The Asymmetric Impact of Negative and Positive Attribute-Level Performance on Overall Satisfaction and Repurchase Intentions Author(s): Vikas Mittal, William T. Ross, Jr. and Patrick M. Baldasare Source: Journal of Marketing, Vol. 62, No. 1 (Jan., 1998), pp. 33-47 Published by: American Marketing Association Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1251801 . Accessed: 18/12/2014 21:27 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. . American Marketing Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of Marketing. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 140.120.52.117 on Thu, 18 Dec 2014 21:27:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions VikasMittal,WilliamT. Ross, Jr.,& PatrickM.Baldasare of Negative and Positive Attribute-Level Performance on Overall Satisfaction and Intentions Repurchase The Asymmetric Impact The relationship between attribute-level performance, overall satisfaction, and repurchase intentions is of critical importance to managers and generally has been conceptualized as linear and symmetric. The authors investigate the asymmetric and nonlinear nature of the relationship among these constructs. Predictions are developed and tested empirically using survey data from two different contexts: a service (health care, n = 4517) and a product (automobile, n = 9359 and n = 13,759). Results show that (1) negative performance on an attribute has a greater impact on overall satisfaction and repurchase intentions than positive performance has on that same attribute, and (2) overall satisfaction displays diminishing sensitivity to attribute-level performance. Surprisingly, results show that attribute performance has a direct impact on repurchase intentions in addition to its effect through satisfaction. Consider the following scenarios: ?In an effortto increasecustomersatisfaction,an automotive manufacturer consistentlyhas increasedperformanceon engine power.However,resultsshow that, after a while, increasesin the ratingsfor performance on enginepowerdo not yield correspondingchanges in satisfactionratings. Designerswonderif theyshouldkeepinvestingresourcesto enhanceenginepower. *A physicianhistoricallyhas receivedhigh performance ratsuchas politeness,timeliness,reliabilityof ingson attributes service,andcorrectdiagnosis.Inthecurrentsurvey,shefinds thatperformance ratingson timelinessaredown,butareup on all the otherattributes.However,she also findsa sharp declinein heroverallsatisfaction ratings.She is puzzledas to on a singleattributeis not offset why negativeperformance on a hostof otherattributes. by positiveperformance These scenarios highlight typical dilemmas that managers face at customer-drivenorganizationswhere the goals are to design productsand services with attributesthat maximize customer satisfaction.These goals generally are operationalized by multiple-regressionmodels that identify key attributes into which managers should invest resources to enhance overall satisfaction (e.g., Bolton and Drew 1991; Hanson 1992; Wittink and Bayer 1994). The assumption underlyingsuch "key driver"models is that there is a symVikasMittal is a visiting assistantprofessor, Graduate Schoolof Kellogg Northwestern William T.Ross,Jr.is anassociate Management, University. Patrick M.Baldasare is President andChief professor, Temple University. Executive Officer of TheResponseCenter,a division of TeleSpectrum Inc.Theauthors thehelpful comments andsugWorldWide, acknowledge Oliva,D.Raghavarao, gestionsof EugeneAnderson, Terry SoumyaRoy, andfouranonymous reviewers. Theauthors thankFrank Mark G. Forkin, andAlanRogersof Consumer Attitude Inc./Research Konkel, Research, DataAnalysis, Inc.forproviding theautomotive data,andJamesCuthrell, Kristine Michael andE.Monsoon fortheir Gerhart, Gupta, JerryKatrichis, support. Journal of Marketing Vol. 62 (January 1998), 33-47 metric and linear relationshipbetween attribute-levelperformanceand dependentconstructssuch as overall satisfaction and purchase intentions. However, what if, at the attribute level, performance and satisfaction were linked asymmetrically?The answer to this question is important for academics and managersalike. Although academics (cf. Anderson and Sullivan 1993; Oliva, Oliver, and Bearden 1995) and practitioners(cf. Coyne 1989) recognize that the overall satisfactionfunction might not be linearand/orsymmetric, no such conclusion can be offered at the attribute level. Yet,it is at the attributelevel thatmanagersmust make decisions. Therefore,our objective is to investigate-theoretically and empirically-the existence and natureof the asymmetric and nonlinearresponse of satisfaction to attribute-level performance.In the first section, we discuss the need for an attribute-levelconceptualization of the performance/satisfaction relationship.That is followed by a discussion of the asymmetricand nonlinearnatureof the relationshipand the development of hypotheses. Then we present results from three studies that test the hypotheses. Multi-AttributeProducts and Satisfaction There are several reasons-theoretical and managerial-to use multi-attributemodels in the context of customer satisfaction (cf. LaTourand Peat 1979; Wilkie and Pessemier 1973). First, consumers are more likely to render evaluations of their postpurchaseexperiences of satisfaction at an attributelevel ratherthan at the productlevel (Gardialet al. 1994). Second, an attribute-based approach enables researchersto conceptualize commonly observed phenomena, such as consumersexperiencing mixed feelings toward a productor service. A consumer can be both satisfied and dissatisfied with different aspects of the same product. Performance/ 33 Negativeand PositiveAttribute-Level This content downloaded from 140.120.52.117 on Thu, 18 Dec 2014 21:27:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Although such phenomena are not easy to model in an overall satisfaction approach, the attribute-level approach provides a simple and elegant solution: Mixed feelings towarda productexist because a consumer may be satisfied with one attributebut dissatisfied with another.For example, in a restaurant,a customer may be highly satisfied with the food but highly dissatisfied with the service at the same time. Third, an attribute-level approach to satisfaction affords researchersa higher level of specificity and diagnostic usefulness comparedwith the productlevel or "overall" approach(LaTourand Peat 1979). For example, Parasuraman,Zeithaml, and Berry (1988) show that measuring various attributesor dimensions of a service provides a better understandingof global constructs,such as service quality. Similarly, Boulding and colleagues (1993) suggest that quality is multidimensional and different dimensions of quality are averaged together in some fashion to producean overall assessment of quality. However, both works treat the relationship between attributesand overall quality as linear and symmetric. Fourth, the higher specificity and diagnostic usefulness of multi-attributemodels also ensures that academic research will find more welcome applications among managers who generally work at the attribute level ratherthanat an overall level (e.g., Hanson 1992; Wittink and Bayer 1994). Finally, there is some evidence that attribute-level performance/disconfirmation and overall satisfaction are qualitatively different constructs (Oliva, Oliver, and Bearden 1995), and, if treatedinterchangeably, "global customer satisfaction responses may mask specific product issues" (Oliva, Oliver, and Bearden 1995, p. 26). Thus, studying satisfaction at the attributelevel can help extend both conceptual and empirical understandingof the phenomenon. The Asymmetric Effect of Product Performance and Satisfaction: Theory and Hypotheses When examining the theoretical and analytical importance of the link between attribute-levelperformanceand overall satisfaction, it is importantto recognize thatthe relationship could be asymmetric (cf. Anderson and Sullivan 1993; Oliva, Oliver, and Bearden 1995). One unit of negative performanceon an attributecould have a greatereffect on overall satisfaction or repurchaseintentions than a corresponding unit of positive performance.Similarly,in a given set of attributes,negative performanceon a single attributecould outweigh positive performance on many other attributes combined. Oliver (1993) finds that (1) attribute-levelsatisfaction and dissatisfaction significantly affect overall satisfaction with a product(automobile)and a service (an undergraduate course offering) and (2) attribute dissatisfaction has a larger weight than attributesatisfaction for the productI but not for the service. However,no theoreticalmotivation for the observed disparity between the impact of attributesatisfaction and dissatisfaction is provided. In the tStandardized loadingswere as follows:attributesatisfaction and dissatisfactionfor the automobile(.135 and -.175, respectively)andthecourse(.130 and-.059, respectively). next sections, the theoretical logic is developed along two lines of reasoning.One is based on prospecttheory (Kahneman and Tversky 1979), and the other is rooted in cognitive researchthat examines the memorabilityof positive versus negative events. Prospect Theory Prospecttheory(Kahnemanand Tversky 1979) is a descriptive theoryin which all of the alternativesthata personfaces are reducedto a series of prospects that are evaluatedindependently on the basis of an S-shaped value function (see Figure 1). As depicted in Figure 1, prospect theory postulatesthat peoples'judgments display (1) referencedependence(carriers of value are gains and losses from a referencepoint) and (2) loss aversion (the functionis steeper in the negative than in the positive domain). In addition, evaluations display diminishing sensitivity (marginal values of both gains and losses decrease with their size). The two key propertiesof the value function for our discussion are loss aversion and diminishingsensitivity. The loss aversionbuilt into prospecttheorysuggests that losses loom largerthan gains (Einhom and Hogarth 1981). Psychologically, a one-unit loss is weighted more than an equal amountof gain. In a satisfactioncontext, negative outcomes on attributeperformanceshould carrymore weight in the overall satisfactionjudgmentthanequal amountsof positive outcomes on attributeperformance.For example, if a car's mileage were to decrease by 10 miles per gallon, it would have a greaterimpacton the overall satisfactionjudgment than if the car's mileage were to increase by 10 miles per gallon. Thus, negative performanceon an attributewill loom largerthanpositive performanceon the same