COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 1 COMM 495: Communication Capstone Professor Office Office Hours Office Phone Dr. Brian H. Spitzberg COMM Bldg 201 TTh 10-11:30, Th 1-2, & by Appt. 619.594.7097 (email preferred) Semester Schedule # Classroom Mailbox in: FALL 2013 20854 LA-2203 (Library Annex) COMM BLDG. 236/237 E-mail Texts spitz@mail.sdsu.edu Class Time Tue 4-6:40 Objectives 1. Stacks, D. W. & Salwen, M. B. (Eds.). (2009). An integrated approach to communication theory and research (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge. 2. i>clicker “clicker” and current semester registration (register through Blackboard!). 3. Additional readings specified in syllabus. This course seeks to both refresh and assess knowledge competencies in regard to the field of communication, especially as represented in the SDSU School of Communication. Think of it as “The 150 things every student should know about communication,” covering the following: I. CORE COMMUNICATION COMPETENCIES: DISCIPLINE: Demonstrate knowledge of key persons and events in the history of the discipline of communication; COMMUNICATION: Specify and differentiate key principles regarding the definition and nature of communication, language, nonverbal communication, and their contexts; THEORIES: Specify and differentiate the nature of theories, their criteria for evaluation, and paradigms; METHODS: Specify and differentiate key assumptions and practices of the major methodological paradigms in the field of communication; RELATIONSHIPS: Specify and differentiate key principles of core societal relationships formed through communication; SOCIETY: Specify and differentiate key principles of societal uses of communication (e.g., persuasion, political communication, rhetorical movements, intercultural communication). II. CORE PROFESSIONAL COMPETENCIES: Demonstrate the following competencies. ABILITY TO WRITE: Demonstrates proficiency in grammar, syntax, semantics, academic voice, application of APA style guidelines. ABILITY TO FORMULATE CLAIMS: Demonstrates ability to articulate researchable claims specifying the interrelationship among variables. ABILITY TO ARGUE COMPETENTLY: Demonstrates ability to articulate comprehensive arguments that include relevant and appropriate claims, warrants, and evidence, and argue ethically (i.e., originality/avoiding plagiarism). ABILITY TO CONDUCT SCHOLARLY RESEARCH: Demonstrates ability to locate and appropriately cite and list recent, relevant, and reasonable scholarly research, consisting mostly of peer-reviewed journal sources. ABILITY TO SELECT RESEARCH TOPICS RELEVANT TO THE COURSE: Content demonstrates relevance to the assignment and to the communication-based focus expected of the assignment. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 2 COMM 495: CAPSTONE SCHEDULE* Week 1 Date F:13 08-27 Course Overview; ESSENTIALS 2 09-03 ESSENTIALS, Quiz on plagiarism and APA reference style 3 09-10 4 09-17 Disciplinary History Architecture of The Discipline: Theories, Paradigms (“Islands”) Disciplinary Introduction: Theories, Paradigms, methodologies FALL 2013 Readings Th: Quiz Ch. 16 Chs. 1-2 Ch. 15 http://danielciurel.files.wordpress.com/2011/10/21stcenturycommunication2.pdf 5 09-24 Disciplinary Introduction: cont. [GUEST LECTURER] Chs. 3-4 http://danielciurel.files.wordpress.com/2011/10/21stcenturycommunication2.pdf Powers Spitzberg Renegar; Levine; Scheibel; Agne & Tracy 6 10-01 7 10-08 8 10-15 9 10-22 10 10-29 Argument & Persuasion Ch. 23 11 11-05 Conflict Management Ch. 27 12 11-12 Organizational Communication 11-19 Intercultural Communication Ch. 20 13 11-26 Political Communication Ch. 29 14 12-03 Health Communication 15 12-10 16 12-17 Communication & Careers IMPACCT FINAL EXAM: 1-3 p.m. (1300-1500)*** Spitzberg (Bb) Ch. 34-35 * This Schedule may be revised as the semester ensues. Fair notice will be announced in class and Blackboard, and students are responsible for abiding by the most recent version of the schedule. Paper submission is due (through Blackboard Turnitin, by the beginning of class) NOTE: This is the final exam date! Inform family, friends, and the people who are getting married that travel during this time on this date is not possible because it is scheduled as of the first day of class! ** *** Verbal & Nonverbal Communication Codes: Verbal/Nonverbal, Symbols, Codes, Interaction Interpersonal/Relational Communication Assignments Interpersonal/Relational Communication Discussion of Interim Paper Assignment Ch. 22 Ch. 17 (syllabus) Ch. 18 Th:MIDTERM Th: PAPER** Th: IMPACCT FINAL EXAM COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 3 COMM 495: Assignments & Assessment Assignment Overview Grade Scale Late Work The points are accumulated from the following assignments: Participation and Readings Comprehension: 2 points per class day participation in clicker activities, and points for items on assigned readings. There are 26-28 potential “clicker-eligible” lecture days, so a student can miss 3 class periods without any effect on participation points (and some of those points can be made up by the assigned readings points on other lecture days) (up to & not to exceed 50 points). APA/Plagiarism Quiz: A 10-item objective exam on the APA style of references format for scholarly journals, chapters in scholarly edited books, and scholarly books, and the indicators of plagiarism. (50 points). Brief Proposition Paper: A 2-page version of the final proposition paper, with 2-3 hypotheses, and 5 relevant scholarly journal citations. Submitted online to course Blackboard Turnitin assignment site. Detailed instructions provided (100 points). Midterm Exam: objective/multiple-choice, on lecture/text (100 points). Final Exam: objective, multiple-choice, covering lecture/text (100 points). Extra credit: Up to 10 extra credit points may be available through participation in School of Communication sponsored research projects. These points are added into the total accumulation of points, not to exceed the potential points for the course total indicated in the grade scale below. Further explanation of extra credit procedures follows (up to 10 points). Deduction points: Any written assignments that have not been downloaded by the student within a week of the graded assignments being available in Turnitin to examine feedback will result in a deduction of 5 points from the assignment. Grades are based on a total point system, in which ‘a point is a point, is a point’. Letter grades are assigned based on .60, .70, .80, .90 main cuts, with .x3, and .x7 mid-grade cuts. There is no normative curving. 372-400 = A 348-359= B+ 308-319= C+ 268-279= D+ 360-371= A332-347= B 292-307= C 252-267= D 320-331= B280-291= C240-251= D000-239= F Any paper turned in late will be reduced 10 points per weekday that passes beyond the scheduled due date, excepted only for university or professor recognized excuses. The credibility of excuse will depend in part on diligence in apprising the instructor of the situation. In all instances in which an assignment is late, an email should be sent to the instructor as soon as possible, with a Word version of the assignment attached. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 4 COMM 495: Assignments & Assessment Writing Style Guide Reevaluations Attendance & Participation Clickers This course, like the entire School of Communication, requires all written assignments to use the APA (6th ed.) style guide for references, headings, and other format considerations not otherwise specified by the course assignment. A brief APA style guide will be uploaded to Blackboard for this course, but students are recommended to possess an APA style guide. GRADING WILL TAKE USE OF APA STYLE INTO ACCOUNT. If you feel your written assignment was unjustly evaluated, please notify your instructor within 2 weeks after the return or posting of your assignment grade. An appeal is welcome, which requires: (a) scheduling an in-office appointment, (b) an email at least 1 day prior to that appointment in which you articulate your questions or concerns, along with whatever warrants or data available. Please note, asking for an appeal and providing a rationale for concern does not secure, nor imply, a change of grade. Further, any reevaluation implies a complete reevaluation, meaning that new concerns will be identified that were not initially identified in the assessment, thereby potentiating a grade increase, no change, or a grade decrease. Attendance is counted as participation in clicker activities. Any unexcused absence on exam day will result in a letter grade reduction per school day starting from the day of absence. Excuses are recognized for personal illness serious enough to see a physician (thus, warranting an appointment slip), family crisis, or participation in school-related activities (thus, warranting an official notification from the activity coordinator). The latter requires that prior arrangements be made. The former two are more credible with calls on the day of absence, and personal conference as soon as possible thereafter. Exams will not be given early for any reason. Record the dates of your final exam NOW, and plan travel schedules accordingly. In particular, the final exam date is set. DO NOT SCHEDULE ANYTHING AGAINST THIS DATE! A classroom response system (aka clickers) will be used in this course. Use of a clicker is part of the course participation requirement. Students must purchase and register their clicker prior to the 2nd class meeting, and bring the clicker to class on every day afterward prepared to participate. For the class, students will register their i<Clicker IN Blackboard. Students may not use other students' clickers; doing so is a violation of academic integrity. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 5 COMM 495: Assignments & Assessment Extra Credit Incompletes Because the School of Communication seeks not only to distribute knowledge through teaching, but also generate it through original research, and because participation in such research provides important insights into this process of knowledge generation, students in this class will be allowed up to 10 extra credit points during the semester, out of the total number of points available, based on participation in School of Communication authorized research projects. Points will be provided upon evidence of completed participation, with 2 points for each half-hour of research participation. Students will be able to locate eligible research projects at the following site: https://sites.google.com/site/commsdsuresearch/ 1. Eligibility: Only research projects approved and listed on the site listed above are eligible. 2. Announcement of Opportunities: It is the students' responsibility to avail themselves of such opportunities--ongoing announcements and solicitations on the part of the instructor may or may not be made during the semester as opportunities arise. 3. Availability of Opportunities: Research in a program ebbs and flows. Participation is only available during the active windows of time specified by each study. Opportunities for participation may or may not be available in any particular semester, or at any particular time of the semester. 4. Record of Participation: It is the responsibility of each individual researcher to generate a valid list of student participation to return to the instructor of record. It should be apparent in each research project how the student's participation is to be recorded and evidenced. 5. Grade: No more credit is available than is indicated above—there are no "additional" projects or sources for achieving extra credit in the course. 6. Ethics: Any attempt to falsify participation in research for the sake of receiving unearned credit, or to surreptitiously claim credit for more than one course, are forms of academic dishonesty, and will be a basis for failure of a course and initiation of proceedings with the office of Student Rights & Responsibilities. An “I” grade is assigned when a faculty member concludes that a student cannot meet a clearly identifiable portion of course requirements within the academic term for unforeseen reasons. An incomplete is not provided because a course or schedule is too difficult or because time was not managed sufficiently. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 6 COMM 495: Exams Exam Grading Items that are overly difficult, not discriminating or unreliable are adjusted based on statistical analyses (overall item discrimination, item reliability, item difficulty, response options with greater reliability than the keyed option). There will be no curving of grades after these adjustments are made. Grades are uploaded to Blackboard only after these adjustments are made. Exam Retention Please note that exams in this class will not be returned. However, you are welcome to make an appointment to see and review your exam. All exams and answer sheets will be destroyed at the end of the following semester. Exams Exams are objective format (multiple-choice, true/false), covering lecture, and any materials distributed to the class. The final exam may be cumulative and comparative across topics of the semester. Detailed review sheets will be made available prior to each exam. Optical scanning answer sheets (ParSCORE “small red”) and No. 2 pencils are the student’s responsibility on exam days. No electronic devices (i.e., cellphones, earphones, MP3 devices, cameras, tape recorders, calculators, e-books, e-tablets, laptop computers, or electronic dictionaries) are permitted during exams. Any evidence of any such device in sight of a student will result in that student’s failure on that exam, and if there is evidence of any attempt at recording or copying exam materials, or making use of other stolen materials, the student may be failed for the course and reported to Student Rights & Responsibilities. Tips for First, as an overview of multiple-choice (MC) exams, study comparatively. A Studying for good item has to present a condition (i.e., root) that one and only one Exams answer (i.e., stem) correctly fulfills. However, the remaining stems (i.e., foils) must seem correct to the unstudied mind. For foils to seem correct, they often use words and concepts that are legitimate content for the course, but that do not uniquely fit the condition identified in the item root. A student who merely skims or memorizes materials will see several stems that “look” correct, when only one stem actually legitimately fulfills the item root. In order to know which stem is correct requires not only that the stems are recognized, but also what makes the concepts distinct and different from one another, and how they relate to the condition specified in the root. This means studying comparatively, which in turn suggests several study techniques. (1) Memorization is important, but only a small part of the picture. Memorization helps with definitions, lists, model components, and stages or sequences. However, it does not help much with comparison, contrast, analysis and synthesis. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 7 Cont. (2) When studying a concept, ask how it relates to other similar but different concepts. For example, it would not be unusual for a textbook to discuss both impression formation and attribution theory. These are different areas of research and study, but they are also related. The textbook/lectures may or may not discuss their relationship to one another, but there may still be test items about attribution theory with item foils derived from the materials in the discussion of impression formation. Thus, it is important to analyze the differences between these concepts. (3) Successive integrative outlining may help. This means to outline the class notes into a more concise set of notes, and then outline text chapters into a concise set of notes, and then combining student outlines. This activity leads to compare and contrast “where things belong in relation to each other.” The resulting integrative outline can then serve as a final study document. This practice is time-consuming, but can also be very beneficial. Second, manage test anxiety. Anxiety can diminish test performance. There are many practices that can assist in managing test anxiety, including test workshops conducted by the campus counseling and testing offices, systematic body relaxation, preparation and organization, studying with a companion, using flash-cards, student writing of test items over the materials, cognitive restructuring or visualization, employing additional time for testing, and consultation with the instructor. Third, develop hypotheticals and examples. Not all objective exams use hypothetical examples. However, it may help relate to materials by attempting to apply the concepts of the course to practical experiences or situations. This also helps differentiate subtle distinctions among related concepts. Fourth, study past exams. The professor permits students to look over their own exams. Such review can help “get into the mind” of the instructor. It can also improve familiarity with the exam style, which can help preparation for the next exam. Sometimes patterns are noticed (e.g., missing items later on an exam due to fatigue, missing items earlier than later on the exam due to anxiety, missing syllabus vs. textbook items, missing “second guess” items, etc.). Finally, occasionally a student can demonstrate to the instructor that there is another way of interpreting a concept that fits the conditions of the course materials, and as a result, there may be credit given. Finally, use study groups to assess preparation, but not to study. This is clearly only an opinion, but study groups can be very inefficient, and are not likely to do much good unless the members have studied hard prior to studying as a group. The value of the group is to provide different ways of seeing the material, to test knowledge with questions any given student COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 8 would not ordinarily derive, and to occasionally correct errors in thinking. Overview SDSU Definitions THE ACADEMIC DISHONESTY POLICY OF THE SCHOOL OF COMMUNICATION In any case in which an instructor identifies evidence for charging a student with violation of academic conduct standards or plagiarism, the presumption will be with that instructor’s determination. The instructor(s) will confer with the School Director to confirm the evidence. Once confirmed, the student will be informed and presented with the evidence. Some conditions and terms below clarify the School policy and procedure. “Cheating: Cheating is defined as the act of obtaining or attempting to obtain credit for academic work by the use of dishonest, deceptive, or fraud- ulent means. Examples of cheating include, but are not limited to copying, in part or in whole, from another's test or other examination; discussing answers or ideas relating to the answers on a test or other examination without the permission of the instructor; obtaining copies of a test, an examination, or other course material without the permission of the instructor; using notes, cheat sheets, or other devices considered inappropriate under the prescribed testing condition; collaborating with another or others in work to be presented without the permission of the instructor; falsifying records, laboratory work, or other course data; submitting work previously presented in another course, if contrary to the rules of the course; altering or interfering with the grading procedures; plagiarizing, as defined; and knowingly and intentionally assisting another student in any of the above. Plagiarism: Plagiarism is defined as the act of incorporating ideas, words, or specific substance of another, whether purchased, borrowed, or otherwise obtained, and submitting same to the university as one's own work to fulfill academic requirements without giving credit to the appropriate source. Plagiarism shall include but not be limited to: submitting work, either in part or in whole, completed by another; omitting footnotes for ideas, statements, facts, or conclusions that belong to another; omitting quotation marks when quoting directly from another, whether it be a paragraph, sentence, or part thereof; close and lengthy paraphrasing of the writings of another; submitting another person's artistic works, such as musical compositions, photographs, paintings, drawings, or sculptures; and COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 9 submitting as one's own work papers purchased from research companies.” (source: http://www.sa.sdsu.edu/srr/cheating-plagiarism.html) Intellectual Intellectual contents include all forms of ‘text’ produced by another person or contents persons. It includes: writings, course syllabi, course lectures and recordings of lectures, visual information such as models, videos, lyrics, software, etc. Intellectual The syllabus, lectures and lecture outlines are personally-copyrighted Property intellectual property of the instructor, which means that any organized recording for anything other than personal use, duplication, distribution, or profit is a violation of copyright and fair use laws. Proper source Proper attribution occurs by specifying the source of content or ideas. This is attribution done by (a) providing quotation marks around text, when directly quoted, and (b) clearly designating the source of the text or information relied upon in an assignment. Self-plagiarism Students often practice some form of ‘double-dipping,’ in which they write on a given topic across more than one course assignment. In general, there is nothing wrong with double-dipping topics or sources, but there is a problem with double-dipping exact and redundant text. It is common for scholars to write on the same topic across many publication outlets; this is part of developing expertise and the reputation of being a scholar on a topic. Scholars, however, are not permitted to repeat exact text across papers or publications except when noted and attributed, as this wastes precious intellectual space with repetition and does a disservice to the particular source of original presentation by ‘diluting’ the value of the original presentation. Any time that a writer simply ‘cuts-and-pastes’ exact text from former papers into a new paper without proper attribution, it is a form of selfplagiarism. Consequently, a given paper should never be turned in to multiple classes. Entire paragraphs, or even sentences, should not be repeated wordfor-word across course assignments. Each new writing assignment is precisely that, a new writing assignment, requiring new composition on the student’s part. Solicitation for Any student who solicits any third party to write any portion of an assignment ghost writing for this class (whether for pay or not) violates the standards of academic honesty. The penalty for solicitation (regardless of whether it can be demonstrated the individual solicited wrote any sections of the assignment) is F in the course and the student will be reported to Student Rights and Responsibilities. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 10 Secondary citations Useful Aides Secondary citation is not strictly a form of plagiarism, but in blatant forms, it can present similar ethical challenges. A secondary citation is citing source A, which in turn cites source B, but it is source B’s ideas or content that provide the unique basis for the claims the student intends to make in the assignment. For example, assume there is an article by Jones (2006) in the student’s hands, in which there is a discussion or quotation of an article by Smith (1998). Assume further that what Smith seems to be saying is very important to the student’s analysis. In such a situation, the student should always try to locate the original Smith source. In general, if an idea is important enough to discuss in an assignment, it is important enough to locate and cite the original source for that idea. There are several reasons for these policies: (a) Authors sometimes commit citation errors, which might be replicated without knowing it; (b) Authors sometimes make interpretation errors, which might be ignorantly reinforced (c) Therefore, reliability of scholarly activity is made more difficult to assure and enforce; (d) By relying on only a few sources of review, the learning process is short-circuited, and the student’s own research competencies are diminished, which are integral to any liberal education; (e) By masking the actual sources of ideas, readers must second guess which sources come from which citations, making the readers’ own research more difficult; (f) By masking the origin of the information, the actual source of ideas is misrepresented. Some suggestions that assist with this principle: When the ideas Jones discusses are clearly attributed to, or unique to, Smith, then find the Smith source and citation. When the ideas Jones is discussing are historically associated more with Smith than with Jones, then find the Smith source and citation. In contrast, Jones is sometimes merely using Smith to back up what Jones is saying and believes, and is independently qualified to claim, whether or not Smith would have also said it; in such a case, citing Jones is sufficient. Never simply copy a series of citations at the end of a statement by Jones, and reproduce the reference list without actually going to look up what those references report—the only guarantee that claims are valid is for a student to read the original sources of those claims. A good place to learn about plagiarism is the tutorial on academic integrity at http://plagiarism.org/plagiarism-101/overview/; and at http://www.yorku.ca/tutorial/academic_integrity/caseintro.html A good place to learn about APA writing and citation style is: http://owl.english.purdue.edu/owl/resource/560/01/ A good place to learn about making better arguments is: http://www.readwritethink.org/files/resources/interactives/persuasion_map/ COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 11 TurnItIn.com The papers in this course will be submitted electronically in Word (preferably 2007, .docx) on the due dates assigned, and will require verification of submission to Turnitin.com. “Students agree that by taking this course all required papers may be subject to submission for textual similarity review to TurnItIn.com for the detection of plagiarism. All submitted papers will be included as source documents in the TurnItIn.com reference database solely for the purpose of detecting plagiarism of such papers. You may submit your papers in such a way that no identifying information about you is included. Another option is that you may request, in writing, that your papers not be submitted to TurnItIn.com. However, if you choose this option you will be required to provide documentation to substantiate that the papers are your original work and do not include any plagiarized material” (source: language suggested by the CSU General Counsel and approved by the Center for Student’s Rights and Responsibilities at SDSU). Consequences Course failure: Soliciting or reproducing a whole paper, paragraph, or of Plagiarism or large portions of unattributed materials without proper attribution, Cheating whether represented by: (a) multiple sentences, images, or portions of images; or (b) by percentage of assignment length, will result in assignment of an “F” in the course in which the infraction occurred, and a report to the Center for Student Rights and Responsibilities (CSRR2). Assignment failure: Reproducing a sentence or sentence fragment with no quotation marks, but with source citation, or subsets of images without source attribution, will minimally result in an “F” on the assignment, and may result in greater penalty, including a report to the CSRR, depending factors noted below. In this instance, an “F” may mean anything between a zero (0) and 50%, depending on the extent of infraction. Exacerbating conditions—Amount: Evidence of infraction, even if fragmentary, is increased with a greater: (a) number of infractions; (b) distribution of infractions across an assignment; or (c) proportion of the assignment consisting of infractions. Exacerbating conditions—Intent: Evidence of foreknowledge and intent to deceive magnifies the seriousness of the offense and the grounds for official response. Plagiarism, whether ‘by accident’ or ‘by ignorance,’ still qualifies as plagiarism—it is all students’ responsibility to make sure their assignments are not committing the offense. Assistance: Evidence that the student was not the original author of the work, due to soliciting the assistance or composition of another person or persons. Exceptions: Any exceptions to these policies will be considered on a caseby-case basis, and only under exceptional circumstances. HOWEVER, THERE ARE NO EXCUSES ALLOWED BASED ON IGNORANCE OF WHAT CONSTITUTES PLAGIARISM, OR OF WHAT THIS POLICY IS COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 12 Comportment The School of Communication, as a representative of SDSU and higher education, expects students to engage in behavior that enhances the classroom learning environment. The Instructor is responsible not only to the individual student, but to the collective group of students who constitute a class. This means that behavior disruptive to the classroom instruction is not tolerated. For the sake of the other students, the instructor may be required to intervene under various circumstances. Among the actions that are considered disruptive to the learning environment are: The use of cellphones and/or computers/laptops/tablets, whether for conversation, correspondence, emailing, texting, tweeting, or other activities (e.g., social media/Facebook), and when not directly related to the course and its instructional objectives, materials, or contents; Side conversations or discussion in a manner distracting to the instructor or fellow students; Ongoing or unrestricted interruption of instructor or fellow students, or otherwise attempting to monopolize classroom time or discussion; Reading, sleeping, snoring, moving about, yelling, harassing, bullying, or otherwise engaging in activities disrespectful of the instructor or students, or unrelated to the course, materials, or contents; Entering late, leaving early, or leaving often during lecture, especially when in a disruptive manner; Activity that in any way could be considered grossly inappropriate, threatening or dangerous. Certain other activities may be acceptable, but only with permission or by direction of the Instructor, who retains the authority to specify relevant restrictions. Such activities include: Filming, taping, or otherwise recording the class; Accessing the Internet during class; Use of computers/laptops/tablets may be permitted, but only if the students are seated in the front row(s) of the classroom. The Instructor reserves the right to establish additional reasonable expectations deemed necessary to maintain optimal learning conduct in the classroom. Each faculty member is the primary arbiter of classroom comportment. The faculty member has the authority to enforce this policy in a manner deemed suitable to the particular class in question. For example a student texting in class may be requested to turn the phone in to the instructor for the remainder of the class, or a student using a laptop or IPAD to access Facebook may be asked to close and shut down the technology for the remainder of the period. Should repeat offenses occur, with fair warning, each faculty member will determine fair and appropriate consequences for these behaviors. Should an emergency occur or require monitoring, or if students observe violations of this policy, they are encouraged to inform the instructor as soon as possible. Finally, all students are governed by the SDSU policy on cheating and plagiarism. See their coda at: http://csrr.sdsu.edu/cheating-plagiarism.html COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 13 THIS IS SERIOUS! PLAGIARISM IS A CRIME OF CONDUCT, NOT OF INTENT. THIS COURSE WILL HAVE ZERO-TOLERANCE FOR PLAGIARISM! WHY? BECAUSE: 1. A PLAGIARISM POLICY IS PUBLISHED IN THE UNIVERSITY CATALOG; 2. THE SCHOOL OF COMMUNICATION PLAGIARISM POLICY, COMPLETE WITH ELABORATED EXAMPLES, DEFINITIONS, AND CONSEQUENCES FOR TYPES OF PLAGIARISM, IS: a. ON THE SCHOOL OF COMMUNICATION BLACKBOARD SITE, b. ON THE SCHOOL OF COMMUNICATION WEBSITE, AND c. IN THE COURSE SYLLABUS; 3. A POWERPOINT LECTURE PRESENTED IN THIS COURSE ON “THE ESSENTIALS” FURTHER SPECIFIES THE NATURE OF PLAGIARISM; 4. YOU CAN TURN IN YOUR PAPER BEFORE IT IS DUE, SEE ITS ORIGINALITY RATING, FIX IT, AND TURN IT IN AGAIN BY SUBMISSION DEADLINE; 5. FINALLY, YOU SHOULD SIMPLY KNOW THAT IT IS UNETHICAL AND A ‘HIGH CRIME’ IN ACADEME TO MISREPRESENT ANYONE’S WORDS OR IDEAS, THROUGH IMPLICATION, WHETHER INTENDED OR NOT, a. THAT THEY ARE YOUR OWN, OR b. THEY ARE SOMEONE ELSE’S WHEN THEY ARE NOT. A final analogy: If you are taking a driving test to get a license, And you run a stop sign while the instructor is in the car, You fail the test then and there, and must wait for an opportunity to re-take the exam another time. So it is with plagiarism. THEREFORE, THERE IS NO EXCUSE FOR PLAGIARISM, AND NO EXCUSE WILL BE ACCEPTED. YOU ARE FOREWARNED. IF YOU PLAGIARIZE, YOU WILL FAIL THIS COURSE. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 14 COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 15 Source: http://plagiarism.org/plagiarism-101/overview COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 16 ADDITIONAL READINGS: The following readings are listed in the schedule above, and are required readings. They are not available in the bookstore, and must be located by the student using standard research techniques associated with a research university education. Note: The following reading can be located through the library search engines. Learning to use these search engines is part of the competency assumed by this course: Parrott, R. (2004). Emphasizing 'communication' in health communication. Journal of Communication, 54(4), 751-787. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2004.tb02653.x Powers, J. H. (1995). On the intellectual structure of the human communication discipline. Communication Education, 44(3), 191-222. doi:10.1080/03634529509379012 Scarduzio, J. A., Giannini, G. A., & Geist-Martin, P. (2011). Crafting an architectural blueprint: Principles of design for ethnographic research. Symbolic Interaction, 34(4), 447-470. doi:10.1525/si.2011.34.4.447 Note: The following readings can be located through the URL provided in the schedule above, and it is also in the library as a reference work. The following are listed in the order in which they are required: Agne, R. R., & Tracy, K. (2009). Conversation, dialogue, and discourse. In W. F. Eadie (Ed.), 21st century communication: A reference handbook (pp. 177-185). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. Levine, T. R. (2009). Quantitative approaches to communication research. In W. F. Eadie (Ed.), 21st century communication: A reference handbook (pp. 47-64)). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. Renegar, V. R., & Malkowski, J. A. (2009). Rhetorical and textual approaches to communication. In W. F. Eadie (Ed.), 21st century communication: A reference handbook (pp. 49-56). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. Note: The following reading can be located at the end of this syllabus packet: Spitzberg, B. H. (2009). Islands of inquiry. In G. Merrigan & C. L. Huston (Eds.), Communication research methods (2nd ed., pp. xxi-xxiv). New York: Oxford. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 17 INSTRUCTIONS FOR THE “PROP” PAPER A Propositional Parable D: F: D: F: D: F: D: F: D: F: D: F: D: F: D: F: D: F: D: F: Daddy, what is an instinct? An instinct, my dear, is an explanatory principle. But what does it explain? Anything – almost anything at all. Anything you want it to explain. Don't be silly. It doesn't explain gravity. No. But that is because nobody wants "instinct" to explain gravity. If they did, it would explain it. We could simply say that the moon has an instinct whose strength varies inversely as the square of the distance . . . But that's nonsense, Daddy. Yes, surely. But it was you who mentioned "instinct," not I. All right – but then what does explain gravity? Nothing my dear, because gravity is an explanatory principle. Oh. Do you mean that you cannot use one explanatory principle to explain another? Hmm . . . hardly ever. That is what Newton meant when he said, "hypotheses non fingo." And what does that mean? Please. Well, you know what "hypotheses" are. Any statement linking together two descriptive statements is an hypothesis. If you say that there was a full moon on February 1st and another on March 1st; and then you link these two observations together in any way, the statement that links them is an hypothesis. Yes – and I know what 'non' means. But what's 'fingo.' Well, 'fingo' is a late Latin word for 'make.' It forms a verbal noun 'fictio' from which we get the word 'fiction.' Daddy, do you mean that Sir Issac Newton thought that all hypotheses were just made up like stories? Yes – precisely that. But didn't he discover gravity? With the apple? No, dear. He invented it. D: Oh. Adapted from: Heinz Von Foerster (1981). Rigor and imagination: Essays from the legacy of Gregory Bateson. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 18 DON'T PANIC “Hypotheses are the scaffolds which are erected in front of a building and removed when the building is completed. They are indispensable to the worker; but he must not mistake the scaffolding for the building. J. W. von Goethe, Maxims and reflections, 1893 (in Kaplan: Science says) “For every fact there is an infinity of hypotheses. The more you look, the more you see. R. M. Pirsig, Zen and the art of motorcycle maintenance, 1974. “Hypothesis, where successful, is a two-way street, extending back to explain the past and forward to predict the future. What we try to do in framing hypotheses is to explain some otherwise unexplained happenings by inventing a plausible story, a plausible description or history of relevant portions of the world” (Quine & Ullian, 1980, “Hypothesis,” p. 197) “The purpose of models is not to fit the data but to sharpen the questions” S. Karlin, 11th R. A. Fisher Memorial Lecture, Royal Society, April 20, 1983 (in Kaplan: Science says) “Being able to examine our models, critically evaluate them, and even discard them is far more scientifically literate than being able to regurgitate facts for a standardized test. … Ultimately, our models and descriptions of reality must be subject to two overriding criteria. How useful is this model, and how much does this model resemble our observations? Scientific literacy requires an understanding that science is only a model. We have to be able to jettison our models when our critical thinking leads us to that conclusion. Saus, S. (2007). Camelot is only a model: Scientific literacy in the 21st century. Seed, (Sept/Oct), 77-78. “Without a sense, or without the thought, a proposition would be an utterly dead and trivial thing.” Wittgenstein, Blue and Brown Books, 1958, p. 4 “The meaning of exactness best founded in intellectual history is the possibility of constructing a theoretical system of idealized models containing abstract constructs of variables and of relations between variables, from which most or all propositions concerning particular connections can be deduced.” Machlup, 2004, Readings in the philosophy of social science p. 11 “All possible knowledge is only an approximation and does not reach ultimate reality. It only reflects certain aspects of reality in more or less appropriate models.” von Bertalanffy, 1975 Perspectives on general system theory, p. 114 COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 19 Overview The purpose of a proposition (i.e., hypothesis) paper is to develop a number of theoretical propositions, a significant proportion of which concern communication in relationship to the course topic (e.g., conflict, relationships, dark side of communication, communication theory, etc.). The paper in some ways is a review of literature; it is different from typical literature reviews in a few important ways. Most reviews just summarize research to provide a sense of ‘what is known’ in an area. In contrast, the proposition paper attempts to develop explanatory arguments in support of a number of hypotheses. Papers are primarily graded on four areas: (1) Writing: Good writing, composition, style, voice, and format (APA); (2) Proposition wording: Conceptual soundness of the propositions and their wording; (3) Scholarly research: Quality of the scholarly journal research brought to bear; and (4) Explanatory argument: The quality of the explanatory arguments developed for each proposition. For graduate student papers only, a fifth grading component is (5) Model value: The quality of the visual model in summarizing or generating the hypotheses. It is very important to emphasize that “evidence” and “argument” are separate elements of the assessment—evidence backs an argument, but the argument itself has to explain the proposition, independent of the evidence. Of course, papers must also be topical; that is, they must be relevant to the course content. Hypothesizing A hypothesis is a verbal or symbolic statement of relationship between two or more variables (e.g., Trust is positively related to the likelihood of using cooperative strategies during conflicts). A variable is any construct or concept that can be observed to take on different values (e.g., trust, anxiety, self-disclosure, assertiveness, strategy use, etc.). It takes the form of X = fY (i.e., “The value of ‘X’ is a function of ‘Y’). A definition (e.g., self-esteem is the degree to which self is perceived positively), in contrast, characterizes the nature of a variable, but not its relationship to other variables. It takes the form X = Y. All of this is just a fancy way of saying that the proposition paper attempts to develop, through review of scholarly research and personally formulated argument, a series of hypotheses on a given topic. Many of these hypotheses may be already in the existing research literature, they may be derived from conceptual models, or the products of creativity and imagination. There are two keys to this assignment: (1) Developing well-conceived and well-worded hypotheses, and (2) developing reasonable arguments for each hypothesis. Like any argument, it is made credible through the use of causal analysis, strong reasons, evidence, citations, and example. The most important thing is that every hypothesis should be carefully worded (read all these instructions to understand this warning) and arguments should answer two “why” questions: (1) Why are the variables related this way? (2) Why is the hypothesis likely to be true? The word “because” should figure prominently in every explanation. To explain a hypothesis means to make sensible how and why things are related the way they are. Hypotheses themselves explain nothing—they simply describe a relationship between two or more things—not why the relationship exists. Theories are basically sets of interrelated hypotheses that collectively provide an explanation for a phenomenon or process. For example, a COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 20 person may want to explain the hypothesis: “As jealousy increases, the likelihood of relational violence increases.” Guided by an understanding of argument (e.g., see recommendations for writing that follow later in these instructions), this hypothesis could be argued thusly: Jealousy is a complex blend of emotions based on the perception that a person’s valued relationship is threatened by a third party or rival. [Backing]: Retzinger (1991) has reviewed evidence [Grounds]: that violence is likely to result from a combination of anger and shame, but neither alone [Backing]. Because [Warrant]: anger creates an inner sense of expressive frustration and arousal, and shame provides a target for this expression (i.e., the partner and/or the rival), the jealous person is much more likely to engage in violence than non-jealous persons. [Claim]: This hypothesis may not apply to contexts in which strong moral, religious, or public restraints are in place (e.g., even jealous persons tend not to be overtly violent in public places). [Rebuttal]: The rebuttal is not necessary, but sometimes illustrates relative objectivity and openness of the author. In this explanation, other concepts (i.e., the theory of rage as proposed by Retzinger, with its components of anger and rage) are used to make the link between jealousy and violence sensible. More explanation could be provided for these concepts, more evidence reviewed in favor of Retzinger’s hypothesis, and so forth, but the basic elements of explanation and argument are there. The point is that explanation is neither mere restatement nor mere review of research or expert opinion. Please note, there is no need for quotations. CITE OFTEN, QUOTE SPARINGLY (AND PREFERABLY, NOT AT ALL). Modeling Only the graduate assignment requires a model, but undergraduates are certainly encouraged to try their hands at modeling. Modeling is one of the zeniths of scientific reasoning. It incorporates analytic reasoning (i.e., breaking wholes into parts), synthetic reasoning (i.e., [re]assembling parts into wholes), abstract reasoning (i.e., recognizing similar things across levels of abstraction and concreteness), and explanatory or causal reasoning. It is fundamental to theoretical reasoning (i.e., explaining how and why phenomena happen the way they do) and methodological reasoning (i.e., specifying hypotheses to be tested qualitatively or quantitatively, the results of which inform the validity of the underlying theory). Models serve various functions in science as in everyday life. A model is a visual simplification of concepts that exist in both a hypothesized conceptual form and an actual or potential observed form. For example, if you believe that as social skills decrease a person’s ability and motivation to meet others and develop satisfying relationships decreases, and that this in turn increases the person’s loneliness, then four conceptual components each potentially can be arranged as a basic model that looks something like the following: COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 21 Figure 1a. Social Skills Model of Loneliness Shyness Loneliness Social Skill Deficit Social Anxiety Notice that this model could have been formulated differently. People might develop anxiety and shyness through early learning experiences, which then would lead to a deficit of social skills, which thereby make the development of social relationships more difficult, leading to loneliness. This version of the model is represented next. Figure 1b. Affect and Social Skills Model of Loneliness Shyness Social Anxiety Loneliness Social Skill Deficit Still another perspective would predict that as a person experiences loneliness over time, social activity and self-doubts would lead to a crisis in social confidence, social anxiety, shyness, and eventually, a lack of experience and motivation regarding social skills. In this perspective, the chronic experience of loneliness leads to a lack of practice and social experience, which would lead to anxiety, which would lead to shyness, which would lead to an atrophy of social competence. This model would look something like: Figure 1c. Social Withdrawal Model of Social Skills Atrophy Loneliness Social Anxiety Shyness Social Skill Deficit COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 22 Each of these models represents anywhere from three to seven hypotheses, depending on the configuration of arrows (i.e., causal paths). The point is that these represent alternative models of reality, and any one of these models can be reasonably argued and justified. Which model is “true” is irrelevant for the sake of formulating the model—the question is whether a good set of arguments can be developed in support of the model presented. Once a basic model has been developed, it is then relatively simple to start asking additional questions, such as how biology (e.g., sex, age, etc.), culture (e.g., ethnicity, regional identification, etc.), context (e.g., socio-economic status, intact family status, etc.), personal background (e.g., attachment disorder, relational history, etc.), dispositions (e.g., self-esteem, attribution style, etc.), or recent events (e.g., personal loss, relationship conflicts, etc.) influence various model components or relationships. Most models follow simple rules: (1) Components on the left (or top) precede, at some point, the components to the right (or bottom) in time, moving from “antecedents” (causes) to consequents (effects). (2) Every conceptual component should be potentially measurable or observable. (3) Every arrow represents a direct effect of one concept on another (and a potential hypothesis). This effect does not have to be a causal relationship, but it usually implies such an influence. (4) Every component has an arrow headed to at least one other component, and may have arrows to multiple other components. (5) Components can be grouped according to larger components (i.e., multiple indicators of a single concept; e.g., social skills above might be further indicated by coordination, expressiveness, attentiveness, and composure skills). Form, Organization and Style The basics are as follow: Use normal 1-inch margins; Use 11-point Calibri or 12-point Times Roman font; With double spacing throughout; With no more than 2 pages text (not including reference page); With the top two lines of the first page of the paper with five pieces of information; With headers programmed into the paper, indicating a brief title and page numbers; With 2 to 3 propositions—if 3 are developed, they must be in the form of a syllogism; otherwise, just develop 2 propositions; (8) With a minimum of 5 scholarly journal publication references, all of which must be cited in text, and properly listed on the third page “References” list, all in proper APA style. There may be additional references to scholarly books or chapters in scholarly books. (9) The assignment will be submitted to Turnitin—no hardcopy is needed. It is recommended you submit your paper early to receive a Turnitin originality report