Meta-analysis of neuroimaging data Tor D. Wager Columbia University

advertisement
SCANLab
Meta-analysis of neuroimaging
data
What, Why, and How
Tor D. Wager
Columbia University
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Uses of meta-analysis in neuroimaging
• Meta-analysis is an essential tool for
summarizing the vast and growing
neuroimaging literature
Wager, Lindquist, & Hernandez, in press
SCANLab
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Uses of meta-analysis in neuroimaging
• Assess consistency of activation across
laboratories and task variants
• Compare across many types of tasks and
evaluate the specificity of activated regions
for particular psychological conditions
• Identify and define boundaries of functional
regions
SCANLab
• Co-activation: Develop models of functional
systems and pathways
Wager, Lindquist, & Kapan, 2007
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Functional networks in meta-analysis
• Use regions or distributed networks in a priori
tests in future studies
SCANLab
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Meta-analyses of cognitive control
Authors
Chein et al.
Year Method
2002 Density (Gaussian)
Clustering of peaks,
Wager et al. 2003
chi-square
Wager et al. 2004 KDA, Spatial MANOVA
Buchsbaum
2005 ALE
et al.
Chein &
2005 Density (Gaussian)
Schneider
Laird et al. 2005 ALE
Psychological focus
Verbal working memory
SCANLab
Working memory
Attention/task switching
Wisconsin Card Sorting
Practice effects in cognitive
control
Stroop interference
Owen et al. 2005 ALE
N-back working memory
Neumann et
ALE, Co-activation
2005
Stroop interference
al.
"replicator dynamics"
Costafreda
2006 Spatial location
Verbal fluency in left IFG
et al.
Gilbert et al. 2006
Spatial location/Chisquare/classifier
Episodic memory, Multitasking,
Mentalizing in BA 10
Nee et al.
KDA, logistic
regression
Cognitive control/interference
2007
Van
2008
Snellenberg
MKDA/KDA
*
& Wager
Cognitive control and memory
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Meta-analyses of emotion & motivation
SCANLab
Authors
Year Method
Chi-square within
Phan et al.
2002
regions
Murphy et
Spatial location (K-S
2003
al.
Test)
Wager et al. 2003 KDA, Chi-square
Kringelbach
2004 Spatial location
et al.
Psychological focus
Phan et al.
2004 Qualitative
Emotion
Baas et al.
Northoff et
al.
2004 Chi-square
Amygdala lateralization
2005 Clustering of peaks
Self-referential processes
Krain et al.
2006 ALE
Decision-making
Wager et al. 2008 MKDA, Chi-square
Kober et al.
2008
MKDA, Co-activation
*
Emotion
Emotion
Emotion
Reinforcers in OFC
Emotion
Emotion
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Meta-analyses of disorders
Authors
Zakzanis et
al.
Zakzanis et
al.
Whiteside et
al.
Glahn et al.
Year Method
Psychological focus
SCANLab
2000 Effect sizes
Schizophrenia
2003 Effect sizes
Alzheimer's disease
2004 Effect sizes
Obsessive-compulsive disorder
2005 ALE
Working memory in
schizophrenia
Fitzgerald et
2006 ALE
al.
Dickstein et
2006 ALE
al.
Van
Snellenberg 2006 Effect sizes
et al.
Spatial location
Steele et al. 2007
("unwarped")
Depression, DLPFC
ADHD
Schizophrenia and working
memory
Depression, frontal cortex
Valera et al. 2007 Effect sizes
Brain structure in ADHD
Etkin &
Wager
2007 MKDA, Co-activation
Anxiety disorders
Hoekert et
al.
2007 Effect sizes
Emotional prosody in
schizophrenia http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Meta-analyses of language
SCANLab
Authors
Turkeltaub
et al.
Year Method
2002 ALE
Psychological focus
Single-word reading
Jobard et al. 2003 Clustering of peaks
Word reading
Brown et al. 2005 ALE
Vigneau et
2006 Clustering of peaks
al.
ALE, Co-activation
Ferstl et al. 2008
"replicator dynamics"
Turkeltaub
2002 ALE
et al.
Jobard et al. 2003 Clustering of peaks
Speech production
Language, left cortical
hemisphere
Brown et al. 2005 ALE
Speech production
Vigneau et
al.
2006 Clustering of peaks
Language, left cortical
hemisphere
Ferstl et al.
2008
Text comprehension
