Week 8 Lecture Toddler Language and Thinking Representation Language Components of Language Sound Patterns of language Speech perception Productive and Receptive skills First words Vocabulary growth Under and over extension Morphological rules and overregularization Syntax Language in Social Settings—pragmatics Theories of language development Child directed speech/motherese Pretend play Gestures Representational Skills Skills for thinking about, manipulating, and communicating to others about absent objects, past events, abstract thoughts. These skills emerge in toddlerhood but continue to develop for years to come. Children's representational abilities become obvious in their development of language. Language: An abstract, rule-governed system of arbitrary symbols that can be combined in countless ways to communicate information. Photo copyright © 2003 www.arttoday.com. Used with permission. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. The Components of Language Phonology - sounds of a language. Semantics - meanings of words. Morphology - grammatical endings. Syntax - sentence structure. Pragmatics – rules for using language The Components of Language Every language has its own set of phonemes, speech sounds that contrast with one another and can change the meaning of a word. The smallest meaningful units in a language are called morphemes. Pragmatics is the set of rules governing conversation and the social use of language. Productive & Receptive Skills Children need both productive skills and receptive skills to carry on a conversation. The development of receptive skills tends to run slightly ahead of the development of productive skills. Productive skills: Language skills used to put ideas into words. Receptive skills: Language skills used to understand what other people are saying. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Learning the Sound Patterns of a Language The early period of prelinguistic vocalization during the first year of life can be divided into five stages: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. crying cooing vocal play canonical babbling conversation babbling (jargon) Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Learning the Sound Patterns of a Language Two things are needed to prepare children to begin speaking: 1. 2. Must gain control over their speech apparatus to produce speech sounds intentionally. Must learn the phonemes of their particular language by paying close attention to the speech sounds they hear and begin imitating them. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Variation in VOT VOT frequency histograms of voicing categories across six languages (Lisker & Abramson, 1964) English identification functions for VOT stimuli superimposed on VOT frequency histograms English discrimination functions for VOT stimuli Thai identification functions for VOT stimuli superimposed on Thai VOT frequency histograms Thai discrimination functions for VOT stimuli Learning the Sound Patterns of a Language Between 10-12 months, most children start to make the transition from babbling to true speech. Protowords: Vocalizations that seem to have consistent meanings for a child and are used in attempts to communicate, but do not closely resemble adults words in sound or meaning. Photo copyright © 2003 www.arttoday.com. Used with permission. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Learning Words & Their Meanings First Words First words usually refer to: familiar persons body parts animals objects “Mama” “Nose” “Doggie” “Ball” First words may also express: feelings movement social commands not words “Goodboy” “Up” broken down into their component “Gimme!” Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Learning Words & Their Meanings First Words Children differ in the purposes for which they use their first words. Referential style: Words primarily refer to objects and events. Expressive style: Words primarily express social routines. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Learning Words & Their Meanings Vocabulary Growth Average vocabulary for 18-month-olds is 50 words, then there is a dramatic vocabulary increase. Referential children have more obvious vocabulary spurts. Vocabulary spurt: A sudden increase in word acquisition at about 18 months of age. Average productive vocabulary = 8,000 to 14,000 words for the average 6-year-old. Children's receptive vocabularies are considerably larger than their productive vocabularies. Between ages 1 and 6, the average child is learning an average of 5.5 new words per day (40,000 words by age 10 or 11). Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Learning Words & Their Meanings Processes of Word Learning Not until around the vocabulary spurt do children start using categorical words for people, objects, & events. A milestone comes when the child discovers everything has a name. Children's first task in learning words is to extract them from the stream of speech they hear, which leads to some segmentation errors (e.g., “readit the book” ). Segmentation errors: Mistakes in detecting boundaries between words in a sentence. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Wildwood Flower Oh, I'll twine with my mingles and waving black hair With the rose so red and the lilies so fair And the mirltes so bright with the emerald dew The pale and the leader and eyes look like blue I'll twine 'mid the ringlets Of my raven black hair, The lilies so pale And the roses so fair, The myrtle so bright With an emerald hue, And the pale aronatus With eyes of bright blue. I will twine, I will mingle my raven black hair With the roses so red and the lilies so fair And the myrtle so bright with it's emerald hue The pale emanita and the hyssop so blue Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Learning Words & Their Meanings Processes of Word Learning Even before age 2, children can use fast mapping, which is helped by joint attention. Fast mapping: A young child uses context cues to make quick & reasonably accurate guess about the meaning of an unfamiliar word. Joint attention: Tendency for language-learning children and adult conversation partners to share a focus of attention. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Learning Words & Their Meanings Errors in Early Word Learning A common semantic mistake involves errors of underextension. too restricted use of a word example: calling toy trucks “truck” but not full-sized trucks on the road Another common error involves overextension too global of a word use example: calling a bus “truck” Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Learning Morphological Rules Children's first words are usually single morphemes. A language's grammatical morphemes (units of language that change the meanings of words and sentences) are gradually added. Photo copyright © 2003 www.arttoday.com. Used with permission. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Learning Morphological Rules Order of Acquisition First children add -s to nouns and -ing to form present participles. Later they used -ed to form past tense verbs and -s to form the third person singular. The last to appear are those for contractions of the verb to be. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Learning Morphological Rules Productivity & Overregularization Morphological development provides clear evidence that language development involves rule learning. Overregularization: Language errors in which a child applies a morphological rule to a word that is an exception to the rule. • example: “mouses” for mice Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. October14, 2014 Online training for new students From words to sentences Syntax Question asking Passive voice Language in Social Settings—pragmatics Theories of language development Child directed speech/motherese Bilingualism Pretend play Gestures Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Online training for new students Understanding Sexual Assault (45 min) AlcoholEdu (90 min) http://www.sjsu.edu/wellness/ Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Learning to Form Sentences In any system of syntax, individual words belong to particular form classes, such as nouns, verbs, and adjectives. Form class: A category of words in a language that can fill similar syntactic roles in forming phrases and sentences. Syntactic rules specify how words belonging to various form classes can be combined to make phrases, clauses, and sentences. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Syntax Study of the rules by which words are organized into phrases or sentences in a particular language. Referred to as the grammar of the language and allows for more complex expression of thoughts and ideas by making references to past and future events. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. The One-Word Stage When children first begin to speak, they use only one word at a time. When these words are used to communicate a more complex meaning, they are said to function as holophrases. Holophrase: A single word that conveys the meaning of a phrase or sentence. “Mama” = “Here is Mama.” “Mama” = “I want my Mama.” “Mama” = “This belongs to Mama.” Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. First Sentences At 18-24 months, toddlers start to put two words together, tied into the appearance of verbs in the child’s vocabulary. When true two-word sentences appear, they usually take the form of telegraphic speech. Telegraphic speech: A toddler speech style in which words not essential to the meaning of a sentence (articles, conjunctions, prepositions) are omitted. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Further Syntactic Development Brown’s Stages of Early Syntactic Development based on length of utterances I. Express simple semantic & syntactic relationships (1-2 morphemes). “See teddy.” II. Acquire basic grammatical morphemes (2-2.5 morphemes). “See teddies.” III. Variations on simple sentences, incl. questions (2.5-3 morphemes). “Where’s Mommy?” “You can’t come.” IV. Subordinate clauses (3-3.5 morphemes). “I want you to do it.” V. Join simple sentences to form compound sentences (3.5-4). “I had cake and Daddy had ice cream.” Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Question Asking Inflection Changes in word order and addition of words Two types of questions: Yes/no questions: Are you going to school? Wh questions: Where are you going? Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Yes/No Questions Inflection Tag questions The tree died? The tree died, didn’t it? Word order and addition Did the tree die? Learning to ask Wh questions phase one Children will make two word questions ex.Where kitty? Phase two Children will add helping verbs to the question but willl often reverse them. ex.Where kitty is going? Phase three Children will form proper questions. ex.Where is the kitty going? Asking Why Questions You went to the store, why? Why you went to the store? Why did you go to the store? Negation Phase one Children will put a negation in front of the word they want to negate.ex. No kitty. Phase two children will put the negative word into their sentences.ex. That not kitty. Phase three Children will be able to add a negative into a sentence correctly ex. That isn't a kitty. Asking negative questions You are going to the store. Are you going to the store? You aren’t going to the store. Aren’t you going to the store? Native speakers understand these constructions, but even for adults they are hard to explain. Active and Passive Voice Non-reversible passives The boy kicked the ball The ball was kicked by the boy Reversible passives The boy kicked the girl The girl was kicked by the boy Before age 4– semantic strategy and first noun is the subject hueristic Age 4 to 6 –rigid word order, first noun is the subject After age 6 – correct understanding of passive construction, although very rare in production Learning to Use Language Socially Children also acquire linguistic and communicative competence. Linguistic competence involves syntactically and semantically correct use of a language. Communicative competence involves being able to carry on conversations, repair breakdowns in communications, and to use language in socially appropriate ways (as determined by culture). Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. How can children learn a language in such a short time? Environment theories stress environmental factors in language acquisition, including: the language the child hears structure of social interactions physical environment Nativist theories stress inborn, biologically based factors in language acquisition. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Chomsky argued that all languages share structural characteristics because language and the human brain evolved together. Language acquisition device: Chomsky’s term for innate capacities of the human brain that make language possible. • assumed that part of the brain is specially adapted for language learning • ignored social contexts in which language acquisition occurs Most current researchers agree both inborn & environmental factors contribute to language development. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. What the Child Brings to Language Acquisition Evidence For Biological Underpinnings Apparent sensitive period early in life Hemispheric specialization Brain changes at 8-9 months of age Species-specific nature of language Inborn abilities & constraints Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. The Environment of Language Learning Child-directed speech (CDS) = motherese: The modifications adults make in their speech when talking to young children. simpler grammatically includes fewer grammatical errors higher pitch clauses & boundaries more clearly marked by pauses, intonation focus more on objects and present tense events quite redundant includes many questions about objects & events Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. The Environment of Language Learning Impact of Child-Directed Speech (CDS) Simplifies input. Makes clear connections between words and what they refer to. Provides chances for linguistic practice. Serves attentional & affective functions in parent-infant interaction. Does not necessarily improve language development. Bilingualism Initially words learned in two languages may not be separated by language. As children learn grammatical morphemes, auxiliary verbs, and other features of syntax, the languages become separate systems. Vocabulary development in each language may be slower than for monolinguals, but the total vocabulary may be larger. Explicit knowledge of syntax is greater for bilinguals. Cognitive executive functioning and cognitive flexibility is greater for bilinguals. Lexical access is slower for bilinguals. Toddlers’ Pretend Play 14-19 months Pretend play with replica objects (dolls, toy horses, toy cars) increases. 19-24 months Use of substitute objects (using pillow to represent a baby, a block to represent a car) increases. by 24 months Most children can use one substitute object in a pretend scenario (using block to feed a baby doll). later sensorimotor Double substitutions (using a block for a bottle and a pillow as a baby) will appear. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Toddlers’ Use of Gestures around 9 months Simple communicative gestures (such as pointing) normally emerge. 9-12 months Conventional social gestures (like waving bye-bye, nodding yes, shaking the head no) usually appear. 12-18 months Toddlers begin producing symbolic gestures, representing some object or action. 10-18 months Children gradually use more gestures. around 18 months Frequency of gesture use declines. around 24 months Frequency of gesture use levels off. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Toddlers’ Understanding of Iconic Symbols Iconic symbols: Symbols that closely resemble the things they represent. 9 months old Babies seem confused by pictures. They try to grasp objects portrayed in them. 20 months old Babies seem to understand objects in pictures are not physically present. 2 years old When shown a picture of where a toy is hidden, they are rarely able to find it. 2-and-a-half years old When shown a picture of where a toy is hidden, children find it 80% of the time. Copyright © 2004 The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display.