at least 2 days before due date. Use A.P.A. throughout (6th ed.), except for format specifications above. Do not leave big blank spaces between sections or hypotheses. The text should run continuously. Save your work often during writing—there are no excuses for late work. Use the insert running head procedure in Word—do not just type the header at the top of the page for each page. Learn to use internally formatted hanging indents for the references and hypotheses (as opposed to hitting ‘enter’ at the end of a line and tabbing in from the next line), and page and section breaks for new pages (as (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 23 opposed to just hitting ‘enter’ a bunch of times). There is no excuse for ignorance about Word— learn to use its formatting resources and tools! There is no “Title Page;” at the top of the first page insert the following: Last name, First name (Red ID#) Email address Course #: Brief course title Date: Title of Paper Next, some form of introduction is presented. An introduction may briefly examine the history and importance of the concepts, discuss the relevance of these concepts to the course subject, introduce relevant theories, and/or explain any basic terms, assumptions, or limitations that may be important. Assume this class as the audience, which means that the writing should be clear enough for them to comprehend fully the content, and intent, of the paper. Next, the body of the paper consists of the propositions and the arguments for each proposition. This section of the paper proceeds by presenting a proposition and then developing a paragraph (or two) of explanatory argument (warrants; explanations) and support (evidence). The propositions should be numbered and set off from the text (e.g., bolded and/or italicized). Propositions are never prima facie; that is, they never stand on their own. Propositions always require some degree of explanation. Next, the conclusion section develops any of several points, such as a brief summary, a consideration of the limitations of the analysis, a critical conjecture about the status of the theory and/or research associated with the concepts examined, future implications of the analysis, or the importance of the analysis. Finally, the References section will provide the complete citations in A.P.A. style. Please save the paper with the following nomenclature: “C495-PROP-semester-yearLastname-BriefTopicTitle. For example, if it is the Fall 2013 semester, and the paper is on sexual coercion, and your last name is Johnson, then the file would be saved as: “C495-PROP-SP13Johnson-Sexual Coercion” Submission The paper will be turned in electronically, in Word 2007, through Turnitin in Blackboard. If difficulties are experienced turning it in online, it is essential to send an email to the professor with the paper attached, by the time the assignment is due. Types of Hypotheses Below is a series of hypotheses to illustrate how they can differ. These hypotheses may seem sophisticated right now. However, research of a topic has begun, hypothetical relations will begin to emerge either stated directly (i.e., in the form of hypotheses being tested) or indirectly (i.e., implied by the explanation and discussion of the concepts). COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 24 The simplest, and weakest, form of hypothesis is a "non-directional" statement of relationship. For example: H1: Self-esteem is related to communication competence. While this is a hypothesis, it provides minimal information regarding the precise nature of the relationship. Avoid such statements in the paper. A more precise form is: H2: Self-esteem is positively related to interpersonal communication competence. (Note: This can be reworded as: “Persons high in self-esteem are significantly higher in communication competence than persons low in self-esteem”). Hypotheses can also vary by the form (or shape) of the relationship: H3: Motivation, knowledge and skills are curvilinear to perceived communication competence. (That is, they are positively related up to a moderately high range of competence, after which higher values lead to lower perceived competence). Finally, deductive construction allows the development of stronger, and more tightly controlled, theoretical arguments. For example, H4: As [TV exposure] increases, [verbal skills] decrease. [A] [B] H5: As [verbal skills] decrease, use of [physical violence] in conflict increases. [B] [C] H6: As [TV exposure] increases, use of [physical violence] in conflict increases. [A] [C] On the Art of Explanation “We cannot infer the events of the future from those of the present. Belief in the causal nexus is superstition” (Wittgenstein, Tractatus, 5.1361) Explanation implies the identification of some “bridge” (i.e., warrant, “because...,” etc.) that serves as an account of why one concept is related to another concept. It is an “animating mechanism,” that gives life to a concept. For example, one popular concept is that exposure to violence in the media causes violence in society. The natural question is why. Answering the “why” question gets at the concept of explanation. Even though seeing or hearing the equation: “media violence causes societal violence” seems like an explanation, it is not. It is missing the bridge, or animating mechanism. To illustrate, consider a few “non-explanations.” It is not an explanation to say media violence causes societal violence because: (1) media depictions of violence are increasing; (2) COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 25 societal violence is increasing; (3) some people have engaged in crimes they saw on TV (“copy cat crimes”); (4) people believe what they see on TV; (5) research shows media violence increases societal violence. Each of these merely suggests correlation (rather than causation), repeats the original assertion, or simply provides backing for an explanation that is itself missing. To illustrate legitimate explanations, consider the following: Cultivation: Social learning theory claims that people learn both by direct experience (e.g., touching a hot stove) or observation of others (e.g., observing one’s sister touch a hot stove). Since we cannot or do not always directly experience things (e.g., robbing a bank), we look to real and imaginative role models (e.g., media figures and narratives). Thus, the crime and violence in the media provide models, and rewarding ones at that, for us we otherwise would not have had. Cognitive Availability: Our brain can be viewed as a library. If 90% of this library is filled with science fiction, and we are asked to respond to someone’s question for information, we are most likely to give them a fanciful piece of information, because that is what is most represented in our mind’s library. If, in watching media, most of the acts or ways of dealing with conflict we observe are violent in nature, it simply supplies more acts of violence to store in our mental repertoires, and thus, when we find ourselves in a conflict, violent acts are the most “available” to draw upon. Disinhibition: Growing up in a culture is largely about learning what one cannot or is not supposed to do. The very nature of culture is conformity and normative action within a given body of beliefs and behaviors. Exposure to violence in the media may make violence appear more normative than it is, and thus, make engaging in violence seem less deviant. In essence, it is not making violence seem more rewarding (cultivation) or available, but merely makes the violence within a person more acceptable. Fantasy Fulfillment: Freud and others might argue that daily life, as well as the normal tortures of growing up, fill us with tensions that are constantly repressed. These tensions struggle to “get out,” managing to manifest themselves mainly in the form of fantasy. Media violence may give form to these fantasies, making them more vivid and “real,” thereby stimulating the enactment of fantasies in life (e.g., who has not “fantasized” about hitting someone in the face—seeing it done may both “suggest” it, but stimulate the enactment of it). Desensitization: One of the reasons people avoid violence is that it hurts others. However, when violence is everywhere in the media, it can numb one’s sensibilities, and lower one’s ability to empathize. Further, the more cartoonish the violence, the more it seems it does not really hurt anyone. Such exposure to violence may take the “brakes” off using by making violence seem less hurtful, and thus, less costly to use. Excitation: Violence is a thrill. Through 5 million years of evolution, we have come to find violence a potential threat, a potential path to victory, and therefore, arousing. Thus, seeing violence in the media is itself, arousing, and this arousal stimulates our own arousal. Our arousal (adrenaline, muscular tension, etc.) needs to find an outlet, and is expressed in violence. Peer Group Mediation: Research on sexual violence indicates that people who engage in sexual violence are much more likely to have friends who approve of, and have engaged in, sexual violence. The suggestion is that peer groups mediate the transfer of violence. That is, if a COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 26 peer group watches violence in the media, this peer group may then become self-reinforcing through its interaction, thereby serving as the proximal stimulus to its members’ violence. Spillover: Violence in the media provides models of behavior in certain contexts that are then available for generalization to contexts experienced in everyday life, both directly comparable to those observed in the media, as well as contexts largely unrelated. Thus, for example, a person may “copycat” behaviors witnessed in the media, but may also extend the function of the behaviors observed in one context (e.g., a drop kick in a professional wrestling program) to another context (e.g., a dispute with one’s brother). Cultural Chaos: Violence in the media may reflect the very breakdown of society, norms, and culture. If everyone is violent to everyone in the media, then this communicates a sense of despair, hopelessness, alienation and angst to the viewing public. This has the effects of both (1) inciting some to exploit the lawlessness (e.g., looting during natural disasters), (2) join the crowd (e.g., mob violence during the L.A. riots), (3) bystander apathy providing a permissive environment for violence. Each of these explanations offers slightly and sometimes substantively distinct sources of causation. For example, fantasy fulfillment, excitation and disinhibition tend to assume that we are by nature violent, and all that is needed is something in the media to take the restraints of society away. Cultivation and availability argue media instills violence in us. Peer-group mediation and cultural chaos tend to view media as having an effect on society at large, which only then affects our individual behavior. In all these examples concepts are elaborated to make sensible the link between violence in the media and in society. Co-occurrence makes no sense without an explanation, and it matters which explanation is offered. Do all of the explanations above seem equally believable? Were a politician to make a speech on fantasy fulfillment would it seem as reasonable as the cultivation explanation? Why? The “theory” cannot be evaluated until its explanatory rationale has been specified, and the brief examples above illustrate how significant, and distinct, such explanations can be. Finally, use theories, use concepts discussed in class, use concepts, rather than personal experience, anecdotes, examples, or intuitive explanations. The concepts and theories discussed in class represent an attempt to provide explanatory frameworks specifically to explain hypotheses. So use theories when possible to help explain the hypotheses. As Another Example Some explanations seem, on the surface, to make complete sense. For example, the following argument was offered by a student: First-born children often reach a vocabulary of 50 words at a faster rate than later born children, with a range variation of 14 to 21 months (Pine, 1995). First-born children develop a greater range an understanding of respective words at a younger age, while later born children tend to develop “frozen phrases.” “Frozen phrases” are utterances that contain two or more words that have not previously occurred alone in the child’s vocabulary, or that contain one such word, provided it has not occurred in the same position in a previous multi-word utterance (Pine, 1995). Since first-born children achieve a greater vocabulary earlier, they tend to COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 27 take on a mentor role with younger siblings, thus mentally stimulating themselves (Travis & Kohli, 1995). (Source: Anonymous.) Here the student did a good job of evidencing that first-born children develop a higher vocabulary level, but the student offered no explanation for why they do so. In this case, the student could have provided explanatory arguments such as the following: Given an assumption of a relatively fixed amount of time available to the parents, first-born children receive more parental attention and interaction, which later born children may be relatively deprived of, as larger numbers of children shrink the available time for any one child. Further, it is reasonable to assume that parental vocabulary resources are more valuable than sibling resources, due to the relatively advanced nature of parental grammar, and given the “mistakes” that might be built into a sibling’s vocabulary. Further, an older sibling will play some role in mentoring the younger sibling’s linguistic development, and the process of reflection and ruleconformity involved in teaching another person is likely to reinforce self-learning. Finally, the competition brought about by potential older sibling resentment of younger siblings may interfere with the learning of the younger sibling, as conflict and emotional disturbance distracts attention away from the more fundamental aspects of vocabulary building. These explanations build a conceptual substrate or foundation that bridges, or warrants, the claim that first-born children develop higher levels of vocabulary, and prevent such a claim from being purely tautological (i.e., tautology: first born children develop higher levels of vocabulary earlier than later born children because first born children mature faster than later born children). So explanations are the conceptual ladder that bridges evidence and claim so as to make their relationship sensible. They answer the question why. Yet Another Example A student argued in a paper: “Berger and Calabrese (1975) also believe that uncertainty plays a significant role in the preservation and dissolution of close relationships.” My question to the student was: WHY do they say uncertainty is important? Because uncertainty means not being able to predict things. Not being able to predict things means not knowing what effects behaviors have in the world. Not knowing what effects behaviors or actions have means not knowing how to control the world, or make the world work better through self-initiated actions. This results in the potential for bad things to happen, which creates anxiety. So, Berger and Calabrese say that uncertainty creates anxiety, and therefore we are motivated to reduce this uncomfortable state of anxiety by reducing our uncertainty. This is what is meant by an explanatory argument. It answers why people seek to reduce uncertainty, and it does this through a chain of causal predicates, each of which is linked thematically, coherently to the next, until a logical framework exists in which the proposition—the relationship between concepts, is sensible and satisfies the question: “but why?” COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 28 SELECTED EXAMPLES OF PUBLISHED HYPOTHESIS PAPERS Berelson, B., & Steiner, G. A. (1973). Human behavior: An inventory of scientific findings. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World. Berger, C. R., & Calabrese, R. J. (1975). Some explorations in initial interaction and beyond: Toward a developmental theory of interpersonal communication. Human Communication Research, 1, 99-112. Burgoon, J. K., & Jones, S. B. (1976). Toward a theory of personal space expectations and their violations. Human Communication Research, 2, 131-146. Burr, W.R., Leigh, G. K., Day, R. D., & Constantine, J. (1979). Symbolic interaction and the family. In W. R. Burr, R. Hill, F. I. Nye, & I. L. Reiss (Eds.), Contemporary theories about the family (pp. 42-111). New York: Free Press. Deutsch, M. (1949). A theory of co-operation and competition. Human Relations, 2, 129-152. Fincham, F. D. (1992). The account episode in close relationships. In M. L. McLaughlin, M. J. Cody, & S.J. Read (Eds.), Explaining one’s self to others: Reason-giving in social context (pp. 225244). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Fisher, R. J. (1990). Chapter 5: An eclectic model of intergroup conflict. The social psychology of intergroup and international conflict resolution (pp. 87-115). New York: Springer-Verlag. Greene, J. (1984). A cognitive approach to human communication: An action assembly theory. Communication Monographs, 51, 289-306. Kaplowitz, S. A. (1978). Towards a systematic theory of power attribution. Social Psychology, 41, 131-148. Keyton, J., Ford, D. J.,& Smith, F. L. (2008). A mesolevel communicative model of collaboration. Communication Theory, 18, 376-406. Liden, R. C., & Mitchell, T. R. (1988). Ingratiatory behaviors in organizational settings. Academy of Management Review, 13, 572-587. Mack, R. W., & Snyder, R. C. (1957). The analysis of social conflict--Toward an overview and synthesis. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 1, 221-248. Millar, F. E., & Rogers, L. E. (1988). Power dynamics in marital relationships. In P. Noller & M. A. Fitzpatrick (Eds.), Perspectives on marital interaction (pp. 98-120). Philadelphia: Multilingual Matters Ltd. Phillips, A. P., & Dipboye, R. L. (1989). Correlational tests of predictions from a process model of the interview. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 41-52. Rodgers, R. H. (1987). Postmarital reorganization of family relationships: A propositional theory. In D. Perlman & S. Duck (Eds.), Intimate relationships: Development, dynamics, and deterioration (pp. 239-268). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Schlenker, B. R., & Leary, M. R. (1982). Social anxiety and self-presentation: A conceptualization and model. Psychological Review, 92, 641-669. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 29 Seibold, D. R., & Spitzberg, B. H. (1982). Attribution theory and research: Formalization, critique, and implications for communication. In B. Dervin & M. J. Voight (Eds.), Progress in communication sciences (Vol. 3, pp. 85-125). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Spitzberg, B. H. (1994). Intercultural effectiveness. In L. A. Samovar, & R. E. Porter (Eds.), Intercultural communication: A reader (7th ed., pp. 347-359). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Stech, E. L. (1979). A grammar of conversation with a quantitative empirical test. Human Communication Research, 5, 158-170. Wyer, R. S., Jr. (1980). The acquisition and use of social knowledge: Basic postulates and representative research. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 558-573. Wyer, R. S., Jr., & Srull, T. K. (1986). Human cognition in its social context. Psychological Review, 93, 322-359. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 30 SELECTIONS FROM SPITZBERG'S "CHAMBER OF HORRORS" Try to avoid the following pitfalls: (1) avoid statements of non-relationship (e.g., 'X is not related to Y'); (2) avoid including explanations in the hypothesis itself (e.g., "X is positively related to Y because of theory Z's rationale that..."); (3) avoid using prescriptive (e.g., "S's should do X when Y occurs") or purely descriptive non-probabilistic (e.g., "X may/can be related to Y") wording; (4) avoid introducing terms into the hypothesis that have not been adequately explained or defined yet; and (5) avoid introducing categorical variables that distort the form of the relationship (e.g., "LOW X is positively related to HIGH Y's"); (6) avoid introducing set/subset redundancies in multiple hypotheses (e.g., P1: Females disclose more than males; P2: Androgynous females disclose more than androgynous males). Below are some of the "best of the worst" that have crossed my tired eyes. H1?: Knowledge also relates to mindlessness in an inverse parabolic function, in that they are positively related to each other until the mindlessness ceases to be mindless. H2?: Empathizing with co-workers strengths, weaknesses, and working style relates negatively towards escalating conflict resulting in an inability to see another's view. H3?: Equity restoring to a relationship is negatively related to low self-esteem individuals. H4?: Reactions in adolescents relate back to anxious, neurotogenic, disparging, masochistic, and manic depressive parents. H5?: Individual’s interaction with a caregiver as a child affects their relationship bondage with a potential partner as an adult. H6?: The purpose of negotiation is to resolve a disagreement between two or more parties. H7?: In certain negotiation circumstances nonverbal aspects of communication assume a greater significance than does verbal language. H8?: Levels of immediate physical and psychological trauma suffered by victims will vary. H9?: Child abuse is determined by the factors involved. H10?: The use of threats as an influence tactic does not exacerbate conflicts in general, but is limited to the occasions on which they are used. H11?: Conflicts are likely to be influenced by power. H12?: Sex differences are loosely related to relationship goals. H13?: Attraction is positively related and influential in choice of sexual partners. H14?: Males are negatively related to female interviewers of equal status; whereas females are not affected significantly to status and the sex of the interviewer. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 31 H15?: Mindlessness is negatively related to information processing in interactions. H16?: Deception is negatively related to the truth. H17?: Proximity increases physical closeness. H18?: Viewing the mediatior’s role as folkloric trickster is positively related to their job title. H19?: Guests of a television talk show depend on the reaction of the audience for support of their heinous actions, therefore after the show is over whether or not they take the advice of the audience and change their way, depends on the reaction they received. H20?: Family hour will need to be changed back to true family hour. H21?: Hip hop is a response to conditions of property and disempowerment. H22?: Media coverage of the violent topics in Rap music helps alert people to the possible problems. H23?: Families no longer have a need for face-to-face communication. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 32 WHAT NOT TO DO WITH HYPOTHESES What follows are examples of flawed hypotheses taken from previous student papers. Each hypothesis is analyzed to illustrate common mistakes observed in the conceptualization and verbalization of hypotheses. Students are responsible for studying these examples, and thereby avoiding the types of mistakes indicated. If there are questions about any of the issues raised, please raise them at the appropriate time in class or professor office hours. Recall that a hypothesis is any statement of relationship between or among variables. A variable is a concept that takes on different values. The entire function of science is to make the nature of the relationships among things understandable. The more precise the statement of relationship, the more understandable the hypothesis is. What follows is an effort to assist in making the hypotheses more precise. 1. MOOD IS POSITIVELY RELATED TO SEXUAL INITIATION. Overly generic variables: "Mood" may be positive or negative. Since mood is not specified, the hypothesis, as worded, implies that any increase in mood state will correspond to an increase in sexual initiation. Thus, as worded, an increase in depression increases sexual initiation, as would sadness anger, and so forth. The key is to understand that the term "mood" is a concept that can take on different values or levels (e.g., low to high, negative to neutral to positive, etc.). A hypothesis is an attempt to specify how their values or levels correspond to the values or levels of another concept, in this case, the occurrence of sexual initiation. The hypothesis intends to say: "The experience of positively valenced or labeled states is positively related to the likelihood of sexual initiation." 