J Comp Physiol A (1999) 185: 367±372 Ó Springer-Verlag 1999 REVIEW K. C. Catania A nose that looks like a hand and acts like an eye: the unusual mechanosensory system of the star-nosed mole Accepted: 31 May 1999 Abstract The star-nosed mole (Condylura cristata) has a snout surrounded by 22 ¯eshy and mobile appendages. This unusual structure is not an olfactory organ, as might be assumed from its location, nor is it used to manipulate objects as might be guessed from its appearance. Rather, the star is devoted to the sense of touch, and for this purpose the appendages are covered with thousands of small mechanoreceptive Eimer's organs. Recent behavioral studies ®nd that the star acts much like a tactile eye, having a small behavioral focus, or ``fovea'' at the center ± used for detailed explorations of objects of interest. The peripheral and central nervous systems of the mole re¯ect these behavioral specializations, such that the small behavioral focus on the nose is more densely innervated in the periphery, and has a greatly enlarged representation in the somatosensory cortex. This somatosensory representation of the tactile fovea is not correlated with anatomical parameters (innervation density) as found in other species, but rather is highly correlated with patterns of behavior. The many surprising parallels between the somatosensory system of the mole, and the visual systems of other mammals, suggest a convergent and perhaps common organization for highly developed sensory systems. Key words Cortical magni®cation á Somatosensory cortex á Development á Evolution á Behavior Introduction The star-nosed mole is renowned for its unusual nose, which consists of 22 mobile, ¯eshy appendages that surround the nostrils. This peculiar nasal specialization K.C. Catania Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, 301 Wilson Hall, 111 21st Ave South, Nashville, TN 37240, USA Tel.: +1-615-322-7491; Fax: +1-615-343-8449 is unique among mammals and raises several basic questions about star-nosed mole biology. What is the star and how is it used by the mole to explore its dark underground environment? What kinds of sensory receptors are associated with the star? How is information from the star represented in the neocortex? Such questions have been the subject of a number of recent studies exploring the sensory biology of this species and its relatives (Catania et al. 1993; Catania 1995a, b, c, 1996; Catania and Kaas 1995, 1996, 1997a, b). Each aspect of the sensory system, including the mole's behavior, the structure of the nose and sensory receptors, and the physiology and anatomy of the cortex, has provided some important insight into these basic questions. But the greatest insights come from combining these studies. Unusual specializations in the cortex are explained by behavioral observations. Anatomical comparisons between the star and its cortical representation reveal unexpected parallels between the mole's somatosensory system and the primate visual system. These results provide an example of how a specialized species can reveal general principles of brain organization. Star-nosed mole behavior reveals a ``tactile fovea'' Star-nosed moles live in wetlands in the eastern United States and Canada. They build extensive tunnel systems with their heavily clawed forelimbs, and seldom come to the surface. Moles are insectivores with a high rate of metabolism, and they must ®nd and eat a large number of invertebrate prey each day. As would be predicted from their fossorial lifestyle, they have small eyes and a tiny optic nerve. Thus a major challenge to their survival is ®nding sucient quantities of often small prey in their dark underground tunnels. The star seems to have evolved mainly for this purpose, and may be the most sensitive touch organ among mammals. When moles search for food or explore their environment, the star is in constant motion and is repeatedly touched to the substrate or objects of interest. Each 368 touch consists of raising the nose upward while the nasal rays swing backward, then swinging the rays forward while the star is brought into contact with the substrate. This behavior is very rapid; star-nosed moles may touch ten or more dierent places each second. Similar behavior in other moles without a ``star'' has been described as tapping the ground with the nose (Nagorsen 1996). When a prey item ± such as an earthworm ± is encountered, the rays are repeatedly touched to the prey as it is bitten, torn up, and eaten. When prey is missed by the star, by even a millimeter or two, the mole proceeds past without apparent notice (whether searching