptable trials were those initiated within an acceptable timing window (Fig. 2A). The stimulus-tone (S-T) interval was the time between movement cue and the onset of the fourth tone; it was the preparation time forced by the paradigm. The stimulus-response (S-R) interval was the time between movement cue and movement onset, the preparation time actually used by the subject. The timing error [(S-T) – (S-R)] reflected the accuracy of movement initiation time. Subjects were provided with visual feedback to facilitate the timing of movement initiation within the acceptable timing window (Fig. 2B). A trial was not included in the analysis if the subject anticipated and moved before the movement cue (i.e., a negative S-R interval). This kind of timing error constituted less than 9% of all acceptable responses per subject. In addition, acceptable trials had to be completed within a preestablished MT criteria. A hierarchy of trial errors was used such that “too slow” was provided only after trials in which movements were initiated within the timing window criteria, but the MT was greater than the MT criteria. The initial MT criterion for an acceptable trial was <450 ms. As each subject demonstrated the capability to move faster, (i.e., very few “too slow” errors) the criterion MT was decreased. Actual MTs were much shorter than criterion (control: 262±52 ms, stroke: 315±63 ms). As seen in Fig. 3, in three of the four different preparation time bins, subjects in both groups were able to produce rapid responses without overt corrections during the movement. Sixty-four acceptable trials constituted a “block”. Unacceptable trials were tallied, but not used for analyses. Trial blocks were differentiated by two conditions. In the predictable condition, targets 1, 2, 3, and 4 were presented in serial order, and, in the unpredictable condition, targets were presented in a pseudo-randomized order (e.g., target 2, 4, 1, 3). Initially, each subject performed 11 trial blocks: three predictable condition blocks (number 1, 6, 11) and eight unpredictable condition blocks (number 2–5, 7–10). Inherent in the TRP is the precise control over the preparation interval. This is accomplished by imposing a movement start time (fourth tone). However, precise timing of movement onset is a difficult task for most subjects, even for those without neural deficits. Therefore, we established an acceptable window around the fourth tone (timing window) that was used during data collection (see explanation above). Subjects may, however, systematically adjust the movement start time within the acceptable timing window. For example, subjects could move late when the preparation interval is short, or move early when the preparation interval is long, resulting in a high negative correlation between timing error and S-T interval. To control for this, after each successfully completed block, the subject was shown a “timing error” × “S-T interval” plot along with the Pearson correlation coefficient. The goal was to complete each block with the smallest correlation possible. Subjects repeated trial blocks until they produced 11 blocks similar to their best (i.e., lowest) correlation achieved during the testing period (average block timing-error correlation range by subject r=0.41 to –0.07). In order to ensure that subjects were not disregarding response accuracy to meet these timing demands, a second plot of “absolute error (AE)” × “S-T interval” was displayed. Acceptable trial blocks were those with a significant accuracy correlation (r>0.20) in the unpredictable condition (average block accuracy-correlation range by subject r=–0.53 to –0.28). Trial blocks chosen for analysis where those that met the timing and accuracy criteria for the unpredictable condition (eight “best” blocks) and the timing criteria for the predictable condition (three “best” blocks). 367 Fig. 3 Individual trial responses for one control subject (A) and one stroke subject (B). For all plots, the four targets (long dashed lines) at ±20° and ±45° are as displayed in the top plot in A; extension movements are positive, and flexion movements are negative. Time 0 is the onset of trial sampling 100 ms before movement-cue presentation. The three rows represent responses in three of the four preparation intervals. Top rows are responses to each of the four targets with <100 ms preparation interval; middle rows are responses with between 100–199 ms preparation interval; bottom rows are responses with between 300–400 ms preparation interval. Short dashed lines are the short target movements, and solid lines are the long target movements Note that the aforementioned procedures included modifications to the standard TRP protocol (e.g., Favilla et al. 1990) necessary to assure that acceptable trials were those where the preparation interval was most effectively controlled. In summary, (1) for the practice phase, a minimum criteria were used for accuracy and timing error; (2) for the test phase, specific criteria were used for trial acceptance; (3) timing-error and -accuracy scatter plots and correlations were displayed as feedback after each completed