Single-word reading
Word reading
ALE, Co-activation
Text comprehension
"replicator dynamics"
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Meta-analyses of other stuff
SCANLab
Authors
Joseph
Grezes &
Decety
Kosslyn &
Thompson
Year Method
2001 Spatial location
2001 Qualitative
Action
2003 Logistic regression
Visual imagery
Nielsen et al. 2004
Gottfried &
Zald
Nickel &
Seitz
Petacchi et
al.
Psychological focus
Object recognition: category
specificity
Kernel
density/multivariate
Cognitive function
2005 Spatial location
Olfaction in OFC
2005 Clustering of peaks
Parietal cortex
2005 ALE
Auditory function, cerebellum
Average maps in
CARET
Lewis
2006
Tool use
Postuma &
Dagher
2006 Co-activation
Basal ganglia
Zacks
2008
Mental rotation
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
SCANLab
Using meta-analysis to evaluate consistency:
Why?
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Locating emotion-responsive regions
164 PET/fMRI studies, 437 activation maps, 2478 coordinates
SCANLab
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Why identify consistent areas?
• Making statistic maps in neuroimaging studies
involves many tests (~100,000 per brain map)
• Many studies use uncorrected or improperly
corrected p-values
SCANLab
Long-term Memory
# of Maps
P-value thresholds used
Uncorrected
Corr.
How many false positives?
A rough estimate: 663 peaks, 17% of reported activations
Wager, Lindquist, & Kaplan, 2007
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Consistency
SCANLab
Emotion: 163 studies
Consistently
Activated
Reported
peaks
regions
163
studies
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Ventral surface
Lateral surface (R)
Medial surface (L)
SCANLab
vmPFC
Gyrus rectus
Central sulcus
dmPFC
pOFC
BF
rdACC
Pre SMA
pgACC
dmPFC
PCC
OCC
CM, MD
sgACC
mTC
sTC
aINS
latOFC
lFG
Kober et al., in press, NI
Deep
nuclei
TC
vmPFC
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
SCANLab
Using meta-analysis to evaluate specificity:
Why?
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Disgust responses: Specificity in insula?
Insula
SCANLab
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Disgust responses: Specificity in insula?
SCANLab
Search Area: Insula
Feldman-Barrett & Wager, 2005; Phan, Wager, Taylor, & Liberzon, 2002;
Phan, Wager, Liberzon & Taylor, 2004
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Meta-analysis plays a unique role in
answering…
SCANLab
The Neural Correlates of Task X
• Is it reliable?
– Would each activated region
replicate in future studies?
– Would activation be insensitive
to minor variations in task
design?
• Is it task-specific?
– Predictive of a particular
psychological state or task type?
– Diagnostic value?
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
SCANLab
Using meta-analysis to evaluate consistency:
How?
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Meta-analysis: Multilevel kernel density estimate (MKDE)
Monte Carlo:
Expected maximum proportion
Under the null hypothesis
Peak coordinate locations (437 maps)
Permute blobs
within study
maps
…
Damasio, 2000
Liberzon, 2000
Kernel convolution
SCANLab
Wicker, 2003
Apply threshold
E Significant regions
Weighted
average
…
Comparison indicator maps
Wager, Lindquist, & Kaplan, 2007; Etkin & Wager, in press
Proportion of activated
Comparisons map
(from 437 comparisons)
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
MKDA: Key points
SCANLab
• Statistic reflects consistency across studies. Study
comparison map is treated as a random effect.
Peaks from one study cannot dominate.
• Studies are weighted by quality (see additional
info on handouts for rationale)
• Spatial covariance is preserved in Monte Carlo.
Less sensitive to arbitrary standards for how
many peaks to report.
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Whether and how to weight studies/peaks
MKDA analysis weights by sqrt(sample size) and study
quality (including fixed/random effects)
Study quality