attribute. On the basis of these theories, we propose FIGURE 1 Asymmetric Impact of Attribute-Level Performance Negative Performance on Attribute II i OverallSatisfaction/ RepurchaseIntentions 34 Journalof Marketing,January1998 This content downloaded from 140.120.52.117 on Thu, 18 Dec 2014 21:27:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Positive Performance on Attribute on an attributewill havea greater H ,,:Negativeperformance impacton overallsatisfactionthanpositiveperformance on thesameattribute. on an attributewill have a Hlb:Negativedisconfirmation greaterimpacton overallsatisfactionthanpositivedison thesameattribute. confirmation In addition, on the basis of prospect theory,overall satisfaction also should display diminishing sensitivity toward attributeperformance.That is, at high (low) levels of performance, positive (negative) performanceon an attribute should not affect satisfaction as dramaticallyas it does at lower levels of performance.This hypothesis is similar to the diminishing returns hypothesis in classical economics and also is depicted in Figure 1. Therefore, H2:Overallsatisfactionwill displaydiminishingsensitivityto changesin the magnitudeof performancefor a given In otherwords,at highlevelsof positiveor negattribute. ativeperformance on an attribute, overallsatisfactionwill be affectedless thanat intermediate levelsof performance on thatattribute. Memory for Negative Versus Positive Instances Researchers distinguish between transaction-based and cumulativesatisfaction(Bitnerand Hubbert1994). Cumulative satisfaction typically is conceptualized as an overall judgment based on several transactionswith a product or service. To the extent that all aspects of the transactionsare not equally accessible (Gardialet al. 1994), the overall satisfactionjudgment will vary on the basis of the accessibility of a given aspect. Memory accessibility is a function of stimulus salience, among other things (Taylor 1982). Evidence shows that negative informationis more perceptually salient than positively valenced information,is given more weight than positive information, and elicits a stronger physiological response than positive information (Peeters and Czapinski 1990). Similar psychological operations should occur for customer satisfactionbecause satisfactionis linked to memorybased processing (Yi 1990). To the extent that attributes with negative performancewill be more perceptuallysalient than attributes with positive performance, attributes with negative performanceshould have a greater impact on the cumulative satisfactionjudgment. Thus, within a given set of attributes,the relative impact of each attributewill be asymmetric.Consequently,when combined, attributeswith negative performanceshould have a greaterimpacton overall satisfactionthan theircorrespondingattributeswith positive performancecombined. On the basis of the previous discussion, we hypothesize that H3a: Cumulatively, attributeswith negative performancewill have greaterimpactson overallsatisfactionthanattributeswithpositiveperformance. Along similar lines, a diminishingsensitivity hypothesis between overall satisfaction and performance on various attributescan be proposed. In a given set of attributes,each additionalinstance of positive performanceon an attribute will have a smaller impact than the other attributes.Conversely, each additional instance of negative performance should have a correspondinglysmaller negative impact on overall satisfaction.Thus, willdisplaydiminishing sensitivityto H3b:Overallsatisfaction additionalinstances of negative or positive performance. In otherwords,each additionalinstanceof positiveor shouldhave a smallerimpacton negativeperformance overallsatisfaction. Repurchase Intentions Attribute-level performanceshould affect satisfaction and repurchase intentions differently (e.g., Ostrom and Iacobucci 1995). One reason could be that satisfaction and repurchaseintentions are qualitatively different constructs. Satisfaction may be merely a judgment with cognitive and affective dimensions, whereas repurchase intentions also have a behavioral component. Based on the consumer's goals (e.g., Mittal et al. 1993), performanceon a certain attributemay become crucial for repurchaseintentions but not satisfaction. For example, consider the case of a patient who is satisfied with all aspects of the service provided by his or her primary care physician (PCP), except that the patient has now relocated to another area far from the PCP's office. When it comes time to renew and choose a PCP, the patient might indicate high overall satisfaction with the PCP but still might choose another PCP, because performance on a critical attributehas changed. In other words, though the performance on "distance of PCP's office from home" had a negative impact on repurchase intentions, it has a small or virtually no impact on overall satisfaction. One reason for such a discrepancy can be found in the attributionliterature(Folkes 1988). Here, the patient may attribute"poor" performanceon "distance of PCP's office from home" to him or herself or to causes other than the PCP and thus not change his or her overall satisfaction rating; nevertheless, the repurchaseintentions of the patient change. Thus, will H4:Overallsatisfactionand attribute-level performance haveseparateanddistincteffectson repurchase intentions. However, the relative magnitudeof overall satisfaction and attribute performance should be driven contextually, and no specific predictionsare made in that regard.H4 simply states that attribute-levelperformanceand overall satisfaction will have separate and distinct impacts on repurchase intentions. Next, we proposean asymmetriceffect of attribute-level performance on repurchase intentions. Previous research (e.g., Oliva, Oliver, and MacMillan 1992) shows that overall satisfaction and performanceare related nonlinearly to repurchase intentions or loyalty. Feinberg and colleagues (1990, p. 113) found that across several productcategories "the probability of repurchase was not isomorphic with either positive or negative service experiences." Research also shows that satisfaction and dissatisfactionhave different affective consequences (Oliver 1993), which may be related differentiallyto repurchaseintentions.Furthermore, the same psychological principlesthatoperatein the context of satisfaction also should operate in the context of repurchase intentions.Therefore, intentionswill be affectedasymmetrically H5:Repurchase by attribute-levelperformance.Negative performanceon attributeswill havea greaterimpacton repurchase intentionsthanpositiveperformance. Performance/35 Negativeand PositiveAttribute-Level This content downloaded from 140.120.52.117 on Thu, 18 Dec 2014 21:27:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions In the next sections, we presentresults from three largescale studies that test the preceding hypotheses. A unique feature of all three studies is that they use data from commercial studies conducted by firms measuring satisfaction with their productor service. Study 1 Data for this study were providedby a large health maintenance organization (HMO) in the Northeastern United States. The HMO collected these data as partof its ongoing patient satisfaction measurementprogram. For this study, the entire data set, which contained informationfrom 4517 telephone interviews conducted among patients enrolled with the HMO, was used. These interviews were conducted using Computer-Assisted-Telephone-Interview, which enabled the interviewerto probe, clarify, and follow up on responses to open-ended questions. Because responses to the open-ended questions were a vital part of the measurement process, interviewerswere asked to exercise greatcaution in recordingand following up on them. The sampling framefor the data set consisted of patients who subscribedto 501 PCPoffices affiliatedwith the HMO. A random sample was drawn from each PCP's patient list. Thus, in effect, a proportionate-stratified sample, with each PCP as a stratum,was drawn. After contact with a household had been established, up to two members per household were interviewed as long as they evaluated different PCPs. Thus, each household provided only one evaluation per PCP.The qualified respondentwas an adult from a family whose members had visited a PCP within the past 12 months. All 4517 interviews were used in the analysis. Although the exact responseratefor this survey could not be ascertained,typical responseratesfor such surveys are more than 70%. Input from the managersat the HMO and informal comparisons with data from other waves of the study show that the sample is representativeof the population served by the HMO. Measures The database contained measures for several constructs. There were two dependentmeasuresfor this analysis: overall satisfaction and repurchaseintentions."Overallsatisfaction with the medical care at the PCP's office" was operationalized as a five-point scale (1 = very dissatisfied, 5 = very satisfied). Repurchaseintentionor a patient'sintention to switch to a different PCP was operationalizedas a yes or no response to the following question: "Do you intend to switch to a different primarycare physician when you next have an opportunity?"Although both these measures are single-item scales, there is considerableprecedentfor using single-item measures in the context of large-scale satisfaction studies. LaBarberaand Mazursky (1983) discuss the issue of using single- versus multi-itemscales for measuring overall satisfaction and conclude that, in large-scale survey research,the use of multi-itemscales actually may decrease the quality of measurementratherthanenhanceit. Similarly, in their large-scale study of driversof customer satisfaction in the computer industry,Kekre, Krishnan,and Srinivasan (1995) use a single-item measure for overall satisfaction. Finally, Yi (1990), in his review of satisfaction research, compares the test/retest reliabilities of several multi- and single-item scales and finds that the test/retestreliabilityof single-item scales is acceptable (.55 to .84). For these reasons, we believe the measuresare adequate. Independentvariableswere createdon the basis of openended data providedby patients.In a free-thoughtelicitation task duringthe telephone interview,respondentswere asked about theirexperiences with variousfeaturesof theirvisit to the PCP.The interviewerasked them to clarify and expand their thoughts. These thought listings provided the basis for the independentvariablesfor the first analysis.As shown in Table 1, each thought was coded in a distinct category. When