block of trials (J. Gordon, personal communication); and (4) these correlation coefficients were used to choose trial blocks for analysis. Data analysis Three dependent variables were computed: (1) constant error (CE), (movement offset – target position), (2) absolute error (AE), i.e., |CE|, and (3) direction error, i.e., the number of responses in the opposite direction of the intended target. For wrong direction responses, AE was recalculated using the correct target extent in the direction of the response (i.e., the virtual target). This was done in order to dissociate the error due to direction specification from that due to extent specification. Each block had 16 different trial types, resulting from a combination of the four targets, and the four S-R interval bins. Each block contained approximately four trials of each trial type. The number of trials included in each S-R bin varied somewhat as a result of acceptable timing errors. For each subject, the mean AE was calculated for acceptable trials with the same trial type and block condition. The mean AE of each trial type was calculated from approximately 32 trials across the eight blocks in the unpredictable condition and approximately 12 trials across the three blocks in the predictable condition. To produce a full complement of acceptable test trials (i.e., 704 trials with specific S-T interval, target, and condition characteristics), each subject performed between 12 and 21 blocks. Each subject returned to the lab between 4 and 8 days and performed between two and four blocks each day. In all, each subject worked between 6.3 and 13.4 h and performed between 2196 and 4455 trials. There were no differences between stroke and control group means for either number of hours or trials in which they participated (Students t test: t=–0.62, –1.93, P>0.05, respectively). Statistical analyses were used to evaluate the time course of extent and direction specification for the stroke and control groups. To evaluate extent specification, a mixed model analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used with AE as the dependent variable. For analysis, the between-subject factor was group (stroke, control) and the two within-subject factors were target (1, 2, 3, 4) and S-R bin (1, 2, 3, 4). Post-hoc orthogonal contrasts were performed on significant effects from the ANOVA to determine the locus of the effect. To determine direction specification, a stepwise logistical regression with main (group and S-R bin) and interaction terms (group × S-R bin) was used with the frequency of wrong direction responses as the dependent variable. 368 Fig. 4 A Bar graph of mean absolute error (AE) (±SEM) for responses within each stimulusresponse (S-R) interval for each group in the predictable condition. B Bar graph of mean AE (±SEM) for responses within each S-R interval for the unpredictable and predictable conditions. Data are averaged across 12 subjects and four targets. C Bar graph of the mean AE (±SEM) for responses to the long and short extent targets within each S-R interval in the unpredictable condition. Data are averaged across 12 subjects. D Bar graph of mean AE (±SEM) for responses to the long and short extent targets in the unpredictable and predictable conditions. Data are averaged across 12 subjects. Mean AE (±SEM) long targets: unpredictable condition =12±1.0°, predictable condition =9.0±0.9°; short targets: unpredictable condition =5.4±0.4°, predictable condition =5.3±0.2° Results Target effect in predictable and unpredictable conditions Predictable-condition responses are described first by comparing between groups and then for all subjects. This is followed by a description of the unpredictable-condition responses first for correct and then for wrong direction responses. General response characteristics are described first for all subjects, followed by the group differences that highlight the contribution of the ipsilateral S-M areas in the movement parameter-specification process. A target effect was obtained for AE in both the predictable condition, P=0.047, and the unpredictable condition, P=0.0002. As expected, responses to the shortextent targets were more accurate [range: 4.6±0.4° (mean±SEM) to 6.2±0.5°] than those to the long-extent targets (11.2±1.0° to 13.1±1.1°) for both correct and wrong-direction responses. Post-hoc orthogonal contrasts revealed that mean AE was similar for targets of the same extent, P>0.05, and different for targets of different extents, P<0.05, in the unpredictable condition. Therefore, for all further analyses, responses to each of the two similar-extent targets were combined. Predictable-condition responses The predictable condition was the control condition providing a measure of how well subjects performed the task without the imperative programming demand. The stroke group demonstrated slightly higher extent errors (~3°) than the control group across all response intervals (Fig. 4A); however, this difference was not reliable for response accuracy, (P=0.12). All subjects preplanned their responses before the movement cue, therefore, response accuracy (P=0.22) was