 weight
Weighted
proportion of P  CIM   c N c 

c 
Sample
size
for

activating
c
 c N c  map c
 c

studies
SCANLab
Weighted
average

Activation
indicator (1 or
0) for map c
c 1 Fixed effects
c  0.75 Random effects

http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Monte Carlo Simulation
• Simulation vs. theory (e.g. Poisson process)
• Simulation allows:
SCANLab
– Non-stationary spatial distribution of peaks
(clumps) under null hypothesis; randomize blob
locations
– Family-wise error rate control with irregular
(brain-shaped) search volume
– Cluster size inference, given primary threshold
Monte Carlo:
E(max(P|H0))
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Compare with Activation Likelihood Estimate
(ALE), Kernel Density Analysis (KDA)
Peak coordinates
Combined across
studies
Kernel convolution
Density kernel

OR
ALE kernel
SCANLab
Apply significance
threshold
Peak density or
Significant results
ALE map

Ignores the fact that some studies report more peaks than others!

Density kernel: Chein, 1998; Phan et al., 2002; Wager et al., 2003, 2004, 2007, in press
Gaussian density kernel + ALE: Turkeltaub et al., 2002; Laird et al., 2005; others
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Comparison with other methods
MKDA
KDA/ALE
SCANLab
• Statistic reflects consistency
• Peaks are lumped together, study
across studies. Study comparison
is fixed effect. Peaks from one
map is treated as a random
study can dominate, studies that
effect. Peaks from one study
report more peaks dominate.
cannot dominate.
• Studies are weighted by quality
• No weighting, or z-score
weighting (problematic)
• Spatial covariance is preserved in • Spatial covariance is not
Monte Carlo. Less sensitive to
preserved in Monte Carlo.
arbitrary standards for how many
Effects of reporting standards
peaks to report.
large.
See handouts for more comparison points
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
ALE approach
• Treats points as if they were Gaussian
probability distributions.
• Summarize the union of probabilities at each
voxel: probability of any peak “truly” lying in
that voxel
SCANLab
P(X1  X 2 ... X n )  1 P(X)  1 P(X1) * P(X 2 ) * ...P(X n )
P(Xi ) is the probability that peak Xi lies in a given voxel
The bar indicates the complement operator
Null hypothesis:
P(X)  0
No peaks lie in voxel
Alt hypothesis:
voxel
P(X)  0
At least one peak lies in
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
ALE meta-analysis
• Analyst chooses smoothing kernel
• ALE analysis with zero smoothing:
SCANLab
– Every voxel reported in any study is significant in
the meta-analysis
• Test case: 3-peak meta analysis, one peak
activates in voxel:
P(X1) 1,P(X2 )  0,P(X3 )  0
ALE statistic:
Highest
1 Pr(X )  1 (0)* (1)* (1)  1 possible value!