queried,the projectmanagerfor this study indicatedthatthe categories were determinedby the managersat the HMO in conjunction with the project director. The process is to review all the open-endeddatafrom a randomlychosen subset of interviews (usually 10%) and develop the categories so that they are mutuallyexclusive and collectively exhaustive. Then the coders code a portionof the data using these categories. After these data are coded, the coders and managers meet to see if the categories are "working,"that is, if the categories are descriptive and capturethe "essence" of what respondents said. Furthermore,when making categories, the managersensure that all attributesand thoughts are coded. In any given study,fewer than2% of the thoughts are forced into the "other/not specified" category. The process of determining categories is done carefully, especially given the high cost of datacollection and the strategic value of the informationcontained in these data. For the purpose of this analysis, each category was coded as 1 if a respondentgave a response in that category and 0 otherwise. Three professionalcoders, each with more than 2-3 years of coding experience, coded the categories. However, each coder coded a separate set of respondents. Because the coding already had been done before the data set was made available for this study,no follow-up analyses could be done to assess the reliability and agreementof the coding. Therefore,to assess the agreementamong the three coders, they were asked to code data from 337 respondents from a subsequent wave of a similar study conducted for another HMO in the same geographic area. For these 337 respondentsof the pilot study, the three coders were in perfect agreementfor 81% of the surveys.That is, coders coded every category identically for 81% of the respondents.Furthermore,Coders I and 2 matchon 84.3% of the responses, Coders 2 and 3 match on 85.5%, and Coders I and 3 match on 85.2%. The associated Kendall's tau correlationwas as follows: .84 for Coders 1 and 2, .85 for Coders I and 3, and .85 for Coders 2 and 3 (all significant at p < .01). With these limitations in mind, it can be said that such an approachto understandingsatisfactionhas been used by otherconsumersatisfactionresearchers(Bitner,Booms, and Tetreault1990; Gardialet al. 1994). This approach,though more unstructured than rating scales, is advantageous because it capturesattributeperceptionsthat are not contaminatedby the preconceived notions that a researchercould impose on the respondentsby cuing them regardingspecific attributes.The thought categories then were classified fur- 36 / Journalof Marketing,January1998 This content downloaded from 140.120.52.117 on Thu, 18 Dec 2014 21:27:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions ther as positively or negatively valenced. The positively valenced items for each respondentwere summed to create a new variable, POSITIVE. Similarly, a variable called NEGATIVEwas createdby summing all the negative items for each respondent.The positive/negative classification of each thoughtcategory is indicatedin Table 1. Analysis and Results The general strategy employed for testing the proposed asymmetry consists of computing separate estimates for positive and negative performanceand statisticallycomparing the absolute magnitude of the positive and negative estimates. The absolute magnitude is compared because negative performance should be related negatively to the dependentconstructs,and positive performancerelatedpositively to the dependent constructs. Therefore, to get a proper comparison, the "signs" of the estimates must be ignored.As an illustration,a test of the asymmetrybetween the composite variables POSITIVE and NEGATIVE is done, in which the coefficient for variable POSITIVE is expected to have a positive sign, and the coefficient for the variable NEGATIVEis expected to have a negative sign. > [bposITIVEI sugTherefore, testing whether IbNEGATIVEI gests the following hypothesis: (1) HA: IbNEGATIVEI> IbPOSITIVEI- The correspondingnull hypothesis can be stated as follows: (2) Ho: IbNEGATIVEI < IbPOSITIVEI To test HA, two rival models are compared to see whether Ho can be rejectedstatisticallyin favor of HA.One < IbPOSITIVEI model formally constrainsIbNEGATIVEI as sugin whereas in the the coefficients are free other, gested Ho, to vary as suggested in HA. Comparingthe performanceof the constrained model to that of the unconstrainedmodel provides a test of the asymmetry.If the model with the constraintis rejected in favor of the alterative model (i.e., Ho is rejected in favor of HA), it can be concluded that the TABLE 1 and Their Relative Thought Categories Impact on Overall Satisfaction with Medical Care Positive Mentions Negative Mentions Coefficient Thorough/attentive Spends time withme/personable Not afraidto referto anotherdoctor Listens to his/her patients Convenientoffice hours Convenientoffice location of doctor Alwaysavailable/availability No wait/sees patients rightaway Coefficient Matched Categories .11*** Not thorough .17**** Doesn't spend enough time/rushesyou in and out .07 Won'tgive referrals/trouble getting referrals .03 Not interested in patients/doesn'tlisten .07 Office hours are not convenient .02 Distance of office is inconvenient .15*** Not available/isn'talways in office .14** Waittoo long/officetoo crowded -.66**** -.40**** -.77**** -.94**** -.37**** .07 -.67**** -.38**** Unmatched Categories Good, excellent doctorcare Knowledge/educated/competent Explainseverythingwell/answersquestions Helpful Good bedside manner Professional Friendly/nice/courteous Concerned/caring/sympathetic Good withkids Makes me comfortable/easyto talkto Verypatient Sincere/honest/straightforward Outgoing/goodpersonality/sense of humor Is warm/kind Is an old-fashionedtype of doctor Calls back rightaway/keeps in contact/followsthorough Alwaysavailableduringemergency Promptnessof service Has been our doctorfor a long time I like him/her Staff is very nice/good Cleanliness of office/building *p < .10;< **p.05** < .26**** .02 .12*** .13* .13 .13 .11*** .22**** .19**** .06 .15 .15 .25* .19 .14 No followup/doesn'ttell you results Doctor/staffoverbooksappointments Poor staff/problemwithstaff Problemswith HMO Incorrectdiagnosis/unnecessarytesting -.65**** -.60*** -.84**** -.35**** -.99**** .17*** .32 .02 .33**** .20*** .14** .05 * **** < . ; p 001; Model R2 = .23, F = 30.9, error df = 4481, p < .0001. Negativeand PositiveAttribute-Level Performance/ 37 This content downloaded from 140.120.52.117 on Thu, 18 Dec 2014 21:27:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions absolute magnitude of the coefficient for NEGATIVE is > IbposITIVE greater than POSITIVE, or that IbNEGATIVEI Next, the results are described. Negative versuspositive attributeperformance(H/a). A dummy-variableregression was conducted using all of the thought categories as independentvariablesand overall satisfaction as the dependent variable.2As shown in Table 1, there were 30 positively valenced thoughtcategories and 13 negatively valenced thought categories. On the basis of these, the following model was estimated: (3) Overall Satisfaction= Intercept+ Attpos ... + Attpos30 + Attnegl ... + Attneg13 In this model, the impactof each attributeon overall satisfaction is calculated individually.Furthermore,positively valenced attributementions are expected to have positive signs, whereas negatively valenced attributementions are expected to have negative signs. To test Hla, we must test whether the absolute impact of a negative mention for an attribute is greater than the absolute impact of a positive mention for the same attribute.The model is significant (F = 30.9, p < .0001) and is reportedin Table 1. Several findings of interestemerge from this table. First, as shown in the first eight rows of Table 1, there were eight attributesfor which there were both negative and positive categories. Therefore,for these eight attributes,Hia could be tested. For all eight pairs, the negatively valenced attributes have a larger(in the absolute sense) coefficient than the corresponding positively valenced attributes.We used the statistical test explained previouslyto test whetherthe negative coefficient for each pair of attributesis largerthan the corresponding positive coefficient for the same attribute;the null hypothesis in Equation2 was rejectedfor seven of the eight pairs at p < .001. For example, "always available/ availability of doctor"(.15, p < .01) has a smaller impacton patient satisfaction than "not available/isn't always in his office" (-.67, p < .0001). Similarresultscan be noted for (1) "not afraid to refer to anotherdoctor"(.07, p < .26) versus "won't give referrals/troublegetting referrals"(-.77, p < .0001) and (2) "spends time with me/personable"(.17, p < .0001) versus "doesn't spend enough time/rushesyou in and out" (-.40, p < .0001). Only for "convenient office location," for which both the positive and negative coefficients were nonsignificant, was the hypothesis not supported. These results support Hia. However, because this study is only able to provide a sense of the direction of a person's evaluation on each attribute,a full test of H,, cannot be done. In other words, because there are no data on thei,rgnitude of positive or negative performanceon each attribute, additional testing for this hypothesis is required.Recall that though Hla is stated in terms of varying levels of performance on each attribute,there are no data on varying levels of performancein this study. Nevertheless, these results are encouraging and point toward tthetype of investigationthat is carriedout in Study 3. 