not dependent on preparation time (Fig. 4B, striped bars). Direction errors were rare, contributing to only 1% of the total responses in this condition. Unpredictable condition Extent specification of correct direction responses All subjects. In the unpredictable condition, as the S-R interval increased, all subjects were able to use movement-cue information to improve response accuracy. A conclusion that can be drawn from this is that, with longer preparation intervals, subjects planned their responses within the S-R interval. For correct direction responses, accuracy was dependent on the imposed preparation time, P<0.00001, (Figs. 3, 4B, dark bars). Post-hoc orthogonal contrasts revealed that accuracy increased be- 369 Fig. 6 A Bar graph of mean absolute error (AE) (±SEM) for responses to the long and short extent targets for the stroke and control groups. Mean AE (±SEM) long targets: stroke =15.4±2.4°, control =8.8±1.5°; short targets: stroke =5.5±0.8°, control =5.3±1.0°. B Bar graph with group mean AE (±SEM) within each stimulus-response (S-R) interval for correct direction responses Fig. 5 A Scatter plot of constant error (CE) for a full complement of correct direction responses for S1 (control subject) and (B) S8 (stroke subject) in the unpredictable condition. The horizontal line at 0° represents the target. For these two subjects, the default position was the short target extent. C Scatter plot as in A and B for S4 (control subject) in the unpredictable condition. For this subject, the default position was between the short and long target extents. S-R Stimulus-response interval tween the first and second S-R intervals (P=0.001) and between the second and third S-R intervals (P=0.002), but not between the third and fourth S-R intervals (P=0.32) (Figs. 3, 4B). Thus, response accuracy did not improve when preparation time was 300 ms or greater. For all subjects, responses to the short targets were relatively accurate regardless of preparation time, whereas responses to the long targets increased in accuracy as preparation time increased (Figs. 3, 4C). This resulted in a significant target × S-R-interval effect for AE, P=0.004. These findings suggest that the short target extent was programmed in advance as the default and the long target extent was specified from the default extent as the S-R interval increased. In fact, AE for responses to the short extent targets was similar to that found in the predictable condition, whereas AE for responses to the long extent targets was somewhat higher in the unpredictable condition than in the predictable condition (Fig. 4D). Indeed, 10 of the 12 subjects used the short target extent as the default and specified only the long target extent during the S-R interval (see, for example, Fig. 5A, B). The remaining two subjects (S4 and S5, both control) used a default position in-between the short and long target extents and appeared to specify both short and long target extents during the S-R interval (see, for example, Fig. 5C). Group differences. Absolute error to the long targets was greater for the stroke group than the control group, P<0.05, whereas AE to the short targets was similar between the groups, P=0.05 (Fig. 6A). This finding suggests that subjects in the stroke group were as capable as control subjects of specifying the default extent in advance of the stimulus, but had difficulty scaling that default response to the long extent within the imposed S-R interval (Fig. 3). However, the time course of extent 370 Fig. 7 A Scatter plot of constant error (CE) for a full complement of responses to the long extent targets in the unpredictable condition for S2 (control subject) and (B) S7 (stroke subject). The horizontal line at 0° represents the target. Note that wrong-direction responses (WDR bracketed) are clustered at the default extent (~–65°). Only correct-direction long-extent responses are progressively scaled from the default to the correct target extent as stimulus-response (S-R) interval increases specification was similar for both groups, P=0.75 (Fig. 6B). Extent specification of wrong direction responses All subjects. Wrong direction responses for both the short and long targets were positioned near the short target extent, resulting in AE to the virtual target of 6.4±0.24° (mean±SEM) for responses to short targets and 21.5±0.33° for responses to long targets; the latter approximated the difference between the short and long targets. Wrong direction responses did not converge on the correct target extent as preparation time increased. Responses to long target extents in the correct direction were progressively scaled to the correct target extent, while responses in the wrong direction remained near the default extent regardless of preparation time (Fig. 7A, B). For all subjects, mean AE remained relatively stable across preparation intervals, P=0.14 (Fig. 8A). Group differences. Overall, the stroke group produced responses with a larger AE [15.5±0.42° (mean±SEM)] Fig. 8 A Bar graph of mean absolute error (AE) (±SEM) to the virtual target for wrong-direction responses within each stimulusresponse (S-R) interval. Values represent mean responses of all 12 subjects in the unpredictable condition. B Bar graph of mean AE (SEM) to the virtual target for each target (short, long) and group (C control, S stroke) within each S-R interval. C Frequency of direction errors as a percent of the total responses within each S-R interval for the stroke and control groups in the unpredictable condition than did the control group (12.2±0.34°), P<0.0001. Further, as the preparation interval increased, control subjects showed minimal scaling of extent, for only long target responses. In contrast, subjects with stroke did not scale response extent as preparation interval increased for either target (Fig. 8B). This group difference in wrong-direction extent errors was reliable, as evidenced by a triple interaction between group, target, and preparation interval (P<0.03). Although the control group showed some capability to scale these wrong-direction responses to the long target extent, that response was markedly truncated (~4.7° change across preparation interval, see open bars in Fig. 8B) compared with that for correct direction responses (~10.7° change across preparation intervals). 371 Direction specification All subjects. Overall, approximately one-fourth of all responses were in the opposite direction of the designated target. The frequency of direction errors decreased as preparation time increased from 0 to 400 ms, χ23=534.13, P<0.0001 (Fig. 8C). If direction was not specified, the chances of moving in either of the two possible directions should have been equal. In fact, when preparation time was <100 ms and specification was expected to be minimal, the percentage of responses in the wrong direction was close to 50% for both groups (Fig. 8C). Alternatively, when preparation time was greater than 300 ms, the frequency of wrong direction responses for all subjects was less than 10% of all responses within that S-R interval. Group differences. Overall, across all preparation intervals, the stroke group was 20% more likely to make an error in direction than the control group, (χ2=7.287, P<0.008). Figure 8C shows the group differences in direction specification across the four preparation intervals. For the shortest interval (<100 ms), the stroke group was 1.2× (20%) more likely to make a direction error than the control group. For the longest interval (300–400 ms), the group differences were greater, with the stroke group 1.9× (90%) more likely to make a direction error than the control group. More importantly and in contrast to extent specification, the time course for direction specification was significantly different for the two groups [χ2=34.14, P<0.0001). Figure 8C shows that the reduction in frequency of direction errors proceeded more gradually for the stroke group (solid bars) than for the control group (open bars) over the preparation interval. The group difference in the rate of decrease in the frequency of direction errors was greatest between the second (100–199 ms) and third (200–299 ms) preparation intervals (Fig. 8C). The control subjects showed a 30% reduction in the number of direction errors, while those with stroke only showed a 19% reduction in the number of direction errors over the same preparation time (i.e., 100–299 ms). This group difference in the time course shows that the added preparation time (especially >200 ms) was more beneficial for control subjects than for those with stroke of the ipsilateral hemisphere. Discussion The TRP allowed an examination of the nature of the processes by which visually cued, goal-directed arm movements are specified in advance of movement. With the TRP approach, we found clear evidence of programming deficits in individuals with ipsilateral S-M area brain damage; these deficits impaired both movement extent and direction accuracy. Subjects with unilateral SM area damage were less accurate primarily in the unpredictable condition where imperative planning was required. In the predictable condition when specification time was not constrained, movements using the ipsilateral limb were nearly as accurate as those of control subjects. Therefore, deficits in movement-parameter specification observed in the unpredictable condition were related to the preparation phase and not a reflection of impairments in visual cue identification, movement initiation/execution, or stimulus-response transformation processes. These findings will be discussed first in relation to our three proposed alterations in the specification process and, second, with respect to the role of the ipsilateral S-M areas in programming of goal-directed actions. Consistent with the first suggested alteration, when time to prepare was limited, subjects with stroke produced more direction errors and were consistently less accurate at specifying the long target extent. When the time for imperative response planning was constrained to <400 ms, both extent and direction specification were compromised for those with ipsilateral S-M area damage. Our second proposed alteration suggested that, if imperative planning challenges the resources for parameter