in water or soil), arguing against the hypothesis that the star functions to detect prey from a distance through electroreception (Gould et al. 1993). With the naked eye, the details of foraging and prey encounters are dicult to discern, but slow motion videotaped behavior of moles locating small prey reveal a stereotyped sequence of nose movements and a behavioral focus or ``fovea'' on the star (Fig. 1). Ray 11, the center-most ventral ray (Fig. 2) is preferentially used to explore prey items, acting in a manner analogous to the fovea in the visual system of other mammals. For example, whenever a prey item is ®rst contacted with the peripheral rays (rays 1 through 10) the nose is then shifted so that further detailed explorations are made with ray number 11 (Fig. 1A). No food item is eaten without ®rst being explored with ray 11. But since ray 11 is one of the smallest rays on the nose, food is usually Fig. 1A±C The use of the nose while foraging for food. A A schematic representation of the nose with the rays numbered from 1 to 11, shown in relation to a prey item (gray oval). The ®rst touch made contact with rays 4 and 5 as the mole discovered the food item. The second touch was centered on the 11th central pair of rays, after which the food was eaten. For brevity, the prey is shown changing position relative to the star ± but in reality, the star is moved relative to the prey. This sequence of movements is characteristic of prey encounters, which often include multiple touches with the 11th pair of rays. B A ¯ow diagram illustrating the progression of touches from the lateral rays to the central 11th ray before the food is eaten. C An example of the distribution of touches across the nose for ten consecutive preys encounters, showing the preferential use of the 11th and surrounding rays. B and C from Catania and Kaas 1997b ®rst encountered by the larger peripheral rays which together make up most of the surface area of the star. This stereotyped sequence of movements is very rapid. Moles can touch a small prey item with the peripheral rays, shift the star for several additional touches with ray 11, and take the prey into the mouth, all in about 400 ms (Catania and Kaas 1997b). The division of the star into peripheral touch and central touch seems analogous to the retina in the visual system of other mammals. In primates, for example, photoreceptors in the peripheral retina typically detect potentially important stimuli ®rst, and then a saccade brings the image onto the higher resolution, denser region of photoreceptors of the retinal fovea. Does the anatomy of the nasal rays also re¯ect the dierent roles they play in behavior? The peripheral anatomy and sensory receptors of the star Although the star is a specialization of the distal portion of the nose, it is obviously not an olfactory structure. At ®rst glance, one might imagine the nose acting as an extra hand. But the rays do not contain muscles or bones and are not used to manipulate objects or capture prey. They are controlled through tendons by a complex series of muscles that attach to the skull, and their role seems to be purely mechanosensory. For this purpose, they are composed almost entirely of a series of small mechanosensory organs called ``Eimer's organs''. Under the scanning electron microscope, these organs are visible in a honeycomb pattern of epidermal domes on the surface of each nasal ray (Fig. 2). Eimer's organs are very sensitive to light touch (Catania and Kaas 1995, 1997a) and they are found on the snout of every member of the family Talpidae that has been examined (Catania 1995b). However, the star-nosed mole has many more Eimer's organs than any other species and their distribution across the nasal rays is unique: while most moles have at most a few thousand Eimer's organs surrounding their nostrils, the star-nosed mole has over 25 000 on the star. Each organ is supplied by a number of primary aerents, thus the star is very densely innervated. There 369 A. 2 1 B. Terminal Swellings of Free Nerve Endings C. 3 4 Epidermis 5 N 6 Dermis 11 Merkel Cell Neurite Complex Encapsulated Corpuscle 7 10 9 8 Fig. 2A±C The nose and sensory organs of the star-nosed mole. A The nose is composed of 22 appendages, or ``rays'' that surround the nostrils (N). There are 11 symmetric pairs, numbered from 1 to 11 on each side. The rays are used by the mole to explore its dark underground environment through touch. The rays can be moved through tendons that connect to facial muscles, and they are repeatedly touched to the substrate as the mole moves through its system of tunnels. B Each ray is covered