2Acorrelationmatrixforall thoughtcategoriesshowedthehigh- Second, differentattributesof the service encountercontribute differently to overall satisfaction. For example, "cleanlinessof office/building" (.05, p = .78) is not consequential, whereas "no follow-up/doesn't tell you results" (-.65, p < .0001) has a greaterimpacton overall satisfaction. Several othercomparisonscan be made with similarresults. These echo earlier findings (Bolton and Drew 1991) that service attributesmay be core, facilitating, or supporting and provideconfidence in our results. Third, with an R2 of 23%, the overall variation explained by the model appears to be low. However, this could have resultedpartiallyfrom loss of informationresulting from the dichotomousnatureof the independentvariable (Cohen 1983). Furthermore,because the main concern in this study is to test the asymmetry hypothesis ratherthan explain variationin the dependentvariable,the low R2 is not particularlytroublesome. Cumulative impact and diminishing sensitivity (H3a and H3b).To test H3aand H3b,the following equation was estimated: + b2(POSITIVE)2 (4) Overallsatisfaction= bi(POSITIVE) + b3(NEGATIVE)+ b4(NEGATIVE)2. In Equation4, POSITIVEand NEGATIVEare the sums of positively and negatively valenced mentionsof attributes, respectively. Therefore, bl and b3 model the asymmetric cumulative impact of positive and negative performanceon overall satisfaction. To test for diminishing sensitivity, the squaredtermsare introduced(cf. Hamilton 1992). Thus, for positive performance,bl and b2 model diminishingsensitivity, whereas for negative performance, b3 and b4 model diminishing sensitivity. These tests (for H3a and H3b) are describedin detail next. Cumulative impact of number of positive or negative attributementions(H3a).As statedpreviously,bI and b3 can be used to test the asymmetryfor the cumulative impact of negative and positive mentionsof attributeson overall satisfaction statedin H3a.Because negative mentionsof attribute performanceare hypothesized to have greater impacts on overall satisfactionthanare positive mentions,lb3\shouldbe greaterthan Ibli.Similar to the test of Hia, two rival models are comparedto test whether the model with the constraint Ib31< Ibil performsbetter or worse than the unconstrained model. If the constrained model performs worse than the unconstrainedmodel, the constraintis rejected, which supports|b31> Ibil.Results of the analysis are shown in Table 2. The coefficients for POSITIVEand NEGATIVEare .29 and -.72, respectively. Both are in the expected direction and significantat p < .0001. Moreover,theirabsolute magnitude is as hypothesized;the absolute value of the coefficient for NEGATIVEis greaterthan the absolute value of the coefficient for POSITIVE.This was confirmed by comparingthe constrainedand unconstrainedmodels as explained previously. The model with the constraintlb31< Ibll was rejected in favor of the unconstrainedmodel, where lb3l> Ibjl(Fi,4477 = 50.37, p < .0001). Thus, H3ais supported;3cumulatively, est con-elation to be .21. A majorityof the correlationswere non- significant(p > .05) andbelow .10 in absolutemagnitude. Thus, multicollinearitydoes not appearto be an issue here. Furthermore, thethoughtcategoriesarenot linearlydependenton one another. 3These models also were estimated using standardizedcoefficients. The results for the models with standardizedcoefficients were practicallyidentical to the results reportedhere. 38 / Journalof Marketing,January1998 This content downloaded from 140.120.52.117 on Thu, 18 Dec 2014 21:27:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions TABLE 2 Regression Results for Satisfaction With Medical Care Variable was used alone, the function becomes a curve that "tapers off' when the squaredterm (b2) is included in the equation. This occurs because every increase in bl is countered by a smaller (because of the squared term) decrease in b2. For example, the estimate for bl is .28, whereasthe estimate for b2 is -.04. Considerthe effect of increasingpositive performance for one and two attributes. In the absence of the squared term, the beneficial impact of one attributeis .29 and of two attributesis .58; however, with b2, the beneficial impact increases but at a decreasingrate. Using both bl and b2, the beneficial impactof one attributeis .25, calculatedas .29(1) - .04(1)2, whereas the impactof two attributesis .42, calculatedas .29(2) - .04(2)2. Thus, inclusion of the squared term capturesdiminishing sensitivity. Similar results apply for b3 and b4 in the domainof negative performance.Results based on this analysis are reportedin Table 2. As seen in Table 2, all four coefficients are statistically significant and in the predicted direction, which thereby supports H3b. However, a key concern when using these data to test H3bis that supportfor H3bmay be an artifactof the way the variablesPOSITIVEand NEGATIVEwere constructed.Some of the attributesthat went into the construction of the variable may be inherently less importantor salient than others. Thus, the test for H3bmay be simply an artifactof the lower importanceor salience of some attributes used in the constructionof the cumulativevariablethan others. Additional tests, described in the Appendix, were used to test for this possibility. In these tests, the variables POSITIVEand NEGATIVEwere constructedin eight differentways, and H3bwas tested eight differenttimes. Therefore, including the base case described in Table 3, a total of nine tests were conducted for H3b.All nine tests supported the diminishing sensitivity in the positive performance domain, but only five supporteddiminishing sensitivity for negative attributeperformance.Therefore,erringon the side Standardized Regression Standard Coefficient Error t-statistic p-value Intercept POSITIVE NEGATIVE POSITIVE2 NEGATIVE2 4.24 .29 -.72 -.04 .07 R2 = .23; F4,4477 = 330.65, .02 .02 .05 .01 .02 199.48 13.87 -13.48 -7.78 3.01 .0001 .0001 .0001 .0001 .0030 p < .0001. Test for constraint for Ho in Equation 2: F1,4477= 50.37, p < .0001 (one-tailed test). negative performance on attributes has a significantly greater impact on overall satisfaction than positive performance on attributes.The regression coefficients show that for this particularservice, the deleterious effect of an additional instance of negative performanceon attributesoutweighs the beneficial effect of positive performanceby a marginof two to one (-.72 versus .29). Diminishingsensitivity(H3b). This hypothesis suggests a nonlinearrelationshipbetween overall satisfactionand performance on additionalattributes.This relationshipis modeled using the polynomial function describedin Equation4. In Equation4, bl and b2 test for diminishing sensitivity in the domain of positive performance. The linear positive coefficient bl models the positive impact of performance, whereas b2, the coefficient for the squared term, models diminishing sensitivity by creatinga "dampening"effect on the slope of bl. Instead of being a straight line as when bl Logistic Regression TABLE 3 for Intention to Switch to a Different PCP Variable Model 1 Attribute Intercept -1.50 Model 2 Overall Satisfaction 4.17 (.07) POSITIVE NEGATIVE -.66 (.05) 1.21 (.08) Overall satisfaction Chi-square (d.f.) AIC (.23) 3.14 (.24) -.43 (.05) 708.7 (2) (p < .0001) 2905.22 Model 3 Attributeand Overall Satisfaction -1.45 (.06) 989.9 (1) (p < .0001) 2606.10 .76 (.08) -1.15 (.06) 1182.2 (3) (p < .0001) 2417.71 % correctly classified 86.0% 88.1% 89.5% Allcoefficientestimates are significantat p < .001; standarderrorsare in parentheses. It could be argued that POSITIVEand NEGATIVE are not 2 but 43 variablesbecause they are composed of multiplebinaryvariables.Therefore, the AICalso was recalculatedafterpenalizingModels 1 and 3 for43 ratherthan 2 variables.The results for model fitwere virtuallyidentical to the one reportedin the table. Negativeand PositiveAttribute-Level Performance/ 39 This content downloaded from 140.120.52.117 on Thu, 18 Dec 2014 21:27:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions of conservatism, the conclusion is that diminishing sensitiv- ity holds for positive performancebut not for negative performanceon attributes. These results suggest that overall satisfaction displays diminishingsensitivity to each additionalinstanceof positive performancebut not necessarilyto negative performance.In other words, though the relative benefit of positive performance on additionalattributesmay decrease,each additional instanceof negative performancemay be equallydeleterious. Repurchase intentions (H4 and H5). Because the measure for switching intentions was a dichotomousvariable, a logistic regressionanalysis was conductedto test H4and H5. Three models predicting intention to switch to a different PCP were estimated. Note that though the hypotheses are stated in terms of repurchase intentions, results are discussed in terms of intention to switch to maintain consistency with the data. The models used for testing H4 and H5, which are shown in Table 3, are as follows: NEGATIVE}; (5) Model1:intentionto switch= f POSITIVE, (6) Model2: intentionto switch= f{Overallsatisfaction); NEGATIVE, (7) Model3: intentionto switch= f{POSITIVE, Overallsatisfaction}. In Model 1, only POSITIVEand NEGATIVEwere used as predictorvariables. In Model 2, only overall satisfaction was used as a predictorvariable.The thirdmodel used POSITIVE, NEGATIVE,and overallsatisfactionas predictorvariables. Note that these models are concernedonly with asymmetry,not diminishingsensitivity;therefore,only the cumulative variablesPOSITIVEand NEGATIVEare used in these models. Results from these models are reportedin Table 3. Results for Model 1, the attributes-onlymodel, confirm the hypothesis that negative attributesare more consequential for intention to switch than are positive attributesof the service. Thus, H5 is supported. Next, all three models in Table 3 were examined to test H4.The fit of the models was examined using the Akaike InformationCriterion(AIC statistic) of model evaluation(Bollen 1989).The AIC evaluates alternativemodels while accountingfor the numberof parameters in rival models; it penalizes models with many rather than few free parameters.Models with smaller AIC values provide a better fit. The AIC for each of the three models is shown in Table 3. The overall satisfactionmodel (Model 2) provides a better fit in predicting repurchase intentions than does the attribute performance model (Model I); the AIC fit for Model 2 is smaller thanthatof Model 1. However,Model 3, which includes both attributeperformanceand overall satisfaction, outperformseither model. The value of the AIC for Model 3 is smaller than either the overall satisfactionor the attribute performance model. Thus, H4 is supported.4 4These results also were verified by dividing the data set in two parts: one with 3517 and the other with 1000 people. The three models were estimated with the first data set and then used to predict the value for intentionto switch in the holdoutsample of 1000 people. The coefficient estimates were virtually identical to the ones reportedin Table 4, and the percentagescorrectly classified for the three models were 65%, 69.9%, and 78.9%, respectively. These results again supportthe hypotheses. Attributeperformanceand overall satisfactionhave separate and distinct effects on repurchaseintentions. Summaryof resultsand limitations.The resultsfrom this study support the hypothesized asymmetry and provide a strong indicationof the existence of diminishingsensitivity, at least on a cumulative basis. Negative performanceon seven of eight pairs of