2. LOW SELF-ESTEEM IS POSITIVELY RELATED TO INTERPERSONAL HOSTILITY. Mixing variable labels: This is one of the most common, yet subtle problems in writing hypotheses. By specifying only "low" self-esteem, this hypothesis ignores "medium" and "high" self-esteem. When two-thirds of a variable's possible values are removed, its ability to "relate" to anything is eliminated because there is no real opportunity for the variable to vary beyond a very narrow range of values. The above statement intends to say simply: "Self-esteem is negatively related to interpersonal hostility" or "Low self-esteem persons are significantly more interpersonally hostile than high self-esteem persons." Think about what the terminology means! If something is categorized as "low" it needs to be compared to something that is "high." As an additional note, it is unclear if self-esteem relates to the frequency with which all hostile actions occur, the intensity of hostile actions, the duration of hostile actions, all three, some combination, or some other aspect of hostility. Hostility is likely to take on many different features. It assists the hypothesis to make these features clear in the hypothesis. Thus, "Self-esteem is negatively related to the frequency and breadth of interpersonally hostile behaviors." COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 33 3. SELF-DISCLOSURE IS POSITIVELY RELATED TO INTIMACY. Overly unidimensional concepts: On the face of it, this is sensible, right? Yet, this hypothesis is deeply flawed. First, anyone looking into the two concepts involved here, self-disclosure and intimacy, should soon find that they are multidimensional. This means that a valid use of the term actually implies many distinct but related concepts. For example, self-disclosure has been found to vary importantly in terms of breadth (number of topics), depth (intimacy of topics), reciprocity (degree to which partner discloses in response), honesty, and valence (positive or negative). Similarly, intimacy can be viewed in terms of domain (e.g., recreation I, sexual, social, emotional, etc.) or type (e.g., caring, commitment, interdependence, physical, etc.). The point is not that student papers have to agree with "the" view with which I am familiar, but that any responsible research effort will uncover the complexity (i.e., multidimensionality) of the concepts. In this case, the hypothesis implies that all forms of self-disclosure are equally related to all forms of intimacy. Is this sensible? It seems unlikely that as disclosure of negative information increases that sexual intimacy increases, yet this is exactly what the hypothesis implies until it is specified. Second, this is a perfect candidate for a curvilinear relationship. That is, extremely low or extremely high amounts of disclosure are likely to impede or inhibit intimacy respectively. Yet, this hypothesis implies that even at extremely high levels of disclosiveness that intimacy will also be extremely high. Third, would this hypothesis apply reasonably to all contexts? For example, does it apply to task-oriented groups, or superior-subordinate, or disengaging, relationships? Unlikely. 4. MALES ARE POSITIVELY CORRELATED TO APPRECIATION OF OBSCENE HUMOR. Concepts not variable: "People" are not variables; their characteristics are. This hypothesis intends to compare 'maleness" to "femaleness. It makes the mistake of correlating categories. Categories are nominal level variables, whereas correlation requires ordinal level variables (recall COMM 350?). Gender, religion, ethnic group, etc. are categorical variables, and as such, should be framed as "difference" hypotheses rather than correlational hypotheses. Thus: "Males appreciate obscene humor significantly more than females." 5. FRIENDSHIPS ARE HIGHLY VALUABLE TO OLDER MALES AND FEMALES. Overly definitional: This is overly definitional in form. First, does it imply the friendships are more valuable to older persons than younger person? (e.g., "The value of friendships for males and females increases as age increases"). Second, if a relationship is being identified that holds for all members of a set (i.e., persons), there is no need to specify all categories of that set (i.e., "males and females"). Thus, "The value of friendships increases as age increases." Third, to what is friendship "valuable"? Valuable in psychological health, physical health, social health, etc.? Thus, for example, "As age increases, the perceived value of friendship in preventing depression and loneliness increases." COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 34 6. ADULTERY, ALCOHOL ABUSE, AND FINANCIAL DIFFICULTIES ARE POSITIVELY RELATED TO DIVORCE. ‘NS’ hypothesis: This qualifies in the realm of an “NS” hypothesis (“No Shit!”). Technically there is nothing wrong with this hypothesis. However, it is about as unsurprising, and therefore, uninformative, as a statement can be. It’s a little like saying “Oxygen is important to human life.” Science has a healthy respect for demonstrating what is supposed to be true actually is true. But come on! Put a little imagination into the paper and come up with a statement that allows parents to think their children are learning something not already known without the degree. 7. AS MISUNDERSTANDINGS INCREASE, POOR COMMUNICATION INCREASES. Tautology: It is a little like saying that “X is true because it is X” (i.e., something is taken to be true because of the way in which it is defined). In this hypothesis, the question arises because for most commonsense notions, “misunderstanding” would be viewed as a form or subset of “poor communication.” Thus, if X “includes” Y, then it tends to make little logical sense to say that with more X, there will be more Y, since this is virtually true by definition of X. 8. A COWORKER WILL USE MORE INDIRECT COMPLIANCE-GAINING STRATEGIES WHEN ADDRESSING A SUBORDINATE. Lacking comparative condition: This sounds sensible. However, it is incomplete. Whenever the words “more” or “less” are used, the question arises “less (or more) than what?” In this case, it is not entirely clear which of the following phrases should be added to the end of the hypothesis: “...THAN WHEN ADDRESSING A SUPERIOR,” “...THAN DIRECT STRATEGIES,” or “THAN A COWORKER.” Complete the comparison implied by “more” or “less.” 9. SCHOLARS BELIEVE THAT ALCOHOLISM IS A DISEASE, WHILE OTHERS BELIEVE IT TO BE A BAD HABIT. Descriptive, not relational: This fails in three important senses. First, although it sounds like it is saying something substantive about the nature of alcoholism, it really is only saying something about what certain people believe about alcoholism, which was not the point of the paper. Second, it is not about communication, and therefore is not very topical to the assignment or major. Third, the hypothesis is definitional in content, not a hypothesis. There is no “relationship between variables” being described here. This hypothesis is stating something about what alcoholism is rather than what it is related to and how it is related. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 35 A NOTE ABOUT STRUCTURALLY CORRECT SYLLOGISMS THAT ARE NOT THEORETICALLY SOUND. A student constructed the following propositions in syllogistic form: H1: A strong relationship with in-laws is positively related to the quality of a marriage. H2: The quality of a marriage is positively related to the similarity of the partners’ socioeconomic status. H3: Therefore, a strong relationship with in-laws is positively related to the socioeconomic status of the couple. This is a structurally sound syllogism (i.e., [A] [B] [C]), but it makes no conceptual or explanatory sense as a coherent whole. It makes sense that similarity in socio-economic factors might lead to a ‘stronger’ (e.g., more satisfying) relationship, but it makes little sense to suggest that a stronger relationship would lead to a partner who was previously uneducated, poor, or from male to become educated, rich, and female. SES factors (sex, education, wealth, etc.) are distal variables—variables that have long duration and tend to precede current relationship status. So the implicit order of the syllogism could easily be made more sensible: H1: Similarity in partners’ SES is positively related to partner relationship satisfaction. H2: Relationship satisfaction is positively related to satisfaction with in-law relationships. H3: Therefore, similarity in partners’ SES is positively related to satisfaction with in-law relationships. Or: H1: Similarity in partners’ SES is positively related to likelihood of satisfaction with in-law relationships. H2: In-law relationship satisfaction is positively related to satisfaction with relational satisfaction. H3: Therefore, similarity in partners’ SES is positively related to relational satisfaction. The point is to consider the validity of a syllogism both at a structural (i.e., formal logic) and explanatory (i.e., narrative) level. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 36 MODELS AND MODELING: A FUNDAMENTAL SCIENTIFIC LITERACY Some notes for graduate student papers: “Understanding that our scientific knowledge is ‘only’ a model is the key to true scientific literacy. Knowing this tells us that our science has built-in limitations, but that it does resemble reality in very fundamental ways. More importantly, that understanding gives us permission to use our models when they are useful— and permission to discard them when they no longer meet our needs. … Ultimately, our models and descriptions of reality must be subject to two overriding criteria: How useful is this model, and how much does this model resemble our observations. Scientific literacy requires an understanding that science is only a model.” Martin, T. W. (2007, Sept/Oct). Scientific literacy and the habit of discourse. Seed, pp. 77-78. Modeling is one of the zeniths of scientific reasoning. It incorporates analytic reasoning (i.e., breaking wholes into parts), synthetic reasoning (i.e., [re]assembling parts into wholes), and abstract reasoning (i.e., recognizing similar things across levels of abstraction and concreteness). It also is fundamental to theoretical reasoning (i.e., explaining how and why phenomena happen the way they do), and methodology (i.e., specifying hypotheses to be tested qualitatively or quantitatively, the results of which inform the validity of the underlying theory). Modeling involves developing a visual “map” of a “territory.” The territory is rarely known entirely, and in fact, sometimes it is only vaguely understood. The territory may have been observed in various ways, but seldom has it been observed in its entirety. Thus, a map is needed to navigate our way through this territory, and to guide the conduct of research into that territory, to see to what extent our map does a good job of representing that territory. Every territory, however, has a potentially infinite number of potential maps. Consider San Diego County. There are topographical, weather, road, tectonic activity, and photographic maps of the territory. Every one of these maps represents San Diego, but does so from a different point of view. A tectonic history map of San Diego is not likely to be very useful to someone seeking how to get from a house address in Ramona to a destination in the Gaslamp. Likewise, a road map is not likely to be very useful to a person seeking to predict where the next earthquake will occur. Furthermore, “the map is not the territory.” That is, the map is never the exact same thing as the territory. A map that completely represented a territory would be the territory. Thus, maps are abstractions. They are intended to be abstract representations. They are generally intended to have some degree of abstract generality as well. A map that only represents an extremely small and detailed event or place, and only represents a “snapshot” has limited value. Maps may change as the territory changes (this is why roadmaps are updated frequently), but they are usually intended to have some validity and value over time and space. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 37 The graduate version of this assignment involves the development of a type of map, which is here referred to as a model. Specifically, a model will be developed and arguments in defense of the model will be developed. The model will be developed to represent some communication process directly relevant to the course. Models vary by a number of various dimensions, any or all of which may be useful to consider as you ponder what kind of model to create. A partial list of such dimensions is explained below: (1) Abstraction Level: Models vary by the scope or level of abstraction they seek to represent. Marx’s theory of capitalist systems and social conflict was a very broad-based macro model of how societies evolve when property is privately owned. Janis’s mezzo model of groupthink attempts to explain why and how highly cohesive groups tend to reach premature consensus. Andersen’s cognitive valence model was developed to represent at a micro level how minute changes in intimacy behavior by one partner influences the response behaviors of the other partner. (2) Complexity: Some models are relatively simple, and hypothesize only a few relationships. For example, Spitzberg, Canary and Cupach predicted that the use of conflict strategies influences competence judgments, which then influence relationship quality. These three concepts produce a relatively small number of specific predictions. In contrast, Turner synthesized numerous theories and propositions to develop a model of social conflict that involves over 20 concepts and dozens of hypotheses. (3) Testability: Some models are relatively difficult to observe or test, and are therefore conjectural. Freud’s theories of the id, ego, and superego are highly conjectural because by definition, the unconscious is difficult to observe or test. In contrast, Cupach and Metts developed a model of how face-saving strategies are used to disengage from a relationship, which is relatively easy to test through surveys, narratives, or other techniques. A relatively easy litmus test for this dimension is to see how other scholars have “observed” (tested, measured, rated, scaled, coded, interviewed, etc.) the concepts in your model, if at all. If a concept has been previously studied by other scholars, then it can be tested or observed. If it is still a “hypothetical” concept, then it may be more conjectural in nature. (4) Recursiveness: Some models are strictly linear. That is, they progress from cause to effect. Other models are more processual, such that at various steps a given result may “feed back” into the process and influence prior processes. For example, a linear model might propose that lonely people are more likely to seek out parasocial relationships in the media than non-lonely people. In this case, loneliness causes media consumption behavior in a linear manner. In contrast, a model might propose that as lonely people consume more media, they are spending less time in social contexts, and therefore lose their social skills and confidence in their social abilities. In this model, then, the loneliness causes media behavior, which causes more loneliness, which causes more media behavior, and so forth. (5) Modeling Topoi: In general, all models have the potential for five basic types of concepts (variables, components, factors): a. Causes (also often referred to as antecedents, or “independent variables”): These are factors that influence subsequent events. As causes change, subsequent events COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 38 change. Causes typically are further distinguished in two forms: distal and proximal. Distal causes are causes that occur “deep” in the process, typically meaning “far back” in time. Proximal causes are “closer” to the events or process being explained. So, for example, in modeling relationship aggression, distal causes might include whether a person grew up observing parent-to-parent aggression, and being the recipient of parental aggression. In contrast, proximal causes might include the amount of stress a person is experiencing in a job or school, and whether or not alcohol has been recently consumed. b. Context: The context is all those factors that influence how the situation is defined by those participating in a communicative process. Spitzberg, for example, has argued that contexts typically involve issues of culture, relationship type, physical situation, and goal or function of the communicative process. Related types of contextual factors might include time (e.g., slow cultures vs. fast cultures, early in a relationship vs. later in a relationship, etc.), activity level (e.g., a party vs. a classroom), or other such features. c. Individual Characteristics: These are features of the individuals involved that may influence how a process of communication unfolds. These features sometimes can be classified as causes or context factors, but may also be treated separately. For example, Spitzberg proposes that a person’s motivation, knowledge and skills increase the likelihood of engaging in communication that is perceived as competent by self and others. The motivation, knowledge, and skills themselves can be accounted for in part by the individual’s proximal and distal experiences (e.g., a person who received bad parenting growing up—i.e., a distal factor—may be least motivated to communicate competently in situations involving a partner who is behaving in ways similar to those parents—i.e., a more proximal factor). Traditionally, psychology has distinguished these as “traits” (i.e., stable predispositions to experience and react to the world in certain ways—e.g., low IQ) and “states” (i.e., temporary ways of experiencing a situation—e.g., situational shyness). d. Process: What are the communicative behaviors or processes being explained by the model? What do the causes and context influence a person or persons to do? For example, exposure to media violence and coming from a violent family background may intersect contexts of personal conflict to produce violent behavior as a process. e. Outcomes: What results from the confluence of these various components? For example, Spitzberg’s model proposes that as motivation, knowledge, and skills of interactants increase, they are more likely to engage in communication that is perceived to be appropriate and effective. These perceptions of appropriateness and effectiveness, in turn, are likely to result in greater attraction, persuasion, and relationship satisfaction and development. Thus, there are outcomes of competent interaction. (6) Component Functions: The various components described above can generally play a moderating or a mediating role. A moderating role means that the relationship between X and Z is significantly altered by the introduction or consideration of the variable Y. In contrast, a mediating role means that the relationship between X and Z disappears when the variable Y is introduced or considered. For example, a person who experienced COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 39 parental violence as a child is more likely to engage in violence against a current romantic partner. However, if that person who experienced violence as a child is also consuming alcohol, that significantly increases the likelihood of engaging in violence against a current partner. Thus, alcohol moderates the relationship between childhood abuse and adult abuse. In contrast, the relationship between race and violent crime (Whites engage in less, Blacks engage in more) tends to disappear when controlling for socioeconomic status (SES), and thus, SES is a mediating variable. (7) Directionality: Some models represent relationships in indeterminate form, whereas other models specify the exact type of relationships anticipated. For example, a model might represent a link (or line) connecting “exposure to media violence” and “viewer’s violent behavior.” This link can be viewed as bi-directional (e.g., as a person’s exposure to violence increases, this person’s likelihood of engaging in violence increases, but also, as a person’s violent behavior increases, the more this person seeks out media content that reflects such violence). This link can also be specified as a particular type of relationship (e.g., consuming violent media increases the likelihood of behaving violently, but the reverse is not true—once a person engages in violence, consumption of violent media does not necessarily increase or decrease). Generally speaking, models consist of components and connections. The components represent some variable or process, and the connections consist of arrows or lines. A directional arrow indicates that a given variable causes or directly influences another variable. Thus, X Y means that X causes or influences Y. If the arrow is reversed (i.e., X Y), it means that Y causes or influences X. A double-sided arrow means that the relationship is reciprocal. If a sign is provided for an arrow (i.e., - or +), this indicates the type of relationship involved. A positive (or direct) relationship means that as X increases, Y increases, and as X decreases, Y decreases. A negative relationship means that as X increases, Y decreases, and as X decreases, Y increases. There is a third type of relationship that often describes communication processes—curvilinear. Although there are many types of curved lines that could represent the relationship between concepts, one of the most common is that as X increases, Y increases, to a point, beyond which, more X results in less Y. For example, as a person talks more and more, generally speaking, we tend to find this person more attractive and competent. However, as a person begins to talk during 80 or 90% of the time in a conversation, the more we tend to view this person as egotistical and narcissistic. Thus, talk time in a conversation is curvilinear to perceived competence (i.e., as talk time increases, perceived competence increases, to a point, beyond which, perceptions of competence decrease). (8) Formality: There are many potential ways of defining formality in a model, but for the purposes of this assignment, formality is the extent to which the model can be translated into hypotheses or specific propositions. For the most part, the more that the connectors of a model have directional and sign-specific connectors, the more formal the model will be. Informal models simply illustrate variables that affect one another, but do not specify how or in what specific ways each of the components is related to the other components of the model. Formal models specify not only which components affect which other components (and by implication, what components are not related to one another), they specify the exact predicted type of relationship among components of the model. COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 40 (9) Spatial Logic: Most (but not all) models follow relatively simple rules: (1) Components on the left precede, at some point, the components to the right in real time (except when there are feedback loops or arrows). (2) Every arrow represents a direct effect of one concept on another. This effect does not have to be a causal relationship, but it usually implies such an influence. (3) Every component must have an arrow headed to at least one other component, and may have arrows to multiple other components. (4) The spatially closer two components are to one another, the more closely they are associated with one another than to components further away from one another. (5) Components can be hierarchically grouped according to larger components (i.e., multiple indicators of a single concept; e.g., social skills above might be further indicated by coordination, expressiveness, attentiveness, and composure skills). (6) Every arrow is a potentially testable hypothesis (or proposition). The graduate student version of this assignment requires that a model of some communicative process be developed, explained, and defended. The following minimum requirements apply: (1) The model must be in some central way directly related to communication. (2) There must be at least five components with at least one component each representing cause, context, individual factor, process, and outcome. More components can be provided, but there must be at least one component that fits each of these component types. (3) There must be at least five directional arrows. There can be far more, but at least five must be provided. (4) At least 10 formal propositions emerging from the model must be formally stated as hypotheses. More propositions can be articulated, but there must be at least five. (5) Each component must have at least one scholarly journal article that provides backing for arguments related to that component. (6) The complete model must be capable of being summarized in an abstract 500 words or less. (7) The model must be rendered in a visual form (preferably using the drawing tools in Word, or Powerpoint, which is subsequently saved as an image and imported into Word). (8) The paper, and the reference list, must be in A.P.A. (5th ed.) format, with 1-inch margins, double-spaced, and either 11-point Arial (normal) or 12-point Times Roman font. The pages of the paper should have a running head, which includes serial pagination. (9) The entire paper cannot be more than 7 pages in length (not including appended article ‘first-page prints’), such that: a. Title page (= 1 page; which includes student name, red id#, class #, class title, semester/year, and of