specification after ipsilateral S-M area damage, one expected and parsimonious solution would be to invoke serial rather than parallel specification of direction and extent. In general, both groups showed a similar and gradual specification of extent and direction (see Figs. 6B and 8C), which suggests a predominantly parallel specification process. One surprising finding in this regard was that, for both groups, extent specification of wrong direction responses was either truncated or aborted, as these responses did not fully converge on the correct extent as preparation time increased. While this could be interpreted as suggesting a serial specification strategy, it alone does not suffice to dismiss a parallel specification scheme. Our results suggest that at least partial specification of extent occurred before direction was completely specified (i.e., wrong direction responses were specified at the short target extent, see Fig. 7). Indeed, it is possible that parallel specification is the default control strategy for both groups and that it is aborted when a direction error is detected (see Fig. 8B). In previous TRP investigations, young, healthy adults showed a parallel specification of direction and extent of isometric force pulses (Favilla et al. 1989, 1990; Ghez et al. 1989; Hening 1988a). Responses in both the correct and incorrect directions converged on the target extent as preparation time increased. Recently, Favilla and De Cecco (1996) showed that the time course of direction specification of a reaching movement (two directions and two extents) in healthy adults was not influenced by the need to simultaneously specify extent. While their results support a model of parallel and independent channel processing for direction and extent, it does not preclude the possibility that extent scaling was completed after movement onset. Indeed, Fu et al. (1995) recently showed a temporal parcellation of direction and extent specification in the activity of M1 and PM cortex cells in primates. Consistent with Fu et al. (1995) and others, there is support in the literature for a serial and hierarchi- 372 cal movement-parameter selection process (Fu et al. 1993; Megaw 1972; Riehle and Requin 1989). Neural activity was recorded as monkeys performed arm-aiming movements to targets with different direction and extent characteristics. Directionally selective neurons in PM cortex were active during the preparation period prior to movement. During this preparation period, very few neurons were recorded which clearly processed prior information about movement extent. During movement, M1 and PM activity was correlated with both direction and extent (Fu et al. 1993). In addition, when the monkeys were provided with advance information about the correct movement direction, RT decreased more than when they were provided with information about the correct movement extent (Riehle and Requin 1989). One critical issue and possible explanation for the discrepancy in these findings is the MT across these studies. If MT is quite long, extent can be scaled and corrections made during the movement with only a rudimentary or partial specification made during the preparation interval. In contrast, if MT is quite short, a more complete specification is necessary during the imperative preparation interval. In our case, the fact that all subjects were hypometric with short S-R intervals (see Fig. 5) suggests that only partial specification was made during the imperative interval. Although parallel specification of kinematic parameters is possible and most studies support such a strategy, it might not be optimal under all task conditions. In our study, the perceived penalty for direction errors may have been greater than extent errors when subjects were producing observable arm movements (as opposed to isometric force pulses). Also, the MT was considerably longer (~300 ms) under these movement conditions than for the force-pulse conditions (~100 ms) used in previous work. In a subsequent paper, we have applied an early measure of response magnitude similar to that used by Ghez and colleagues to ferret out the degree to which preplanned specification and later compensatory adjustments contribute to final position (Fisher BE, Winstein CJ, Velicki MR, submitted). We find that those with ipsilateral S-M area deficits are more dependent on the preplanned specification of extent than are the healthy control subjects, who can also correct on-line to achieve a more accurate final position (Fisher et al. 1998). The group difference in the time course for direction, but not extent specification is an important finding for two reasons. First, it provides further support for the independent specification of extent and direction, and, second, it highlights a potential role of the ipsilateral S-M areas in the movement parameter specification process. This first point will be discussed in relation to our third possible alteration in the parameter-specification process. The potential role of the ipsilateral S-M areas in this process will be discussed in the last section. Our third proposed alteration was that