attributeshas a greater impact on overall satisfaction than does positive performance.This is also true of the cumulative impact of negative versus positive attributes. Similarly, intentions to switch also are affected asymmetrically by positive and negative performance. Overall satisfaction appearsto display diminishing sensitivityto attributeperformancein the domainof positive performancebut not negative performance;however, this conclusion cannot be offered for different levels of performance within each attribute.Finally, both overall satisfaction and attributeperformancehave separate and distinct impactson repurchaseintentions. These results should be viewed in light of the study's limitations.First, there were no data on expectationdisconfirmationat the attributelevel; this precludedtesting Hlb, a limitation that is addressed in Study 2. Second, attributelevel performance was measured using 0,1 categories, which precludedmeasurementof differentlevels of performance for an individualattribute.Thus, Hla and H2, which are statedfor differentlevels of performancewithin an individual attribute,could not be tested. Study 3, in which performancelevels are measuredfor each attribute,fully examines Hia and H2. Third, Study 1 was conductedin a service setting; therefore,it could be argued that the results of this study would not necessarily generalize to a product-related setting. Although there is no theoretical reason to expect this, testing these resultsin a productsetting should increase confidence in the generalizability. Study 2 The purposeof this study is twofold: (1) to corroboratethe findings of Study 1 in a different setting-automobiles-and (2) to test Hlb, which postulatesan asymmetric impact of positive and negative disconfirmationon attributes on overall satisfaction. It is important to provide a separate test of Hlb, because both performance and disconfir- mation have been found to have distinct impactson overall satisfaction (Yi 1990). Sample Data for this study were obtained from a mail survey of new car buyers throughout the United States. This survey is part of an ongoing tracking study conducted by a domestic automotive manufacturer. This study uses a mail survey, and the response rate for a typical wave of this study ranges from 36% to 40%, which is similar to the industry standard for satisfaction surveys in general. For example, Mittal and colleagues (1993) report results from a large-scale automotive study with a response rate of 33.5%, Hall (1995) reports results from a patient satisfaction context with a response rate of 41.1%, and LaBarbera and Mazursky (1983) report results from grocery item shoppers with a response rate of 25%. 40 / Journalof Marketing,January1998 This content downloaded from 140.120.52.117 on Thu, 18 Dec 2014 21:27:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions The database included responses from 9359 new car buyers in one wave of the larger study. These respondents were selected randomlyfrom the largerdatabaseof 250,000 respondents that the organizationhas accumulated.A chisquare test on key demographicsand t-tests on the dependent variableshowed no differencebetween the largerdatabase and the random subsample. Furthermore,informal comparisons with other industry databases and inspection by managersin the automotiveresearchindustryconfirmed that the sample was nationallyrepresentativeof automobile buyers. Measures Overall satisfaction was measuredon a ten-point scale with the following anchors: 1 = very dissatisfied and 10 = completely satisfied. Furthermore,disconfirmation measures were available on ten attributesthathad been selected by the firm on the basis of qualitativeresearch.Attribute-leveldisconfirmationwas measuredon a three-pointscale in which respondentswere asked to indicate whetherperformanceon each attributewas "betterthan what you expected," "about what you expected," or "worse than what you expected." Operationalizingdisconfirmationin this way facilitates a comparison of the asymmetric impact of positive versus negative disconfirmationin a mannerthat is consistent with previous studies (cf. Oliver and DeSarbo 1988). This measure was coded as a dummy variable such that "betterthan expected" was coded as (1,0), "worse than expected" was coded as (0,1), and "aboutas expected" was coded as (0,0). With this coding scheme, the asymmetric effects between positive and negative disconfirmationon each attributecan be compared; the neutral level serves as the "baseline" against which the coefficients for "above" or "below" expectations are computed (cf. Pedhazur 1982). In other words, the estimates for the neutrallevel (aboutas expected) for each attributeare confounded with the interceptterm. Analysis and Results A dummy-variableregression was conducted to test Hlb. The specific equation that was estimated is (8)Overall Satisfaction = Intercept + bll AttributeI (aboveexpect.) + b21 Attributel (below expect.) + ... b1 10 Attributelo (above expect.) + b2 Io Attribute 10(belowexpect.)- In Equation8, bli representsthe relative impact of positive disconfirmation on attributei, whereas b-i represents the relative impact of negative disconfirmationon the same attributeon overall satisfaction. Using this formulation,the impact of each attribute on overall satisfaction was estimated. This data set had expectation/disconfirmationdata on ten separateattributes,which resultedin 20 specific coefficients, one for positive and one for negative disconfirmation on each of the ten attributes.Results of the analysis are shown in Table 4 and are discussed subsequently in two parts. First, the substantive results related to HIb are discussed; second, the overall model, in terms of the variance explained and the differentattributesincluded in the model, is discussed. Recall that Hib postulatesthat negative disconfirmation on an attributewill have a greaterimpact than positive dis- Regression Attribute TABLE 4 Results for Study 2 Dummy-Variable Regression Coefficients Positive Negative Disconfirmation Disconfirmation Comfort Ease of getting in and out of vehicle Driver'sseating comfort Ride quality Maneuverability/ handling -.86*** .24*** -.39*** .34*** -.29* -.42** .11* .06n.s. -.67*** .16** Quietness of engine -.39** .07n.s. -.62*** -1.43*** -.39** -.32*** .08n.s .42*** .01n.s .08n.s. Powerand pickup Qualityof vehicle Brakes operation Transmission R2 = .18; F20,9339 = 104.70, p < .0001. ***p< .0001; **p < .01; *p < .05; n.s.p > .10. confirmation on the same attribute.Table 4 supports this hypothesis. For all ten attributes,the absolute value of the coefficients is greaterfor negative disconfirmationthan for positive disconfirmation.For example, the impact of negative disconfirmationon "comfort"is three times largerthan the impact of positive disconfirmationon the same attribute (-.86 versus .24). Negative disconfirmationon "quality of vehicle" affects overall satisfaction three times more than positive disconfirmation(-1.43 versus .42). Equivalent results can be noted for other attributes, though the magnitude of the asymmetry between positive and negative disconfirmationvaries somewhat. For example, the magnitudeof the asymmetryis smaller for "ease of getting in and out of vehicle" (-.39 versus .34) than for "power and pickup"(-.62 versus .08). Overall, the results supportHlb. More interesting,for many attributes,positive disconfirmation has a nonsignificant impact (e.g., "ride quality," "quietness of engine"). For these attributes, it seems that there is no additionalbenefit in exceeding customer expectations, though the negative consequences of not meeting expectations are relatively high. These results also confirm the findings of Study 1 that differentattributes have varying degrees of impact on overall satisfaction. For example, the impactof negative disconfirmationon "quality of vehicle" (-1.43, p < .0001) is much largerthan negative disconfirmationon "driver'sseatingcomfort"(-.29, p < .05) or "ride quality" (-.42, p < .01). Again, this suggests that attributescould be classified as core or facilitatingattributes on the basis of the magnitudeof the asymmetry. However, the results of both Studies I and 2 must be scrutinizedfurtherin light of the fact thatthe models used in .bothstudies explain a small amount of variation(23% and 18%,respectively) in overall satisfaction.There are several possible reasons for this. One explanation is that when dummy variablesinstead of regularlyscaled items are used to operationalize attribute-levelperformanceor disconfirmation, the overall varianceexplained by the model generally decreases because of !oss of informationfor the indePerformance/41 Negativeand PositiveAttribute-Level This content downloaded from 140.120.52.117 on Thu, 18 Dec 2014 21:27:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions pendentvariable(Cohen 1983; Cox 1980). In the context of satisfactionliterature,only one study has examined the issue of scale categories and explained variation. Wittink and Bayer (1994) conducted a quasi-experimental study to investigate different measurementsystems for satisfaction research. They found that the system using dichotomized variables for measuring attribute-level performance (e.g., high versus low) had a lower R2 than the one using a fivepoint scale for measuringattribute-levelperformance.Thus, it can be arguedthatthe lower R2 is simply an artifactof the measurementsystem adoptedhere; still, we cannot rule out the alternative explanation that all possible attributesthat explain overall satisfactionwere not included. This alternative explanation will be examined by comparingthe results from Studies 2 and 3, because both pertainto overall satisfaction with an automobileand measuresimilar attributes. Study 3 Study 3 included data from 13,759 respondentswho filled out a satisfaction survey in the context of the automotive industry. Before describing the study, it is important to understandthe specific rationalebehind including Study 3. Although the results of the previous studies indicate some support of the central thesis-namely, the asymmetry and diminishing returns-of this work, they do not test adequately for both asymmetry and diminishing returns for each individual attribute(Hia and H2). For example, Study 1 shows diminishing sensitivity but only for all attributes combined, whereas Study 2 is unableto test for diminishing sensitivity. Therefore,to providedirectevidence for Hla and H2, this thirdstudy is reported. Sample Analysis for Study 3 was based on a customer satisfaction data set of a major automotive manufacturerin the United States. This data set was assembled using a mail