course, a title that represents the topic of the model). b. Model visual figure (= 1 page) c. Proposition list (= 1 page) d. Abstract (= 1-2 pages) e. Reference list (= 1-2 pages) f. Appendix: The first page of every article cited must be attached, either as a photocopy of the actual published article first page, or as a .pdf of the article first page. The first page is NOT the journal issue’s table of contents, or the publisher’s online information COMM 495: Capstone (Sched#-20854; LA-2203; Tue 4-6:40)—p. 41 page about the article—it is the first page upon which the title, author(s) and usually the abstract of the article begins. (10) The abstract provides both explanation and argument in support of the model. The idea is to explain the overall model (i.e., “make sense” of it), and provide key arguments along the way for as many of the key links or relationships as possible within the space allotted. Arguments will typically consist of backing (i.e., sources, evidence, citations, quotations, statistics), claim (i.e., the arrow or proposition), and warrant(s) (i.e., the reason why the claim is sensible or valid). Sample models will be displayed in class, illustrating some of the many ways in which communication theories, processes, and concepts can be visually modeled and explained. Students may adapt already existing models, but when doing so, must cite the existing model, and explain how the student model differs from, or advances, the existing model. COMM 495: Capstone—p. 42 Name: Mortimer Shnerd (Red ID: xxx-xx-xxx) Email: wild&crazy@girlsgonewild.com Class: COMM 495 Date: today’s date YOUR PAPER TITLE HERE (e.g., “IT’S ALL ABOUT ME”: NARCISSISM, ENTITLEMENT, AND SURVEILLANCE) In mythology, Narcissus was a hunter renowned for his beauty and very self-aware of this feature. Feeling himself so attractive, those who loved him were viewed as undeserving. Nemesis, realizing this personal flaw, lured Narcissus to a body of water in which Narcissus was able to gaze at his own reflection. Falling in love with the reflection, Narcissus perishes, unable to remove himself from the gaze of self-love. In modern parlance, narcissism is a personality trait indicating a grandiose sense of self, and it influences a variety of interpersonal behaviors. H1: Narcissism is positively related to relational proprietariness and entitlement. New media have introduced the potential for anyone to construct an idealized identity in online environments. In such a context, narcissism is both enabled and reinforced. Narcissism is a grandiose and exaggerated sense of uniqueness, need for recognition, and entitlement (Salzman, 1993). This need for love and recognition leads paradoxically to a defensiveness to criticism or deprivation, resulting in reactionary aggression toward any source perceived as threatening the face of the narcissist (Twenge & Campbell, 2003). Narcissistic defensiveness is commonly manifest in strategies such as confrontation and derogation of others, while reactions to a lack of positive feedback are often characterized by threats and antagonism (Twenge & Campbell, 2003). Narcissism is likely to lead to a lack of empathy for partners, and a belief that the partner is obliged to serve the narcissist’s needs. As such, narcissism is expected to produce a tendency to view a partner as a type of relational object, or property. Therefore: COMM 495: Capstone—p. 43 H2: Relational proprietariness and entitlement are positively related to romantic cybersurveillance of relational partner(s). Proprietariness refers to a set of beliefs and values that view a partner as a form of property (Hannawa, Spitzberg, Wiering & Teranishi, 2006). Statements as benign as “you belong to me,” can co-occur with more chilling statements such as “you’ll never escape from me.” Closely related are beliefs representing rights or responsibilities that obtain to property, such as “I have a right to know where you are all the time” and “if I can’t have you, no one can” (Hannawa et al., 2006). The underlying beliefs and values reflect various forms of entitlement. Entitlement is a belief that, like property, a person can legitimately control a partner’s behavior, access and use information and social relationships. One way in which such entitlement could find expression is cyber-surveillance, a process of frequent or obsessive monitoring of another’s social network site(s) for the purposes of uncertainty reduction. It is distinct from cyberstalking, in which the intent is to evoke fear or communicate threat (Spitzberg & Hoobler, 2002). Instead, cyber-surveillance is primarily intended to provide the ‘lurker’ information about a potential partner’s activities, commitment, or relational intentions. Therefore: H3: Narcissism is positively related to romantic cyber-surveillance of partner(s). Cyber-surveillance is likely to increasingly become a source of conflict in people’s relationships. Such surveillance not only potentially provides information that permits a partner’s commitment to a relationship to be questioned, but when a partner’s surveillance is discovered, it may communicate a lack of trust. The negotiation of self and of relationship will likely continue to become a contested site for the emergence of relationship problems, as people adjust to the ‘brave new world’ enabled by such media. COMM 495: Capstone—p. 44 References Hannawa, H. F., Spitzberg, B. H., Wiering, L., & Teranishi, C. (2006). “If I can’t have you, no one can”: Development of a relational entitlement and proprietariness scale (REPS). Violence and Victims, 21(5), 539-560. DOI:10.1891/vivi.21.5.539 Salzman, L. (1993). Narcissism and obsessionalism: An interpersonal psychoanalytic approach. In J. Fiscalini & A. L. Grey (Eds.), Narcissism & the interpersonal self (pp. 241-253). New York: Columbia University. Spitzberg, B. H., & Hoobler, G. (2002). Cyberstalking and the technologies of interpersonal terrorism. New Media & Society, 14, 67-88. DOI: 10.1177/14614440222226271 Stern, L. A., & Taylor, K. (2007). Social networking on Facebook. Journal of the Communication, Speech, & Theatre Association of North Dakota, 20, 9-20. Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2003). “Isn’t it fun to get the respect that we’re going to deserve?” Narcissism, social rejection, and aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(2), 261-272. DOI:10.1177/0146167202239051 COMM 495: Capstone—p. 45 PERFORMANCE ASSESSMENT SCALE (PAS) WRITING/APA: FORM: Demonstrates proficiency in grammar, syntax, semantics, academic voice, and application of APA style guidelines. Form displays…: CLAIMS/PROPOSITI ONS--CONTENT: Demonstrates ability to articulate researchable claims specifying the interrelationship among variables. Form displays…: CLAIMS/ ARGUMENT: CONTENT: Demonstrates ability to articulate comprehensive arguments that include relevant and appropriate claims, warrants, and evidence. Form displays…: RESEARCH/ DATA: CONTENT: Demonstrates ability to locate and appropriately cite and list recent, relevant, and reasonable scholarly research, consisting mostly of peerreviewed journal sources. Form displays…: 1: 0-20 (F) multiple types & instances within type of writing or grammatical errors in expository text, &/or displays inconsistency in rule application; frequent re-editing or rephrasing to achieve more professional voice is suggested. 1: 0-20 (F) Key claims that are not clearly articulated or delineated. Propositions fail by level of scaling, relationship, or syllogistic entailment. “Object lessons” or “list of horrors” are repeated. 1: 0-20 (F) Key claims of source(s) not clearly articulated or delineated. Specific reference to passages in the book(s) is not consistently provided, or not provided in sufficient detail or accuracy to test the claims. 1: 0-20 (F) No more than one or two directly or peripherally related external sources brought to bear on the validity of the selected source claims, &/or those sources applied are distantly relevant to source claims; &/or sources lack recency, relevance, or scholarly imprimatur. 2: 21-40% (D) moderately low level of professional voice, composition, and grammatical form with moderate number of errors, inconsistency of rule application, or required editing (intermediate to scales 1 & 3). 3: 41-60% (C) Few types & instances within type of writing or grammatical errors in expository text, &/or inconsistency in rule application; occasional reediting or rephrasing to achieve more professional voice is suggested. 3: 41-60% (C) Only minor or one or two claims or propositions need editing for sake of clarity. 4: 61-80% (B) Moderately high level of professional voice, composition, and grammatical form with moderately few errors, inconsistency of rule application, or required editing (intermediate to scales 3 & 5). 5: 81-100% (A) Writing displays consistent use of professional voice, composition, and grammatical form, and APA style and format. 4: 61-80% (B) Moderately high level of relationship specification or implications of claims or propositions, or makes few errors in wording (intermediate to scales 3 & 5). 5: 81-100% (A) Propositions that are both logically sound, and sophisticated in their thematic connection &/or articulation of complex relationships. 2: 21-40% (D) moderate level of incomplete, vague, or poorly evidenced arguments or claims (intermediate to scales 1 & 3). 3: 41-60% (C) Some key claims of the source(s) articulated or delineated, but there are some inconsistencies in the detail, gravity, or evidential basis provided in explicating the claims. 4: 61-80% (B) Moderately high level of consistent, coherent, and consistent development of comprehensively formulated arguments in support of primary claims (intermediate to scales 3 & 5). 5: 81-100% (A) Consistent, coherent, and consistent development of comprehensively formulated arguments in support of primary claims. 2: 21-40% (D) Form displays: moderately low number and quality of location, citation, and listing of recent, relevant, and reasonable scholarly sources (intermediate to scales 1 & 3). 3: 41-60% (C) At least 1 to 2 studies relevant to each selected proposition, argument, component, or source claim are provided, &/or some sources applied are distantly relevant to source claims; &/or sources lack recency, relevance, or scholarly imprimatur. 4: 61-80% (B) Form displays: moderately high level of development of sound, articulated, and evidential warrants for claims, with high status and appropriate sources (intermediate to scales 3 & 5). 5: 81-100% (A) Each major claim is evidenced by sources high in scholarly credibility (i.e., relevance, recency, peer review, etc.) 2: 21-40% (D) Moderately low level of relationship specification or implications of claims or propositions, or makes several errors in wording (intermediate to scales 1 & 3). COMM 495: Capstone—p. 46 TOPICALITY: Content demonstrates relevance to the assignment and to the communicationbased focus expected of the assignment. CONTENT & SKILLS: Demonstrates knowledge and skills of a competent ‘citizen’ communicator germane to course in applying learning objectives to courserelevant topical or audience domain. PRESENTATION PERFORMANCE: Demonstrates the ability to orally express ideas, thoughts, claims, propositions, arguments, and evidence in a competent verbal and nonverbal communication MODEL: Model provides theoretical clarity, synthesis, heurism, and potentially researchable hypotheses and/or questions. 1: 0-20 (F) There is no discernible direct or indirect link to the process of human communication, and/or no direct or indirect fulfillment of the specified assignment. 1: 0-20 (F) Demonstrates no representation of lecture, text, or syllabus-based content or learning objectives of course in the content of the paper. Paper lacks topicality or internalization of principles or claims established in course. 2: 21-40% (D) There is a minor or indirect discernible link to the process of human communication, and/or to the specified assignment. 3: 41-60% (C) There is a partial direct discernible link to the process of human communication, and/or fulfillment of the specified assignment. 4: 61-80% (B) There is a substantial direct discernible link to the process of human communication, and/or fulfillment of the specified assignment. 5: 81-100% (A) The entire project displays a direct discernible link to the process of human communication, and/or fulfillment of the specified assignment. 2: 21-40% (D) Form displays: intermediary (to scales 1 & 3) mastery of course content, claims & established principles and practices, and learning objectives. 4: 61-80% (B) Form displays: intermediary (to scales 3 & 5) mastery of course content, claims & established principles and practices, and learning objectives. 1: 0-20 (F) Demonstrates neither awareness of nor ability to integrate competent verbal and nonverbal communication into an oral presentation of express ideas, thoughts, claims, propositions, arguments, and evidence. 1: 0-20 (F) Model paths &/or components are vaguely conceptualized, &/or interrelationships are organized in confusing or unfalsifiable ways, &/or model excludes important relevant areas of research. 2: 21-40% (D) Demonstrates little awareness of and ability to use competent verbal and nonverbal communication to orally express ideas, thoughts, claims, propositions, arguments, and evidence. 3: 41-60% (C) Demonstrates moderate representation of lecture, text, or syllabus-based content or learning objectives of course in the content of the paper. Paper demonstrates only moderate topicality or internalization of principles or claims established in course. 3: 41-60% (C) Demonstrates an awareness and ability to use competent verbal and nonverbal communication to orally express ideas, thoughts, claims, propositions, arguments, and evidence with a minimal confusion and lack of clarity. 3: 41-60% (C) Model components &/or paths synthesize more than one major areas of work, articulate a number of researchable hypotheses, and demonstrates potential theoretical potential. 5: 81-100% (A) Demonstrates excellent representation of lecture, text, or syllabus-based content or learning objectives of course in the content of the paper. Paper demonstrates direct and central topicality or internalization of principles or claims established in course. 5: 81-100% (A) Demonstrates a mastery of competent verbal and nonverbal communication to orally express ideas, thoughts, claims, propositions, arguments, and evidence. 2: 21-40% (D) Model components &/or paths show some places in which existing concepts &/or relationships have been synthesized, but most of the model is derivative or lacks clear innovative analysis, or excludes some important relevant areas of research. 4: 61-80% (B) Demonstrates an intermediate awareness of and ability to employ competent verbal and nonverbal communication to orally express ideas, thoughts, claims, propositions, arguments, and evidence. 4: 61-80% (B) Model components &/or paths synthesize several areas of work, specifies an openended number of hypotheses, and suggests the seeds of a formal theoretical system. 5: 81-100% (A) Model constitutes a self-contained & hierarchical theoretical system with formally articulated & falsifiable propositions, & significant heuristic potential for theoretical expansion. COMM 495: Capstone—p. 47 Note: Ethical practice is presumptive; evidence of plagiarism counts against all evaluative standards and jeopardizes all credit. PAPER GRADING FEEDBACK BOILERPLATE Argument: Needs evidence/citation: This is a very specific empirical claim and needs evidence/backing (i.e., a citation). Comparative clause: If you have a “more than” or a “less than” formulation, you must specify the comparison group; otherwise it the question arises: “more than what?” or “less than what?”. Tautological: This is argument by definition—which makes the argument rather tautological— entity X is more Y because X’s engage in more Y. It’s a little bit like saying “Democracies are governments that engage in participative representation, so democracies are positively related to greater participation.” This doesn’t tell us anything other than what a democracy is. We need to know something about the nature of what factors lead to more democratic societies or governments for us to understand what democracy is. Thin conclusion: A little thin as a conclusion. Future directions? Research limitations? Theoretical implications? Grammar/Style: Nonparallel pronouns: “They” and “them” and “their” are PLURAL pronouns. If they are used to refer to “him,” “her,” “individual,” “you,” “me,” “I,” “one,” “person,” or other such singular pronoun, then it is nonparallel, and grammatically incorrect. Personalized “pronouns”: Just me? Gee. Avoid the generic “you” (and other self-referential pronouns, such as “we” and “our”). Of course it “personalizes” writing style [please note, the former sentence could have been written “Of course, you may think it personalizes your writing style.”]. But “you” also implies that whatever is being claimed [whatever you claiming] is specific just to the reader, and not more generally. Removing the “you” transforms the [your] claims into more universal and more assertive rhetorical forms. Rhetorically it is tempting to identify with the reader/audience, but scholars are attempting to establish general principles, and their writings are intended for not only a universal audience, but for all time. “We,” and “our,” “you,” “us,” and “I”, etc., refer to particular people reading the text—not anyone else, which seems exclusionary and temporary. Generic pronoun “one”: For the most part, it is best to avoid use or reliance on the generic pronoun “one” (as in: “One should not use generic pronouns,” or “Pronouns are often misused by someone”). The term is passive in construction, and creates complications because the word has multiple potential referents or meanings. That/Which: The word “which” is intended for interrogatives (i.e., questions; e.g., “Which of the following is not a style of conflict management?”) and for unrestricted clauses (e.g., “The word ‘which’ is intended for unrestricted clauses, which are set off by a comma.”). So, if the word “that” fits the sentence, use it instead. Effect/Affect: OK, so I know that each term has two distinct meanings, but they are distinct both within and between them. It really ought not be too difficult to learn the four meanings, and when they are appropriate. Effect (verb): rarely used because it is awkward, it means “to cause” (e.g., “The use of disclosure can effect positive change in relationships”). Effect (noun): an outcome or result of some cause (e.g., “Disclosure has the effect of producing greater satisfaction in relationships”). Affect (verb): to influence (not to cause, which is specific, but to influence, have some unspecified ‘effect’ upon) or change something (e.g., “Disclosure affects satisfaction in a variety of ways”). Affect (noun): a fairly technical term used in social sciences as a synonym for “emotion” (e.g., “Jealousy is an affect blend of insecurity and fear”). COMM 495: Capstone—p. 48 Media/Medium: The term “media” refers to the plural—i.e., multiple means through which communication occurs. The singular form is “medium.” It is common for laypersons to make this mistake, but it would be nice if a major in communication might recognize the difference. Apostrophe: The apostrophe is your friend! No contractions are permitted in APA, but possessives require the apostrophe. Contractions: APA does not allow contractions, except in quotations or deliberately informal speech (e.g., in titles). Single ‘space’ after punctuation: APA calls for a single space after words and punctuation—not two spaces. Page breaks: Learn to create page breaks rather than slop a bunch of empty spaces into the formatting! Header: Learn to insert legitimately ‘embedded’ headers rather than just typing them at the tops of page breaks. Often times: Given that “often” means something happening many “times,” isn’t this a redundant phrase? It: Try not to begin paragraphs with the indefinite pronoun “It,” as “it” has no immediate referent for the reader. Quotation Practices: Voice: Part of developing your argumentative competence is promoting your own ‘voice’ in formulating your writing. One of the ways of doing this is to subordinate authors whenever reasonable to do so. This means putting them in a parenthetical position at the end of the sentence, rather than drawing attention to the fact that you are relying on someone else to do your thinking and writing for you. There are obvious reasons for noting authors explicitly, such as when citing specific findings of specific studies, but otherwise, they can usually be subordinated. Quotation bracketing: Quotation marks surround the quote—not the authors of the quote. The authors are not part of the quotation itself. Page #!: Direct quotations require page numbers. Otherwise, you are expecting any reader to have to read an entire article or even book in order to verify your quotation! Minimize quotations: Apparently you didn’t see the comment in the instructions: “please note, there is no quotation, and no need for quotation. Quotations should be used extremely sparingly—instructors want to see the students’ writing, not someone else’s.” Page #: You only use page numbers for direct quotations, but if there is a direct quotation, there must be a page number (web-based html sources are not allowed as sources, so you should have .pdf or hardcopy with pages). Scholarly sources: The paper instructions state: “Evidence claims with scholarly journal sources… There must be a minimum of three A.P.A. style (6th ed.) citations* of scholarly journal references you used to support your explanations of the propositions (there may be additional citations and the additional sources do not need to be from journals).” Textbook cites: Undergraduate textbooks are not considered scholarly because they are written for a relatively “lay” audience, rather than for scholars. URL: For any source available as a .pdf through the library search engines, there is no need to provide a URL address. A .pdf is a digital scan of the original hard copy of the published work, and therefore, it is the same as having the original source. The function of a URL is primarily to assist reader review when the original is not available. COMM 495: Capstone—p. 49 Citation/Reference Formatting Practices: Ruler Tabs!: Learn to use the ruler/tabs or [Ctl-T] to create ‘hanging indents’ for your references—do NOT use hard returns and tab indents, because if there are any changes to font or format, it screws up the formatting. Page #: Direct quotations require a specified page number. Volume #: The Volume # is italicized too in APA; the issue number is not part of APA references (except when the journal is paginated from page 1 for every issue such as with popular magazines; something that rarely occurs among scholarly journals). et al.: The first time you cite the source, cite all the authors (unless there are 6 or more authors). Secondary citations: There should be no secondary citations. If Jones cites Smith, and you only have Jones informing you about what Smith says, but you want to cite Smith, then go find Smith. If it is important enough to include as part of your argument, it is important enough to go find so you can (a) verify its content and claims, and (b) learn more about the claim you are arguing. Retrieval: If you can download the .pdf of a document, this is a legitimate version of the “hardcopy,” and therefore you do not need to include the online retrieval information. Citing the online retrieval information is only necessary for “html” based documents. APA reference format: It is so extremely discouraging to see this. I lecture over it. I provide elaborate instructions, and elaborate example. I even test over it. And yet, it appears that no real attention is paid to the APA format requirements, thereby requiring extensive correction on my part, taking my valuable time and effort away from providing more substantive commentary on your ideas. Formatting matters! Why is it important to do “deep” research on topics? Self-Esteem: You may want to see: Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Does high self-esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or healthier lifestyles? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 4, 1-44. Why is it important to do “deep” research on topics? Self-disclosure: Research decades ago by Wheeless and others demonstrated that self-disclosure occurs along a number of different dimensions, including: (1) depth, (2) breadth, (3) positive valence/content, (4) negative valence/content, (5) honesty/dishonesty, (6) reciprocity, and (7) frequency. So, when it is claimed that “self-disclosure” is positively or negatively related to X, the writer is implicitly claiming that each and every one of these forms of disclosure are similarly related to X. It is highly unlikely, however, that both disclosure of positive things and disclosure of negative things about self are similarly related to much of anything, just as an example. COMM 495: Capstone—p. 50 Why is it important to do “deep” research on topics? Media & Body Image: You may want to see: Levine, M. P., & Murnen, S. K. (2009). “Everybody knows that mass media are/are not [pick one] a cause of eating disorders”: A critical review of evidence for a causal link between media, negative body image, and disordered eating in females. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28, 9-42. It’s about communication: Despite being a communication major, many students continue to commit the fallacy that relationships do not work because they “lack” communication. It is rarely a matter of lacking communication, but lacking competent communication. Remember, abusive relationships have plenty of communication—it’s just bad communication. Avoiding communication is itself sending a message—“I don’t want to talk to you.” Please, please, please, use the concepts we teach in the way in which you understand and appreciate the communication process. Related, research and theory have increasingly been identifying the specific adaptive role that bias and inaccuracy play in the success of certain relationships (see., e.g., [a] Boyes, A. D., & Fletcher, G. J. O. (2007). Metaperceptions of bias in intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 286-306. [b] Spitzberg, B. H. (1993). The dialectics of (in)competence. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 137-158. [c] Spitzberg, B. H. (1994a). The dark side of (in)competence. In W. R. Cupach & B. H. Spitzberg (Eds.), The dark side of interpersonal communication (pp. 25-49). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.). COMM 495: Capstone—p. 51 DIFFERENTIATING SCHOLARLY JOURNALS FROM POPULAR PERIODICALS FEATURES Authorship SCHOLARLY JOURNALS POPULAR PERIODICALS Author(s) typically experts or specialists in field, from recognized academic institution, with ‘terminal academic degrees’ Articles usually reviewed and critically evaluated by a board of experts in the field (i.e., refereed, or “peer reviewed”); statement of article submission procedures provided. The “gold standard”: blind peer review A reference list (works cited) and/or footnotes are always provided to ground the article in the existing research literature Written in the technical or theoretical jargon of the field for scholarly readers (e.g., professors, researchers, students, etc.) Articles typically more structured, may include ‘boilerplate’ sections (e.g., abstract, literature review, method, results, conclusion, references or bibliography) Longer articles, providing in-depth analysis Author typically a staff writer or journalist; credentials often not provided Special Features Illustrations that support the text such as tables of statistics, graphs, maps, or photographs, labeled numerically as Table 1, Table 2, etc. Illustrations with glossy or color photographs; typically include advertisements between articles Serialization Typically, “Vol.” and “Issue” numbers are identified, and pagination of the articles is continuous from one issue to the next within a volume or year. Each new issue begins with page 1, and individual issues most likely referred to by “month” and/or day/date rather than volume(issue) numbers Vs. Textbook Textbooks, particularly undergraduate textbooks, are written for students— not for scholars. They are NOT acceptable for citation in proposition paper assignments. Scholarly books and scholarly edited books, are written for other scholars and tend to be heavily referenced throughout. Editors Credits/ Citations Language/ Audience Format/ Structure Length Articles are not evaluated by experts in the field, but by the staff editors A reference list is typically not provided, although names of reports or references may be listed for “suggested reading” Written in non-technical language for anyone to understand; written for broad appeal Articles often do not follow a specific format or structure Shorter articles, providing broader overviews of topics Adapted from SDSU Library handout COMM 495: Capstone—p. 52 A SAMPLING OF POSSIBLE CONCEPTUAL ENTRIES INTO AN ENCYCLOPEDIA OF COMMUNICATION TOPIC/ENTRY (N > 700) Acculturation processes & communication Accuracy Action Assembly Theory Activist Media Addiction and Exposure Advertising, History of Aesthetics Affective Disposition Theories Affects and Media Exposure Agenda Building Agenda Setting Effects Aging and Cognitive Processing Aging & Message Production & Processing Americanization of media Anxiety-Uncertainty-Management Theory Apologies, Remedial Episodes Appraisal Theory Argumentative Discourse Aristotle (Murray?) Arrangement and Rhetoric Art as Communication Artifacts Attending to Mass Media Attention to Media Content by Life-Span Attention Attitude – Behavior Consistency Attitude Accessibility Attitudes and Values, Media Effects on Attitudes Attribution Processes Audience Segmentation Bad News, Communicating Political Communication Bi- and multilingualism Bias in the News Black Feminist Media Studies Body Images in the Media Branding Bureaucracy and Communication Business Discourse DISCIPLINARY CATEGORY Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Media Production and Content Information Processing and Cognitions Development Communication Exposure to Communication Content Media History Communication Theory and Philosophy Exposure to Communication Content Exposure to Communication Content Political Communication Media Effects Information Processing and Cognitions Information Processing and Cognitions International Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Language and Social Interaction Media Effects Language and Social Interaction Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Visual Communication Popular Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Developmental Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Information Processing and Cognitions Information Processing and Cognitions Media Effects Information Processing and Cognitions Information Processing and Cognitions Exposure to Communication Content Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication Theory and Philosophy Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Media Production and Content Feminist and Gender Studies Reality Perception through the Media Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Organizational Communication Language and Social Interaction COMM 495: Capstone—p. 53 Candidate Image Cartoons Catharsis Theory Celebrity Culture Celebrity Stalking Censorship Change Management and Communication Child Protection, Media Regulations Classroom Instructional Technology Classroom Management Techniques Classroom Power Classroom Questioning Classroom Student-Teacher Interaction Cognition Cognitive Dissonance Cognitive Science Collective Action and Communication Collective Opinion and the Media Comforting Communication Comm Apprehension and Social Anxiety Comm Apprehension in the Classroom Comm App., Intervention Techniques Comm Professions and Academic Research Communication Satisfaction Commodification of the Media Commodity Feminism Communibiology Communication Accommodation Theory Communication Apprehension Communication as a Field and Discipline Communication Education, Goals of Communication Evaluation Research Communication Inequality Communication Management Communication Networks Communication Research and Politics Communication Skills Across The Life Span Comm. Strategies For Empowerment Comm. Technology and Development Communication Technology Communication Theory and Philosophy Communication, Relationship Rules Communicative Aggression Communicator Style Political Communication Visual Communication Media Effects Popular Communication Interpersonal Communication Communication and Media Law and Policy Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Developmental Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Exposure to Communication Content Communication Theory and Philosophy Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Reality Perception through the Media Interpersonal Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Communication as a Field and Discipline Interpersonal Communication Media Economy Feminist and Gender Studies Information Processing and Cognitions Language and Social Interaction Interpersonal Communication Communication as a Field and Discipline Instructional/Educational Communication Development Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Organizational Communication Communication as a Field and Discipline Developmental Communication Development Communication Development Communication Communication and Technology Communication Theory and Philosophy Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication COMM 495: Capstone—p. 54 Communities of Practice Community Integration Community Structure Model Competence/Performance Distinction Compliance Gaining Comprehension Computers-User Interaction Conflict and Cooperation Across Life-Span Conflict as Media Content Conflict Resolution Consistency Theories Construction of Reality through the News Constructivism in Information Processing Constructivism Contingency Model of Conflict Control and Authority in Organizations Conversation Analysis Credibility Effects Crisis Communication Critical Rationalism Cultivation Effects Cultivation Theory Cultural Imperialism Theories Cultural Patterns and Communication Cultural Studies Cultural Studies, Feminist Popular Culture Culture & Comm, Ethnography Culture and Health Communication Culture Industries Culture, Definition and Concepts Cumulative Media Effects Cyberfeminism Cybernetics Dating Relationships Death, Dying and Communication Deception Detection Accuracy Deception in Discourse Deceptive Message Production Decision Making processes in organizations Deduction vs Induction vs Abduction Deep Structure Deliberativeness: Political Communication Delivery and Rhetoric Dependency Theories Language and Social Interaction Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Language and Social Interaction Information Processing and Cognitions Information Processing and Cognitions Exposure to Communication Content Developmental Communication Media Production and Content Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Exposure to Communication Content Media Production and Content Information Processing and Cognitions Communication Theory and Philosophy Strategic Communication Organizational Communication Language and Social Interaction Media Effects Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Communication Theory and Philosophy Reality Perception through the Media Communication Theory and Philosophy International Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Communication Theory and Philosophy Feminist and Gender Studies Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Popular Communication Communication Theory and Philosophy Media Effects Feminist and Gender Studies Communication Theory and Philosophy Interpersonal Communication Developmental Communication Interpersonal Communication Language and Social Interaction Interpersonal Communication Organizational Communication Communication Theory and Philosophy Language and Social Interaction Political Communication Rhetorical Studies Development Communication COMM 495: Capstone—p. 55 Desensitization Design Theory Development and Gender Development Communication Campaigns Development Communication Development Discourse Development, Resistance to Developmental Communication Dialogic perspectives Diffusion of Information and Innovation Digital Divide Digitization and Media Convergence Direct and Indirect Effects Directives and Requests Disability and Communication Disasters and Communication Disclosure and Communication Disclosure in Interpersonal Communication Discourse Comprehension Discourse Markers Discourse Discourse, Cognitive Approaches Discursive Psychology Dissent in organizations Distance Education Diversity in the Workplace Doctor-Patient Talk Double-Bind Communication Drama in Media Content Dual Coding Theory E-Democracy Educational Communication Educational Media Content Educational Television: Children Election Campaign Communication Electronic Mail Emic vs Etic Research Emotion & Communication in organizations Emotion and Discourse Emotion Emotional Arousal Theory Emotions, Media Effects on Empathy Theory English Only Movements Media Effects Language and Social Interaction Development Communication Development Communication Development Communication Development Communication Development Communication Developmental Communication Organizational Communication Media Effects Communication and Technology Communication and Technology Media Effects Language and Social Interaction Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Interpersonal Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Language and Social Interaction Communication Theory and Philosophy Language and Social Interaction Language and Social Interaction Organizational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Language and Social Interaction Interpersonal Communication Popular Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Political Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Developmental Communication Political Communication Media History Communication Theory and Philosophy Organizational Communication Language and Social Interaction Information Processing and Cognitions Media Effects Media Effects Exposure to Communication Content Language and Social Interaction COMM 495: Capstone—p. 56 Enjoyment/Entertainment Seeking Exposure to Communication Content Entertainment Education Exposure to Communication Content Entertainment, Effects of Media Effects Environment and Social Interaction Interpersonal Communication Environmental Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Escapism Exposure to Communication Content Ethnic Media and their Influence Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Ethnicity and Exposure to Communication Exposure to Communication Content Ethnography of Communication Language and Social Interaction Ethnolinguistic Vitality and Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Ethnomethodology Language and Social Interaction Ethos and Rhetoric Rhetorical Studies Evolutionary Theory Exposure to Communication Content Excitation & Arousal Exposure to Communication Content Excitation Transfer Theory Media Effects Exemplification and Exemplars, Effects of Media Effects Exemplification in Health Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Expectancy Violation Interpersonal Communication Expectancy-Value Models Exposure to Communication Content Extended Parallel Process Model Information Processing and Cognitions Eye Behavior Interpersonal Communication Facial Expressions Interpersonal Communication Family Communication Patterns Developmental Communication Family Decision Making Developmental Communication Fandom Popular Communication Fantasy-Reality Distinction Developmental Communication Fear Induction on Children Through Media Content Developmental Communication Fear Induction through Media Content Media Effects Feedback Processes in Organizations Organizational Communication Feminine Mystique Feminist and Gender Studies Femininity and Feminine Values Feminist and Gender Studies Feminist Communication Ethics Feminist and Gender Studies Feminization of Media Content Feminist and Gender Studies Fetishiziation Popular Communication Freedom of Communication Media History Friendship and Communication Developmental Communication Friendship and Peer Interaction Interpersonal Communication Frustration Aggression Theory Media Effects Functional Analysis Communication Theory and Philosophy GLBT Media Studies Feminist and Gender Studies Gaze in Interaction Language and Social Interaction Gender and Discourse Language and Social Interaction Gender and Journalism Journalism Gender and Media Organizations Feminist and Gender Studies COMM 495: Capstone—p. 57 Gender, Representation in the Media Genre Gestures and Kinesics Gestures in Discourse Girl Culture Globalization of the Media Goals & Social Interaction Goals Gossip and Small Talk Groupthink Habituation Hacktivism Hate Speech and Ethnophaulisms Health Behavior Change Health Belief Model Health Campaigns For Development Health Campaigns, Communication in Health Communication and Internet Health Communication Health Communication, Ethics in Health Disparities, Communication in Health Literacy Hermeneutics Hostile Media Phenomenon Human-Computer Interaction Hybridity Theories Identification Identities and Discourse Identity Development and Communication Identity Politics Idiographic vs Nomothetic Science Image Management Image Restoration Theory Imagined Interactions Immediacy Impersonal Effects Implicit Personality Theories Impression Management Individual Differences & Info. Processing Information Technology, Development Information Technology, Economics Of Information Literacy Information Overload Information Processing Feminist and Gender Studies Communication Theory and Philosophy Interpersonal Communication Language and Social Interaction Popular Communication Media Economy Interpersonal Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Language and Social Interaction Small Group/Decision-Making Exposure to Communication Content Communication and Technology Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Development Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication Theory and Philosophy Reality Perception through the Media Communication and Technology International Communication Exposure to Communication Content Language and Social Interaction Developmental Communication Feminist and Gender Studies Communication Theory and Philosophy Organizational Communication Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Interpersonal Communication Nonverbal Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Information Processing and Cognitions Interpersonal Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Communication and Technology Communication and Technology Communication and Technology Communication and Technology Information Processing and Cognitions COMM 495: Capstone—p. 58 Information Processing, Self-Concept Information Processing and Cognitions Information Processing, Stereotypes Information Processing and Cognitions Information Science Communication Theory and Philosophy Information Seeking Exposure to Communication Content Information Society Communication and Technology Information Communication Theory and Philosophy Infotainment Media Production and Content Ingratiation and Affinity Seeking Interpersonal Communication Initial Interaction Interpersonal Communication Interaction Adaptation Theory Interpersonal Communication Interaction Communication Theory and Philosophy Interactional Sociolinguistics Language and Social Interaction Interactivity, Concept of Communication Theory and Philosophy Intercultural & Intergroup Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intercultural Communication in Health-Care Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intercultural Communication Training Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intercultural Conflict Styles and Facework Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intercultural Norms Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Interethnic Relationship in Families Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intergenerational Communication Developmental Communication Intergroup Accommodative Processes Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intergroup Comm. & Discursive Psychology Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intergroup Contact and Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intergroup dimensions of organizational life Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Intermediality Communication Theory and Philosophy International Comm. Association (ICA) Communication as a Field and Discipline International Communication International Communication International Political Communication Political Communication Internet and Popular Culture Popular Communication Internet Use Across Life-Span Developmental Communication Internet, Technology of Communication and Technology Interorganizational Networks Organizational Communication Interpersonal Attraction Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Competence & Social Skills Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Comm., Sex & Gender Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Conflict Interpersonal Communication Intimacy, Communication Characteristics of Interpersonal Communication Intimate Talk with Family and Friends Language and Social Interaction Intimate Violence Interpersonal Communication Intrinsic Motivation and Volition Exposure to Communication Content Invention and Rhetoric Rhetorical Studies Issue Management in Politics Political Communication Issue Voting Political Communication COMM 495: Capstone—p. 59 Jealousy Job Satisfaction, Communication Predictors Knowledge Gap Effects Knowledge Interests Knowledge Management Language Acquisition in Childhood Language and Social Interaction Language and the Internet Language Attitudes in Intergroup Contexts Language Varieties Latitude of Acceptance Leadership in Organizations Learning and Communication Learning Organizations Libel and Slander Limited Capacity Model Linear and Nonlinear Models Linguistic Pragmatics Linguistics Listening Logos and Rhetoric Long-Distance Relationships Marginality, Stigma and Communication Marital Communication Marital Typologies Masculinity and Media Meaning Media and Group Representations Media and Perceptions of Reality Media as Political Actors Media Content and Social Networks Media Content in Interpersonal Comm. Media Dependency Theory Media Events and Pseudo Events Media Literacy Media Messages & Family Communication Media Use Across Life-Span Media Use and Child Development Media Use by Children Media's Role in Society Mediated Social Interaction Mediated Terrorism Mediating Factors Mediation Discourse Interpersonal Communication Organizational Communication Media Effects Communication Theory and Philosophy Organizational Communication Developmental Communication Language and Social Interaction Communication and Technology Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Language and Social Interaction Media Effects Organizational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Organizational Communication Communication and Media Law and Policy Information Processing and Cognitions Media Effects Language and Social Interaction Communication Theory and Philosophy Information Processing and Cognitions Rhetorical Studies Interpersonal Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Feminist and Gender Studies Communication Theory and Philosophy Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Reality Perception through the Media Political Communication Reality Perception through the Media Reality Perception through the Media Media Effects Political Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Reality Perception through the Media Developmental Communication Developmental Communication Developmental Communication Media Systems in the World Interpersonal Communication Political Communication Media Effects Language and Social Interaction COMM 495: Capstone—p. 60 Mediatization of Organizations Theory Mediatization of Politics Mediatization of Society Medium Theory Meeting Technologies Memory and Rhetoric Memory, Message Memory Memory, Person Memory Mentoring Message Design Logics Message Discrimination Message Effects, Structure of Message Production Metaphor Meta-Pragmatics Metonymy Microethnography Mindlessness and Automaticity Models of Communication Modernity Multitasking Navigation Negative Campaigning Negativity Negotiation and Bargaining Network Organizations & Technology News as discourse Nonverbal Communication and Culture Nonverbal Signals, Effects of Objectivity in Science Obsessive Relational Intrusion On-Line Relationships Opinion Leader Order of Presentation Organizational Assimilation Organizational Change Processes Organizational Comm., Critical Approaches Organizational Conflict Organizational Crises, Communication In Organizational Culture Organizational Discourse Organizational Ethics Organizational Identification Organizational Image Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Political Communication Media Effects Communication Theory and Philosophy Organizational Communication Rhetorical Studies Information Processing and Cognitions Information Processing and Cognitions Instructional/Educational Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Media Effects Information Processing and Cognitions Visual Communication Language and Social Interaction Visual Communication Language and Social Interaction Information Processing and Cognitions Communication Theory and Philosophy Development Communication Exposure to Communication Content Exposure to Communication Content Political Communication Media Production and Content Interpersonal Communication Communication and Technology Political Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Media Effects Communication Theory and Philosophy Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Media Effects Media Effects Organizational Communication Organizational Communication Organizational Communication Organizational