• In practice: 10 – 15 mm FWHM kernel
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Comparison across methods: Inference
Property
KDA
ALE
Multilevel KDA
Kernel
Spherical
Gaussian
Spherical
Interpretation of
statistic
Null hypothesis
Num nearby peaks
Interpretation of
significant result
Assumptions
Generalize to
SCANLab
Prob. that at least
one peak nearby
Peaks are not
No peaks truly
spatially consistent activate
More peaks lie near One or more peaks
voxel than
lies at this voxel
expected by chance
Num. study maps
activating nearby
Study maps are not
spatially consistent
A higher proportion
of studies activate
near voxel than
expected by chance
1. Study is fixed
1. Study is fixed
Activation ‘blobs’
effect (homogenous effect (homogenous are spatially
sample of studies) sample of studies) independent under
2. Peaks are
2. Peaks are
the null hypothesis
spatially
spatially
independent under independent under
the null hypothesis the null hypothesis
New peaks from
New peaks from
New study maps
same studies
same studies
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Comparison: Correction and Weighting
SCANLab
Property
KDA
ALE
Multilevel KDA
Multiple
comparisons
FWER
FDR
Weighting
None, or weight
peaks by z-score
None
FWER
(recommended) or
FDR
Weight studies by
sample size,
fixed/random
effects, quality
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Density analysis: Summary
SCANLab
Working memory Executive WM Long-term memory
Memory
Inhibition
Task switching
Response
selection
Wager et al., 2004; Nee, Wager, &
Jonides, 2007; Wager et al., in
press; Van Snellenberg & Wager,
in press
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
SCANLab
Using meta-analysis to evaluate specificity:
How?
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Specificity
• Task-related differences in relative activation
frequency across the brain:
SCANLab
– MKDA difference maps (e.g., Wager et al., 2008)
• Task-related differences in absolute activation
frequency
– Nonparametric chi-square maps (Wager,
Lindquist, & Kaplan, 2007)
• Classifier systems to predict task type from
distributed patterns of peaks (e.g., Gilbert)
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
MKDA Difference maps: Emotion example
SCANLab
Experienced
Perceived
• Approach:
– Calculate density maps for two conditions, subtract to get
difference maps
– Monte Carlo: Randomize blob locations within each study, recalculate density difference maps and save max
– Repeat for many (e.g., 10,000) iterations to get max
distribution
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
– Threshold based on Monte Carlo simulation
Emotion example: Selective regions
Experience > Perception
Perception > Experience
OFC
SCANLab
Amy
TP
IFG
Hy
Midb
OFC
aIns
TP
OFC
vaIns
dmPFC
Amy
mOFC Hy
vaIns
IFG
pgACC
PAG
Hy
Midb
PAG
TP
TP
CB
Amy
OFC
CB
aIns
Amy
Wager et al., in press, Handbook of Emotion
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Task-brain activity associations in meta-analysis
SCANLab
Study
contrast
map
Region/V Task
oxel 1
condition
Study 1
1
Disgust
Study 2
0
Fear
Study 3
1
Disgust
Study 4
1
Happiness
Study 5
0
Anger
…
…
…
Study N
0
Sadness
Measures of association:
Chi-square
• But requires high expected counts
(> 5) in each cell. Not appropriate
for map-wise testing over many
voxels
Fisher’s exact test (2 categories
only)
Multinomial exact test
• Computationally impractical!
Nonparametric chi-square
• Approximation to exact test
• OK for low expected counts
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Nonparametric chi-square: Details
SCANLab
Study
contrast
map
Region/V Task
oxel 1
condition
Study 1
1
Disgust
Study 2
0
Fear
Study 3
1
Disgust
Study 4
1
Happiness
Study 5
0
Anger
…
…
…
Study N
0
Sadness
Idea of exact test:
• Conditionalize on marginal
counts for activation and task
conditions.
• Null hypothesis: no systematic
association between activation and
task
• P-value is proportion of nullhypothesis possible arrangements
that can produce distribution
across task conditions as large as
observed or larger.
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Nonparametric chi-square: Details
SCANLab
Study
contrast
map
Region/V Task
oxel 1
condition
Study 1
0
1
Study 2
Study 3
Study 4
Study 5
…
Study N
0
1
0
…
1
Disgust
Fear
Disgust
Happiness
Anger
…
Sadness
Permutation test:
• Permute activation indicator
vector, creating null-hypothesis
data (no systematic association)
• Marginal counts are preserved.
• Test 5,000 or more samples and
calculate P-value based on
observed null-hypothesis
distribution
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Density difference vs. Chi-square
• Relative vs. absolute differences
SCANLab
Experience
Perception
Voxels (one-dimensional brain)
Chi-square
Density
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Can we predict the emotion from the pattern of
brain activity?
Experienced
SCANLab
Perceived
• Approach: predict studies based on their pattern of
reported peaks (e.g., Gilbert, 2006)
• Use naïve Bayesian classifier (see work by Laconte; Tong; Norman;
Haxby). Cross-validate: predict emotion type for new studies
that are not part of training set.
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Classifying experienced emotion vs. perceived
emotion: 80% accurate
SCANLab
Experience
EXP vs. PER
DMPFC vs. Pre-SMA
PAG vs. Ant. thalamus
Perception
Deep cerebellar nuc. vs.
Lat. cerebellum
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Outline: Why and How…
• Consistency: Replicability across studies
SCANLab
– Consistency in single-region results: MKDA
– Consistency in functional networks: MKDA + Co-activation
• Specificity and “reverse inference”
– Brain-activity – psychological category mappings for
individual brain regions:
MKDA difference maps; Nonparametric Chi-square
– Brain-activity – psychological category mappings for
distributed networks
Applying classifier systems to meta-analytic data
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Extending meta-analysis to connectivity
SCANLab
Study
contrast
map
Region/V
oxel 1
Region/V
oxel 2
Study 1
1
0
Study 2
0
0
Study 3
1
1
Study 4
1
1
Study 5
0
0
…
…
…
Study N
0
1
N = 45
Region 1
No
Region 1
Yes
Region 2
Yes
6
23
Region 2
No
12
4
Co-activation: If a study
(contrast map) activates
within k mm of voxel 1, is it
more likely to also activate
within k mm of voxel 2?
Measures of association:
Kendall’s Tau-b
Fisher’s exact test
Nonparametric chi-square
Others…
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Kendall’s Tau: Details
• Ordinal “nonparametric” association between two
variables, x and y
• Uses ranks; no assumption of linearity or normal
distribution (Kendall, 1938, Biometrika)
• Values between [-1 to 1], like Pearson’s correlation
SCANLab
 N 1

4  min( rank(x), rank(y))  i 
1
   i1


N(N 1)




Tau is proportion of concordant pairs of observations
sign(x diff. between pairs)= sign(y diff. between pairs)
Tau = (# concordant pairs - # discordant pairs) / total # pairs

http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Meta-analysis functional networks: Examples
• Emotion: Kober et al. (in press), 437 maps
SCANLab
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Meta-analysis of
emotion
Acknowledgements
Lisa Feldman
Barrett
Statistics
Martin Lindquist
Meta-analysis of
cognitive function
SCANLab
Ed Smith
Tom Nichols
Luan Phan
Steve Taylor
Israel Liberzon
Students
Hedy Kober
Lauren Kaplan
Jason Buhle
Jared Van Snellenberg
Derek Nee
John Jonides
Ed Smith
Funding agencies:
National Science Foundation
National Institute of Mental Health
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
SCANLab
Weighting
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Whether and how to weight studies/peaks
• Studies (and peaks) differ in sample size, methodology,
analysis type, smoothness, etc.
• Advantageous to give more weight to more reliable
studies/peaks
SCANLab
• Z-score weighting
– Advantages: Weights nominally more reliable peaks
more heavily
– Disadvantages: Small studies can produce variable
results. Reporting bias: High z-score peaks are high
partially due to error; “capitalizing on chance”
• Must convert to common Z-score metric across different
analysis types in different studies
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Whether and how to weight studies/peaks
• Alternative: Sample-size weighting
SCANLab
– Advantages:
• Weights studies by the quality of information their peaks
are likely to reflect
• Avoids overweighting peaks reported due to “capitalizing
on chance”
– Disadvantages: Ignores relative reliability of various
peaks within studies
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
SCANLab
MKDA vs. KDA vs. ALE:
Comparison chart
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
SCANLab
More details on reverse inference
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Is brain activity diagnostic of a particular
psychological state?
SCANLab
Pleasure?
Punishing wrongdoers
Forward
inference
Reverse
inference
Brain activity
P(Brain | Psy)
Given a psychological
We observe
state
brain activity
P(Psy | Brain)
Can we infer
Given brain
psychological pleasure?
activity
‘Forward’ and ‘reverse’ inference are not the same!
Reverse inference requires comparing across many
psychological states!
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
The predictive value problem: Worked example
For a brain region to be used as a marker of pleasure
SCANLab
– The brain region must respond consistently to pleasure
– The brain region must respond specifically to pleasure (not activated
by other things)
Non-pleasure
Pleasure
P(Brain|no pleasure) = .4
1-Specificity
Ventral caudate
P(pleasure) = .1
Prior
P(Brain|Pleasure) = .9
Forward inference; Sensitivity
Caculate reverse inference:
P(Pleasure|Brain) = .2
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
SCANLab
More details on connectivity
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
SCANLab
More details on MKDA difference maps and
nonparametric chi-square maps
http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/
Download