survey methodology. Similarto the data in Study 2, these data were collected as partof an ongoing trackingstudy to assess satisfaction with various aspects of the vehicle ownership experience. A survey was mailed to people who owned their vehicle for two or more years. The responserate for this survey varied from wave to wave but rangedbetween 38% and 42% for the last three waves. In the survey,respondentsratedtheiroverall satisfaction with the vehicle and evaluatedperformanceon six different attributes:brakes, transmission,power and pickup, vehicle quality, quietness, and interiorroominess. Data from 13,759 respondents who answered this section of the survey were made available for this analysis. Similar to Study 2, the demographic profile of these respondents was compared with the demographic profile of the larger database from which they were extracted. No statistically significant differences were found in terms of age, sex, income, race, or geographic location (for all, p > .5). Measures There were two main constructs: overall satisfaction and attribute-level performance.Overall satisfaction was measured on a ten-point scale (10 = completely satisfied, I = completely dissatisfied). Performanceon each of the six attributeswas measuredon an eight-pointscale (4/3 = very satisfied; 2/1 = somewhat satisfied; -1/-2 = somewhat dissatisfied; and -3/-4 = very dissatisfied with performanceon the attribute).Note that this scale is a satisfactionscale and therefore does not measure performance directly at the attributelevel. However, recent studies in the area of customer satisfaction show that though attribute-levelsatisfaction and performanceare conceptually distinct constructs, their measures are highly correlated.For example, Spreng, MacKenzie, and Olshavsky (1996) find high correlation between attribute-level satisfaction and performance for camcorders.In their study, the correlationbetween performance and satisfaction for "versatility"was .72 (p < .01), and "picture"was .80 (p < .01). Furthermore,both performance and satisfaction ratingson each attributehad similar correlations with overall satisfaction. Bitner and Hubbert (1994, p. 85) find a high interfactor correlation (.94) between satisfaction and quality/performanceand conclude that "thisfinding providesevidence for the notion thatoverall service satisfaction and service quality may be tapping elements of the same construct."Thus, for the sake of measurement,the use of the satisfactionscale as a surrogateof attribute-level performance is reasonable for the current study. However, this is in no way designed to suggest that the conceptual distinction between performanceand satisfaction at the attributelevel should not be maintained. Recall that the scale being used to measure attributelevel performanceis an eight-pointscale; on this scale, positive ratingson an attributereflect positive performanceon that attribute, whereas negative ratings indicate negative performanceon that attribute.For each attribute,there are four levels or grades of positive performance(1, 2, 3, 4) and four grades of negative performance(-1, -2, -3, -4). Thus, the effect of differentlevels of performanceand diminishing sensitivity can be modeled for each individualattribute. Analysis and Results The analyticstrategyfor this study was adaptedfromAnderson and Sullivan (1993). Accordingly, the asymmetricand diminishing impact of each attributeon overall satisfaction is modeled as follows: (9) OverallSatisfaction= Intercept + LN_PERFi + LP_PERFi ... LN_PERF6+ LP_PERF6. In Equation 9, the measurement variable for each attribute is decomposed into LP_PERF and LN_PERF, where L denotes the natural logarithm of each measure, P_PERF or N_PERF (for positive and negative performance, respectively). Thus, performanceon each attribute is decomposed into positive and negative performanceas follows: If performance on the first attribute is negative, the measurement variable for negative attribute performance, LN_PERFI, is equal to -In (-PERFI),5 and 5Thenegativesign for PERFIfor negativeperformance makes the finaloutcomepositiveandallowsthe use of the naturallogarithmof the number.Forexample,if thereis a negativeperformanceratingof -4, thensettingthevariableto -(-4) yields+4, for whichit is possibleto takethe naturallogarithm.Recallthatnaturallogarithms do notexist for negativenumbers. 42 / Journalof Marketing,January1998 This content downloaded from 140.120.52.117 on Thu, 18 Dec 2014 21:27:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions LP_PERFJis equal to zero. If performanceon an attribute is positive, then LP_PERF is equal to In (PERFI), and LN_PERFI is equal to zero. Thus, if the first attribute received a rating of -4, LN_PERFi = -In {-(-4)}, and LP_PERFI = 0. Conversely, if an attribute was rated 3, LP_PERFI = ln{3}, and LN_PERFI = 0. Note that in this analysis plan, two coefficients are estimated for each attributefor a total of 12 coefficients. This attribute-levelmeasurementplan accomplishestwo goals. First,it ensures thatall of the coefficients will be positive; thus, for example, the coefficients for positive and negative performanceon a given attributewill be positive. This makes the interpretationof resultsmoreconvenientand therefore managerially useful. Thus, if the coefficient for negative performance is larger than the coefficient for positive performance on an attribute, the hypothesized asymmetry would be supported.Second, the naturallogarithm transformationcaptures diminishing returnor sensitivity (Anderson and Sullivan 1993). For example, if the coefficient for positive performanceon an attributeis significant, it can be interpretedas supportingthe diminishing sensitivity hypothesis for positive performance on that attribute. At this point, it is necessary to explain why this plan of analysis ratherthan the one used in Equation4 was adopted to model the asymmetry and diminishing sensitivity. First, to use an analytic plan similar to Equation 4, four coefficients would have been requiredto model the asymmetry and diminishing returnson each of the six attributes(positive, negative, positive2, and negative2 for each attribute); thus, the use of a plan similar to Equation4 would necessitate the estimation of 24 coefficients. Equation 9, conversely, requiresthe estimationof only two coefficients per attributefor a total of 12 coefficients. Second, the use of squared terms in polynomial equations such as Equation4 introduces severe multicollinearity(e.g., positive and positive2 for an attribute are likely to be highly correlated). Although such multicollinearitymay not be a serious issue for a single attribute,when modeling several attributes,it becomes difficult to detect and control for (e.g., Wittinkand Bayer 1994). Thus, Equation9 not only providesa parsimonious representationof the model (12 instead of 24 terms), but also circumventsissues relatedto multicollinearitythat would arise because of the use of squaredterms along with the base terms (e.g., positive and positive2) for several attributes.Finally, the use of Equation9 not only complements the results of Study 1 by testing the asymmetry and diminishingsensitivity for each individualattribute,but also compares attribute-level results with those reported by Andersonand Sullivan (1993). Given this measurementand analysis plan, each coefficient in Equation 9 should be positive. The asymmetry is tested by constrainingthe positive and negative coefficients for each attributeto be equal (LN_PERFj= LP_PERFj), determining whether the constraint can or cannot be rejected,and reportingwhetherLN_PERFj> LP_PERFj.If the constraintis rejectedand the absolute size of the coefficient for negative performanceis greaterthanthe coefficient for positive performance,then the asymmetryis supported. Furthermore,the naturallogarithmtransformationcaptures diminishingsensitivity, and a statisticallysignificant coefficient indicates support for the diminishing hypothesis. Results for these analyses are reportedin Table 5 and discussed next. Asymmetrybetween positive and negative performance (Hla). The results in Table5 show that all estimates are statistically significant. Forfive of the six attributes,Hla is supported because LN_PERF > LP_PERF and the constraint LN_PERF = LP_PERF is rejected. A comparison of the coefficients shows that the magnitudeof the asymmetry is different for each attribute.Thus, the magnitude of the asymmetry is much larger for "transmission"(.04 versus .35) than for "quietness"(.11 versus .23). Similar to the results of Study 2, "vehicle quality"has the largest impact on overall satisfaction(even after two years of ownership). Diminishing sensitivity. The logarithm transformation facilitates testing the diminishingsensitivity hypothesis. As the values get more extreme, a logarithmicfunction tapers off and thus resembles the diminishing sensitivity curve. Thus, significant coefficients that are based on the natural logarithm indicate support for the diminishing sensitivity hypothesis (cf. Anderson and Sullivan 1993). However, TABLE 5 Regression Results for Study 3 Attribute Brakes Transmission Power and pickup Vehicle quality Quietness Interiorroominess LP_PERF LN PERF (Regression coefficients for positive performance on an attribute) O.On.s. .04* .06** (Regression coefficients for negative performance on an attribute) .24*** .35*** .21*** 1.41*** .11*** (LN_PERF= LP_PERF) Yes*** Yes*** Yes* 1.86*** .23*** Yes*** Yes* .49*** .17* Reject Constraint? Yes*** R2 = .65; F12,13758= 2210.1, p < .0001. ***p< .0001;**p< .01;*p< .05;n.sp> .10. Performance/ 43 Negativeand PositiveAttribute-Level This content downloaded from 140.120.52.117 on Thu, 18 Dec 2014 21:27:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions whetherthe logarithmictransformationaddedany additional explanatorypower to that of a model without diminishing returnsalso was checked. Anotherbaseline model in which attributeperformancehad not undergonea logarithmictransformation also was estimated. Results from both models were virtuallyidentical. For the baseline model-that is, the model without the log-transformedvariables-R2 = .65 and F12,13758= 2210.9 show equally good fit. A chow-test confirmedthatthe model with diminishingreturs did not fit significantlybetterthanthe model withoutdiminishingreturns. Therefore,even thoughthe model with diminishingreturnsis statistically significant, it can be concluded that it does not add much to the explanatorypower of the model. These results show that the relationship between attribute-leveloutcomes and overall evaluationsis complex. These results have several implications for research