with many hundreds of domed sensory organs called ``Eimer's organs''. These are apparent in a honeycomb distribution on the skin surface at high magni®cation. The rays are essentially composed of these sensory organs which are densely innervated by a branch of the infraorbital nerve. C Each Eimer's organ is a specialization of the epidermis that contains an array of sensory receptors, including an encapsulated corpuscle, a merkel cell-neurite complex, and a series of intra-epidermal nerve endings that terminate in swellings at the apex of the central cell column. Plate A from Catania and Kaas 1995 are over 100 000 myelinated ®bers innervating the relatively small star, and a substantial cross section of each ray is taken up by a large nerve branch (Catania 1995a). The internal structure of the star-nosed mole's Eimer's organs is illustrated in Fig. 2C. Each organ is a roughly 40-lm swelling of the epidermis that contains a central column of keratinocytes. At the bottom of each organ, in the connective tissue of the dermis, there is a single encapsulated corpuscle. Just above this, among the keratinocytes that form the base of the central cell column, there is a single merkel cell-neurite complex. Within the cell column, a series of nerve ®bers branch from three myelinated ®bers in the dermis and ascend along the margins of the column to terminate in a circular arrangement of swellings just below the outer layer of epidermis. This concise geometric arrangement of terminal nerve swellings is entirely enclosed and encapsulated by a single circular keratinocyte at the apex of the cell column and the overlying outer layer of epidermis is tightly sealed by many desmosomal adhesions (Catania 1996). While the response properties of the separate nerve terminals at the apex of Eimer's organ have not been Myelinated Fibers 10µm Eimer's Organ characterized, recordings from the cortex show the star to be highly responsive to several dierent features of ®ne tactile stimulation (Catania and Kaas 1995). The super®cial nerve terminals in each Eimer's organs are ideally positioned to be stimulated by pressure to the top of the cell column when the rays are touched against an object. The Eimer's organs have the same basic structure across the dierent rays on the star. Ray 11, the behavioral focus of the nose, does not have a higher density of organs, or more organs than other rays, as might be guessed from its important role in search behaviors. In fact, because it is relatively short and has little caudal surface, it has fewer Eimer's organs than almost any other ray. Ray 11 has about 900 Eimer's organs on its surface while some of the lateral rays have well over 1500 (Catania and Kaas 1997b). Rather than having more sensory organs, a dierent way in which a skin surface may be more sensitive to mechanoreceptive input is by increasing its innervation density and decreasing each aerent's receptive ®eld. To test for this possibility, the number of Eimer's organs and the number of myelinated ®bers innervating each ray were counted and compared (Fig. 3). Rays 1 through 9 each had about 4 ®bers per Eimer's organ, while rays 10 and 11 had signi®cantly higher innervation densities of 5.6 and 7.1 ®bers per organ, respectively. Thus there is a peripheral specialization of the rays most used for exploratory behaviors, such that ray 11 has a much higher innervation density than more lateral rays. Previous studies of the organization of the somatosensory cortex, where touch information in mammals is processed, have found a correlation between peripheral innervation density and the size of the corresponding representation or map of the sensory surface in cortex. To investigate this relationship in moles, we examined the organization of the mole's somatosensory cortex and related these areas to the anatomy of the star (Catania and Kaas 1997b). Average Fibers per Eimer's Organ 370 8 Fibers per Organ Anatomical proportions 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Ray Number Fig. 3 The ratio of myelinated ®bers to Eimer's organs from the 11 rays of the nose (from four moles). While rays 1 through 9 have an average of roughly 4 ®bers per Eimer's organ, the 10th and 11th have signi®cantly higher innervation densities of 5.6 and 7.1 ®bers per organ, respectively (from Catania and Kaas 1997b) The representation of the star in cerebral cortex To explore the organization of the somatosensory cortex in star-nosed moles, neuronal responses were recorded using microelectrodes in anesthetized moles, while the nose and body were gently stimulated with small probes (Catania et al. 1993; Catania and Kaas 1995). A large area of cortex responded to stimulation of the star, while smaller areas responded to the rest of the body and the limbs (Fig. 4). After mapping the locations of receptive ®elds from the star and body of the mole onto the somatosensory areas, the moles were perfused and their cortex was ¯attened, sectioned parallel to the cortical surface, and stained for the metabolic enzyme cytochrome oxidase, which often reveals cortical subdivisions. In the area of cortex that contained an electrophysiological map of the star, a set of dark stripes was found in brain sections processed for cytochrome oxidase (Fig. 5). Each stripe corresponded precisely in location to an area that responded to a single nasal ray in the microelectrode recordings. Thus the representation of the star in somatosensory cortex is visible in brain sections, much as the representation of whiskers in rodents is visible as a set of cortical barrels in their cortex (Woolsey and Van der Loos 1970). However, dierences between the barrel cortex in rodents and the star representation in moles were immediately apparent. First, multiple maps of the star were seen in sections of the mole's cortex. As in all mammals, each half of the body is represented in the opposite cerebral hemisphere, but in each mole hemisphere there were at least two clearly visible sets of 11 stripes representing the contralateral star. In some favorable cases, a smaller third set of stripes was also apparent (Catania and Kaas 1995, 1996). The most prominent set of stripes corresponded to the primary somatosensory cortex, S1. The second set Cortical proportions Fig. 4 Cortical magni®cation in star-nosed moles. The upper drawing shows the anatomical proportions of the various body parts. The lower drawing of a ``moleunculus'' shows the relative size of each body part as it is represented in somatosensory cortex. As would be predicted from its high innervation density and behavioral importance, the nose dominates the cortical representation. The forelimb also has a large cortical representation, probably re¯ecting its important sensory role in the excavation of tunnels, rather than in locating prey (from Catania and Kaas 1996) of stripes was a mirror image of the ®rst, and corresponded to the second somatosensory area, S2 (Catania and Kaas 1995). S1 and S2 are somatosensory areas found in all mammals (Kaas 1987, 1995), including rodents, but there is not a visible representation of each whisker representation in S2 of rodents. The third set of stripes also responds to tactile stimulation of the nose, but it has not yet been fully mapped because of its small size and lateral location in the brain. The presence of two and perhaps three visible maps of the star in different somatosensory areas of the mole is unique, and may be a specialization related to the very high innervation density and large number of mechanoreceptors in the star. Another unusual but not entirely unexpected ®nding was a hugely enlarged cortical representation of the 11th nasal ray. This was seen in electrophysiological recordings (Catania and Kaas 1995, 1996), and is also strikingly clear in the cytochrome oxidase pattern of stripes visible in brain sections (Fig. 5). This ray often takes up over 25% of the entire S1 cortical representation of the nose, despite its relatively small size on the star. Behaviorally important sensory surfaces often have greatly enlarged cortical representations relative to their anatomical size. But these enlargements are generally attributed to their higher innervation densities, rather than speci®c in¯uences of behavior. For example, a direct linear correlation has been found between the size of cortical barrels in rodent somatosensory cortex and the number of aerents innervating the corresponding whisker on the face (Welker and Van der Loos 1986). To 371 A. b 4 3 5 6 7 2 8 1 10 9 11 B. 8 4 5 6 7 9 10 3 2 1 Average Area of Cortex Per Afferent (µm2) C 150 11 S1 Cortex per Fiber (Afferent Magnification) 120 90 60 30 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Ray Number explore this relationship in moles, the area of each ray's cortical representation was measured and compared to its corresponding peripheral innervation (Fig. 5C). Cortical representations re¯ect behavior, rather than innervation density While ray 11 was found to have a higher innervation density than the lateral rays (Fig. 3), this increased Fig. 5A±C The cortical representation of the star. A A rotated view of the left side of the nose (dorsal is to the left, lateral is up) oriented to match the nose representation in