Communication Organizational Communication Organizational Communication Organizational Communication Organizational Communication Organizational Communication Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement COMM 495: Capstone—p. 61 Organizational Metaphors Organizational Communication Organizational Structure Organizational Communication Organizational Symbolism Organizational Communication Organization-Public Relationships Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Organizations, Cultural Diversity In Organizational Communication Paradigm Communication Theory and Philosophy Paralanguage Interpersonal Communication Para-Social Interactions and Relationships Exposure to Communication Content Parental Mediation Strategies Developmental Communication Participative Processes in Organizations Organizational Communication Participatory Action Research Development Communication Participatory Communication Development Communication Party Political Communication Political Communication Pathos and Rhetoric Rhetorical Studies Patient-Provider Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Pedagogy, Communication in Instructional/Educational Communication Perceived Reality as Comm. Process Reality Perception through the Media Perceived Reality as a Social Process Reality Perception through the Media Perception Exposure to Communication Content Personal Communication by CMC Communication and Technology Personality & Exposure to Communication Exposure to Communication Content Personality Development & Communication Developmental Communication Personalization of Campaigning Political Communication Persuasion and Resistance Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Persuasion Media Effects Phatic Communication Language and Social Interaction Physical Effects of Media Content Media Effects Planned Behavior, Theory of Information Processing and Cognitions Planned Social Change & Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Playing Exposure to Communication Content Pluralistic Ignorance and Ideological Biases Reality Perception through the Media Pluralistic Ignorance Reality Perception through the Media Politainment Political Communication Politeness Theory Interpersonal Communication Political Advertising Political Communication Political Cognitions Political Communication Political Communication Culture Political Communication Political Communication Systems Political Communication Political Communication Political Communication Political Consultant Political Communication Political Cynicism Political Communication Political Discourse Political Communication Political Economy of the Media Media Economy Political Efficacy Political Communication COMM 495: Capstone—p. 62 Political Knowledge Political Language Political Media Content, Quality Criteria Political Media Use Political Personality in Media Democracy Political Persuasion Political Socialization Through The Media Political Symbols Politics in Popular Communication Popular Communication and Social Class Popular Communication Popular Culture and News Media Popular Culture Populism and Responsiveness Pornography, feminist debates on Positioning Theory Postcolonial Theory Postdevelopment Postfeminism Postmodernism and Communication Power and Discourse Power in Intergroup Settings Power, Dominance & Social Interaction Pragmatism Prejudiced & Discriminatory Comm. Presence Press Conference Prevention and Communication Priming Theory Propaganda in World War II Propaganda Propaganda, Visual Communication of Proxemics Public Opinion, Media Effects on Public Sphere Public Sphere, Fragmentation Of Publics, Situational Theory Questions and Questioning Realism Reality and Media Reality Reciprocal Effects Reciprocity & Compensation in Interaction Reification Relational Control Political Communication Political Communication Political Communication Political Communication Political Communication Political Communication Political Communication Political Communication Popular Communication Popular Communication Popular Communication Popular Communication Popular Communication Political Communication Feminist and Gender Studies Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Communication Theory and Philosophy Development Communication Feminist and Gender Studies Communication Theory and Philosophy Language and Social Interaction Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Interpersonal Communication Communication Theory and Philosophy Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Exposure to Communication Content Journalism Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Media Effects Media History Political Communication Visual Communication Interpersonal Communication Media Effects Political Communication Political Communication Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Language and Social Interaction Communication Theory and Philosophy Media Production and Content Media Effects Interpersonal Communication Popular Communication Interpersonal Communication COMM 495: Capstone—p. 63 Relational Dialectics Relational Maintenance Relational Schemata Relational Termination Relational Uncertainty Relationship Development Religion and Popular Communication Remediation Rhetoric and Dialectic Rhetoric and Epistemology Rhetoric and Ethics Rhetoric and Ethnography Rhetoric and Gender Rhetoric and Language Rhetoric and Logic Rhetoric and Media Studies Rhetoric and Narrativity Rhetoric and Orality-Literacy Theorems Rhetoric and Philosophy Rhetoric and Politics Rhetoric and Psychology Rhetoric and Religion Rhetoric and Semiotics Rhetoric and Social Protest Rhetoric and Social Thought Rhetoric and Technology Rhetoric and Visuality Rhetoric of Science Rhetoric, Argument and Persuasion Rhetoric, Epideictic Rhetoric, Nonverbal Rhetoric, Postmodern Rhetorical Criticism Rhetorical Studies Rhetorics, New Rhetorics Right to Know Risk Communication Risk Perceptions Rituals in Popular Communication Schema and Media Effects Schemas Schemata, Knowledge Structures Scripts Secular Social Change Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Interpersonal Communication Popular Communication Communication Theory and Philosophy Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Rhetorical Studies Communication and Media Law and Policy Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Popular Communication Media Effects Information Processing and Cognitions Interpersonal Communication Information Processing and Cognitions Communication and Social/Behavioral Change COMM 495: Capstone—p. 64 Selective Attention Information Processing and Cognitions Selective Exposure Exposure to Communication Content Selective Perception & Selective Retention Exposure to Communication Content Self Presentation Interpersonal Communication Semiotics Communication Theory and Philosophy Sensation Seeking Exposure to Communication Content Sense-making Organizational Communication Sex/Pornography as Media Content, Effects Media Effects Sex and Pornography Online Communication and Technology Sex Role Stereotypes in the Media Feminist and Gender Studies Sexual Compliance-Gaining & Safe Sex Talk Interpersonal Communication Sexism in the Media Feminist and Gender Studies Sexual Violence in Media Feminist and Gender Studies Sexualization in Media Feminist and Gender Studies Sibling Interaction Interpersonal Communication Sign Systems Visual Communication Sign Visual Communication Sleeper Effect Media Effects Soap Operas Media Production and Content Social Behavior, Media Effects on Media Effects Social Capital and Communication in Health Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Social Capital, Media Effects on Media Effects Social Cognitive Theory Exposure to Communication Content Social Comparison Theory Exposure to Communication Content Social Conflict and Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Social Exchange Interpersonal Communication Social Identity Theory Exposure to Communication Content Social Interaction Structure Interpersonal Communication Social Judgment Theory Media Effects Social Marketing Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Social Mobilization Development Communication Social Movements and Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Social Networks Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Social Norms Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Social Perception Reality Perception through the Media Social Stereotyping and Communication Intercultural and Intergroup Communication Social support in Health Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Social Support & Interpersonal Comm. Interpersonal Communication Socialization by Media Reality Perception through the Media Sound Bites Media Production and Content Spectator Gaze Visual Communication Speech Acts Language and Social Interaction Speech Anxiety Instructional/Educational Communication Speech Codes Theory Language and Social Interaction COMM 495: Capstone—p. 65 Speech Fluency and Speech Errors Spin and Double Speak Spin Doctor Spiral of Silence Spirituality and Development Sports and the Media, History of Sports as Popular Communication Stages of Change Model Stakeholder Theory Stereotypes Stereotyping and the Media Stimulus-Response Model Storytelling and Narration Strategic Communication Strategic Framing Structuralism Structuration Theory Student Communication Competence Style and Rhetoric Supervisor-Subordinate Relationships Support Talk Suspension of Disbelief Sustainable Development Symbol Systems Symbolic Annihilation Symbolic Interaction Symbolic Politics Symbolism Systems Theory Tabloidization Taste Culture Teacher Affinity-Seeking Teacher Assertiveness Teacher Clarity Teacher Comforting and Social Support Teacher Communication Style Teacher Confirmation Teacher Feedback Teacher Immediacy Teacher Influence and Persuasion Teacher Self-Disclosure Teacher Socio-Communicative Style Teacher Use of Humor Technologically-Mediated Discourse Information Processing and Cognitions Strategic Communication Political Communication Reality Perception through the Media Development Communication Media History Popular Communication Exposure to Communication Content Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Media Production and Content Reality Perception through the Media Media Effects Language and Social Interaction Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Strategic Communication, PR, Advertisement Communication Theory and Philosophy Organizational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Rhetorical Studies Organizational Communication Language and Social Interaction Exposure to Communication Content Development Communication Popular Communication Popular Communication Communication Theory and Philosophy Political Communication Visual Communication Communication Theory and Philosophy Media Production and Content Visual Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Instructional/Educational Communication Language and Social Interaction COMM 495: Capstone—p. 66 Technology, Social Construction Of Communication and Technology Telephone Talk Language and Social Interaction Televised Debates Political Communication Television as Popular Culture Popular Communication Television for Development Development Communication Terrorism and Communication TechnologiesCommunication and Technology Text and Intertextuality Communication Theory and Philosophy Theory of Reasoned Action Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Third Person Effects Reality Perception through the Media Transcribing and Transcription Language and Social Interaction Two-Step-Flow of Communication Media Effects Uncertainty and Communication Communication and Social/Behavioral Change Uncertainty Management Interpersonal Communication Uncertainty Reduction Theory Interpersonal Communication Uses-and-Gratifications Exposure to Communication Content Verbal Aggressiveness Interpersonal Communication Victimization Media Effects Video Games Popular Communication Videomalaise Reality Perception through the Media Violence as Media Content, Effects of Media Effects Virtual Communities Communication and Technology Virtual Reality Media History Visuals, Cognitive Processing Of Information Processing and Cognitions Voice, Prosody, and Laughter Language and Social Interaction Voyeurism Visual Communication War Propaganda International Communication Women in the Media, Images of Feminist and Gender Studies Women’s Movement and Media Media History Women's Communication and Language Feminist and Gender Studies Zapping and Switching Exposure to Communication Content COMM 495: Capstone—p. 67 Abortion Asian-American Families Cognitive Behavioral Couple Therapy Abuse and Violence in Relationships Assessment of Couples Cognitive Processes in Relationships Abused Women Remaining in Relationships Assessment of Families Cohabitation Assortative Mating Cohesiveness in Groups Attachment Theory Collectivism, Effects on Relationships Attachment Typologies, Childhood Commercial Channels for Mate Seeking Attraction, Sexual Commitment, Predictors and Outcomes Attribution Processes in Relationships Commitment, Theories and Typologies Aunts and Uncles, Relationships With Communal Relationships Balance Theory Communication Accommodation Theory Adult Attachment, Individual Differences Bank Account Model Communication Processes, Verbal Adulthood, Sibling Relationships in Barrier Forces to Relationship Dissolution Communication Skills Advice, Self-Help and Media Advice about Relationships Batterers Affection Behavioral Couple Therapy Communication, Instant Messaging and other New Media Affiliation Behavioral Parent Training Communication, Nonverbal Affiliation in Non-human Species Beliefs about Relationships Communication, Norms and Rules Affinity Seeking Beliefs, Destiny vs. Growth Community Involvement Affirmation Belonging, Need for Comparison Levels African-American Families Bereavement Compassionate Love Age at First Marriage Betrayal Compatibility Aggressive Communication Biological Systems for Courtship, Mating, Reproduction, and Parenting Complementarity Accommodation Accounts Accuracy in Communication Acquaintance Process Adolescence, Romantic Relationships in Adoption Aging Processes and Relationships Agreeableness AIDS, Effects on Relationships Alcohol and Sexual Assault Alcoholism, Effects on Relationships Birth Control, Relational Aspects Blended Families Body Image, Relationship Implications Bogus Stranger Paradigm Communication, Gender Differences in Computer-Mediated Communication Conflict Measurement and Assessment Conflict Patterns Conflict Prevalence and Sources Conflict Resolution Alienation and Anomie Borderline Psychopathology, in Relationships Conflict, Family Alternative Relationship Life-Styles Boredom in Relationships Conflict, Marital Altruistic Love Boston Couples Study Connectedness, Tension with Autonomy Alzheimers, and Relationships Bullying Conscientiousness, Effects on Relationships Ambivalence Capitalization American Couples Study Caregiver Role Anger in Relationships Care-giving across the Life Span Apologies Casual Sex Approach and Avoidance Orientations Celibacy, in Long-term Relationships Arguing Change in Relationships over Time Aristotle and Plato on Relationships Child Abuse and Neglect Arousal and Attraction Children's Peer Groups Arranged Marriages Closeness Contextual Influences on Relationships Cooperation and Competition Coping, Developmental Influences Couple Identity Couple Therapy Couple Therapy for Substance Abuse Couples in Later Life Couples in Middle Age COMM 495: Capstone—p. 68 Coupling Policies Divorce, Co-parenting after Expectations about Relationships Courtship and Dating, Cross-Cultural Differences in Divorce, Effects on Adults Experimental Designs for Relationship Research Courtship, History of Courtship, Models and Processes of Covenant Marriage Criticism in Relationships Culture and Relationships Dark Side of Relationships Dating and Courtship in Adolescence and Young Adulthood Divorce, Prevalence and Trends Double Standard in Relationships Dual-Earner Couples Dyadic Data Analysis Dyssemia Early Years of Marriage Project Economic Pressures, Effects on Relationships Expressed Emotion Extended Families Extradyadic Sex Extraversion and Introversion Facework Facial Expressions Fairness in Relationships Egalitarian Relationships Falling in Love Dating and Courtship in Mid- and Later Life Elder Abuse and Neglect Familiarity Principle of Attraction Dating Services Embarrassment Families, Coping with Cancer Dating, First Date Emotion in Relationships Families, Definitions and Typologies Deception and Lying Emotion Regulation in Relationships Families, Demographic Trends Decision-Making in Relationships Emotion Regulation, Developmental Influences Families, Intergenerational Relationships in Emotional Communication Families, Public Policy Issues and Emotional Contagion Family Communication Emotional Intelligence Family Data, Analysis of Emotionally Focused Couple Therapy Family Functioning Dependence Dependency Paradox Depression and Relationships Deteriorating Relationships Developing Relationships Empathic Accuracy and Inaccuracy Family Life Cycle Developmental Designs (Longitudinal, Cross-Sectional, Retrospective) Empathy Family Relationships in Adolescence Deviance, Relationship Effects Employment Effects on Relationships Family Relationships in Childhood Dialectical Processes Empty Nest, Effects on Marriage Family Relationships in Late Adulthood Disabilities, Chronic Illness, and Relationship Functioning End-of-life, Relationship Issues During Family Relationships in Middle Adulthood Discipline in Families Engagement, as a Relationship Stage Family Relationships in Young Adulthood Discourse Analysis Envy Family Routines and Rituals Disillusionment, in Long-Term Relationships Equity Theory Family Therapy Equivocation Ethical Issues in Relationship Research Family Therapy for ADHD in Children and Adolescents Evolutionary Perspectives on Women's Romantic Attraction Family Therapy for Adult Psychopathology Evolutionary Perspectives, Applications to Relationships Family Therapy for Noncompliance in Children and Adolescents Exchange Orientation Family Therapy for Substance Abuse in Adolescents Display Rules Dissolution of Relationships, Breakup Strategies Dissolution of Relationships, Causes Dissolution of Relationships, Coping and Aftermath Dissolution of Relationships, Processes Diversity in Relationships Division of Labor in Households Divorce and Preventive Interventions for Children and Parents Divorce, Children and Enemies Exchange Processes Excitation Transfer Theory Ex-Partner and Ex-Spouse Relationships Expectation States Theory, Applied to Relationships Fatal Attractions Father-Child Relationships Fear of Death, Relational Implications Feminist Perspectives on Relationships Fertility and Family Planning COMM 495: Capstone—p. 69 Fictive Kinship Homelessness and Relationships Intimacy Field of Availables and Eligibles Honeymoons First Impressions Hooking Up, Hookups Intimacy, Individual Differences Related to Flirting Hormones Related to Relationships Food and Relationships Hostility Forgiveness Hurt Feelings Foster Care, Relationships in Idealization Friends with Benefits Ideals about Relationships Friendship Formation and Development Illness, Effects on Relationships Friendship, Conflict and Dissolution Imaginary Companions Friendships in Adolescence Incest Friendships in Childhood Individuation Friendships in Late Adulthood Infant-Caregiver Communication Friendships in Middle Adulthood Infatuation Friendships in Young Adulthood Information Seeking Friendships, Cross-Sex Ingratiation Friendships, Sex Differences and Similarities Initiation of Relationships Fun in Relationships Gain-Loss Theory of Attraction In-laws, Relationships with Insight-Oriented Couple Therapy Investment Model Iowa Youth and Families Project Isolation, Health Effects Jealousy Job Stress, Relationship Effects Justice Norms Applied to Relationships Kin Relationships Kin Selection Kinkeeping Kissing Language Usage in Relationships Leadership Leisure Activity Lesser Interest, Principle of Lies in Close and Casual Relationships Life Review, Role of Relationships Integrative Behavioral Couple Therapy Life-Span Development and Relationships Interaction Analysis Liking Interdependence Theory Listening Intergenerational Family Relations Loneliness Intergenerational Transmission of Abuse Loneliness, Children Goals in Relationships Intergenerational Transmission of Divorce Loneliness, Interventions God, Relationships with Internet and Social Connectedness Gossip Internet Dating Longitudinal Studies of Marital Satisfaction and Dissolution Grandparent-Grandchild Relationship Internet, Attraction on Loss Gratitude Interpersonal Attraction Love, Companionate and Passionate Group Dynamics Interpersonal Dependency Love, Prototype Approach Guilt and Shame Interpersonal Influence Love, Typologies Happiness and Relationships Interpersonal Process Model of Intimacy Love, Unreciprocated Hard-to-Get Phenomenon Interpersonal Psychotherapy Lust Health and Relationships Interpersonal Sensitivity Maintenance Behaviors in Relationships Health Behaviors, Relationships and Interpersonal Spread of Interracial and Interethnic Relationships Marital Satisfaction and Quality Interracial Friendships, in Adolescence Health, Relationships as a Factor in Treatment Marital Satisfaction, Assessment of Interruptions, Conversational Marital Stability, Prediction of Helping Behaviors in Relationships Intervention Programs, Domestic Violence Marital Typologies Gay, Lesbian, and Bisexual Relationships Gender Roles in Relationships Gender Stereotypes Gender-Role Attitudes Goal Pursuit, Relationship Influences Hispanic/Latino Families Holidays and Relationships Intervention Programs, Satisfaction and Stability Long-Distance Relationships Marketplace Approaches to Courtship, Love and Sex COMM 495: Capstone—p. 70 Marriage and Health Nonverbal Involvement Marriage and Sex Norms about Relationships Marriage Markets Nostalgia Marriage, Benefits of Obsessive Love Marriage, Expectations about Obsessive Relational Intrusion Marriage, Historical and Cross-Cultural Trends Opening Lines Predicting Success or Failure of Relationships Pregnancy and Relationships Prejudice PREP (Premarital Relationship Enhancement Program) Openness and Honesty Prevention and Enrichment Programs for Couples Optimism, Effects on Relationships Privacy Ostracism Proximity and Attraction Mate Preferences PAIR (Process of Adaptation in Intimate Relationships) Project Psychodynamic Theories of Relationships Mate Selection Parent-Adolescent Communication Psychopathology, Genetic Transmission of Materialism and Relationships Parental Investment Theory Media Depictions of Relationships Parent-Child Communication about Sex Psychopathology, Influence on Family Members Media Influences on Relationships Parent-Child Relationships Psychotherapists, Relationships with Mediation, Marriage Dissolution Parenthood, Transition to Public Policy and Relationships Memories and Relationships Parenting Rape Mental Health and Relationships Personal Idioms Rapport Mentoring Programs Personal Relationships Journals Reassurance-Seeking Mentoring Relationships Personal Relationships, Defining Characteristics Reciprocity of Liking Marriage, Transition to Matching Hypothesis Mate Guarding and Poaching Metacommunication Military and Relationships Personal Space Reciprocity, Norm of Rejection Minding the Relationship Personality Traits, Effects on Relationships Rejection Sensitivity Misattribution Perspective-Taking Relational Aggression Money and Relationships Persuasion Relationship Distress and Depression Mood and Relationships Pet-Human Relationships Relationship Messages Morality and Relationships Physical Attractiveness Stereotype Mother-Child Relationships Relationship Science, Disciplines Contributing to Physical Attractiveness, Defining Characteristics Relationship Types and Taxonomies Motivation and Relationships Physical Attractiveness, Role in Relationships Religion, Spirituality, and Relationships Narcissism, Effects on Relationships Physical Environment, Effects on Relationships Repairing Relationships Need Fulfillment in Relationships Physicians, Relationships With Negative Affect Reciprocity Play Fighting Negative Interactions During Late Life Polygamy Negotiation Popularity Neighbor Relations Pornography, Effects on Relationships Neuroticism, Effects on Relationships Positive Affectivity Newlyweds Post-Divorce Relationships Nonverbal Communication, Status Differences Power Distribution in Relationships Multi-Generational Households Mutual Cyclical Growth Power, Predictors of Remarriage Resilience Resource Theory Respect Responsiveness Retirement, Effects on Relationships Revenge Rewards and Costs in Relationships Risk, in Relationships Rochester Interaction Record COMM 495: Capstone—p. 71 Role Theory and Relationships Shyness Stalking Romanticism Sibling Relationships Story-telling Rules of Relationships Similarity in Ongoing Relationships Stress and Relationships Safe Sex Similarity Principle of Attraction Satisfaction in Relationships Singlehood Substance Use and Abuse in Relationships Secret Relationships Single-Parent Families Secret Tests of Relationship Status Social Allergies Secrets Social Anxiety Security in Relationships Social Capital Self-Concept and Relationships Social Comparison, Effects on Relationships Self-Disclosure Suicide and Relationships Symbolic Interaction Theories Systems Theories Taboo Topics Teacher-Student Relationships Technology and Relationships Social Convoy Theory Temperament Social Exchange Theory Touch Social Identity Theory Transference Social Inequalities and Relationships Transformation of Motivation Social Isolation Transgressions Social Learning Theory Trust Self-Verification Social Networks, Changes in Relationships Turning Points in Relationships Sex and Love Social Networks, Dyad Effects on Sex Differences in Relationships Sex in Established Relationships Social Networks, Effects on Developed Relationships Understanding Sex Ratio Social Neuroscience Unmitigated Communion Sex-Role Orientation Social Penetration Theory Validation in Relationships Sexual Aggression Social Relations Model Values and Relationships Sexual Communication between Adults Social Skills in Childhood Vengeance Sexual Dysfunctions Social Skills, Adults Virginity Sexual Harassment Social Support and Health Vulnerability-Stress-Adaptation Model Sexual Intent, Perceptions of Social Support, Intervention Groups Waist-to-Hip Ratio and Attraction Sexual Intercourse, First Experience of Social Support, Nature of Warmth, Interpersonal Sexual Motives Socialization Weak Ties Sexual Prejudice Socialization, Role of Peers Weddings Sexual Standards Socioeconomic Status Work-Family Conflict Sexuality Socioemotional Selectivity Theory Work-Family Spillover Sexuality and Attachment Sociometer Hypothesis Workplace Relationships Sexuality in Adolescent Relationships Sociosexual Orientation Sexually Transmitted Diseases and Relationships Speed-Dating Self-Esteem, Effects on Relationships Self-Evaluation Maintenance Model Self-Expansion Theory Self-Monitoring and Relationships Self-Presentation Self-Regulation in Relationships Shared Activities Stage Theories of Relationship Development Uncertainty Reduction Theory Unconditional Positive Regard COMM 495: Capstone—p. 72 Islands of Inquiry Foreword: Spitzberg, B. H. (in press). Islands of inquiry. Foreword for: G. Merrigan & C. L. Huston. Communication research methods (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. By Brian H. Spitzberg School of Communication San Diego State University Imagine an island archipelago in the vast, uncharted sea of science. Long ago intrepid explorers from a nation state far, far away settled the islands of this archipelago. Once the various islands were settled, the peoples found themselves separated by shark-infested waters, treacherous reefs and inaccessible ports. Consequently, little commerce today occurs between natives of these separate islands. Over time, the peoples developed alternative customs, rituals, religions, values, dialects, and modes of exchange. Because each island produces slightly different desirable natural resources, the various peoples of these islands face a fundamental choice: do they compete to take the territories across the waters by force, or do they find sufficient commonality to negotiate normative and mutually compatible relations for continued commerce? Conflict is costly, but may be seen as a means to possess the entire archipelago, the entire territory with all the resources and power entailed by the success of such a conflict. In contrast, a negotiated cooperative arrangement may reduce the total resources available to each individual island, but enable greater benefits COMM 495: Capstone—p. 73 by avoiding the costs of waging war and arranging complementary exchanges of the best each culture has to offer. Conflict can make a group stronger by steeling the motives to pull together against the external enemies, yet it can also reveal the weaknesses and fractures of a given group, and potentially, the entire overthrow of one’s own cherished culture. The methodological “cultures” of social scientists are a lot like these separate island cultures. They each have their rules, customs, beliefs and values. Each knows the others exist, but they engage in relatively little commerce and often view each other with suspicion and incredulity. Conflict, or at least indifference, occurs more often than cooperation. Social science began in the ancient, perhaps primal, desire to understand the world around us. Long ago, Eastern and Western traditions evolved across and into various eras, cultures and locales of enlightenment. As it was increasingly realized that scientific methods for understanding the world could be cumulative and increasingly valid, the approaches to understanding the physical world were increasingly extended to investigating the social world. These scholars eventually evolved into “tribes” of methodological and theoretical disciplines and associations. These tribes settled distinct islands of academe, often only dimly aware of the practices and beliefs of the tribes occupying the academic programs across continents, universities, colleges, departments, and even hallways and faculty room tables. The methods by which these tribes became acculturated and accustomed became claims to their natural resources of the “truth(s)” of the world, and the academic prestige implied by successful claims to this domain. Over time, these different methods have more often fomented indifference, alienation, and occasional struggles for respect, rather than negotiated COMM 495: Capstone—p. 74 cooperation. Scholars peer derisively at the alien practices of the heathen tribe across these methodological divides, and chant the righteousness of their own personal beliefs and customs. The domain of truth is often viewed as a limited resource, and any successful claims by other tribes result in territory no longer available to conquer except through renewed conflict. These territorial skirmishes often strengthen the spirits of believers and sometimes eliminate more destructive or flawed cultural customs of certain tribes; but often the ongoing battles serve no higher purpose than to fuel the conflict itself. The tribes intuitively understand that identifying a foil, or a common enemy, helps reinforce the resolve of the group. The destructiveness of the conflicts is typically exacerbated by the tendency of the different cultures to employ distinct symbols, vocabularies and dialects. Misunderstandings become common, even when negotiation efforts are pursued in the interest of cooperation. Social scientists have developed different methodological idioms of scholarly inquiry. These methodological practices represent distinct cultures, sometimes cooperating, but more often competing, to claim the larger territory of social science. Even when representatives of these distinct cultures claim publicly the importance of “getting along,” in private conversations with those of their own tribes, the rhetoric generally becomes incendiary and resentful of the others’ intrusions into territories more “rightly” reserved for one’s own endeavors. Competition for the sake of competition may have reached the limits of its evolutionary value. Two millennia have helped hone a verdant array of methodological islands. Productive progress in the future may well require more than a mere truce. Instead, the academic archipelago of social sciences may need a common bill of rights, a common sense of collective purpose and a common recognition of each other’s contributions. Unfortunately, such a COMM 495: Capstone—p. 75 revolution is not in the immediate offing. Before such a revolution can occur, however, bridges must be forged between and among the academic islands. This textbook lays the preliminary pontoons, in two important ways: First, by locating the nature of methods in the nature of argument, and second, by representing the broader scope of methods currently employed by the communication discipline. By locating the central underlying architecture of all methods in the structure of arguments, this text helps decode the Rosetta Stone of methodological languages, the symbolic intersection through which negotiations for collective commerce in the pursuit of knowledge must progress. No matter what else a method attempts to accomplish, it must rely upon, and establish the validity of, its practices through argument. Every method guides the production, collection and analysis of data, which consist of artifact(s), observation(s), case(s), example(s), or counts of something. But data alone prove nothing. Data only become meaningful in the crucible of argument, which connects the data through warrants to claims. Warrants are the reasons, rationale, or answer to the question “why” should I believe the claim being made by this research. The claim is the conclusion, or the particular proposition (e.g., hypothesis, value judgment, belief statement, etc.), which contextualizes the reasonableness of the data in connection with a claim. The claim, once established, may then become the warrant for subsequent arguments. Warrants are the bridges between data and claim, and claims so established serve as bridges to further arguments. This textbook examines ways of knowing as arguments. When a scholar has reached a conclusion, it stands as a privileged claim—a claim that this scholar’s method has provided specialized insight. Scholars apply a specialized method that they have apprenticed in their COMM 495: Capstone—p. 76 education to master, and this method serves as a way of privileging their voice compared to any given layperson’s view of the world. This does not invalidate the layperson’s views—it only suggests that methods provide a more reasoned or systematic approach to knowing than the average person will have had the opportunity or expertise to apply to making claims about some particular topic of investigation. There are many ways of scholarly knowing, but four illustrative paradigms in the communication discipline consist of discovery, conversation/textual analysis, interpretation, and critical approaches. These will be defined and detailed more extensively throughout the text, but for now, they can be illustrated in general ways. The discovery method assumes a singular objective reality, and although no method can reveal this objective truth in the social world, the discovery method uses various methods of objectification, including experiments, control, and quantification in an attempt to inch ever closer to that reality. The conversation/textual method assumes that because communicators accomplish everyday life based only on the behaviors they display through their communication (as opposed to reading each other’s minds), researchers can understand such behavior best by observing and precisely analyzing such naturally occurring activities. The interpretive method assumes that reality is socially constructed; that there are as many realities as there are people perceiving and influencing such perceptions through their communication. The critical method assumes that reality is always influenced by underlying systems of often hidden influence and power, and such structures must be evaluated through an evaluative perspective that reveals these hidden forces, thereby presenting opportunities for pursuing more noble or practical ends. If these COMM 495: Capstone—p. 77 paradigms are analyzed through the lens of the rationales they rely upon, they might look something like the following arguments: COMM 495: Capstone—p. 78 Figure 1. Diagramming the underlying arguments of different ways of knowing. DATA: All methodological paradigms engage in systematic selection, collection, observation, and interpretation of empirical communication-relevant data. WARRANT: Given these data, the methodological rationale provides justification for rendering the following claim(s)… CLAIM: Given these data and warrant(s), each of the following paradigms claims that…: From the interpretive paradigm perspective, this is a new frame for understanding these texts and their implications. From the discovery paradigm perspective, these data reveal new information about the generalizable nature of reality. From the critical paradigm perspective, these data enlighten the role of constraints, power, and exploitation, and point to possibilities for improving communication, society, and the human condition. Each paradigm or method can be further elaborated into its own particular rationale. The discovery paradigm presupposes that in any given process, there is a set of causes and effects, and that methods properly designed to manage or control for subjectivity of the researcher(s), and translating observations into quantifiable measurements, can reveal something about how causes associate with such effects. This approach to knowing implies an argument such as the following. COMM 495: Capstone—p. 79 Figure 2. Diagramming the underlying argument for the discovery paradigm. DATA: Communication is a cause, and is caused by, complex but specifiable underlying processes. CLAIM: Therefore, probability, measurement, design, theoryand hypothesis-testing render generalizable conclusions about cause-effect relations. WARRANT: Given statistical or experimental control of extraneous, mediating and moderating variables, causal influences can be identified to a reasonable level of probability (by excluding null and competing hypotheses). As another example, let’s say that in meticulously observing everyday conversation, you recognize a highly complex process through which people achieve social life. That is, apologies, compliments, requests, and the “events” of everyday life are accomplished through a subtle choreography of move and countermove of behavior. In such a dance, thoughts, values and beliefs are actually irrelevant to uncovering the structure of such accomplishments. An interactant cannot peer into your mind during a conversation; he or she recognizes an apology through the structure of the behavior observed, and therefore, such behavior is also observable by a researcher. The data of everyday accomplishments exists in behavior. If this is accurate, then it seems reasonable that all inferences about what conversationalists are attempting to achieve through interaction is exclusively “available” to others through their behavior. Several arguments could be derived from this rationale, but consider for the moment the following: COMM 495: Capstone—p. 80 Figure 3. Diagramming the underlying argument for the conversation/textual analysis paradigm. DATA: Everyday conversation is accomplished exclusively through mutually observable behavior. CLAIM: Subjective thoughts of interactants are irrelevant to understanding how interaction is accomplished. WARRANT: Interactants have no access to others’ subjective thoughts, yet accomplish everyday tasks through interaction. In contrast, interactants often make judgments about what others are doing through their behavior. In so doing, sometimes making one attribution rather than another may be an important determinant of how a person behaves in response to others’ behavior. For example, if you think you deserve an apology from someone, and this person provides what seems a cursory or inappropriate apology, you are likely to devalue this person’s apology. If you think this person provided an insincere apology because he or she thought you didn’t deserve an apology, you might begin disliking this person. Further, you might respond by seeking further apology, or avoid interacting with this person in the future. In short, your attributions or subjective thoughts about this person’s behavior directly influence your interaction with this person. Consequently, an argument can be derived as follows: COMM 495: Capstone—p. 81 Figure 4. Diagramming the underlying argument for the interpretive paradigm. DATA: Subjective thoughts affect motives and means of interaction. CLAIM: Subjective thoughts are essential to understanding how interaction is accomplished. WARRANT: Interactants seek to understand others’ behavior by making subjective attributions about why the others behaved/said what they did. Finally, the world may reveal disparities and distortions that imply underlying forces at work, which sustain themselves through power, deception, manipulation, and bias. Such hidden forces require critics to expose them, and to provide an evaluative standard against which such exploitative practices might be revealed and the victims of such distortions thereby liberated to empower their own interests. The rationale underlying this paradigm might look like the following. These various arguments lead to very different claims (i.e., methods) of understanding social interaction. For example, to the conversation analyst, only people’s naturally-occurring behaviors count as data, whereas in ethnographic or interpretive methods, both thoughts and behaviors count as data. Discovery researchers will experiment with people, whereas interpretive and conversation analytic researchers take people’s behavior for what it is, or was, COMM 495: Capstone—p. 82 and do not seek to introduce new stimuli to the investigation context. It follows that what counts as a theory can differ substantially across these paradigms. In one, behavior explains behavior, whereas in another, perceptions and feelings explain behavior. Conversation analytic researchers don’t see the relevance of the findings of survey (discovery) researchers, and survey researchers have difficulty seeing how to generalize the conclusions of conversation analysts without a cognitive theoretical context. Figure 5. Diagramming the underlying argument for the critical paradigm. DATA: There are forms of exploitation, deprivation, corruption, and distortion in life. CLAIM: Critical inquiry and evaluations need to reveal these forms of exploitation, thereby pointing the way to better ways of pursuing life. WARRANT: Such disparities and distortions could only be sustained to the extent that they are hidden or masked by powerful interests, groups, and individuals. So it is with all methodological arguments—they are the ways we know, and the ways we choose to know often seem to preclude other ways of knowing. The collective practices and vocabularies of scholars represent what Thomas Kuhn (1970) referred to as paradigms, and he believed them to be incommensurable. That is, a paradigm answers all the questions it needs to, in much the same way a religion is meant to do, and consequently, there is no need to COMM 495: Capstone—p. 83 borrow from another paradigm, just as most people find no need to borrow religious beliefs from another religion. So the islands upon which these researchers dwell are isolated by the arguments they make. But maybe, just maybe, if they recognize this fact, they can begin to understand why they reside on different islands in the first place, and how their respective tribes differ. Maybe they can begin to see how their arguments relate to each other, and begin a dialogue through which cooperation, rather than conflict, can be begun (Craig, 1999). Perhaps they can learn to “talk each other’s language” (Kuhn, 1970), and thereby better understand their differences. This textbook, by excavating the underlying basis of these differences, lays the initial bridges (and warrants) for this dialogue to begin. This dialogue is facilitated to the extent the various tribes and their customs are known. Arguments are most competent when adapted to their audiences, including those with whom we argue (i.e., other practitioners), as well as other interested parties (e.g., granting agencies, foundations, the media, etc.). Another strength of this text is a fair representation of the domain of the discipline. Few survey textbooks, for example, do justice to conversation analysis, or only give shallow consideration of critical and rhetorical methods. In contrast, this textbook recognizes the legitimacy of these methods as equivalent because their endeavors are predicated on the same discourse of argument. As Walter Fisher (1978) claimed, all arguments are ultimately erected on the foundations of underlying values, and “no analytically grounded hierarchy of values will ever claim universal adherence” (p. 377). Nevertheless, having an understanding of the multiple cultures of values with which one may seek congress, the better the dialogue can become in the service of that engagement. COMM 495: Capstone—p. 84 Nowhere is this dialogue more important than in the initial enculturation of students, beginning your own intrepid voyages into the often turbulent waters of the communication discipline. Just as it is easier developmentally to learn multiple languages early in the process of language learning, so it is easier to accept multiple methodologies before any single methodological argument has fortified its armaments and defenses against the other scholarly cultures with which it competes. Distinct cultural groups need not engage in similar practices to reap the benefits of mutual understanding and cooperation. The potential distance and competition among islands of inquiry is an old concern. Scholars have previously suggested that the key differences are in the questions that each culture seeks to answer. The types of questions asked can be important frames for arguments—such as determining what kind of argument represents a sensible response. People have suggested, for example, that there need be no competition between religion and science (Gould, 1999) or science and humanities (Gould, 2003; Miller, 1975), because these magisteria ask different types of questions, and thereby avoid encroachment upon one another’s territories. Others, however, have suggested that the differences between religion and science, for example, go far deeper than just the questions asked—they go to issues such as their orientation to skepticism, openness to new knowledge and discovery, and the degree to which faith is placed in preserving the past versus accumulating, revising, and correcting, and accumulating the past (Fuchs, 2001). In the case of the arguments posed for the methods encountered in this text, for the most part, the big questions addressed are the same—why do people communicate the ways they do, and how does such communication affect the human condition? Instead, the major differences in the paradigms of inquiry examined in this text have COMM 495: Capstone—p. 85 to do with ways of answering any such questions, and this “how” question is addressed by different methods, representing different kinds of arguments for pursuing understanding. The journey is not an easy one. Forging relations with strange cultures and territories seldom is. There are many barriers that even the most motivated and capable among us face in building bridges across the waves and shoals of these methodological divides (Bryman, 2007). On the other hand, there are usually great benefits to developing an acquaintance and ongoing relationship with these cultures. Eventually, with enough trade, commerce, and experience in multiple cultures of research, scholars may become truly multi-lingual and multi-cultural, appreciating their indigenous culture and yet fully appreciating and engaging other cultures as well. There are many fruits of knowledge to taste and experiences to pursue that can only derive from encounters with those beyond the borders of our own comfortable domains. Therefore, go forth, and may you find the value of the voyage worthwhile. And for the few of you who will ever get the privilege of applying such arguments in the service of knowledge, may you get some glimpse of the excitement that derives from knowing something no one else knows, of discovering something no one else has discovered, or of seeing further than anyone else has previously seen. The risks of being wrong are great, but the potential of charting new routes or discovering new islands, or even building new bridges, holds its own rewards for those willing to venture forth with a spirit of scholarly adventure. This text will help you greatly along your way, providing as it does the charts and compass needed for the voyage ahead. Brian H. Spitzberg School of Communication COMM 495: Capstone—p. 86 San Diego State University 30 December 2002 References Bryman, A. (2007). Barriers to integrating quantitative and qualitative research. Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 1, 8-22. Craig, R. T. (1999). Communication theory as a field. Communication Theory, 9, 119-161. Fisher, W. R. (1978). Toward a logic of good reasons. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 64, 376-384. Fuchs, S. (2001). What makes sciences “scientific”? In J. H. Turner (Ed.), Handbook of sociological theory (pp. 21-35). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum. Gould, S. J. (1999). Rocks of ages: Science and religion in the fullness of life. New York: Ballantine. Gould, S. J. (2003). The hedgehog, the fox, and the magister’s pox: Mending the gap between science and the humanities. New York, NY: Harmony Books. Kuhn, T.S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago. Miller, G. R. (1975). Humanistic and scientific approaches to speech communication inquiry: Rivalry, redundancy, or rapprochement. Western Speech Communication, 39, 230-239. COMM 495: Capstone—p. 87