the stroke group would demonstrate a different time course compared with that for the control group. As described above, this prediction held up for direction specification, but not extent specification. Our results show that the time course for direction specification was different between groups, while that for extent specification was similar. Such a difference in time course for extent and direction provides further support for a model of semi-independent processing of movement parameters (cf. Favilla and De Cecco 1996). Using the TRP paradigm, the time course naturally involves the pre-movement interval. If direction specification primarily utilizes this pre-movement time frame and extent specification can also use the interval after movement onset, then a group difference in direction-, but not extent-parameter specification for the pre-movement interval would be more likely than if both parameters were fully specified during the exact same time interval. The more gradual specification time course of direction for the stroke group, especially during the 100– 300 ms pre-movement interval, compared with that for the control group suggests a potential role for the ipsilateral S-M areas in the chronometrics of imperative programming (see Fig. 8C). Chen et al. (1997a) induced timing errors in both simple and complex piano sequences using rTMS of the ipsilateral M1 on either side, suggesting involvement of the ipsilateral motor cortex in processing of complex motor programs. We now focus the discussion on the role of the ipsilateral S-M areas in the planning and organization of goaldirected actions. Independent of the time course, the error in extent and the frequency of direction errors were greater in the stroke group than the control group. What could possibly explain these findings in imperative programming in the arm ipsilateral to the lesion? While the answer is speculative, we offer a few logical and testable hypotheses. The first explanation is based on the assumption that the ipsilateral hemisphere is important for imperative programming and considers what the nature of the contribution might be. The second explanation is based on the assumption that the contribution of the ipsilateral hemisphere to imperative programming is disturbed as a result of the negative effects (i.e., artifact) of plasticity triggered by the brain damage itself. First, anatomical data indicate that the ipsilateral motor pathway from M1 to the spinal cord projects primarily to axial and proximal muscles (Colebatch and Gandevia 1989). Indeed, there is little evidence for direct ipsilateral connections from M1 to distal hand muscles. However, functional imaging and physiological studies including rTMS show that ipsilateral M1 is involved in the performance of complex finger movements (e.g., Chen et al. 1997a; Kim et al. 1993). One interpretation of this apparent discrepancy is that ipsilateral M1 is not directly involved in activating spinal motoneurons required to execute these movements, but is more involved in the planning and the higher-order organization of movements (Chen et al. 1997b; Haaland and Harrington 1990). This role for the ipsilateral hemisphere is consistent with our findings of significant group differences for the unpredictable (complex, imperative) condition and not the predictable (simple) condition where imperative planning is likely not required. 373 A second possibility is that the ipsilateral effect is a by-product of plasticity in the unaffected hemisphere. If the unaffected hemisphere after stroke shows plastic changes in motor output organization, one negative effect of this plasticity could be a decreased or disordered organization of the contralateral (affected) or ipsilateral (unaffected) S-M areas involved in motor planning and higherorder organization. Such a possibility could explain the findings in this study and might be the result of cortical reorganization similar to that described in cases of repetitive strain injury (e.g., Byl et al. 1996). Indeed, there is some suggestion that ipsilateral motor pathways may play a role in functional recovery from stroke (Chollet et al. 1991; Lee and van Donkelaar 1995; Lemon 1993). In two positron emission tomography (PET) studies, the stroke group had significantly increased rCBF in the ipsilateral SM1 with movement of the affected hand, but not the unaffected hand (Chollet et al. 1991; Weiller et al. 1992). However, in a later report from the same group, the patients with the ipsilateral SM1 activation also had mirror movements in the unaffected hand when they moved the affected hand (Weiller et al. 1993). Several studies using TMS found no evidence that the ipsilateral fast corticospinal tract was responsible for recovery of affected limb function (Netz et al. 1997; Palmer et al. 1992; Turton et al. 1996). Indeed, ipsilateral responses with TMS of the undamaged hemisphere were more common among patients who had poor recovery than in those patients with good recovery (e.g., Turton et al. 1996). While there is evidence that the motor outputs in the unaffected hemisphere are significantly changed after