the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) of the right hemisphere (below). B A tangential section through layer 4 of the primary somatosensory nose representation in cortex, processed for the metabolic enzyme cytochrome oxidase. The pattern of 11 rays on the nose is mirrored in the brain as a series of 11 dark stripes. Note, however, the 11th subdivision in the brain is greatly enlarged relative to the size of the 11th ray on the nose. The disproportionate representation of the 11th ray, and to a lesser extent the adjacent rays, re¯ects their behavioral importance (Fig. 1). C The average area of cortex in S1 per primary aerent for each nasal ray. The representations of the rays in cortex are not proportional to their respective innervation densities. Rather, the 11th and surrounding rays have greater areas of cortex per aerent. The pattern of ``aerent magni®cation'' is very similar to the distribution of touches across the nose during feeding behaviors (Fig. 1C), suggesting a possible role for behavior in shaping these cortical areas during development. Plate C from Catania and Kaas 1997b innervation density did not account for the size of the 11th subdivision in somatosensory cortex (Catania 1995c; Catania and Kaas 1997b). Ray 11 contained about 7% of the Eimer's organs on the star, received about 11% of the nerve ®bers innervating the star (accounting for the higher innervation density per organ), but took up about 25% of the cortical representation of the star. Thus ray 11 is specialized both in the periphery and in the cortex, by a higher innervation density and a larger cortical representation per aerent, respectively. These ®ndings are dierent from the results of similar investigations in rodents (Welker and Van der Loos 1986), but nevertheless may be a common condition in mammalian somatosensory cortex that is simply dicult to quantify in most species where the areas of cortical representations and corresponding innervation densities of skin surfaces are dicult to measure. This preferential magni®cation of primary aerents has been termed ``aerent magni®cation'', to distinguish it from the traditional term ``cortical magni®cation'' (Catania and Kaas, 1997b). The latter is used to describe enlarged representations of a skin surface in the corresponding cortical representation, but does not take into account innervation density. The pattern of aerent magni®cation of the rays (Fig. 5C) is strikingly similar to the distribution of touches across the mole's nose when exploring prey (Fig. 1C). The relationship between peripheral innervation density and cortical representational area has long been debated for the visual system of primates (Malpeli and Baker 1975; Drasdo 1977; Myerson et al. 1977; Perry and Cowey 1985; Silveira et al. 1989; WaÈssle et al. 1989, 1990). The ®ndings in moles support the most recent ®ndings in primates (Azzopardi and Cowey 1993) which report a disproportionately large cortical representation of ganglion cells from the retinal fovea. The relative degree of aerent magni®cation is similar in the two sensory systems (Catania 1995c). Thus there seem to be parallels between visual system organization in primates and somatosensory organization in moles, perhaps re- 372 vealing a general aspect of the cortical representations of important inputs across dierent sensory systems. All of these ®ndings raise a number of additional questions about star-nosed mole sensory biology. For example, why have multiple cortical representations of the nose, rather than one large representation? Does adding new representations of the nose increase computational eciency through parallel processing of different aspects of touch? These representations are also topographically interconnected both in the same hemisphere and across hemispheres to the representation of the contralateral star. The ability to identify each respective ray representation in multiple maps will allow future studies to determine the circuitry of this system in detail (with the aid of anatomical tracers) to determine the degree of parallel or hierarchical organization of the cortical representation. Another area of future research includes the functioning of Eimer's organs. The concise geometric arrangement of nerve terminals at the apex of each organ may be used to detect the surface features of objects in the 10- to 100-lm size range. If so, the sensory world of the star-nosed mole could include a new realm of perceptions not usually considered salient to mechanosensation, such as microscopic textures and surface features previously considered impossible to detect by mammalian touch. However, the