and practice that are discussed next. Implications for Research Satisfaction research at an attribute level typically has viewed attributes as unidimensional and classified them accordingly.That is, most typologies classify attributeson the basis of their weight or importancein determiningoverall satisfaction(cf. Bolton and Drew 1991; LaTourand Peat 1979). These results show that typologies that classify attributeson the basis of their weight alone might not be Summaryof results. The results supportthe asymmetry enough. More specifically, there is a need to develop a between different levels of negative and positive attribute typology that enables researchers to understandwhy the performancebut are inconclusive with regardto diminishing magnitudeof the asymmetryis differentfor differentattribreturns.One importantpartof the results is thatfor "roomiutes. That is, is the observed asymmetryrelated to the utilness" a reversalof the hypothesizedasymmetryis observed. ity-preserving or enhancing qualities of an attribute (cf. The implications of this asymmetry for developing a theoKahn and Meyer 1991)? By their very nature, utility-preretical typology of attributesas they relate to overall satisserving attributes(e.g., correctdiagnosis by a doctor,safety faction are discussed in a subsequentsection. in air travel) seem to have a higher potential for negative In addition, Study 3, which has a higher R2 than Study disconfirmation,whereas utility-enhancingattributes(e.g., 2, is based largely on the same attributesas Study 2. "Intedoctor is humorous, entertainmentaboard a flight) have a rior roominess" is the only attributethat is not measured higher potential for positive disconfirmation.Accordingly, directly in Study 2. However, it can be arguedthat in Study in Study 3, "interiorroominess"would be a utility-enhanc2, attributessuch as "comfort,""ease of getting in and out of ing attribute,whereas the other attributesmight classify as vehicle," "driver'sseating comfort,"and "ridequality"caputility-preservingattributes.Yet anotherreasoncould be the ture most of the aspects of "interiorroominess"from Study level of performanceand disconfirmationobserved in the 3. Similarly,though Study 1 has four times the attributesof past. Thus, for airline safety, consumers typically observe Study 3, the variance explained by Study 3 is much higher. high positive performance/disconfirmation (i.e., air travel is Thus, it seems reasonableto infer that the lower R2 obtained safe). If this expectation suddenly were disconrelatively in Studies I and 2 is an artifactof informationloss due to firmed negatively, then it would become salient and affect coarse measurementscales ratherthanexclusion of relevant overall satisfaction. If a customer frequents a restaurant attributes. where the service is consistently below expectations (i.e., negative performance)and receives bad service again on his or her next visit, there would be little furtherimpact. However, if the customer suddenly received top-qualityservice, We investigate the link between attribute-level perforit would have a great impact on overall satisfaction. This mance/disconfirmation,satisfaction, and repurchaseintenline of logic also helps explain why there was a reversalof tions. Using three large-scale data sets, several hypotheses the hypothesized asymmetry for "interior roominess." It about these relationshipswere tested. The results are sumcould be that in the past, automobilecustomersalways have marized as follows: expected low performance on interior roominess; thus, *Both overall satisfactionand repurchaseintentionsare encountering high performance on "interior roominess" affectedasymmetrically and causes a higher level of disconfirmation(in the positive by attribute-level performance disconfirmation.That is, negative performance/disconfir- direction). However, these ideas are speculative and their mationon an attributehas a greaterimpactthan positive resolution is beyond the scope of this research.The develperformance/disconfirmation. opment of a theoreticallydriven typology thatenables a res*Regardingdiminishing returns,the results are mixed. For all olution of these findings should be a welcome addition to attributescombined, there is supportfor diminishing returns satisfaction research. in the domain of positive performancebut not negative performance (Study 1). When tested for diminishingreturnsfor Analytically, these results call into question previous each individualattribute(Study 3), there is supportfor diminconceptualizations that treat the relationship between ishing sensitivity for both positive and negative performance. attribute-levelperformanceand overall satisfactionas symHowever, data also show that a linear conceptualizationfits metric and linear(cf. LaTourand Peat 1979). More broadly, the data as well as the diminishing returnconceptualization. these results have implicationsfor multi-attributemodels of *Finally, results show that, in additionto the impact mediated consumer decision making and choice (cf. Kahnand Meyer by satisfaction, attribute-level performance has a direct 1991; Wilkie and Pessemier 1973). Typical models of deciimpact on repurchaseintentions. This result calls into quession making do not distinguishbetween attributeson which tion previous models that assume that the impact of performance on repurchaseintentionsis mediatedby overall evalua consumer has experienced positive or negative perforations (e.g., satisfaction). mance. Depending on this past experience, the weights of Discussion 44 / Journalof Marketing,January1998 This content downloaded from 140.120.52.117 on Thu, 18 Dec 2014 21:27:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions the attributesin subsequent decisions could shift dramatically. For example, a consumer who has experienced positive performanceon comfort in the decision to purchasea car may weight it less than another who has experienced negative performanceon comfort. Previous experiences of attribute-levelperformancethus serve as importantcontextual cues for subsequent decisions and choices. Applying these findings to multi-attributemodels of consumerchoice is a key directionfor furtherresearch. Similarly,these results also point towarda careful reexaminationof satisfactionand its consequentbehaviors,such as retentionand word-of-mouthactivity. Previousconceptualizations have assumed these relationshipsto be linear and symmetric (Yi 1990). However, satisfaction's impact on these behaviors could be asymmetric and nonlinear.High levels of satisfaction might not increase retention,but high levels of dissatisfaction might have a large and deleterious impact on retention.An asymmetricconceptualizationalso could help explain the curious finding that most firms are experiencing, that is, high rates of customer defection despite high rates of customer satisfaction (Reichheld 1996). High dissatisfaction with a product might prompt high levels of information search for alternative brands, whereas high levels of satisfaction might not decrease the amount of informationsearch substantially;this asymmetry has implicationsfor considerationset formationand choice. Given the increased emphasis on linking satisfaction research to the bottom line (Reichheld 1996), these issues warrantcareful attention. Finally, the finding that attribute-levelperformancealso has a direct impact on repurchaseintentionsnot only is surprising but also calls into question previous models of satisfaction. These models (for a review, see lacobucci et al. 1996) presumethat attribute-levelevaluations affect behavioral intentions but only through an overall satisfaction judgment. This finding raises several importantquestions that need more research. First, the conditions under which the direct impact of attribute-level outcomes is larger (smaller) than the one mediatedthroughsatisfactionshould be outlined. Second, it could be that the mediating role of overall satisfaction is different for different attributesand dependenton the magnitudeand directionof the asymmetry. Developing a typology of attributes should help resolve these issues. Implications for Practice The key implication for managers is that they no longer should view attribute performance with a neutral eye. Although positive and negative performanceon an attribute are two sides of the same coin, each side of the coin buys a different amountof overall satisfactionor repurchaseintentions. This asymmetry should be kept in mind in trying to manage attribute-levelperformance.The strategic implication of this result is clear: To maximize overall satisfaction and repurchase intentions, managers should optimize and not maximize attribute-levelperformance.For example, for any given attributeit is more importantto eliminatenegative performancefirst and then focus on increasingperformance in the positive direction. Similarly, in a given set of attrib- utes, focus on attributesfor which customersare experiencing negative performance and then allocate resources to maximizing performanceon attributesfor which consumers are experiencing positive performance. Concluding Comments Despite the initial wave of skepticism with satisfaction research among practitioners and academics, it now is acknowledged that customer satisfaction and retention are key strategic imperatives and not fads (e.g., Honomichl 1993; Reichheld 1996). Increasingly,researchersalso are finding that the relationships between satisfaction and its antecedents and consequences are complex (cf. Anderson and Sullivan 1993; Coyne 1989; Oliva, Oliver, and Bearden 1995). Therefore, understandingthe complexity of these interrelationshipshas become a key strategicimperativefor most firms (e.g, Jones and Sasser 1995). We hope that this work represents a small step in that direction and will encourage further research aimed at understandingthese complexities. Appendix Follow-up tests for H3bwere conducted to ensure that the reported results are not due to the differing salience or importance of some of the attributes used in the study. Salience is operationalizedas the frequency with which an attributeis mentioned (higher numberof mentions= higher salience), whereas importance is operationalized as the magnitudeof the regressionweight (largerregressioncoefficient = higher importance).To check for this possibility, the cumulative variables were reconstructed, excluding those attributesthat were not importantor salient. To check for the salience bias, the first set of variables was constructedfrom attributeson the basis of the number of mentions. With number of mentions as a proxy for salience, only those attributesthatreceived