stroke (e.g., Netz et al. 1997), these pathways seem to be of little significance for recovery, as the existence of these ipsilateral responses are not correlated with clinical improvement. In summary, the role of the ipsilateral S-M areas in the parameter specification process was revealed in a condition of imperative programming using the TRP protocol. Subjects with unilateral stroke-related brain damage were consistently less accurate than age-matched control subjects in the specification of movement extent and direction for ipsilateral arm aiming movements. In addition, the time course for direction, but not extent specification was altered in the stroke group compared with the control group. Together, the group differences here and in previous work (cf. Winstein and Pohl 1995; Winstein et al. 1999) strongly suggests that the ipsilateral S-M areas contribute both to the specification of an optimal plan or task-relevant parameter set (i.e., motor program) and, to some degree, the chronometrics (i.e., time-course) of the specification process. Acknowledgements We thank Karen Altman, MD for reading the MRI and CT scans; Jim Gordon for valuable comments on an earlier version of this paper and for suggesting the particular timing error by S-R interval feedback plots; John L. Morrison for statistical consultation; Beth Fisher for characterizing the lesion locations from the brain scans; and all the participants, especially the stroke group, for their time and effort toward this study. Funding for this project was provided in part from a grant to CJW and MRV from the California Physical Therapy Fund. References Byl NN, Merzenich MM, Jenkins WM (1996) A primate genesis model of focal dystonia and repetitive strain injury. I. Learning-induced dedifferentiation of the representation of the hand in the primary somatosensory cortex in adult monkeys. Neurology 47:508–520 Carmon A (1971) Sequenced motor performance in patients with unilateral cerebral lesions. Neuropsychologia 9:445–449 Chen R, Gerloff C, Hallett M, Cohen LG (1997a) Involvement of the ipsilateral motor cortex in finger movements of different complexities. Ann Neurol 41:247–254 Chen R, Cohen LG, Hallett M (1997b) Role of the ipsilateral motor cortex in voluntary movement. Can J Neurol Sci 24:284– 291 Cheyne D, Weinberg H, Gaetz W, Jantzen KJ (1995) Motor cortex activity and predicting side of movement: neural network and dipole analysis of pre-movement magnetic fields. Neurosci Lett 188:81–84 Chollet F, DiPiero V, Wise RJ, Brooks DJ, Dolan RJ, Frackowiak RS (1991) The functional anatomy of motor recovery after stroke in humans: a study with positron emission tomography. Ann Neurol 29:63–71 Colebatch JG, Gandevia SC (1989) The distribution of muscular weakness in upper motor neuron lesions affecting the arm. Brain 112:749–763 Dee HL, Van Allen MW (1973) Speed of decision-making processes in patients with unilateral cerebral disease. Arch Neurol 28:163–166 Deiber MP, Passingham RE, Colebatch JG, Friston KJ, Nixon PD, Frackowiak RSJ (1991) Cortical areas and the selection of movement: a study with positron emission tomography. Exp Brain Res 84:393–402 Domasio H, Domasio AR (1989) Lesion analysis in neuropsychology. Oxford University Press, New York Favilla M, De Cecco E (1996) Parallel direction and extent specification of planar reaching arm movements in humans. Neuropsychologia 34:609–613 Favilla M, Hening W, Ghez C (1989) Trajectory control in targeted force impulses. VI. Independent specification of response amplitude and direction. Exp Brain Res 75:280–294 Favilla M, Gordon J, Hening W, Ghez C (1990) Trajectory control in targeted force impulses. VII. Independent setting of amplitude and direction in response preparation. Exp Brain Res 79:530–538 Fisher BE, Winstein CJ, Velicki MR (1996) Evidence for feedforward processing in the control of rapid aiming when time to program is constrained. Soc Neurosci Abstr 22:2041 Fisher BE, Winstein CJ, Velicki MR (1998) Amplitude scaling in rapid aiming movements is impaired after unilateral sensorimotor area damage. Soc Neurosci Abstr 24:1663 Fu QG, Suarez JI, Ebner TJ (1993) Neuronal specification of direction and distance during reaching movements in the superior precentral premotor area and primary motor cortex of monkeys. J Neurophysiol 70:2097–2116 Fu QG, Flament D, Coltz JD, Ebner TJ (1995) Temporal encoding of movement kinematics in the discharge of primate primary motor and premotor neurons. J Neurophysiol 73:836–854 Georgopoulos AP, DeLong MR, Crutcher MD (1983) Relations between parameters of step-tracking movements and single cell discharge in the globus pallidus and subthalamic nucleus of the behaving monkey. J Neurosci 3:1586–1598 Georgopoulos AP, Crutcher MD, Schwartz AB (1989) Cognitive spatial-motor processes. 3. Motor cortical prediction of movement direction during an instructed delay period. Exp Brain Res 75:183–194 Ghez C, Hening W, Favilla M (1989) Gradual specification of response amplitude in human tracking performance. Brain Behav Evolut 33:69–74 Haaland KY, Harrington DL (1989) Hemispheric control of the initial and corrective components of aiming movements. Neuropsychologia 27:961–969 374 Haaland KY, Harrington DL (1990) Complex movement behavior: toward understanding cortical and subcortical interactions in regulating control processes. In: Hammond GE (ed) Cerebral control of speech and limb movements. Elsevier Science, Amsterdam, pp 169–200 Haaland KY, Harrington DL, Yeo R (1987) The effects of task complexity on motor performance in left and right CVA patients. Neuropsychologia 25:783–794 Hening W, Favilla M, Ghez C (1988a) Trajectory control in targeted force impulses. V. Gradual specification of response amplitude. Exp Brain Res 71:116–128 Hening W, Vicario D, Ghez C (1988b) Trajectory control in targeted force impulses. IV. Influences of choice prior experience and urgency. Exp Brain Res 71:103–115 Kalaska JF, Caminiti R, Georgopoulos AP (1983) Cortical mechanisms related to the direction of two-dimensional arm movements: relations in parietal area 5 and comparison with motor cortex. Exp Brain Res 51:247–260 Kim SG, Ashe J, Hendrich K, Ellermann JM, Merkle H, Ugurbil K, Georgopoulos AP (1993) Functional magnetic resonance imaging of motor cortex: hemispheric asymmetry and handedness. Science 261:615–617 Kitamura JI, Shibasaki H, Takagi A, Nabeshima H, Yamaguchi A (1993) Enhanced negative slope of cortical potentials before sequential as compared with simultaneous extensions before sequential as compared with simultaneous extensions of two fingers. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 86:176–182 Kunesch E, Binkofski F, Steinmetz H, Freund HJ (1995) The pattern of motor deficits in relation to the site of stroke lesions. Eur Neurol 35:20–26 Kurata K (1993) Premotor cortex of monkeys: set- and movementrelated activity reflecting amplitude and direction of wrist movements. J Neurophysiol 69:187–200 Lee RG, Donkelaar P van (1995) Mechanisms underlying functional recovery following stroke. Can J Neurol Sci 22:257–263 Lemon RN (1993) Stroke recovery. Curr Biol 3:463–465 Megaw ED (1972) Direction and extent uncertainty in step-input tracking. J Mot Behav 4:171–186 Netz J, Lammers T, Homberg V (1997) Reorganization of motor output in the non-affected hemisphere after stroke. Brain 120:1579–1586 Palmer E, Ashby P, Hajek VE (1992) Ipsilateral fast corticospinal pathways do not account for recovery in stroke. Ann Neurol 32:519–525 Pohl PS, Winstein CJ, Onla-or S (1997) Sensory-motor control in the ipsilesional upper extremity after stroke. Neurorehabilitation 9:67–69 [Corrigendum, Neurorehabilitation 9:245–249] Rao SM, Binder JR, Bandettini PA, Hammeke TA, Yetkin FZ, Jesmanowicz A, Lisk LM, Morris GL, Mueller WM, Estkowski LD (1993) Functional magnetic resonance imaging of complex human movements. Neurology 43:2311–2318 Riehle A, Requin J (1989) Monkey primary motor and premotor cortex: single-cell activity related to prior information about direction and extent of an intended movement. J Neurophysiol 61:534–549 Roland PE, Larsen B, Lassen NA, Skinhoj E (1980) Supplementary motor area and other cortical areas in organization of voluntary movements in man. J Neurophysiol 43:118–136 Shibasaki H, Sadato N, Lyshkow H, Yonekura Y, Honda M, Nagamine T, Suwazono S, Magata Y, Ikeda A, Miyazaki M (1993) Both primary motor cortex and supplementary motor area play an important role in complex finger movement. Brain 116:1387–1398 Tartaglione A, Inglese ML, Bandini F, Spadavecchia L, Hamsher K, Favale E (1991) Hemisphere asymmetry in decision making abilities. Brain 114:441–1456 Turton A, Wroe A, Trepte N, Fraser C, Lemon RN (1996) Contralateral and ipsilateral EMG responses to transcranial magnetic stimulation during recovery of arm and hand function after stroke. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 101:316– 328 Vanman E (1992) The timed response paradigm for interface with an arm manipulandum [computer program]. USC Movement Performance and Learning Laboratory Department of Biokinesiology, Los Angeles Velicki MR, Winstein CJ, Altman K, Pohl PS (1993) Trajectory parameter specification in subjects post stroke. Soc Neurosci Abstr 19:546 Weiller C, Chollet F, Friston KJ, Wise RJS, Frackowiak RSJ (1992) Functional reorganization of the brain in recovery from striatocapsular infarction in man. Ann Neurol 31:463–472 Weiller C, Ramsay SC, Wise RJS, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RSJ (1993) Individual patterns of functional reorganization in the human cerebral cortex after capsular infarction. Ann Neurol 33:181–189 Winstein CJ, Pohl PS (1995) Effects of unilateral brain damage on the control of goal-directed hand movements. Exp Brain Res 105:63–174 Winstein CJ, Grafton ST, Pohl PS (1997) Motor task difficulty and brain activity: an investigation of goal-directed reciprocal aiming using positron emission tomography. J Neurophysiol 77: 1581–1594 Winstein CJ, Merians A, Sullivan K (1999) Motor learning after unilateral brain damage. Neuropsychologia 37:975–987 Wyke M (1967) Effect of brain lesions on the rapidity of arm movement. Neurology 17:1113–1120 Wyke M (1971) The effects of brain lesions on the performance of bilateral arm movements. Neuropsychologia 9:33–42