most striking result of these studies is the similarity between the patterns of behavior in moles (Fig. 1C) and the pattern of aerent magni®cation of the rays (Fig. 5C). The sizes of the cortical modules representing the rays are not correlated with the anatomical parameters traditionally assumed to be driving these specializations, but rather are highly correlated with the patterns of mole behavior (see Catania and Kaas 1997b for more details). Here we may be able to examine the role of behavior in brain development to see if it is in fact driving the enlargement of cortical areas, or whether these behavioral and anatomical parameters are somehow independently matched to one another during development. Acknowledgements Thanks to Jon Kaas and Glenn Northcutt for their support, guidance, and many fruitful collaborations in the course of these studies. Neeraj Jain, Christine Collins and Melanie Catania provided helpful comments on the manuscript. Special thanks to Hrathkus and Bill Catania for their help capturing starnosed moles. All experiments and animal care procedures were approved by the Vanderbilt University Animal Care Committee and follow the National Institute of Health guide for the care and use of laboratory animals. References Azzopardi P, Cowey A (1993) Preferential representation of the fovea in primary visual cortex. Nature (Lond) 361: 719±721 Catania KC (1995a) The structure and innervation of the sensory organs on the snout of the star-nosed mole. J Comp Neurol 351: 549±567 Catania KC (1995b) A comparison of the Eimer's organs of three North American moles: the hairy-tailed mole (Parascalops breweri), the star-nosed mole (Condylura cristata) and the eastern mole (Scalopus aquaticus). J Comp Neurol 354: 150±160 Catania KC (1995c) Magni®ed cortex in star-nosed moles. Nature (Lond) 375: 453±454 Catania KC (1996) Ultrastructure of the Eimer's organs of the starnosed mole. J Comp Neurol 365: 343±354 Catania KC, Kaas JH (1995) The organization of the somatosensory cortex of the star-nosed mole. J Comp Neurol 351: 536±548 Catania KC, Kaas JH (1996) The unusual nose and brain of the star-nosed mole. Bio Science Rep 46: 578±586 Catania KC, Kaas JH (1997a) The organization of somatosensory cortex and distribution of corticospinal neurons in the eastern mole (Scalopus aquaticus). J Comp Neurol 378: 337±353 Catania KC, Kaas JH (1997b) Somatosensory fovea in the starnosed mole: behavioral use of the star in relation to innervation patterns and cortical representation. J Comp Neurol 387: 215±233 Catania KC, Kaas JH, Northcutt RG, Beck PD (1993) Nose stars and brain stripes. Nature (Lond) 364: 493 Drasdo N (1977) The neural representation of visual space. Nature (Lond) 266: 554±556 Gould E, McShea W, Grand T (1993) Function of the star in the star-nosed mole, Condylura cristata. J Mammal 74: 108±106 Kaas JH (1987) The organization and evolution of neocortex. In: Wise SP (ed) Higher brain functions. Wiley, New York, pp 347± 378 Kaas JH (1995) The evolution of isocortex. Brain Behav Evol 46: 187±196 Malpeli JG, Baker FH (1975) The representation of the visual ®eld in the lateral geniculate nucleus of Macaca mulatta. J Comp Neurol 161: 569±594 Myerson J, Manis PB, Miezin FM, Allman JM (1977) Magni®cation in striate cortex and retinal ganglion cell layer of owl monkey: a quantitative comparison. Science 198: 855±857 Nagorsen DW (1996) Opossums shrews and moles of British Columbia. UBC Press, Vancouver, British Columbia Perry VH, Cowey A (1985) The ganglion cell and cone distributions in the monkey's retina: implications for central magni®cation factors. Vision Res 25: 1795±1810 Silveira LCL, Picanco-Dinz CW, Sampaio LFS, Oswaldo-Cruz E (1989) Retinal ganglion cell distribution in the cebus monkey: a comparison with the cortical magni®cation factors. Vision Res 29: 1471±1483 WaÈssle H, Grunert U, Rohrenbeck J, Boycott BB (1989) Cortical magni®cation factor and the ganglion cell density of the primate retina. Nature (Lond) 341: 643±646 WaÈssle H, Grunert U, Rohrenbeck J, Boycott BB (1990) Retinal ganglion cell density and cortical magni®cation factor in the primate. Vision Res 30: 1897±1911 Welker E, Van der Loos H (1986) Quantitative correlation between barrel-®eld size and the sensory innervation of the whiskerpad: a comparative study in six strains of mice bred for dierent patterns of mystacial vibrissae. J Neurosci 6: 3355±3373 Woolsey TA, Van der Loos H (1970) The structural organization of layer IV in the somatosensory region (SI) of mouse cerebral cortex: the description of a cortical ®eld composed of discrete cytoarchitectonic units. Brain Res 17: 205±242