the top 3, 4, 5, or 6 mentions were included. To clarify, POSFRQ3 and NEGFRQ3used only those attributesthat received the first, second, and third highest number of mentions. POSFRQ4 and NEGFRQ4 also included the attributethat received the fourthhighest numberof mentions, and so on. To check for the importance bias, the cumulative variables were constructedon the basis of their imputedimportance.With the regression weight in Table 2 as a proxy for an attribute's importance, variables called POSREG5 and NEGREG5 were constructed using those attributes that had the five highest regressionweights for the positive and negative categories. Similar variablesalso were constructedon the basis of the top six and seven regressionweights. These strategies of integrationare consistent with prior theoreticaland empirical research(e.g., Dawes 1979). Furthermore,satisfaction researchers(e.g., Oliver 1993) have used additive-linearcombinationsto integrateperformance on several attributes.To summarize,eight additionalsets of variables were constructed: POSFRQ or NEGFRQ3-6, POSREG or NEGREG5-7, and POSWTD or NEGWTD5. Next, the following model was estimated for each set of variables: Performance/ 45 Negativeand PositiveAttribute-Level This content downloaded from 140.120.52.117 on Thu, 18 Dec 2014 21:27:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions TABLE A1 Summary of Various Tests for Asymmetry and Diminishing Sensitivity Restrictions Test Variable R2 -.98*** .15* .14 9.8, -.78*** .07n.s. .15 4.9, .0300 -.73*** -1.38*** -.78*** .02n.s. .28*** .08n.s. .17 .18 .18 -.13*** -.09*** -.08** -1.22*** -1.21*** -.80*** .20*** .22*** .05n.s. 6.4, 50.9, 16.9, .19 .21 .14 -.04*** -.72*** 44.2, .0001 66.6, .0001 24.9, .0001 .07** .23 50.4, b2 POS3FRQ (salience) .58*** -.16*** POS4FRQ (salience) .53*** -.13*** POS5FRQ (salience) POS5RG (importance) POS6FRQ (salience) .49*** .54*** .44*** -.11*** -.14*** -.09*** .52*** .46*** .35*** .29*** POS6RG (importance) POS7RG (importance) POSMTCH(importance) POSALL (all attributes/initial case) ***p< .0001; **p< .001; *p < .05; n.s.p> .10. (F-statistic, significance) b4 bl Overall satisfaction= bi(POSITIVE)+ b2(POS[TIVE)2 + b3(NEGATIVE)+ b4(NEGATIVE)2. The results are described in Table Al. In all of the eight additional cases plus the initial case, lb31> Ibll, which indicates that negative performance has a greater impact than positive performance as shown in the columns labeled bi and b2 in Table A 1. This result was significant at p < .03 in all nine cases (see the column labeled "Restrictions Test" in Table Al). These results show that the conclusion about H3 is not a methodological artifact. Regarding diminishing sensitivity, both b2 and b4 have the expected sign in all nine cases. Furthermore, b2 was sig- b3 .0020 .0100 .0001 .0001 .0001 nificant for all of the nine models (p < .001), but b4 was significant only for five of the nine models. To ascertain the "joint" statistical significance on the basis of these nine models, a combined test such as Fisher's combined test would be appropriate (Wolf 1986). However, because the nine comparisons are not independent, such a test of significance cannot be done. Therefore, erring on the side of conservatism, we conclude that over the nine tests, b4 is not significant, whereas b2 is. Thus, diminishing sensitivity holds for positive performance but not for negative performance on attributes. REFERENCES Anderson, Eugene W. and Mary W. Sullivan (1993), "The Antecedents and Consequences of Customer Satisfaction for Firms,"MarketingScience, 12 (Spring), 125-43. Bitner, Mary Jo, BernardH. Booms, and Mary Stanfield Tetreault (1990), "The Service Encounter: Diagnosing Favorable and Unfavorable Incidents,"Journal of Marketing, 54 (January), 71-84. and Amy R. Hubbert(1994), "EncounterSatisfactionVersus Overall Satisfaction Versus Quality: The Customer's Voice,"in Service Quality:New Directions in Theoryand Practice, RolandT. Rust and RichardL. Oliver, eds. London:Sage Publications,72-94. Bollen, KennethA. (1989), StructuralEquationswith Latent Variables. New York:John Wiley & Sons. Bolton, Ruth N. and James H. Drew (1991), "A MultistageModel of Customers' Assessments of Service Quality and Value," Journal of ConsumerResealrch,17 (March), 375-84. Boulding, William, Ajay Kalra, Richard Staelin, and ValarieA. Zeithaml (1993), "A Dynamic Process Model of Service Quality: From Expectations to Behavioral Intentions,"Journal of MarketingResearrch,30 (February),7-27. Cohen, Jacob (1983), "The Cost of Dichotomization,"Applied 7 (Summer), 249-53. Psychological Measulremlent, Cox, Eli P., III (1980), "The Optimal Number of Response Alternatives for a Scale: A Review,"Journal of MarketingResearch, 27 (November), 407-22. Coyne, Kevin (1989), "Beyond Service Fads-Meaningful Strategies for the Real World,"Sloan ManagementReview, 39 (Summer), 69-76. Dawes, Robyn M. (1979), "The RobustBeautyof ImproperLinear Models in Decision Making," AmlericaniPsychologist, 34 (July), 571-82. Einhorn,Hillel J. and Robin M. Hogarth(1981), "BehavioralDecision Theory: Processes of Judgment and Choice," Annual Review of Psychology, 32, 53-88. Feinberg,RichardA., RichardWiddows,MarlayaHirsch-Wyncott, and Charles Trappey (1990), "Myth and Reality in Customer Service: Good and Bad Service Sometimes Leads to Repurchase," Journal of CustomerSatisfaction, Dissatisfaction alnd ComplainingBehavior, 3, 112-14. Folkes, Valarie S. (1988), "Recent AttributionResearch in Consumer Behavior: A Review and New Directions,"Journal of ColsunLmerResearch, 14 (March),548-65. Gardial, Sarah Fisher, D. Scott Clemons, Robert B. Woodruff, David W. Schumann, and Mary Jane Burns (1994), "Comparing Consumers'Recall of Prepurchaseand PostpurchaseProduct Evaluation Experiences,"Journal of ConsumerResearch, 20 (March),548-60. Hall, Melvin F. (1995), "Patient Satisfaction or Acquiescence? Comparing Mail and Telephone Survey Results," Jou1rnalof Health Care Marketing, 15 (Spring), 54-61. Hamilton,LawrenceC. (1992), Regression with Graphics:A Second Course in Applied Statistics. Belmont, CA: Duxbury Press. Hanson, Randy (1992), "Determining Attribute Importance," Quirk'sMarketingResearch Review, 6 (October), 16-18. Honomichl, Jack (1993), "Spending on Customer Satisfaction Continuesto Rise," MarketingNews, 27 (April 12), 17-18. 46 / Journalof Marketing,January1998 This content downloaded from 140.120.52.117 on Thu, 18 Dec 2014 21:27:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Iacobucci, Dawn, Amy L. Ostrom, BridgetteM. Braig, and Alexa Bezjian-Avery(1996), "A CanonicalModel of ConsumerEvaluations and Theoretical Bases of Expectations,"Advances in Services Marketingand Management,5, 1-44. Jones, Thomas 0. and W. Earl Sasser, Jr. (1995), "Why Satisfied Customers Defect," Harvard Business Review, 73 (November/December),88-99. Kahn, BarbaraE. and RobertJ. Meyer (1991), "ConsumerMultiattributeJudgmentsunderAttributeWeight Uncertainty,"Journal of ConsumerResearch, 17 (March),508-22. Kahneman,Daniel and Amos Tversky (1979), "ProspectTheory: An Analysis of Decisions Under Risk," Econometrica, 47 (March), 263-91. Kekre, Sunder, Mayuram S. Krishnan, and Kannan Srinivasan (1995), "Drivers of Customer Satisfaction for Software Products: Implications for Design and Service Support,"Management Science, 41 (September), 1456-70. LaBarbera,Priscilla A. and David Mazursky(1983), "A Longitudinal Assessment of Consumer Satisfaction/Dissatisfaction: The Dynamic Aspect of the Cognitive Process," Journal of MarketingResearch, 20 (November), 393-404. LaTour,Stephen A. and Nancy C. Peat (1979), "Conceptualand Methodological Issues in Consumer SatisfactionResearch,"in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 6, William D. Perrault Jr., ed. Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Research, 431-37. Mittal, Vikas, Jerome M. Katrichis, Frank Forkin, and Mark Konkel (1993), "Does Satisfaction With Multi-AttributeProducts Vary Over Time? A Performance Based Approach," in Advances in ConsumerResearch, Vol. 21, Chris T. Allen and Deborah Roedder John, eds. Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 412-17. Oliva, Terence A., Richard L. Oliver, and William O. Bearden (1995), "The Relationship Among Consumer Satisfaction, Involvement, and Product Performance,"Behavioral Science, 40 (April), 104-32. , and lan C. MacMillan (1992), "A Catastrophe Model for Developing Service SatisfactionStrategies,"Journal of Marketing, 56 (July), 83-95. Oliver, Richard L. (1993), "Cognitive, Affective, and Attribute Bases of the Satisfaction Response," Journal of Consumer Research, 20 (December), 418-30. and WayneS. DeSarbo(1988), "ResponseDeterminantsin Satisfaction Judgments,"Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (March),495-507. Ostrom,Amy and Dawn Iacobucci (1995), "ConsumerTrade-Offs and the Evaluationof Services,"Journal of Marketing,59 (January), 17-28. Parasuraman,A., ValarieZeithaml, and LeonardL. Berry (1988), "SERVQUAL:A Multiple-ItemScale for MeasuringConsumer Perceptions of Service Quality," Journal of Retailing, 64 (Spring), 12-40. Pedhazur, Elazar J. (1982), Multiple Regression in Behavioral Research, 2d ed. New York:Holt, Rinehartand Winston. Peeters, G. and J. Czapinski(1990), "Positive-Negative Asymmetry in Evaluations: The Distinction Between Affective and InformationalNegativity Effect," European Review of Social Psychology, 1, 33-60. Reichheld, Fredrick(1996), The LoyaltyEffect: The HiddenForce Behind Growth, Profits, and Lasting Value. Boston: Harvard Business School Press. Spreng, Richard A., Scott B. MacKenzie, and Richard W. Olshavsky (1996), "A Reexaminationof the Determinantsof ConsumerSatisfaction,"Journalof Marketing,60 (July), 15-32. Taylor,Shelly (1982), "TheAvailabilityBias in Social Perception," in JudgmentUnder Uncertainty:Heuristics and Biases, Daniel Kahnemanet al., eds. Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress, 190-200. Wilkie, William L. and EdgarA. Pessemier(1973), "Issuesin Marketing's Use of Multi-AttributeAttitude Models," Journal of MarketingResearch, 10 (November), 428-41. Wittink,Dick R. and LeonardR. Bayer (1994), "The Measurement Imperative,"MarketingResearch,6 (Winter), 14-23. Wolf, Fredric M. (1986), Meta-Analysis: Quantitative Methods for Research Synthesis.London:Sage Publications. Yi, Youjae(1990), "A CriticalReview of ConsumerSatisfaction," in Review of Marketing 1990, Valarie A. Zeithaml, ed. Chicago: AmericanMarketingAssociation, 68-123. Performance/47 Negativeand PositiveAttribute-Level This content downloaded from 140.120.52.117 on Thu, 18 Dec 2014 21:27:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions