Prolegomena to Heritage Linguistics Executive summary , Maria Polinsky

advertisement
, Maria PolinskyUniversity of Illinois at
Urbana-Champaign 2Harvard University
Executive summary
Linguistics seeks to discover how grammatical knowledge is manifested in the
brain, and which components may be universal. Many linguists believe in the
universality of grammar, arguing that children receive too little input to develop
language’s full complexity without some inherent structuring in the brain. (This
is sometimes called the “poverty of stimulus” problem.) Language acquisition
becomes even more complex when one considers the various degrees of fluency
that are achieved. How much language exposure is needed to become a “native”
speaker? And what about the role of age? Second-language learners who
acquired the language after puberty, regardless of their level, persist in making
mistakes that even the youngest of native speakers do not. In Benmamoun,
Montrul, and Polinsky (2010), referred to as BMP below, the focus is on heritage
speakers – speakers of a language who interrupted or otherwise incompletely
acquired their first language (or one of their first languages, in the case of
children exposed to multiple languages from birth). For these speakers, one of
the languages eventually becomes the primary language and the other weakens;
this finding is at odds with the linguistic assumption that first languages are
stable in adult speakers. Usually, the majority language of the country/region
becomes the primary language, and the minority (incompletely acquired)
language becomes the secondary. Depending on the situation, the heritage
speaker might not receive formal education in the heritage language, which then
remains rooted in a specific context such as the home and/or immediate
community. Despite the fact that heritage speakers are given the same early
linguistic exposure as native speakers, the interruption of their acquisition limits
their grammatical competence, and this problem worsens by attrition as time
passes. This group lies between L1 and L2 learners, making them an extremely
useful source of linguistic information about acquisition and native competence.
Heritage speakers in the United States are usually immigrants or children of
immigrants, with heritage speakers of Spanish constituting the largest heritage
population in the country. Heritage speakers are a diverse group, and using them
in linguistic analysis is complicated. For example, incorrect self-reporting of
language level may occur because they lack metalinguistic awareness, which
further complicates the use of grammaticality judgment tests. Typically, we must
deploy multiple linguistic diagnostics to study these speakers. It is also
important to understand the language of their parents, first generation
immigrants, because this is where their exposure to heritage language comes
from. If we want to be serious about helping heritage speakers maintain their
bilingualism and their bicultural identity, we need to be cognizant of their
environment, their language exposure, language attitudes, and language use.
Prolegomena to Heritage Linguistics
1Elabbas Benmamoun11, Silvina Montrul
2
Despite many challenges, the study of heritage language learners offers many benefits. It allows us
to develop language policies and build more effective language programs. It lends insight into the
cues that lead people to categorize a speaker as native or non-native. It also reveals much about
the process of language acquisition generally. For example, studies show that mere exposure to a
language during the critical period is not enough to acquire native competence; the quality and
quantity of exposure matter as well. (Being bilingual from birth sometimes actually results in a
lower ability in the heritage language than being monolingual with a second language added later.)
BMP analyze in detail six ways in which heritage speakers diverge from native speakers:
phonology, lexical knowledge, morphology, syntax, case marking, and code-switching.
• Phonology is generally an area in which heritage speakers excel, but a “heritage accent”
may develop due to incomplete acquisition and attrition. This area has been understudied
thus far. The authors suggest several fruitful directions for research.
• Lexical knowledge among heritage speakers is often much weaker than among native
speakers; research on which basic lexical categories are most difficult for heritage speakers
may help clear up the controversy over whether a noun-verb distinction is a component of
Universal Grammar. (Studies so far seem to support the distinction even in these incomplete
grammars.)
• orphology, particularly inflectional morphology, is an area in which heritage speakers
often have the most trouble. Preliminary research suggests some generalizations: They
appear to struggle more with nominal morphology than verbal morphology (although there
are differences as well within verbal morphology); agreement, aspect, and mood are much
weaker areas in heritage speakers than is tense.
• Syntax is more likely to be completely acquired, but some areas are problematic. Notably,
complex syntax such as recursion and higher CP projections tends to be worse in heritage
speakers. In languages with pro-drop, heritage speakers often lose that feature, possibly due
to the difficulty of establishing syntactic dependencies. This also leads to issues with binding
that may be manifested in problems with anaphors. Heritage speakers may also have
difficulties with word order, passive constructions, and comprehension of relative clauses.
(Some of these may originate from a neglect of the relevant morphology.)
• Case marking is a common problem for heritage speakers. Inherent case may be highly
affected, revealing that semantics does not escape weakness. Additionally, fine-grained
semantics contrasts (such as definiteness, quantification, and genericity) are difficult for some
heritage speakers. (We must note that these generalizations could stem from the influence of
English, such that heritage speakers in countries with a different dominant language may
exhibit different patterns. Further research in other contexts is required.)
• Code-switching among heritage speakers occurs when two languages (or language varieties)
are present in the same discourse segment. Code-switching involves embedding one language
within the syntax of another; because it requires knowledge of many syntactic properties, code
switching can be used as a tool for gauging heritage speakers’ knowledge of their languages.
Although they acknowledge that the data is far from comprehensive, BMP highlight three
factors that help shape heritage grammars: incomplete acquisition, attrition, and transfer from
the dominant language. Incomplete acquisition (due to insufficient language input during
childhood) often produces adult heritage speakers who pattern like child learners with regards to
certain language features. Attrition is a much more controversial factor in heritage languages,
partly because it is unclear at what point acquisition stops, but also because there is so much
variation in heritage language. (For example, are there degrees of attrition in which features are
present to lesser degrees than in full language?) BMP suggest that attrition may be present when
a heritage speaker demonstrates difficulty with a language property that should have been fully
acquired by age 4-5 (e.g., relative clauses). Finally, BMP highlight language transfer, which
occurs when there is interference between the first language and the primary language. This is
well documented in second language research, but should also be considered in the analysis of
heritage languages. BMP discuss four ways in which heritage language research can benefit
the linguistic community. First, heritage speakers allow us to investigate the phenomena that
researchers have traditionally studied in children’s language use, but with test subjects that are
more sophisticated and manageable. We can also test more complex theoretical issues, such as
the relationship between agreement and case. (For example, should we classify the ergative as
an inherent or as a structural case? Preliminary research suggests that structural case is more
vulnerable than subject-verb agreement and inherent case, but agreement and structural case do
not pattern together and may be disassociated – heritage language research could provide
insights into the encoding of temporality and ergativity patterns.) Second, heritage language
studies that compare the grammatical competence of heritage speakers and L2 learners may
have significant benefits. Heritage speakers have a distinct advantage when it comes to
phonological competence, but no clear advantage in morphosyntactic competence. L2 learners
make fewer mistakes in written language than heritage speakers, and tend to perform better in
tasks that require metalingistic knowledge of the language. Besides its value to theoretical
linguists, heritage language research can benefit the field of education. Notably, because many
heritage speakers sign up for second language classes in their heritage language, educators need
to understand how to serve this large group of students. When special heritage tracks are
created, they would benefit from materials that are “purpose-built” to address the strengths and
weaknesses of heritage learners. Finally, there are social benefits. On one hand, these accrue to
the heritage language communities that wish to teach and maintain their languages and cultures.
On the other, heritage speakers can be a resource of communication skills that benefit the nation
in our
increasingly globalized world.
White Paper: Prolegomena to Heritage Linguistics
1, Silvina Montrul2 , Maria
Elabbas Benmamoun1
Polinsky
1
2
Abstract
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Harvard
University
Linguistic theory and experimental studies of language development rest heavily on the notion
of the adult, perhaps linguistically stable, native speaker. Native speaker competence and
performance are typically the result of normal first language acquisition in a predominantly
monolingual environment, with optimal and continuous exposure to the language. The question
we pose in this article is what happens when access to input and opportunities to use that native
language are less than optimal during language development. We present and discuss the case of
heritage speakers, i.e., bilingual speakers of an ethnic or immigrant minority language whose
first language does not typically reach native-like attainment in adulthood. By examining the
linguistic knowledge of these individuals, we question long-held ideas about the stability of
language before the so-called critical period for language development, and the nature of the
linguistic system as it develops under reduced input conditions. We present an overview of
heritage speakers’ linguistic system and discuss several competing factors that shape this system
in adulthood. We also call attention to the tremendous potential this population offers for
linguistic research, the language teaching profession, and for society in general.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 2
Acknowledgments
This paper is the result of several years of thinking, research studies (some of which were
complete dead ends), and joint conversations the three authors have held together at various
conferences and, most importantly, at the Heritage Language Summer Institutes which inspired
this work. The Institutes, the first of which was held at UC Davis in 2007, were funded by the
National Heritage Language Resource Center at UCLA, and we are very grateful to the Center for
the generous support we have received. This paper is just a small token of our gratitude.
Elabbas Benmamoun’s work has also been supported by NSF grant BCS 0826672.
Silvina Montrul’s work has been supported by the University of Illinois Campus Research Board
and by NSF grant BCS-0917593, ARRA (to Silvina Montrul, Rakesh, Bhatt and Roxana Girju).
Maria Polinsky’s work has been supported by the Center for Research in Language at UCSD, the
Faculty of Arts and Sciences at Harvard University, and by the Max-Planck Institute for
Evolutionary Anthropology. We are also grateful to our postdoctoral fellows and all the graduate,
undergraduate research assistants who have worked on many of the research projects discussed in
this paper.
We have benefited enormously from discussing this work with many colleagues all over
the world and presenting it at different venues; it would be impossible for us to name everyone
here, but we are thankful for their help and insight.
Last but not least, we are thankful to the heritage speakers who participated in the observations
and studies reported here, time and again reluctantly, sometimes with cheer, more often with
puzzlement. This work would not have been possible without you, and we hope that it is a small
step towards giving you a louder voice.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 3
1 Introduction
heart of the
What do we know when we know a language? This question is at the
debate about natural language. The usual answer is that we know a system of sounds (or
gestures/signs) that are put together in a systematic fashion to make up meaningful
linguistic units which in turn can be, to a large extent, manipulated and combined to form
more complex linguistic units, such as phrases, sentences, and extended discourse. The
main bone of contention has been about the nature of the system at work and whether the
system at the core of our linguistic knowledge (i.e., what enables us to produce and
comprehend linguistic stimuli) is specific to language or is a fundamental part of our
general cognitive abilities. There is no question that within a speech community, the socalled normal native speakers (those with no linguistic deficits who have been exposed to
their native language from childhood) share a linguistic system that enables them to
communicate with each other, to process each other’s linguistic input, and to transmit the
system to the next generation. Moreover, when compared cross-linguistically, linguistic
systems display shared properties in the structure of their phoneme inventories, types of
prosodic units, phonological processes, morphology, word order, displacement of
constituents, use of set expressions, etc. Linguistic research since the 1960’s has centered
on how that knowledge, or “linguistic competence”, develops in native speakers, as well
as on the properties of the presumably stable adult system (Chomsky 1959, 1965).
While native speaker competence is the main object of study in theoretical
linguistics and developmental psycholinguistics, the precise characterizations of a native
speaker and his/her linguistic knowledge remain elusive to this day (Davies 2003,
Paikeday1985). Nonetheless, virtually everyone intuitively recognizes a native speaker
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 4
upon seeing or hearing one. To begin with, a prototypical (educated) native speaker has a
“native” pronunciation and a sizable and comprehensive vocabulary. He or she speaks in
grammatical sentences (except for the occasional slip of the tongue), does not omit or
misplace morphemes, recognizes ambiguity and/or multiple interpretations and pragmatic
implications of words and sentences, and is attuned to his or her sociolinguistic
environment (social class, social context, gender, register, etc.). Such a native speaker is
readily accepted by members of his/her speech community (which can be as wide as a
language when you are the only other speaker of German stranded in Sri Lanka, or as
narrow as the jargon of a particular high school). However wide or narrow the
boundaries, the use of language to indicate “otherness” or “sameness” is a powerful
social tool. This judgment would not be possible without an understanding of natural
language design.
How does grammatical knowledge come about? The general idea is that humans
are uniquely endowed with the ability for language. Researchers disagree on whether this
ability represents a special language faculty or whether it is part of a more general
cognitive pre-wiring that allows us to learn how to talk about things past, present, and
future. Researchers also disagree as to how this ability came about—was it the result of a
slow evolutionary process, or was it the result of an abrupt change, some kind of a
linguistic “big bang”? (See Fitch 2010 for an illuminating discussion.) But whatever
disagreements linguists may have about the source and the evolution of the capacity for
language, they agree that language is unique to humans and that it is spectacularly
displayed from birth in such a way that toddlers who cannot feed themselves are quite
capable of commenting on the food they want or do not want.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 5
Some components of linguistic systems are fairly robust and have structural
underpinnings that are likely to be universal. Again, linguists differ in accounting for
such universality. One school of thought, often associated with innateness, attributes this
commonality to Universal Grammar, a limited set of pre-wired rules for organizing
language that is cognitively available to every human at birth (Chomsky 1965, Pesetsky
1999, Pinker 1994, see also Cook & Newson 2007 for a helpful introduction). The other
school of thought relates structural commonalities observed across languages to general
principles of human communication or frequency of patterns in the input (Elman et al.
1996, Tomasello 2003, a.o.). Regardless of the explanatory mechanisms behind the
similarities of natural language design, the similarities themselves are widely accepted by
practicing linguists.
With regard to areas of variation, the idea within the innateness camp is that some
types of variation are due to general principles (parameters) whose values are fixed
through exposure to the relevant language. Thus, while environment and linguistic input
do play a role in shaping the overall system, they do not fully determine it. According to
the so-called poverty of stimulus problem (see fn. 16 below), there are many complex and
subtle aspects of language that are underdetermined by the input and cannot possibly be
learned on the basis of input frequency exclusively (see Crain & Thornton1998, Guasti
2002, O’Grady 1997 for relevant examples).
Regardless of the acquisition model assumed, one must ask how much and what
quality of exposure to a language is necessary in order to acquire that language
“natively”. There seems to be a consensus that native speakers are different from nonnative speakers with regard to their mastery of the linguistic system, with degrees of
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 6
fluency varying according to the age of initial exposure to the language. Speakers who
have been exposed to their language since birth and have used the language continuously
since that age seem to have a fully developed system for the production and processing of
the phonological, morphological, syntactic and discourse patterns of their languages. In
other words, native speakers attain, for lack of a better term, complete acquisition of their
native language system, which provides them with the generative capacity to use and
process their language in all its richness and complexity.
Adult non-native speakers, on the other hand, though they may display advanced
fluency in the second language, tend to exhibit persistent signs of non-target acquisition,
particularly in areas of phonology, inflectional morphology, and syntax-pragmatics.
Further, signs of non-target acquisition may manifest themselves differently in a
speaker’s competence vs. performance. For example, non-native speakers may master
wh-movement in English when asked to judge sentences in a grammaticality judgment
task (White & Genesee 1996), but in spontaneous oral and written production they may
still continue to display problems with subject-auxiliary inversion, such as failing to
consistently invert the subject and the auxiliary verb in the matrix clause, or displaying a
tendency to apply inversion in subordinate clauses with indirect questions, as in the
example below:
(1) Do you know when is my test going to be graded?
An interesting case study is discussed by Lardiere (2007). Patty, the subject of the
case study, is a Chinese speaker who has been living in an English-immersion
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 7
environment for almost half of her adult life (more than 20 years). Patty exhibited nativelike acquisition of English wh-movement constructions and relative clauses, yet produced
overt past tense morphology in obligatory contexts with only 34.6% accuracy, a clear
sign of fossilization (arrested development).
While many of the errors that second language learners make can be traced back
to influence from their native language (otherwise known as L1 transfer), other errors are
developmental and common to first and second language learners of different languages.
In addition, second language learners also display degrees of convergence on the target
grammar that appear to be related to age of first exposure to the second language and
degree of language use (Hyltenstam & Abrahamsson 2003), as well as to the relation
between the first and the second target language (Birdsong & Molis 2001). In general,
post-pubescent second language learners rarely attain complete mastery of the target
language, and this outcome sets them apart from native speakers who do attain complete
mastery of their language.
A word of caution is in order here. There have been arguments that even native
speakers may not attain full mastery of some constructions (Green & Morgan 2005;
Dabrowska 1997; 2010). It is also assumed within recent work on exemplar-based
approaches to language acquisition (Tomasello 2003, a.o.) that language acquisition is a
continuous process (i.e., there may not be a critical period, though we are not sure that
this is indeed the claim). The main point in this text is that regardless of whether the
terms “complete” vs. “incomplete” acquisition accurately capture the dichotomy between
the two types of speakers, the dichotomy exists.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 8
Because a critical difference between the two groups has to do with age of
acquisition and amount of exposure to the target language, age as a variable has been
taken to determine significantly the extent of ultimate attainment, which is typically
characterized as complete in a native speaker but as incomplete in a non-native, secondlanguage speaker. Our goal in this paper is to further question the long-held linguistic
assumption about the stability of the first language in adults. As we stressed earlier,
several approaches within theoretical linguistics, psycholinguistics, developmental
psycholinguistics, second language acquisition and bilingualism rest on the notion of
complete and stable native speaker competence, acquired under conditions of continuous
exposure and use of the language. Here, we investigate what happens when access to
input and opportunities to use the native language are less than optimal during language
development. In doing that, we also hope to show that linguistic theory has many reasons
to pay attention to the population we introduce here: heritage speakers.
2 Heritage Speakers There is a group of speakers whose linguistic capacity does not easily
fit into the
dichotomy between “complete” and “incomplete”, and who have not received the same
degree of attention in the theoretical linguistics literature until recently (Polinsky 1997,
2006, 2008a,b; Montrul 2002, 2004, 2008). In the context of the United States, heritage
speakers are early bilingual speakers of ethnic minority languages who have differing
degrees of command of their first or family language, ranging from mere receptive
competence in the first language to balanced competence in the two languages. A typical
profile of a heritage speaker is that of a child who was born outside the parents’ home
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 9
country or left the home country before the age of eight. At least someone in the family
speaks with the child in the heritage language, but the child is more likely to speak
English or is more comfortable in English; this level of comfort in English increases as
s/he goes through middle and high school, often at the expense of the home language
(Cho et al. 2004).
The terms “heritage language” and “heritage speaker” are fairly new, and they are
still poorly understood outside of the USA, where similar concepts are denoted by the
1phrases
“minority language/speaker”. Although the terms are new, the phenomenon has
probably been with us as long as language contact has existed and migrations have
happened; heritage language development is a common outcome of bilingualism, with
one of the languages becoming much weaker than the other.
As this paper discusses different variants of language, it is important to introduce
some distinctions we will use below. First language (L1) and second language (L2) are
distinguished by the temporal order of acquisition. In case of simultaneous bilinguals, we
can speak of two L1s, although this is not uncontroversial; the critical point is that over
the lifetime of a bilingual, one of the two languages typically wins over and the other
becomes somewhat weaker depending on experience and degree of language use. An old
quote from Einar Haugen comes to mind: “native competence in more than one
language… is an ideal, theoretical model: few, if any, actually achieve this” (Haugen
1987: 14). The second distinction we need is that between the primary and the secondary
1 The term ‘heritage speaker’ originated in
Canada (Cummins 2005). The phenomenon dates back at least as far as the semi-speakers of
Gaelic described by Dorian (1979), who show many of the features we attribute to heritage
speakers; earlier examples could probably be found as well.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 10
languages, which are differentiated by the prevalence of usage. Thus, if an individual
learns language A as his/her first language and speaks it predominantly throughout the
adult life, that language is both first and primary. If an individual dramatically reduces
the use of his/her first language A and switches to using language B, then A is
characterized as this person’s first/secondary language, and B becomes the
second/primary language.
Another important distinction concerns the socio-political status of the language.
The majority language is typically the language spoken by an ethno-linguistically
dominant group in a country or a region. It has a standard, prestigious, written variety
used in government and the media, and it is the language imparted at school. Minority
languages are the languages spoken by ethnolinguistic minority groups; they typically
have no official status, they have lower prestige, they may not enjoy wider use beyond
restricted contexts, and they are not typically taught in schools. Immigrant languages are
minority languages; the societally dominant language (e.g., English in the United States)
is the majority language.
The next distinction we will be using is that between full language, or a language
one has acquired completely (with target-like ultimate attainment), and an incompletely
acquired language, which presupposes that certain areas of competence are lacking, for
reasons that we will examine below. Within the full language, there is a further
distinction between the baseline language (the language that an individual is exposed to
as a child) and the standard or norm, if one exists, for the language.
Let us now tie up all these distinctions together. A heritage speaker can be a
sequential bilingual: someone who grew up hearing (and possibly speaking) their L1 but
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 11
who early on started using L2 as their primary language. A heritage speaker can also be a
simultaneous bilingual who is strongly dominant in the majority language, the main
language of the wider speech community.
The pathways to the weakening of one’s language may vary in infinite ways, but
what they all have in common is the transition from a first language that happens to be a
minority language to a secondary language that is a majority language. This type of
asymmetric bilingualism is not typically observed when the first language is a majority
language and the second language is a minority language.
The best known and most widely used definition of heritage speakers under a
narrow conception of the term is Valdés’s (2000): “individuals raised in homes where a
language other than English is spoken and who are to some degree bilingual in English
and the heritage language.” Although the original definition is English-centered, any
other majority language can be substituted for English, cf. Rothman (2009). The crucial
criterion is that the heritage language was first in the order of acquisition but was not
completely acquired because of the individual’s switch to another dominant language.
The other critical component of this definition has to do with identifying continua of
proficiencies, reflecting the tremendous variation in heritage language proficiency
observed by several researchers (see Polinsky & Kagan 2007).2
2 Many heritage speakers narrowly defined also
turn to their heritage language as a subject of re-learning. Carreira & Kagan (in press) show that
such learners’ top priorities include: learning about their cultural and linguistic roots, the ability
to communicate better with family and friends in the United States., professional reasons, and
purely pragmatic goals, such as fulfilling their college language requirement.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 12
Like regular adult native speakers, heritage speakers are exposed to the heritage
language early in childhood (within the critical/sensitive period) and that exposure,
particularly in the home and among extended family, may be sustained for a number of
years depending on the family and the availability of a speech community. However,
heritage speakers are different from native speakers in important respects. First, in
addition to the heritage language, these speakers are also exposed to the societally
dominant language. While exposure to the dominant language may begin in the home, it
is most prominent outside the home, and particularly in school. Thus, while full native
speakers are exposed to their first language at home and then receive schooling in it,
heritage language speakers do not typically have access to education in their family
language. This asymmetry obviously raises the issue of the impact of schooling on later
language development, as well as whether the encroachment of the dominant language on
the heritage language can shape the nature of the adult grammar of the heritage language.
Furthermore, the exposure to the heritage language is usually different from the
exposure to the dominant language. The heritage language may be confined to some
specific contexts, typically the home and immediate community (if there is one), and
therefore the language content may revolve around themes that are more associated with
those contexts (e.g., family relations). By contrast, exposure to the dominant language in
heritage speakers tends to be more contextually diverse, particularly when children start
attending school and interacting in the dominant language to talk about different topics
with different interlocutors (teachers, friends, etc.) and through different media (written
texts, internet, popular culture, etc.).
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 13
However, it is an empirical question whether mastery of a particular linguistic
construction or form should depend on the context of exposure or literacy. For example, a
priori it should not matter in what context the learners/acquirers have been exposed to
agreement, binding, and word order patterns. Whether we utter a full sentence at home or
we write one at school, we have to use subject-verb agreement the same way. Presumably
the same syntactic constraints would govern the form of these structures regardless of the
context of exposure. Therefore, being exposed to the heritage language in the home on a
sustained basis for a number of years early in life should enable the heritage language
speaker to develop the basic ingredients for native-like competence. Yet, as mentioned
earlier, optimal exposure in the heritage language is usually reduced and even interrupted
at some point in early to late childhood, before the closure of the critical period at or
before puberty. Certain aspects of grammatical competence, most notably inflectional
morphology and complex syntax, are highly vulnerable to attrition and incomplete
acquisition in this population (Anderson 1999, Benmamoun et al. 2008, Bolonyai 2007,
Håkansson 1995, Montrul, Foote & Perpiñán 2008, O’Grady et al. 2001, Polinsky
2008a,b, Bar-Shalom & Zaretsky 2008).
Because heritage speakers vary tremendously in their range of proficiency in their
heritage language, some can be quite non-native-like, displaying incomplete knowledge
of several structural and pragmatic aspects of their language. In this respect, heritage
speakers resemble adult second language learners, who, as we discussed, are often
doomed to achieve incomplete mastery of the second language. It is therefore not
coincidental that within language teaching circles, heritage speakers are very often placed
in classes originally designed for second language learners. The understanding is that
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 14
they do not have full mastery of their heritage languages and can continue to learn it like
non-heritage adult second language learners. However, some universities in the United
States offer special tracks in their second language programs specifically geared for
heritage language learners in languages like Arabic, Chinese, Russian, Hindi or Spanish.
The assumption behind a special heritage language track is that this group of learners
usually comes into the classroom with some cultural or linguistic knowledge of the target
language, and therefore they can benefit from advanced and focused instruction that
3would
presumably accelerate their language (re-)learning process. Some language
teachers, program directors and heritage language practitioners hold the belief that early
language experience brings an added advantage to heritage language learners wishing to
regain command of their native language in a formal environment.
Heritage speakers straddle the boundaries between first and second language
acquisition, which makes them extremely valuable in testing fundamental questions about
the nature of linguistic knowledge, the correlation between age of acquisition and
3 There are two other reasons behind this policy.
The first reason is that teachers want to avoid having students with uneven levels of competence
in the target language, which is the case when heritage learners are put together with
non-heritage learners who may have no prior exposure to the target language and its culture. The
second reason is that there is an increase in the push by educational institutions and the
government (as evidenced by the increase in federal funds dedicated to language training, the
number of federally funded National Language Resource Centers, and the increase in the
number of teacher training programs for college language instructors) to improve the second
language proficiency levels of second language learners, particularly for the so-called critical
languages such as Arabic, Chinese, Hindi/Urdu, Korean, etc. The heritage population seems to
provide the best pool of students who have the potential to attain high proficiency in a relatively
short period of time, or so the thinking goes.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 15
linguistic competence, the nature and role of input that contribute or not to native-like
competence, and the interplay between the heritage language and the dominant language.
In other words, by studying this language group and comparing it to native speakers of
majority languages, L1-acquiring children, and adult second language learners, we can
begin to determine what it means to be a native speaker and what conditions are needed
to develop the competence that characterizes one.
3 Identifying heritage speakers: A response to variance As we saw in the previous section, a
heritage speaker may be someone born to an
immigrant family in the country of their dominant language (in our examples, the United
States) or someone who arrived in the United States as a young person, early enough to
learn English as a bilingual and not as a foreign language learner—the cut-off age for this
is assumed to be around age four or five (Schwartz 2004, Unsworth 2005, Cho et al.
2004). Thus, the identification of a heritage speaker is partially based on their
biographical information, although this may not always be a good predictor of their
proficiency (Polinsky 1997, 2006). This section will consider further indicators of
heritage speaker-hood.
Another common denominator shared by many (though not all) heritage speakers
has to do with a lack of literacy in the heritage language. Of course this is relevant only
for the subset of languages that have literacy and schooling traditions—a heritage speaker
of Seneca who was growing up on a reservation in the United States in the early twentieth
century had no recourse to literacy. But for heritage speakers of languages such as
Korean, Persian, Arabic, Russian, or Swahili, absent or insufficient schooling outside of
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 16
the metropole creates a much greater divide between them and their peers in Korea, Iran,
Egypt, Russia, or Kenya, respectively. Moreover, for some languages, for example the
colloquial dialects of Arabic, there is no significant written media, and the written
language exists exclusively in a formal variety that is significantly different from the
spoken varieties (Ferguson 1959, Benmamoun 2000). The effect of literacy has been the
subject of intense scrutiny in recent literature, and the bottom line is unsurprising:
exposure to literacy helps language development and, possibly, language retention (Cho
& Krashen 2000, Oller & Eilers 2002; Bialystok et al. 2005; Kondo-Brown 2009;
Delgado 2009, and references therein). Still, there are different ways of providing that
exposure, and not all of them may be equally effective. However, just as with
biographical data, data on literacy alone are not sufficient to identify heritage speakers.
One might expect that heritage speakers should be able to identify themselves as
such, and so it would seem uncontroversial to rely on heritage speakers’ self-assessment
of their linguistic ability. However, self-reporting requires a substantial degree of
linguistic and metalinguistic awareness, which may be greater in speakers who had
advanced education in English. Moreover, a large number of people just mark themselves
in the middle when they are not certain how to rate.
Heritage speakers capable of metalinguistic judgments often offer an intriguing
case of what could be called “misjudgment”: those speakers who are quite fluent in the
heritage language often assess their own knowledge as low, whereas those who are in fact
low-level think they are quite good. Below we present two charts showing a strong
inverse correlation between a subject’s self-assessment and their speech rate, which, as
we will show, is a good linguistic indicator of proficiency. Russian and Korean subjects
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 17
were asked to rate their proficiency in their heritage language on a scale from 1 to 5, and
were then shown a short video clip that they had to talk about; based on the productions
about this video clip, we calculated their speech rates. Of course, it is difficult to draw
generalizations from a couple dozen heritage speakers, all of them college students.
Moreover, research on Spanish and Hindi heritage speakers shows the opposite
relationship: the heritage speakers’ self-assessment correlates very well with an
independent measure of proficiency developed for these purposes (per cloze tests, which
we will discuss below; Montrul et al. 2010). Nevertheless, reverse metalinguistic
awareness may occur: the more a heritage speaker contemplates his/her L1, the more s/he
is aware of his/her strengths and deficiencies, while those speakers who are quite happy
with themselves may have not given enough thought to the issue of language proficiency.
If this is on the right track, it illuminates a new dimension to the overall notion of
metalinguistic awareness: it is generally assumed that bilinguals have a high degree of
such awareness (Galambos & Hakuta 1988; Campbell & Sais 1995, a.o.)4 but it may well
be that the threshold of minimal exposure relevant for heritage speakers is not sufficient
for the development of linguistic introspection. The fact that different subgroups of
heritage speakers may differ with regard to their metalinguistic awareness is consistent
4 Many studies of metalinguistic awareness in
bilinguals are based on observations of or experimental work with children. We would like to
underscore that adult heritage speakers are linguistically different from children and such
differences are not always in adults’ favor. The attrition or reanalysis of the language system
that we have observed in adult heritage speakers (see section 4 below)) may have a negative
effect on their metalinguistic awareness as well.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 18
with the overall variance observed within this group of speakers and calls for further
investigation.
Figure
1. Correlations between self-assessment (1-5 scale) and speech rate
(word/min), Korean (20 subjects), r = -.695
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 19
Figure
2. Correlations between self-assessment (1-5 scale) and speech rate
(word/min), Russian (31 subjects), r = -.82
Assessment by native speakers is an area where heritage speakers are perhaps
more easily identified. Anecdotally, most native speakers can identify that someone does
not speak exactly like another native speaker in a very short amount of time, somewhere
under a minute of hearing them speak. Given that heritage speakers are usually
reasonably good at the language’s phonology and have strong advantages in that domain
(see below), it is critical to understand what cues the native speakers react to. So far,
there have been few instrumental studies on the phonetics of heritage languages (we will
review those below) and it is to be hoped that the development of such research will bring
us closer to understanding what exactly constitutes a ‘heritage accent’.
Recent research on heritage languages has focused on identifying purely linguistic
diagnostics that would allow us to measure a heritage speaker’s knowledge of their home
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 20
language, as well as their proximity to the baseline. Although the progress made in this
area has been quite modest, several promising diagnostics have emerged.
Speakers of relatively high proficiency can be assessed by ways of wellestablished cloze tests (Taylor 1953). Different researchers have developed such tests for
Spanish, Russian, Italian, German, Hindi, Romanian, and Polish. These tests can be
relatively easily constructed, drawing upon similar tests used for the assessment of
second language learners. It is often important to compare second language learners and
heritage speakers, which again makes cloze tests a useful instrument. Cloze tests
recommend themselves by many good properties: are easy to administer and rate, they
provide a good platform for comparing groups of speakers and subject pools in different
studies, and they seem to provide a good independent measure of proficiency. Some
disadvantages have to be noted, though. First, heritage speakers of lower proficiency
often have trouble with cloze tests, particularly those that include a reading/writing
component (auditory cloze tests may be better in this situation). Second, it is not always
clear what component of language competence cloze tests actually target; as a result, one
would imaging using a battery of cloze tests, and setting a lower or upper limit on the
number of tests becomes a contentious issue.
Another prominent diagnostic seems to be speech rate, which we have already
brought up in conjunction with self-assessment (see Figures 1, 2 above). The relevance of
speech rate on proficiency is attested by a study of gender restructuring in heritage
Russian (Polinsky 2008b), which showed that heritage speakers fell into two distinct
groups: those who maintained the baseline three-gender system of noun classification
(with various adjustments), and those who radically reanalyzed the baseline system as a
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 21
two-gender system. Reanalysis of the baseline three-gender system as a two-gender
system was strongly correlated with a lower speech rate (Figure 3). The motivation
behind this correlation is rather straightforward. Lower proficiency speakers have more
difficulty in accessing lexical items, which slows down their speech. As we will show
below, knowledge of lexical items and grammatical knowledge are also correlated. Not
surprisingly, problems in lexical access are accompanied by difficulties in constructing
phrases and clauses; spontaneous speech is punctuated by pauses, repetitions, and false
starts. All of this has an immediate effect on the speech rate.
Figure
3. Rate of speech (words/min) in baseline controls, speakers who
maintained the three-gender system, and speakers who switched to a two-gender system for
Russian nouns (Polinsky 2008b)
Although correlations such the one as shown in Figure 3 need to be tested with
more grammatical phenomena and different heritage languages, they offer the promise of
relatively simple diagnostics of language proficiency which can be used both in the lab
and in the classroom.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 22
Speech rate may be a promising method of classification, but it may be difficult to
determine in the lowest-proficiency heritage speakers, who sometimes are reluctant to
produce any language whatsoever. Another diagnostic of proficiency in heritage
languages that may be even more powerful is lexical proficiency. Polinsky (1997, 2000,
2006) observed a strong correlation between a speaker’s knowledge of lexical items,
measured in terms of a basic word list (about two hundred items), and the speaker’s
control of grammatical phenomena such as agreement, case marking, aspectual and
temporal marking, pro-drop, co-reference, and embedding, with grammatical knowledge
measured by deviations from the baseline in spontaneous speech. This correlation was
replicated in later studies with forced-choice judgments (Polinsky 2005). The correlation
is supported by results from several heritage languages, including Russian, Polish,
Armenian, Korean, and Lithuanian (see also Godson 2003).
The correlation between level of lexical and grammatical knowledge is not
exclusive to heritage language competence; it has also been proposed as a measure of
early child language competence (Bates et al. 1994; Thal et al. 1996, 1997; Fenson et al.
2000). If structural attrition and lexical proficiency are correlated, lexical proficiency
scores, which are relatively easy to obtain, can serve as a basis for the characterization
and ranking of incomplete learners on a continuum (rather than in a discrete group).
While the studies mentioned here used an extensive 200-word list, the emerging trend has
been to also use standardized tests meant for the assessment of language development,
such as the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT), widely employed in the speech and
hearing sciences. A more recent and welcome development is the HALA (Hawaii
Assessment of Language) test developed by William O’Grady and his collaborators. The
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 23
HALA is based on picture naming, thus avoiding the interference of the dominant
language; it also taps into lexical and grammatical proficiency (O’Grady 2009).
Another good diagnostic of heritage language proficiency relates to the manner
and length of exposure to the baseline. These two characteristics seem interrelated in
ways which may not be fully understood yet. With respect to the manner of exposure, it is
natural to expect that speakers who grew up surrounded by the baseline language in the
metropole, broadly construed, should differ somehow from those who grew up in an
immigrant community in the U.S. or any other country where a different language is
dominant. The exposure to the language in the metropole is inevitably greater than that in
immigrant communities where bilingualism is prevalent, so one would expect a heritage
speaker who spent her first five years of life in Korea to have an advantage over an
American-born Korean heritage speaker. Au & Oh (2005) found that speaking the
majority language before age five seems to put linguistic minority children at a small, but
measurable, risk for poorer heritage language skills during adolescence. Likewise, the
length of exposure to the baseline should also matter, because the longer the exposure to
the baseline, the greater the baseline input to the heritage speaker, cf. Montrul (2002).
Preliminary results show that the effects of context/modality and length of
exposure are not easily separable. The only group that seems to have a distinct language
advantage is heritage speakers who grew up in the metropole and had exposure to the
baseline language at least until the end of the critical period. All other groups are more or
less indistinguishable; crucially, exposure to schooling in the heritage language after
immigration does not seem to have any serious effect on their proficiency, as shown by
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 24
preliminary studies of Korean Sunday schools (Kwon & Polinsky 2005; Lee 2008) and
Chinese Sunday schools (Wang 1996, Xiao 2006, Weger-Guntharp 2006).
Like any other normally developing child, heritage language speakers were
exposed to their heritage language early in life and received input during the critical
period. If the inability to reach native-like attainment by second language learners is
related to late onset of acquisition (usually at or after puberty), clearly heritage speakers
do not have that problem because they presumably received exposure to their native
language since birth. However, as we have seen so far, early language experience is a
necessary but not a sufficient condition for complete language acquisition in heritage
speakers. Amount and quality of exposure during the critical period matter as well.
As in second language acquisition, age effects are also relevant in heritage
language acquisition. While the younger the age at which second language acquisition
begins the more native-like the outcome is likely to be in adults, Montrul (2008) argues
that a different situation obtains in heritage language acquisition: the younger the
exposure to the majority language and reduction of exposure to the minority language the
greater the degree of partial attainment of the minority language by heritage speakers.
This tendency is reflected in the results of Montrul’s (2002) study on knowledge of tense
and aspect in Spanish. Those heritage speakers who were exposed to both Spanish and
English since birth were less accurate with the interpretations of preterite and imperfect
tenses in Spanish than those heritage speakers who were exposed to Spanish exclusively
during the pre-school period and began learning English later, in elementary school. In
turn, these two groups were also less target-like than the group of heritage speakers who
moved to the United States between the ages of 8-12. And studies of internationally
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 25
adopted children, another special subgroup of heritage speakers, show that the earlier the
age of adoption and complete interruption of input in the native language, the more
abrupt and severe the loss of the first language (Montrul 2008).
Finally, we would like to turn to grammaticality judgments. Their role in
indentifying a particular speaker as “heritage” and in placing such a speaker on a
continuum of proficiency that would faithfully reflect the significant variability in
heritage populations has been disputed. On the one hand, grammaticality judgments are a
well-established tool of linguistic research: linguists have long relied on the comparison
of acceptability between two or more minimally different language forms (Chomsky
1965; Schutze 1996; Marantz 2005; Sprouse & Almeida 2010). Theoretical linguists
make the choice themselves, via introspection, or ask a small group of like-minded
linguists. Experimentally inclined researchers increase the size of their subject pool, do
not ask themselves, and try to exclude contentious linguists. A possible shortcoming of
the experimental approach is that most subjects are typically undergraduates who have
been trained to take various tests and make choices, and thus their responses might be
colored by this training. Critics of the introspective, or small-scale approach counter that
the population linguists test is not representative of the general users of language
(Dabrowska 2010), the empirical evidence is shaky, and the method itself is fraught with
multiple errors (see Sprouse & Almeida 2010 for an overview).
Whether one is wildly opposed to grammaticality judgments or favors their use
whole-heartedly, it is thought that they work well with linguists, graduate students or
trained undergraduates. The critical word here is ‘trained’: all these groups have been
trained to think about the way they speak. Often, heritage speakers are also drawn from
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 26
student ranks, but their metalinguistic awareness of the home language or their
confidence in their knowledge do not match their English skills. This creates a significant
complication in trying to elicit grammaticality judgments from these speakers. When
asked to give such judgments, heritage speakers often refuse to do so, or choose both
forms, or simply admit that they do not know what to choose (cf. Polinsky 2006;
Polinsky & Kagan 2007). This does not mean that this method is completely useless with
heritage populations—some researchers have been able to use it to draw interesting
insights (Montrul et al. 2010, Brehmer & Czachor 2010, a.o.). However, if
grammaticality judgments are used, it is helpful to keep in mind the limitations and
inhibitions that heritage speakers may have: lack of exposure, lack of language use, and
linguistic insecurity. All these factors affect their performance on the judgment task and
often make them into extremely reluctant language consultants: they are asked to judge a
language they do not have full ownership of.
4 Heritage grammatical system at a glance 4.1. Phonology/Phonetics Although it is
traditionally assumed that heritage speakers sound completely
native-like, this assumption does not withstand scrutiny. Several studies of late and early
bilinguals show that native speakers are sometimes judged as non-native after a
prolonged emigration (Major 1992; Schmid 2002; de Leeuw et al. 2010; Hopp & Schmid
submitted). Thus, non-native accent—a heritage accent?—may develop due to low use of
a heritage language and prolonged lack of exposure. This in turn means that native-like
performance is not guaranteed by the acquisition of language from birth: just as in other
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 27
areas of language structure, the degree of use and the amount of exposure seem to play a
role.
Au et al. (2002), Oh et al. (2003), and Knigthly et al. (2003) showed that very low
proficiency Spanish and Korean heritage speakers have pronounced non-native accents in
general, suggesting that pronunciation is affected in heritage speakers to some extent. Oh
et al. (2003) showed that this was true for phoneme production (VOTs) only for speakers
who had limited productive ability in Korean, but not for perception. In Spanish, there
was no difference in VOTs of voiceless stops in low proficiency heritage speakers as
compared to native speakers (Au et al. 2002).
Studies by Khattab (2002) and Saadah (2010) show that children of Lebanese and
Palestinian descents are able to acquire the VOT patterns of their heritage Arabic
varieties, though Lebanese heritage speakers’ VOT patterns displayed properties that
could be attributed to the dominant Yorkshire variety of English that they speak.
In a study documenting phonetic changes in vowel production in Western
Armenian heritage speakers in the United States, Godson (2004) found that the heritage
speakers retained the 5-vowel system of Western Armenian in production. However, the
two front vowels /i/ and /e/ and the central vowel /a/ were not the same as for Western
Armenian native speakers. The values for these vowels were closer to English values than
those of /o/ and /u/. Therefore, while heritage speakers retain their native phonology, the
phonetic values of both vowels and consonants are affected, thus contributing to a
‘heritage’ accent. Another striking feature of heritage speakers examined by Godson has
to do with the leveling of dialectal differences as compared to the baseline. Western
Armenian is a language of the Armenian diaspora, associated with a variety of dialects
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 28
dispersed in the Middle East. As compared to the baseline, heritage speakers of Western
Armenian all sound much more alike, an informal observation that Godson was able to
support by quantitative measures. To date, the pronunciation of heritage speakers remains
an understudied area, but the few available studies suggest that there are measurable
systematic differences between fluent native speakers and heritage speakers worth
investigating in future research, especially to address theoretical debates on the role of
early input for phonological abilities in bilinguals.
4.2. Lexical categories
can also be
Since vocabulary knowledge depends of frequency and exposure, it
highly affected in heritage language grammars. As mentioned above, Polinsky (1997,
2006) reports a correlation between lexical knowledge and extent of morphosyntactic
attrition in heritage speakers of Russian. Hulsen (2000) looked at lexical access in
production and comprehension in Dutch heritage speakers in Australia. In particular, she
investigated lexical retrieval of nouns in a picture-naming and a picture-matching task.
Second generation Dutch speakers differed significantly in both speed and accuracy of
lexical retrieval from both first generation speakers and a control group of Dutch
speakers in the Netherlands. Third generation speakers managed to perform the picturematching task (comprehension) but were unable to perform the picture-naming task
(production). Cognate status of words and frequency played a role in the speed of lexical
retrieval and access.
Every so often some linguists find examples of languages that seem to have no
noun-verb distinction (see Broschart 1997, Gil 2000 for some recent examples) and other
linguists refute their conjectures, essentially by saying that in certain languages the noun-
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 29
verb distinction may be less evident (cf. Lander & Testelets 2006; Arkadiev et al. 2009).
The noun-verb distinction seems to be one of the tenets of Universal Grammar, assumed
to be available in order to posit a difference between nouns and verbs whenever presented
with linguistic data.5 Examining heritage speakers’ knowledge of these basic lexical
categories is important because it can provide additional evidence in support of the nounverb distinction, or help refute the distinction as less fundamental than many researchers
think.
Simple lexical decision studies involving heritage speakers seem to give new
evidence to the idea that the noun-verb distinction is not going away any time soon.
Polinsky (2005) and Lee et al. (in preparation) have shown that heritage speakers of
Russian and Korean show a higher accuracy and response rate with verbs than with
nouns. With a Russian auditory task, Polinsky (2005) did not find a lexical class effect in
the baseline controls—possibly because they were at ceiling in the behavioral study. In a
similar task in Korean, there was a verb facilitation effect for native speakers as well as
heritage speakers.
However, Montrul (2009a) observed the opposite for Spanish: heritage speakers
were faster on nouns and slower and less accurate on verbs. This difference may be
symptomatic of a larger difference across heritage languages, but it may also have some
5 Although the generalization is clear, the
ultimate motivation for the noun-verb distinction remains elusive. The fundamental noun-verb
difference may be due to the distinct cognitive processes of referring and labeling (nouns) and of
predicating, i.e., attributing properties to things, which is what verbs are for (Chomsky 1980,
Williams 1980; Stowell 1995; Bowers 1993; Baker 2003; Hornstein 2009).
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 30
low-level explanations: Montrul’s subjects had generally higher proficiency than the
subjects in the Russian or Korean studies.6
The three studies cited here show that the basic noun-verb distinction seems to
hold even in incomplete grammars. More importantly, we would like to underscore that
this is just one of many instances where an investigation of heritage grammars may yield
results that are of value to the field of linguistics and that can be helpful in theory
construction. In the next section, we will review other findings that are theoretically
significant and some that raise new challenges to the existing theories of grammar.
4.3. Morphology
The sub-module of language that is generally most affected in heritage speakers is
inflectional morphology in languages that exhibit rich morphological systems and regular
and irregular paradigms; as Bar-Shalom & Zaretsky point out, the “[l]oss of languagespecific morphosyntactic structures… is a hallmark of a ‘heritage language’” (2008: 281).
In languages, such as Arabic, with root and pattern morphology (McCarthy 1979) other
issues arise that concern the knowledge of the notion of root and the mapping
mechanisms for linking the root and the vocal melody to the template. In research in
progress by Benmamoun, Albirini, Saadah and Montrul on these specific aspects of
morphology, preliminary results uncovered problems with the notion of root (particularly
6 If proficiency is indeed the deciding factor
here this serves to underscore the methodological point that experimental work on heritage
speakers should pay close attention to the tremendous variance among heritage populations;
studies of heritage speakers should therefore identify their level based on sociolinguistic,
demographic, and linguistic factors (see section 3 for more discussion).
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 31
roots that contain glides and geminate consonants). In addition, heritage speakers, as
opposed to the native controls and second language learners, deploy the feminine suffixal
plural as the default pattern, a finding that Ravid & Farah (1999) reported for child
language.
Numerous studies have documented the fragility of nominal and verbal
inflectional morphology in several heritage languages. In the nominal domain, heritage
speakers exhibit errors with gender agreement in Russian, Spanish and Swedish
(Håkansson 1995, Montrul, Foote & Perpiñán 2008a, Polinsky 2008a,c, Gupol 2009,
Laleko 2010), definiteness agreement in Swedish and Hungarian (Håkansson 1995,
Bolonyai 2007), and case marking in Russian, Korean, and Polish (Polinsky 1997, 2006,
2008b, Song et al. 1997, Laskowski 2009). Similar patterns of erosion are attested in the
verbal domain, including agreement in Russian (Polinsky 1997, 2006), lexical aspect in
Russian (Pereltsvaig 2005; Polinsky 1997, 2006, 2008a,c, Gupol 2009, Laleko 2010),
grammatical aspect in Spanish and Hungarian (Montrul 2002, Fenyvesi 2000, de Groot
2005), mood in Spanish, Russian, and Hungarian (Lynch 1999, Montrul 2007, SilvaCorvalán 1994, Polinsky 1997, 2006, Fenyvesi 2000), and inflected infinitives in
7Brazilian
Portuguese (Rothman 2007).
Morphological deficits in heritage languages are asymmetric; they are more
pronounced and pervasive in nominal morphology as compared to verbal morphology
(see Bolonyai 2007 for the same observation), and within verbal morphology, they seem
to target a subset of categories. To illustrate the nominal-verbal morphological
7 While most of these studies have reported error rates varying from 5% to 70% in oral
production, a growing number of studies suggest similar problems with the comprehension of
morphology.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 32
asymmetries, Hindi heritage speakers make case-marking errors in the range of 23-27%,
while their verbal agreement errors are well under 7% (Montrul et al. 2010). Lowproficiency heritage speakers of Russian have an error rate of about 40% in their nominal
morphology, and fewer than 20% in their verb agreement morphology. Observations on
production in heritage Hungarian (Fenyvesi 2000, de Groot 2005), including the
Hungarian of English-dominant bilingual children (Bolonyai 2007) also point to
significant attrition of nominal morphology (omission of case affixes and the possessive
suffix; overextension of definite forms), and well-preserved verbal morphology,
including agreement marking on the verbs. Within verbal agreement, the forms that are
slightly affected are those with object agreement (Bolonyai 2007; Fenyvesi 2000).
Nevertheless, aspectual and preverbal marking seem to be problematic in heritage
Hungarian. In Korean, a survey of 1,000 written sentences produced by heritage learners
in college classes showed an error rate of about 25% in the use of the nominative and
accusative markers on nouns (omission, replacement by the topic marker, use of the
wrong form) and only about 5% in verbal morphology, mainly in the use of the
interrogative, imperative, and medial forms (Polinsky et al., in prep.). Albinri et al. (in
press) report that Egyptian and Palestinian speakers display native-like command of
subject-verb agreement (respectively at 93% and 97% accuracy rates); heritage Hindi
shows similarly high maintenance of agreement (Montrul et al. 2010).
Within the verbal morphological complex, there seems to be a further asymmetry
among categorical features. Thus, tense marking is unaffected and there are no reports of
tense errors in heritage grammars; some researchers specifically state that tense is robust
in these grammars (Fenyvesi 2000). In addition to agreement marking, which is generally
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 33
affected, heritage speakers make errors in aspectual morphology (Montrul 2002, in press,
Polinsky 2006, 2008c, de Groot 2005), as well as the morphology associated with higher
functional layers in the syntax, namely, mood, polarity, and possibly negation. However,
one significant finding by Albirini et al. (in press) concerns the tendency of heritage
speakers to use a participial form instead of an inflected verbal form. Participial forms do
inflect for gender and number in Arabic, so it is not clear that knowledge of the
inflectional system is the reason for their use, but rather it could be linked to problems
isolating the appropriate inflectional paradigm to go with the tense of the sentence.
The data amassed so far are in need of further verification, both for the heritage
languages that have been studied and those that are still awaiting research. The emerging
asymmetries are intriguing and call for an explanation. A possible explanation for the
asymmetry between the nominal and verbal morphology may have to do with differences
in the nature of these two morphologies. Some researchers have argued that nominal
morphology is post- or extra-syntactic, whereas verbal morphology is directly reflexive
of syntactic structure (cf. Bobaljik & Branigan 2006, Bobaljik 2008). If so, it is possible
that heritage speakers retain the syntactic ability to form predication relations and
recursive structures (the essential properties of Narrow Syntax) but have a reduced
capacity for post-syntactic operations, which would account for the asymmetries.
4.4. Syntax
Syntactic knowledge appears to be more resilient to incomplete acquisition under
reduced input conditions than inflectional morphology. There is a tendency of heritage
language grammars to keep the basic, perhaps universal, core structure of language, while
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 34
aspects of syntax that involve recursion and higher projections of the CP layer (i.e.,
complex syntax) appear to be much less productive and developed in these speakers. In
the word order domain, Håkansson (1995) shows that Swedish heritage speakers have
native-speaker control of the V2 phenomenon, including native command of structural
(or stylistic) variability with verb placement. Montrul (2005) reports that even low
proficiency Spanish heritage speakers know the syntactic constraints on unaccusativity in
their language. Further, the overt pronominal system seems to be quite resilient to
incomplete acquisition (Polinsky 1997). Montrul, Foote & Perpiñán (2008b) investigated
knowledge of wh-movement, subject verb inversion and that-trace effects (illustrated in
(2)) in Spanish heritage speakers. They found that although there were significant
differences between the native and heritage speakers with object and subject extractions,
the heritage speakers were quite accurate with subject-verb inversion and extraction over
a complementizer (that-trace effect), even though Spanish and English differ in this
regard. It is possible to extract a subject from an embedded clause in Spanish when there
is a trace in subject position (2a), while English does not allow a wh-trace in subject
position to follow an overt complementizer (2b):
(2)
a. ¿Quién dijo Ana que rompió la taza?
b. *Who did Ana say that broke the cup?
Turning to less robust areas of heritage grammars, null pronominals seem to be
significantly affected: heritage languages whose baseline is pro-drop are reported to lose
the pro-drop feature or to use it in a more limited manner—for example, Hungarian (de
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 35
Grot 2005), Hindi (Mahajan 2009), Korean (Choi 2003), Tamil and Kabardian (Polinsky
1997), Spanish (Silva-Corvalán 1994, Montrul 2004), Polish (Dubisz 1997, 2001;
Brehmer & Czachor 2010), Arabic (Albirnini et al. in press).8 Sorace finds a more
restricted use of null pronominals in émigré languages (Sorace 2004, Sorace & Serratrice
2009) and attributes this to the attrition of phenomena that lie at the syntax-discourse
interface. If this explanation is on the right track, it behooves us to understand what is so
unique about null pronominals that they seem to be the only affected area of an interface.
One would naturally want to ask what types of interface phenomena are prone to change
under contact. Additionally, is only the conceptual/intentional interface affected by
attrition? What about the sensory/motor interface?
A simpler, alternative explanation for the loss of pro-drop may stem from the
observation, which we will expand on below, that heritage speakers have a general
difficulty in establishing syntactic dependencies, especially when the dependency is at a
distance. A null pronominal is always an element that has to be licensed and identified
(Rizzi 1986), thus the licensing and co-indexation with a DP at a distance causes a
significant problem in heritage grammars.
Difficulties in creating and maintaining a dependency also arise with respect to
binding, which accounts for the difficulties in the interpretations of anaphors in heritage
speakers. The difficulty may vary across heritage languages or across proficiency levels,
or both; Kim et al. (2009, 2010) show that heritage speakers still retain robust control of
the syntactic properties that license local and long distance anaphors in their language.
8 Spanish heritage speakers have been reported
to control the syntactic properties that license null subjects in these languages (Silva-Corvalán
1994, Montrul 2004).
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 36
Polinsky (2006) finds that heritage speakers of Russian often produce the correct
anaphors but have significant problems interpreting binding domains. In terms of crosslinguistic differences, note that the Korean anaphor caki has distinct logophoric
properties, which may help its interpretation, whereas the Russian anaphor sebja is
clause-bound—these parametric differences may explain the difference in the results. It is
hard to draw conclusions on the basis of just two heritage languages; thus, binding, just
as many other areas of grammar, is in need of more empirical data.
As we suggested above, heritage speakers seem to have a particular difficulty in
establishing dependencies between items, especially if these dependencies are at a
distance. As a result, long-distance dependencies such as extraction (relativization, whquestions) and argument displacement (passives) are vulnerable domains in heritage
language grammars. We will first present some research on these areas and then briefly
discuss why these dependencies may be difficult for heritage speakers.
Very little is known about the ability of heritage speakers to deal with Amovement and A-bar phenomena. With respect to A-movement, Polinsky (2009)
compared English-dominant heritage speakers of Russian to age-matched monolingual
Russian controls in a sentence-picture matching task. Subjects matched pictures to
active/passive constructions, with verb-initial and verb-medial orders in Russian:
(3)
a. morjak spas pirat-a
(Active SVO)
sailor.NOM saved pirate-ACC
b. spas pirat-a morjak
(Active VOS)
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 37
c. spas morjak pirat-a
(Active VSO)
‘The sailor saved the pirate.’
(4) a. pirat
spas-en
morjak-om
(Passive SVO)
pirate.NOM saved-PASS sailor-INSTR
b. spasen morjak-om pirat
c. spasen pirat morjak-om
(Passive VOS)
(Passive VSO)
‘The pirate is saved by the sailor.’
The results show that heritage speakers have serious problems when the word order is
different from SVO, regardless of voice, and also have problems with the passive. At first
glance, these results seem parallel to the results obtained for child language and aphasics.
There is an extensive literature documenting the challenges of passives in children’s
comprehension in English (Clark 2003; O’Grady 1997) and in other languages (Demuth
& Kline 2006; Pierce, 1992; Sano et al. 2001; Slobin 1997). Language acquisition
accounts that follow the structural theories of adult language processing, i.e., that
passives and scrambled sentences are derived via movement, explain children’s
difficulties with these constructions as resulting from their inability to form and maintain
syntactic chains (A-chain maturation hypothesis by Borer & Wexler, 1987, 1992) or to
transmit theta-roles (Fox & Grodzinsky 1998). Constraint-satisfaction accounts ascribe
children’s difficulties with passives to the relative rarity of this construction in childdirected speech, the lack of appropriate discourse context (Otsu 1992), and, recently, to a
failure to pay attention to grammatical case markers (Murasagi & Kawamura 2004).
Aphasia patients are another population where A-dependencies are difficult (Drai &
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 38
Grodzinsky 2006). The explanation for aphasic deficits has been linked to the lack of
certain functional projections (Friedmann 2006).
Word order alternations represent another area where heritage speakers display
some vulnerability. Albirini et al. (in press) report data from Egyptian heritage speakers
where the SVO order is predominant, though the language allows for VSO as an
alternative option. The prevalence of the SVO order could be due to transfer from
English, but it could also be due to the complex syntax of the VSO order. The latter order
has been argued to involve movement of the verb past the subject to a higher position
within the sentence (Fassi Fehri 1993, Shlonsky 1997, Benmamoun 2000, Aoun et al.
2010), in which case difficulty with the VSO order may reflect problems with
establishing dependencies between the various verbal positions along the path of verb
movement.
However, when tested in their dominant language, heritage speakers do not show
any problems with a similar task, which means that they certainly have A-chains in their
dominant language. This in turn casts doubt on the purely syntactic explanation for their
problems with passive constructions or raising. If heritage speakers have access to the
relevant functional projection, then their dismal performance on passives may stem from
the same processing strategies that have been identified in child language speakers:
failure to pay attention to the relevant inflectional morphology (cf. Murasugi &
Kawamura 2004 for L1) and subsequent shallow processing which relies on some kind of
a canonical sentence strategy (‘Interpret the first NP as agent and the second NP as
patient’), cf. Hayashibe 1975; O’Grady 1997 for first language acquisition.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 39
Heritage speakers also show difficulties in the comprehension of relative clauses
(see O’Grady & Lee 2007 for Korean, Polinsky 2010 for Russian), and especially object
relatives. Again, one could account for that deficit without relying on the hard to maintain
notion that heritage speakers lack relevant syntactic operations; instead, the problems
with relative clauses may follow from the neglect of morphology.
Case marking seems a particularly vulnerable area in heritage languages;
however, it is unclear whether the problem lies with the syntactic mechanism of case
licensing or with morphological, arguably post-syntactic (Bobaljik 2008) case marking;
we will return to this issue in the next section. A similar dilemma, syntax or morphology,
applies to agreement, which we will also consider below.
4.5. Semantics While most of the existing work on heritage language grammars has centered
on
the areas of morphology and syntax, there is emerging an indication that certain aspects
of semantics are also highly affected in these grammars. One such area is semanticallybased or inherent case. Polinsky (1997, 2006) mentions the Russian genitive of negation,
which is learned late in first language acquisition and is quite infrequent in general.
However, similar erosion has been documented in Spanish (Montrul 2004, Montrul &
Bowles 2009, 2010). Spanish does not have the genitive of negation, but it has
differential object marking (DOM) with animate specific direct objects and dative
subjects with psychological predicates, both instances of inherent case according to
recent analyses. Spanish heritage speakers tend to omit these case markers, which happen
to be the preposition a in both cases. Interestingly, the preposition a, which is also the
dative marker in prototypical dative constructions, is not omitted with indirect objects.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 40
This suggests that inherent case marking may be more affected than structural case
marking.
Another problematic area of emerging interest is the semantics of articles.
Montrul & Ionin (2010, in press) have found that Spanish heritage speakers have a strong
tendency to use bare nouns with generic reference in subject position (which are
ungrammatical in Spanish but grammatical in English), and tend to interpret definite
articles in Spanish as specific in generic contexts. Interestingly, these studies show no
problems with the semantics of articles in inalienable possession constructions (Pedro
levantó la mano ‘Peter raised his hand’), an intriguing result.
Fine-grained semantic contrasts such as definiteness, quantification and genericity
are known to be difficult to first language learners, so it is not too much of a stretch to
expect that they would also be difficult for heritage speakers, especially if their dominant
language deals with these phenomena differently. An interesting case in point has to do
with the generic vs. specific interpretation of nouns appearing with articles or without
articles (bare nouns). It is well known that Germanic languages allow bare plurals in
generic contexts (Dogs have tails), whereas in Romance such bare plurals are generally
impossible (Les chiens ont des queues/*Chiens ont…). Some comparisons of bilinguals
who combine these two languages have already started (Serratrice et al. 2009; Montrul &
Ionin 2010, in press; Kupisch & Pierantozzi 2010; Barton in prep.) but the results are not
always conclusive. However, there seems to be a trend towards the over-acceptance of
bare plurals in inappropriate contexts, which suggests that simple overt markedness may
be implicated.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 41
4.6. Code-switching
Code switching consists of the combination of two languages (or language
varieties) in one clause or one discourse segment. The aspect of code-switching that has
received a great deal of attention involves the embedding of one language within the
syntactic frame of another language (Myers-Scotton 1993). However, to use a language
as a matrix frame requires knowledge of its syntax (word order, dependency and
selectional restrictions, etc.). Thus, code-switching can be an invaluable tool to gauge the
knowledge that heritage speakers have of their heritage language. Code-switching data
discussed in Albirini et al. (in press) show that heritage speakers use their heritage Arabic
varieties (in this instance Egyptian or Palestinian) as matrix languages with English as the
embedded language. Functional elements such as determiners, quantifiers and inflectional
affixes are from the heritage Arabic variety. Consider the sentence below from a
Palestinian heritage speaker:
(5)
kint habba ʔasaaʕed es students
was.1SG liking help.1SG the
fi el homework taaʕhum
in the
of theirs
‘I liked to help the students in their homework.’
The word order is clearly Arabic, with correct categorical selection. For example,
the auxiliary verb kint is followed by a participial form of the root (hbb) which is in turn
followed by an inflected form of the root (sʕ d). The English word homework is followed
by the possessive particle taaʕ which takes a genitive clitic that agrees with the English
word students. Thus agreement is also consistent with Arabic grammar. Also, notice that
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 42
the definite articles for students and homework are es and el respectively, which is
exactly what one would expect in Arabic where the form of the definite article varies
according to the initial consonant of the root; if it is a coronal consonant such as s the
definite articles is realized as a geminate of that consonant; otherwise it is realized as l.
We therefore see the English borrowings conforming to the syntax and phonology of the
heritage language, i.e., the heritage language is used as the matrix language.
The tentative conclusion is that such code-switching shows significant mastery of
the syntax of the heritage language. Whether or not code-switching allows us to
distinguish heritage speakers from second language speakers is just another open
question; we believe that it deserves further investigation (see also section 7).
To conclude this section, we see that, under reduced input conditions, heritage
speakers seem to develop core aspects of their family language, but their grammatical
systems show a marked tendency toward phonological neutralization, lexical restriction,
simplification and over-regularization of complex morphological patterns and restricted
word order. It is also possible that many of these effects could also be triggered by
transfer from English, the dominant language in most of the empirical studies conducted
to date. After all, English has strict SVO order and does not have overt case markers, null
subjects, subjunctive morphology, complex plural morphology, gender, or different types
of reflexive pronouns. Ideally, studies of the same heritage language with different
contact languages should be undertaken to investigate the extent to which transfer from
the dominant language influences the degree of divergence and simplification found in
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 43
heritage language grammars.9 However, there are other factors affecting heritage
languages that may not be related to the dominant language at all, as we discuss next.
5 What determines the shape of heritage grammars?
section, we presented a catalog of problems, both in
In the previous
comprehension and production that have been observed in heritage languages. Because
heritage language research is a very new field, it is inevitable that descriptive
generalizations are sketchy and much more empirical work on individual heritage
languages and across these languages is needed.
It is hard to draw significant conclusions based on sparse data, but at the risk of
sounding presumptuous or precipitous we would still like to consider possible factors that
play a role in the shaping of heritage grammars. Three factors we consider are incomplete
acquisition, attrition, and transfer from the dominant language. We will examine each of
them in turn.
5.1. Incomplete acquisition
We mentioned earlier that a typical heritage speaker displaying many of the
linguistic characteristics described above is a second generation immigrant: a child born
in the host country to immigrant parents or a child who immigrated before the age of 8
years old with their parents, depending on the immigrant group. Crucially, heritage
speakers are early bilinguals who learned the second (majority) language in childhood,
9 Such comparative studies are in their infancy,
but there is already some work on the same heritage (minority) language in contact with
different dominant languages, for example, Hungarian (Fenyvesi 2005) or Polish (Drubisz
2001).
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 44
either simultaneously with the heritage language, or after a short period of predominant
exposure to and use of the minority language at home. A common pattern in
simultaneous bilinguals is that as the child begins socialization in the majority language,
the amount of input and use in the minority language is reduced. Consequently, the
child’s competence in the heritage language begins to lag behind, such that the heritage
language becomes, structurally and functionally, the weaker language. Developmental
delays that start in childhood never eventually catch up, and as the heritage child
becomes an adult the eventual adult grammar does not show ultimate attainment. Even
though complete acquisition is the most natural outcome of monolingual acquisition in
childhood, the same is not necessarily true in bilingual acquisition, where one of the
languages may lag behind in development and end up incompletely acquired. The
clearest indication of incomplete acquisition is when a given language feature causes
problems equally to adult heritage speakers and to child learners at the age of 5 and up
(as this is the age when transfer to the dominant language is most commonly found).
Montrul (2008) defines an individual’s grammar as incomplete when it fails to
reach age-appropriate linguistic levels of proficiency as compared with the grammar of
monolingual or fluent bilingual speakers of the same age, cognitive development, and
social group. A clear example is the acquisition of the subjunctive in Spanish. Blake
(1983) tested monolingual children in Mexico between the ages of 4 and 12 on their use
of the subjunctive in different clauses. He found that between the ages of 5 and 8,
knowledge and use of the subjunctive in these children was in fluctuation: children did
not show categorical knowledge of Spanish subjunctive until after age 10. Heritage
speakers who receive less input at an earlier age and no schooling in the language never
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 45
fully acquire all the uses and semantic nuances of the subjunctive in Spanish, as reported
in many studies (Martínez Mira 2009, Montrul 2007, Potowski et al. 2009, SilvaCorvalán 1994). Silva-Corvalán (2003) presents a longitudinal study documenting
incomplete acquisition of the subjunctive and other verbal forms in bilingual children.
Another example of incomplete acquisition can be found in gender assignment in
Russian. Russian has three genders, and gender assignment is tightly linked to
declensional class (Corbett 1991: Ch. 3; Zaliznjak 1968). For some nouns, gender
assignment easily follows from their declension forms, but for others it is more opaque. A
subset of Russian inanimate nouns, those ending in a palatalized consonant (Cj/C´ ) in the
nominative singular, are particularly challenging; some of these nouns are masculine
(put´ ‘way, path’, mec´ ‘sword’, ogon´ ‘fire’, placš ´ ‘raincoat’), others are feminine
(kost´ ‘bone’, sol´ ‘salt’, krovat´ ‘bed’, ten´ ‘shadow’). As the few examples listed here
show, both subclasses include highly frequent words, many of which are likely to occur
in child-directed speech. Monolingual children make persistent errors in the gender
assignment of these nouns, with some errors continuing until age seven (Gvozdev 1961:
343; see also Comrie et al. 1996: 106, 121). Adult heritage speakers also show persistent
problems with these nouns (Polinsky 2008b). It is significant that both groups have the
same error pattern: feminine nouns are treated as masculine, but not the other way
around. This suggests that there may be language-specific pressures restraining the
assignment of the feminine to nouns that end in a palatal consonant. Children learning
Russian take a long time to acquire all the ingredients of this gender assignment, and
some of these ingredients are due to formal instruction in school. Heritage speakers get
frozen at the incomplete acquisition stage and show the errors that monolingual children
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 46
have a chance to outgrow. Moreover, the lack of schooling in Russian contributes to the
fossilization of this incompletely acquired gender subsystem.
Incomplete, partial, or interrupted acquisition (Montrul 2002, 2006, Polinsky
2007, O’Grady et al., in press; Silva-Corvalán 1994, 2003) is a specific case of language
loss that differs from L1 attrition in both the time in life when the language is affected,
and the extent of the loss. Incomplete acquisition occurs primarily in childhood due to
insufficient input to develop the full L1 system. Attrition implies that a grammatical
system had a chance to develop completely and remained stable for a while before some
grammatical aspects eroded later on, as a heritage speaker was using his/her language less
10 This is the phenomenon we will address next.
and less.
5.2. Attrition
In the previous section we discussed the elements of language design that take a
long time to acquire (possibly due to the tension between competing motivations) and are
therefore never fully attained by heritage speakers. A metaphor one could use here is that
of dancing: an aspiring dancer can learn to waltz but it would take him/her more time and
more exposure to complex tunes to master a tarantella. Contrary to the expectation that
learning your first language is like learning to ride a bicycle—once you know it you
never fall out of that knowledge—heritage speakers show patterns of attrition.
If an adult speaker stops using their language, due to, for example, emigration or
repression (as in the case of the Japanese in the United States during World War WWII),
10 To make the conceptual distinction clearer, we present incomplete acquisition and attrition in
childhood as two distinct scenarios, but we certainly admit that the two do not have to be
mutually exclusive.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 47
it is clear that their language will undergo attrition. Such attrition over the lifespan has
been documented by a number of researchers (Schmid 2002; Schmid et al. 2004, a.o.); it
has long been considered one of the major driving forces behind obsolescence (Dorian
1981, 1989; Seliger & Vago 1991; Sasse 1991, 1992).
Attrition as a culprit in heritage languages is more contested, with some
researchers basically rejecting this possibility and others being more open to it.
The reasons to hesitate are serious. First, it is unclear when acquisition reaches the
point of ultimate attainment, and the ongoing debates on the critical (or sensitive) period
make the decision even more difficult. The second reason to be cautious in applying the
idea of attrition to heritage speakers has to do with the tremendous heterogeneity and
variance in that population. And finally, there is a methodological issue: can a linguistic
feature be considered attrited if it is still represented in a heritage language, albeit to a
lesser degree than in the control population? Or should only categorical differences be
counted as evidence of attrition?
All these considerations suggest that we should proceed with caution, and we
propose that attrition can be sought in those language properties that are fully acquired by
children by age 4-5. If an adult heritage speaker experiences problems with such
language properties there is a great likelihood that these properties underwent attrition
and became weaker over the speaker’s lifespan.
Of course, these considerations have to be somewhat tentative. In order to be
certain that attrition occurred one would need longitudinal data on the same individuals
observed as children and as heritage speaker adults. However, in the absence of such
data, we can rely on the generalizations amassed in the research on first language
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 48
acquisition, including bilingual acquisition, and make sure that our observations are
based on a substantial subject pool—this way an aberrant speaker, who was late to
language acquisition as a child or who was particularly resilient as a heritage adult, would
be canceled out by the other members of the cohort.
An intriguing example of attrition comes from the comprehension of relative
clauses by low-level heritage adults. Relative clauses are generally acquired around age
4, and for some languages even a bit earlier (Tavakolian 1978; de Villiers et al. 1979;
Sheldon 1974; Diessel & Tomasello 2000; Goodluck et al. 2006; Guasti & Cardinaletti
2003, a.o.). In a study which compared the comprehension of subject and object relative
clauses by four groups of Russian speakers (monolingual adults/children, heritage
adults/children), Polinsky (2008c, 2010) found that the children in both groups performed
on par with monolingual adults, almost at ceiling. Heritage adult speakers, however,
performed at chance on object relatives and showed a degraded performance on subject
relatives. This suggests that the grammar of relative clauses underwent attrition in the
language of heritage adults. This result is particularly intriguing given that the child
cohort performed so well and that the experimental conditions were structured in such a
way that transfer from English was ruled out. The reduced (attrited) adult system reflects
well on the seemingly universal preference for subject relatives: heritage speakers of
Russian do particularly poorly on object clauses, but still maintain the subject preference
in relativization.
Brehmer & Czachor (2010) compared three groups of adult Polish speakers:
heritage speakers who were either born in Germany or came to Germany as young
children, Polish-German bilinguals who resettled in Germany after age 15, and
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 49
monolingual Polish speakers. They found an indication of attrition in the use of the
genitive of negation. This genitive occurs in two main contexts in Polish: as the case of
an object under negation and in lieu of the nominative in negative existential and locative
clauses—cf. the affirmative (6a) and negative (6b) locative:
(6)
a. ksiazka byla na stole
book.NOM was on table
‘The book was on the table.’
b. ksiazki nie bylo na stole
book.GEN not was on table
‘The book was not on the table.’
While the genitive of negation in the object position seems to vary greatly with the
accusative in spoken Polish, the use of the genitive in contexts such as (6b) is quite
robust. It is well preserved by the Polish-German bilinguals in Brehmer & Czachor’s
study, but the heritage speakers show gradient attrition: about a quarter of all the cases
where the genitive of negation was required were used or judged wrong. What motivates
this replacement? We hypothesize that it is driven by fundamental differences between
the two cases: the genitive under negation makes a subtle semantic contribution that is
lost to heritage speakers, and they generalize towards a more widely used case. This is
only a conjecture, but Polish offers indirect evidence in its support. Subject idioms with
negation, which would allow the genitive, lose their idiomatic interpretation if the
genitive is replaced by the accusative:
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 50
(7) a. nie
ma
dymu/*dym
bez
ognia
not be smoke.GEN/*NOM without fire
‘There is no smoke without fire.
b. krzykiem
ognia/*ogien nie ugasisz
yelling.INSTR fire.GEN/*ACC not extinguish
‘Talk is cheap.’
Assuming that idiom chunks in non-compositional idioms do not have their own
meaning, the contrasts observed here can be explained if the case alternation makes a
semantic or pragmatic contribution. But this contribution of the genitive under negation is
immaterial to a heritage speaker and the nominative wins out. The scenario, where
attrition is driven by the loss of fine-grained semantic or pragmatic features, has been
proposed for some restructuring in Russian aspect in the speech of higher-proficiency
heritage speakers (Laleko 2010) and may also play a role in the reinterpretation of
interface phenomena as discussed by Sorace (2004, 2005) and Sorace & Serratrice
(2009).
Moving on to teenage heritage speakers, whose competence in their heritage
language is less clear, researchers have commented on a general decline in their language
ability. Such a decline is sometimes confirmed by heritage speakers’ self-assessment; this
decline seems to be linked to an increase in the use of the dominant language in
communication with peers at school and with siblings. Zhou (2000) reports a decline in
the language ability of Vietnamese heritage speakers over two years (from age 14 to age
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 51
16), parallel with the increase in their English competence; a similar decline is reported
for Tagalog by Espiritu & Wolf (2001).
5.3. Dominant language transfer
An important point of contact between heritage speakers and second language
learners, which does not come up in L1 acquisition, is the interplay between the learner’s
first (native) language and the second (target) language. Language transfer, or the nature
of that particular interplay is a foundational issue in second language acquisition
research: to what extent does the first language grammar play a role in shaping the
developing second language grammar? The effects of the native language on the
acquisition of a second language in different levels of linguistic analysis (phonology,
morphology, syntax, semantics, the lexicon) have been extensively documented in second
language acquisition literature over the years (Odlin 1989; White 1989; Gass & Selinker
1992; Schwartz & Sprouse 1996; Jarvis 1998). A similar question arises in other
language contact situations, including pidgin and creole genesis, where phenomena like
lexical borrowings and so-called areal features are the well known cases of linguistic
consequences of language contact. Research on bilingualism and language contact (both
at the social and psycholinguistic level) also suggests that the second language can
encroach into the structure of the native language in systematic ways (Cook 2003,
Pavlenko & Jarvis 2002, Seliger 1996).
In examining the linguistic characteristics of heritage grammars illustrated in
section 4 at all levels of grammatical analysis, the first question that comes to mind is
whether many of the “simplified” characteristics observed in the heritage languages could
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 52
be due to transfer from the dominant majority language. One can easily entertain the
possibility that nominal and verbal inflectional morphology in Spanish and Russian
heritage speakers is eroded because the contact language in most of the heritage speakers
tested to date is English, a language which does not mark gender in nouns or have rich
tense/aspect and mood morphology. The same explanation goes for overuse of overt
subjects and loss of semantically based case in Spanish and Russian, and preference for
SVO over topicalizations. The interpretation of definite articles as having only a specific
interpretation and not a generic interpretation in Spanish is a clear effect of transfer from
English.
At the same time, it is an open question whether most effects of incomplete
acquisition attested in heritage speakers are due to L2 transfer. Research also shows that
some features of heritage speech that are not present in the heritage language are not due
to the influence of the dominant language, such as the simplification of the gender
classification system in Russian (from a 3- to a 2-way distinction), but must be due to
other factors related to linguistic complexity and how this interacts with the nature of
input and the degree of exposure to the input. There are two possible ways to tease apart
these alternatives. One is to test heritage speakers of Korean, Russian or Spanish in two
different language contact situations. If the contact language plays a role, then different
patterns of incomplete acquisition should be observed in the two groups (assuming other
characteristics of heritage speakers are controlled). A recent example of this type of work
is J.-H. Kim’s (2007) study of binding interpretations by Korean heritage speakers in the
United States and Korean heritage speakers in China. The study tested knowledge of
binding interpretations with local and long-distance anaphors in different syntactic
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 53
contexts. In many respects, Chinese and Korean were more similar than Korean and
English. Korean heritage speakers in China were expected to be more accurate with longdistance binding than the Korean heritage speakers in the United States.. Kim found that
the two groups of Korean speakers still had a marked preference for local binding,
regardless of the contact language. If similar findings are replicated with other groups of
heritage speakers in different language-contact contexts, then the effect of the L2 may not
turn out to be as strong, or at least the only factor involved, in shaping the incomplete
grammars of heritage speakers.
Another way to approach this question is by testing heritage speakers whose
majority language is either typologically close to their heritage language (Spanish
heritage speakers in Italy or Brazil, for example) or morphologically and syntactically
more complex (in the sense of McWhorter 2007) than the heritage language (heritage
speakers in non-English speaking countries perhaps). But this is a tricky approach
because “complexity” or “simplicity” cannot be applied to particular languages but rather
to particular linguistic phenomena within a language (i.e., the morphological system,
syntactic flexibility, etc.).
In short, we know that many effects attested in heritage language grammars can
be directly linked to L2 transfer, but this is not always obvious. What appears to be the
dominant language transfer may also be due to universal regression processes or
simplification under reduced input conditions (as attested in the case of creole genesis).
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 54
6 The relevance of heritage languages to theoretical linguistics Let’s step away from
heritage languages for a moment and ask ourselves what
made first language acquisition so valuable to theoretical linguists of all persuasions. The
answer seems to go like this: child language has less irregularity than adult language; it is
less encumbered by external linguistic experience, and therefore allows researchers to see
how the rules and constraints operating in natural language develop more clearly. When a
child over-generalizes, it is not arbitrary, as s/he draws on fundamental principles of
natural language design. When a child makes errors, they are also non-arbitrary, which is
why monsters like (8) never occur (Crain & Nakayama 1987, Legate & Yang 2002):
(8) *Is the woman who singing is happy?
In a nutshell, phenomena observed in child language allow us to see linguistic structure
more clearly than in adult language. But building theories on child language comes with a
cost: children are notoriously difficult to study in an experimental setting and their
production data are largely full of noise.
Unlike children, whose attention is short-lived and fickle, adult heritage speakers
are a user-friendly population; they are motivated and cooperative, they come to the
testing room without a chaperone, and they may even become active participants, not just
experimental subjects, in a study testing the limits of their linguistic knowledge. But just
like children, heritage speakers offer us an opportunity to study a language
unencumbered by too much irregularity that has to be learned over a lifespan, by too
many external factors or non-structural confounds. Their language has the minimal
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 55
scaffolding that has to be there for language to stand and has minimal design features. To
continue with an architectural metaphor, a heritage language has all the structural,
material, and functional design values but very few aesthetic values: it is the minimalist
architecture as compared to the baroque of a full-fledged language with a literary
tradition and a revered norm. A heritage language is what’s left after you have stripped
away everything that is rote learned, driven by tradition, enforced by the norm, and
driven to non-compositionality by many users. This makes it a desirable object of
investigation, and we need to learn how to use it better. As long as it remains under the
auspices of “applied” areas of linguistic inquiry, theoreticians stand to lose.
In this section, we would like to highlight just a few areas in which heritage
language linguistics has a bearing on linguistic theory. These stem from our own work on
the design of heritage languages and naturally reflect our own tastes and theoretical
dispositions.
6.1 Agreement Broadly put, agreement is a relation between two elements within a phrase or
sentence that involves full or partial matching of features such as number, gender, person,
definiteness, (abstract) Case, etc. The instantiations of agreement that have received the
lion’s share of attention within theoretical syntax include verb-subject agreement, verbobject agreement, agreement within noun phrases (with determiners and adjectives), and
agreement on grammatical categories such as complementizers, negation, auxiliaries and
modals.
Since at its core agreement involves dependencies, it is relevant to a number of
issues within theoretical syntax, particularly the role of configuration and the attendant
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 56
structural relations (such as Spec-head, c-command, Agree or government) in
establishing the relation between the agreeing elements. For example, under one version
of current syntactic theory, subject-verb agreement in English relies on the specifier-head
relation between the subject in the specifier of the tense projection and the tense head
which possesses agreement features (Chomsky 1993), as illustrated below:
TP
3
Subject T’ [phi3 features]
T
vP
Agree
[phi-features]
On the other hand, agreement in verb-initial languages such as Arabic or
Hawaiian
(9)
may be due to a government relation between T and the subject, which remains in a lower
projection, probably VP (Koopman & Sportiche 1991). This bifurcation allowed for
accounts for agreement asymmetries that have been uncovered in French participial
construction and Arabic VS vs. SV orders (among others). However, recent minimalist
work has advanced the view that agreement involves only one configuration, namely
Agree, which takes place under c-command. A number of facts support this approach,
chief among them being the phenomenon of long distance agreement (Polinsky and
Postdam 2001, Bhatt 2005), where a head establishes an agreement dependency with a
noun phrase that is relatively deeply embedded within the sentence (or a least not a
clause-mate to it). In addition, this approach entails that there should be no syntactic
differences between VS and SV languages with regard to the configuration that is needed
to establish the agreement dependency. If so, syntacticians can no longer rely on the
contrast between spec-head agreement and Agree and may want to opt for an interface
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 57
explanation for some agreement phenomena. For example, Benmamoun (2000) argues
that the agreement asymmetry between the SV and VS orders in Standard Arabic is due
to how agreement features, particularly number, are realized in the morpho-phonological
component; Franck et al. (2006) suggests that agreement is checked twice, first in syntax,
and then in the phonological component of grammar.
Another important dichotomy that characterizes the syntactic debates about
agreement is the interpretable vs. non-interpretable nature of the formal phi-features we
have mentioned above. Agreement features on heads are considered non-interpretable,
i.e., not critical to the conceptual/semantic interface. That is, a number feature on the verb
does not tell us whether we are dealing with one event or a number of events but only
references the cardinality of the noun the head is in agreement with. On the other hand, a
number feature on a noun is interpretable since it tells us something about the cardinality
of the noun. However, it is not clear how to characterize agreement in null pronominal
languages since there is no other overt noun phrase within the sentence to recover
(identify) the null argument of the verb.
Thus, how agreement manifests itself in heritage languages can be a valuable
testing ground for syntactic theories of agreement. Consider the interpretable/noninterpretable dichotomy. A number of studies (Fenyvesi 2000, Bolonyai 2007, Montrul
2008, Albirni et al. in press) have shown that heritage speakers perform fairly well on
subject-verb agreement as opposed to agreement within the noun phrase or verb-object
agreement. Notice that distance between the two agreeing elements does not seem critical
to the contrast between subject agreement and agreement within the noun phrases. In both
Arabic and Hungarian, agreement within the noun phrase, which is a single projection
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 58
containing the head and the agreement controller, is more vulnerable. This implies that
the spec-head relation (which essentially requires that the two elements be within the
same phrase) is not what accounts for the resilience of subject agreement.11
Why would heritage speakers maintain subject-verb agreement when the features
presumably do not play a role at the conceptual-semantic interface?
One possibility is that the features play a role at the PF interface. For example, in
some languages such as Hungarian and Arabic, a well-formed word must have inflection.
In other words, there are no well-formed bare verbs. By contrast, verb-object agreement
is not critical to the phonological well-formedness of verbs, nor is agreement critical to
the well-formedness of a noun within the noun phrase. However, this does not explain
why heritage speakers do not rely mostly on a default or unmarked form of agreement.
Arabic, for example, has a rich agreement system with default forms such as the third
person form that occurs in the context of null expletive subjects or clausal subjects.
Alternatively, the resilience of subject agreement in pro-drop languages may be due to
the semantic load that agreement carries. Unlike agreement in non-null subject languages,
agreement in null subject languages is sometimes the main means of recovering the
content of the null subject. In some respects, it behaves like an interpretable feature. Of
course, this must remain tentative pending analyses of heritage languages that do not
have null subjects (such as English, French or Russian), but it may be on the right track
and could explain why structural Case, which is non interpretable, is vulnerable (see
section 6.2) but subject verb agreement is not.
11 It would be important to see how heritage speakers perform on long distance agreement and
agreement asymmetries that are sensitive word order, for instance in such languages as Hindi
and Arabic.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 59
Nevertheless, even subject-verb agreement breaks down in contexts where the
subject is not of the canonical type. It is notable that in both Arabic and Hungarian
subject-verb agreement breaks down severely in possessive clauses, as illustrated in (10)
for Hungarian (Bolonyai 2007):
(10)
nek-em
van egy bicikli-Ø
DAT-POSS.1SG
is
a
bicycle.NOM
ʻI have a bicycle.’
The subject (‘bicycle)’ does not carry agreement with the first person possessor. Similar
facts obtain in Egyptian Arabic (Albirini et al. in press):
(11)
kaanet
ʕindaha
was.3SG.FEM at her
talaTTaʕš ar sana
thirteen
year
‘She had thirteen years/she was thirteen years old’
In (11), the copular verb agrees with the possessor rather than the subject (talaTTaʕš ar
sana). Egyptian heritage speakers seem to treat the possessor as subject. This could be
due to transfer from English or to the semantic prominence of the possessor (along the
thematic hierarchy cline). Another alternative explanation is that the agreement
dependency in this context requires more computation than the agreement dependency
involving regular subject verb contexts. In this instance, the speaker must rule out the
possessor as in order to settle on the other NP as the candidate for subject agreement.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 60
Here, the problem would have to do with processing the complex dependencies within
possessive clauses. Evidently, this can only be determined by studying the same patterns
in the heritage speech of speakers of languages that display the Hungarian or Arabic
patterns of agreement in possessive clauses (for example, heritage speakers of Russian in
Israel).
The data available so far does not allow for a definitive answer to why subjectverb agreement is more resilient in canonical contexts. One notable conclusion we can
confidently draw, though, is that agreement at the PF interface seems to be resilient.
Heritage speakers do not have a lot of difficulty with pairing the verb and its formal
feature specification with the correct cell of an agreement paradigm in PF. This is
remarkable because in many languages the agreement paradigms are quite complex (in
Arabic, the imperfective paradigm involves both prefixation and suffixation with the
features of the prefix sometime sensitive to the feature of the suffix. Similarly, in there is
an intricate connection between subject and verb agreement and how they are realized at
PF). In this respect, heritage speakers are different from L2 learners who tend to display
difficulties with agreement paradigms.
6.2. Inherent and structural case
The distinction between structural and inherent case (Chomsky 1981) relies on the
observation that some relationships in case licensing may be purely structural and do not
depend on the thematic relations between the licensing category and the element it
licenses, whereas others have to be defined in terms of thematic relations. Nominative
and accusative cases are standardly assumed to be structural, assigned “solely in terms of
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 61
S-structure configurations” (Chomsky 1995: 114). On the other hand, the genitive or the
dative subject case are associated with theta-marking, hence they represent inherent case.
The status of the ergative case has long been a subject of dispute in linguistics,
with a number of researchers advocating its status as an inherent case assigned by a v
head (Woolford 2006, Legate 2008, Aldridge 2008), and others treating it as a structural
case (Koopman 2008). In an influential paper discussing various criteria that contribute to
the inherent case status, Woolford (2006) suggests that theta marking may not be the
necessary or sufficient criterion of inherent case status, but presents other diagnostics of
inherent case, the strongest of which has to do with case preservation under raising.
Unfortunately, the sole example she presents, from Tongan, is not accepted by all the
speakers, and without that particular empirical piece, not much is left to support the
inherent case status of the ergative.
Heritage language data may offer novel empirical evidence arguing against the
nature of the ergative as an inherent case. The logic of the argument is as follows. We
have solid evidence that some cases in nominative-accusative languages are structural
(nominative, accusative), whereas others are inherent (dative, genitive). If these cases are
treated differently in heritage grammars, one could extrapolate their differential behavior
to the ergative system comparing the ergative and the absolutive.
In heritage Russian and heritage Spanish, object marking with the accusative and
with ‘a personal’ undergo significant attrition (Polinsky 2000, 2006; Montrul & Bowles
2009, 2010). In the meantime, the dative encoding of a sentient subject is retained quite
well. In some varieties of heritage Russian, the accusative form is used in lieu of the
dative and the accusative of the object is replaced by the nominative (Polinsky 2006);
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 62
thus, the sentient subject is marked for case. Heritage Spanish also shows a relatively
robust maintenance of dative subjects, especially in pronominal forms, as in the following
12 example:
(12)
no
le
gusta las instituciones
not 3sg.DAT like the institutions
‘He does not like institutions.’
In the nominal forms, heritage speakers of Spanish judge indirect objects without a as the
least acceptable; they are more tolerant of the omission of a on dative subjects13 and on
animate direct objects (Montrul & Bowles 2009, Montrul et al. 2010). Overall, the data
from these two languages suggest that inherent cases in heritage grammars are preserved
better/to a stronger degree than structural cases.
Let us now turn to Hindi, which is split ergative: it has ergative marking on
transitive subjects in the perfect and nominative-accusative alignment elsewhere. It also
has dative subjects marked by ko. Hindi presents a particularly interesting case because
the marker ko is found both with the dative and with the accusative.
The correct form should be No le gustan las
instituciones, with the verb agreeing with the postverbal constituent. Heritage speakers
consistently make this kind of agreement error, but do not use the wrong clitic form.
13 The same happens in some Spanish dialects, so the change from a dative subject to a
nominative subject is already present in the baseline (Demonte 1995, Fernandez-Soriano 1999,
a.o.). This trend in the baseline, which involves the subject position only, may affect the attrition
of the dative subject in heritage Spanish.
12
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 63
There are two studies of case production in heritage Hindi: Mahajan 2009, based
on the author’s observations, and Montrul et al. 2010, with a strong quantitative
component (Montrul et al. 2010 also employed a grammaticality judgment task).
According to Mahajan, heritage speakers “never miss the marking on the subject
with ko”, e.g.,
(13) John-ko saRak par kelaa
milaa
J-DAT street on banana.MASC found.MASC
‘John found a banana in the street.’
According to Montrul et al. (2010), the marking on the dative subject is omitted in about
22% of production occurrences, and the marking on the indirect object is left out in about
10%; thus indirect objects are least problematic. In both structural positions, the form
represents an inherent case. These data come from a picture description task. In the same
picture description task, heritage speakers left out about 38% of the accusative-marking
ko in contexts such as (14) where it is required as accompanying a specific animate
object:
(14) Mira-ne Ramesh*(-ko) dekhaa
M-ERG R-ACC
saw.MASC
‘Mira saw Ramesh.’
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 64
The use of ko in heritage Hindi thus suggests the same trend we noted earlier: structural
cases undergo greater erosion in heritage production than inherent cases. What about the
ergative -ne, shown in example (14) above?
Mahajan (2009) notes that heritage speakers use the “ergative marking very
randomly—omit it when [it is] needed with [the] subject of [a] transitive perfect verb but
insert it when [they] cannot have it”. In the same picture description task that was used to
get the data on ko, Montrul et al. (2010) show that the ergative is overgeneralized in
about 11% of cases (no overgeneralization is found with ko, which is subject to omission)
and left out in the other 33% of cases. If we compare only the omission and the use of
ergative -ne, without overgeneralization, the omission rate rises to 37%, much higher
than for the dative, and on par with the accusative. This suggests that the ergative patterns
with structural case, not with the inherent case.
Some other ergative languages also show that the ergative is replaced while cases
like dative and genitive are retained better. Thus, in heritage Avar, spoken by Russiandominant younger speakers, one finds the ergative replaced by the dative (the correct
form should be dica in the ergative):
(15)
diye
rucka
rek-ana
1SG.DAT pen.ABS break-PAST
‘I broke the pen.’
This substitution is particularly intriguing because Avar, like many other NakhDagestanian l anguages, has a rich inventory of other marked cases (many of them
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 65
locative); nevertheless, a heritage speaker chooses the marked dative to replace the
likewise marked ergative.
In her description of young people’s Dyirbal, which shows many properties
typical of heritage languages, Schmidt (1985: 47-57) shows that the ergative is pretty
much lost in the language, while the dative and comitative, two arguably inherent cases,
are maintained.
Of course more data on ergative heritage languages are needed, but we would like
to emphasize the general promise that heritage languages hold with respect to a difficult
theoretical problem of case licensing.
6.3. Agreement and Case
As pointed out above, research on heritage languages has uncovered an important
contrast between agreement and structural Case. Subject-verb agreement seems to be less
vulnerable than structural Case (as opposed to inherent Case which appears to be more
resilient). For recent debates in theoretical syntax this result is imminently interesting.
Within the Principles and Parameters framework, in particular, the marriage between
Case and agreement has been rocky with occasional break-ups and reunions. Under
earlier versions of the theory, agreement was critical to Case assignment. This was due,
in part, to the fact that in Germanic and Romance languages, non-finite verbs do not carry
subject verb agreement, particularly person, which appears to point to a correlation
between Nominative Case and agreement on the finite verb. However, with the
proliferation of functional syntactic projections since the seminal paper by Pollock
(1989), this relationship has been called into question. Pollock’s main concern was the
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 66
need for extra space to derive the word order differences between English and French
relative to the position of the verb, adverbs, negation and floating quantifiers. To come up
with this extra space, Pollock split the traditional Infl projection (Chomsky 1986) into a
tense projection and a (subject) agreement projection. Chomsky (1993) added an object
agreement projection. Since Nominative Case is associated with finiteness in a number of
languages and since tense and agreement occupy different positions in the syntactic
representation, the logical conclusion was that agreement is not involved in nominative
Case assignment but tense is.
However, if we posit an agreement projection whose head includes noninterpretable features a serious conceptual problem arises. Most other functional
projections contain heads whose features are interpretable Other than the convenience of
providing extra space for elements to move to derive word order patterns, there did not
seem to be any compelling conceptual reason for positing such an agreement projection.
This conceptual problem led several researchers to argue against the agreement
projection (e.g., Iatridou 1990; Benmamoun 1993; Chomsky 1995). However, in some
later minimalist research (Chomsky 2000), the abandoned association between agreement
and Case was again resurrected, albeit in more subtle form. For the Case dependency (say
between Tense and a DP) to be maintained, they have to be in an Agree relation. Though
the association is indirect it is there nonetheless and suffers from the same empirical
problems that plagued the earlier agreement-as-a syntactic projection-approach.14 This
approach was superseded by a more recent one, which argues for the dissociation of case
14 Languages with sentences where more than
one head carries full agreement also present a problem (Carstens 2001).
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 67
licensing and agreement, on slightly different grounds. The main idea is that Case
features are not present on T but are simply passed down to T from the higher functional
head, C; on the contrary, agreement is an inherent feature of T and thus agreement is
licensed by the T head (Richards 2007, Zeller 2006, Chomsky 2008, a.o.).
The dissociation of tense and agreement makes empirical sense for a number of
languages. For example, in Standard Arabic, tense can occur on negation and agreement
on the verb, which demonstrates that the two must be dissociated and cannot be located
on the same host (Benmamoun 2000). Subject-verb agreement and case licensing in
Scandinavian have long been shown to be separate (Holmberg & Platzack 1995).
Empricial arguments for the dissociation of Case and agreement have been found in
Bantu (Zeller 2006).
Turning to heritage languages, the differential results on the maintenance of case
forms vs. agreement support the conclusion that case licensing and agreement licensing
are dissociated. Otherwise, all else being equal, we would expect them to pattern
together. Thus the data from heritage languages also favor the approach that severs the
connection between agreement and structural Case; these data offer another compelling
argument for the emerging model of Case licensing being separate from agreement.
6.4. Aspectual categories
A fundamental goal of linguistic theory is to provide an explanatorily adequate
model of grammar that could shed light on the properties of the human language faculty,
a model capable of accounting not only for the observed cross-linguistic phenomena and
native speaker judgments, but, crucially, for the facts of language acquisition. Because
heritage language acquisition is a fully systematic and internally rule-governed process,
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 68
linguistic theories that exhibit the capacity to capture and account for the facts of heritage
language acquisition have the potential for a greater explanatory power than those that do
not. Consequently, heritage languages can provide crucial pieces of evidence for
theoretical models of language as expressed through various linguistic phenomena.
This approach is undertaken in Laleko (2010), who draws on empirical data from
heritage Russian speakers to develop a theoretical model of aspect in Russian. On the
basis of experiments targeting production, interpretation, and acceptability of perfective
and imperfective aspectual forms by heritage speakers of Russian and baseline speakers
in the control group, Laleko (2010) puts forward a model of aspect in Russian that posits
multiple layers of aspectual structure. On the lower syntactic level, aspectual distinctions
are tied to lexical aspect, i.e., telicity of the verbal predicate. For verbs that are inherently
specified as telic or atelic, the default aspectual value at this level is calculated based on
the properties of the verb, while in the absence of such specification on the verbal root
itself, compositional telicity of the verbal phrase, including the nominal argument, has the
potential of contributing to the resulting aspectual value of the VP (Laleko, 2008). On the
higher sentential level, the contribution of telicity may be overridden by aspectual
markers (operators), such as the habitual and progressive, or by delimiting perfectivizing
prefixes such as po- and za-. The habitual and progressive force the imperfective
interpretation of telic events, and the perfectivizing prefixes supply an external boundary
to lexically unbounded eventualities. In the absence of sentential aspectual triggers, the
default lexical aspect projects directly onto the sentential level.
The highest level of syntactic structure, which interfaces with discoursepragmatics, is the domain of pragmatically-conditioned aspectual triggers, which are
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 69
sensitive to external contextual factors in mediating aspectual meanings. Here, aspectual
contrasts reflect such notions as the thematicity of the predicate and the illocutionary
force of the utterance. Thus, even in the absence of atelic interpretations of the verbal
phrase at the lexical level or imperfective operators at the sentential level, Russian verbs
may receive imperfective marking for pragmatic reasons, for instance to indicate that the
speaker is merely reporting some fact about a particular event, without regard to its
completion, or to implicate that the result of the action denoted by the predicate has been
annulled. Availability of such pragmatically-conditioned functions of the imperfective
aspect in Russian produces aspectual competition, a situation in which both aspectual
forms are grammatically possible. The competition is successfully resolved in favor in
the imperfective aspect in the presence of the relevant contextual triggers.
Data from monolingual speakers of Russian presented in Laleko (2010) are fully
consistent with the model outlined above. In contrast, evidence from advanced heritage
speakers of Russian reveals a significant reduction of the pragmatically-conditioned
functions of the imperfective aspect. When compared with baseline controls, heritage
speakers exhibit lower acceptability rates for imperfective forms with completed events
even in the presence of contextual discourse-pragmatic triggers of imperfectivity. Further,
heritage speakers are significantly less accurate in their interpretations of the annulled
result implicature. Despite these differences, heritage speakers exhibit no overt
grammatical errors with aspectual morphology in production.
In accounting for the observed findings, Laleko (2010) argues that the three levels
of aspectual structure are affected selectively in heritage language acquisition. The
restructuring of aspect in advanced heritage grammars affects the C-domain: the highest
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 70
level of sentential structure, a domain in which syntactic information is mapped onto
discourse-pragmatic knowledge. As a result, the privative aspectual opposition of
baseline Russian, in which the imperfective aspect is the unmarked member with a wider
contextual distribution, undergoes a shift to an opposition of the equipollent type, where
the distribution of aspectual forms is determined solely by the grammar. Laleko (2010)
refers to this type of reorganization as a covert restructuring of the aspectual system,
because the disintegration of the aspectual system within the C-domain is manifested in
infelicity, rather than strict ungrammaticality, and highly proficient heritage speakers
continue to appear target-like in production even without the complete mastery of the
intricate contextual uses of the Russian imperfective, relying on grammatical cues
instead.
Laleko’s (2010) model of aspect in Russian makes further predictions with
respect to the directionality of aspectual restructuring across the sectors of the heritage
continuum. While advanced heritage speakers seem to exhibit sensitivity to phenomena
mediated in the C-domain (besides aspect, other difficulties in this category include
apparent optionality with null and overt subjects and infelicitous use of overt
determiners), heritage speakers at the lower level of the proficiency continuum are
predicted to diverge from the baseline norm not only on the highest level of aspectual
structure interfacing with discourse-pragmatics, but also on the intermediate level of
sentential aspect, where grammatical aspectual triggers operate. Thus, we expect that
lower proficiency heritage speakers may not be consistently sensitive to sentential
aspectual operators, paying more attention to the default lexical aspect of the predicate.
Consistent with these predictions, existing production data from low-proficiency heritage
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 71
speakers of Russian, such as naturally-occurring examples provided in Polinsky (2006,
2008c), reveal multiple instances of perfective aspectual forms occurring in the presence
of imperfectivizing sentential triggers, such as habitual adverbs, when predicates are
lexically or compositionally telic. Similarly, speakers in the same proficiency group
exhibit loss of some delimiting perfectivizing prefixes (e.g., inceptive -za), which provide
an external boundary to atelic eventualities in baseline Russian.
In tandem with data from high-proficiency speakers, these findings lend support
to a layered model of aspectual structure, in which different components of aspectual
information are mediated at distinct levels.
The encoding of temporality is an important area of vast on-going research within
theoretical linguistics, and research on the expression of aspectual distinctions in heritage
languages provides a unique opportunity for advancing current theories of temporality
based on qualitatively new data; this offers promising implications for furthering our
understanding of the human language capacity and the organization of linguistic
knowledge in the mind.
In examining data from heritage speakers in comparison with data from their
bilingual parents, Laleko (2010) further addresses the role of linguistic input in language
acquisition and maintenance. She finds that bilingual parents of heritage speakers, while
target-like on comprehension tests involving aspectual contrasts at the syntax-pragmatics
interface, exhibit patterns of limited occurrence of imperfective forms with telic
predicates in production. This finding has two important implications. First, it
corroborates the idea that heritage language acquisition and L1 attrition in adulthood are
distinct scenarios with different linguistic consequences: while heritage language
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 72
acquisition creates conditions for a divergent performance along with a reduced
competence, L1 attrition effects in adulthood do not appear to project beyond certain
limitations in performance with respect to the same phenomenon. Second, given that the
variety of the language spoken by the parents of heritage speakers is the primary (and
often only) source of linguistic input in heritage language acquisition, we can raise the
question of whether some of the competence deficits in heritage speakers may be a
categorical response to a quantitatively diminished and qualitatively reconfigured input.
This, in turn, brings us directly to the principally important issue at the intersection of
several distinct fields of inquiry in contemporary linguistics – the impact of age and
circumstances of language acquisition on the outcome of language acquisition. Heritage
language acquisition, which bridges a gap between first and second language acquisition,
as well as between language acquisition and language attrition, can offer a unique
contribution to the existing theories of language development, particularly in situations of
reduced input. The issue of reduced input is of course equally relevant to heritage
linguistics and studies of second language acquisition, and in the next section we will
offer some considerations on similarities and differences between these two fields.
6.5. Heritage language development vs. second language acquisition
Another angle on the study of the grammatical competence of heritage speakers in
the last few years has been to investigate the potential similarities and differences
between heritage speakers and second language learners, as noted by Lipski (1993). Not
only is this comparison theoretically significant for debates on the role of age and input in
bilingual language acquisition and regression, as well as the type of implicit knowledge
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 73
acquired under less than optimal conditions, but such comparison also has great
pedagogical implications for the field of heritage language teaching. The question of
whether early language experience confers advantages to heritage speakers, as opposed to
second language learners who acquire the language much later in their life, has been at
the heart of several studies comparing second language learners and heritage speakers'
grammatical knowledge (Au et al. 2002, Montrul 2010, Montrul, Foote & Perpiñán,
2008). Results on phonological competence indicate advantages for heritage speakers,
who exhibit more native-like pronunciation than second language learners (Au et al.
2002). Results on morphosyntactic knowledge are mixed, with some studies finding no
advantages (Au et al. 2002) and others finding some, depending on proficiency level,
type of structure and type of task (Håkansson 1995, Mikulski 2010, Montrul, Foote &
Perpiñán 2008, Montrul 2010).
Many of these studies have extended theories and methodologies of second
language acquisition to investigate differences and similarities between second language
learners and heritage language learners (Montrul 2005). For example, Montrul (in press)
looked at a perennial problem in second language acquisition and its theoretical
significance: the issue of morphological variability and the source of morphological
errors. A recurrent finding is that postpubescent second language learners often omit or
use the wrong affix for nominal and verbal inflections in oral production, but less so in
written tasks. According to the Missing Surface Inflection Hypothesis (Prévost & White
2000), second language learners have intact abstract representations for this morphology,
but errors stem from problems during production only under communicative pressure (a
mapping or processing deficit). Montrul (in press) discusses collective findings from
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 74
Montrul, Foote and Perpiñán (2008), Montrul (2010) and Montrul & Perpiñán (in press)
showing that variability and instability with gender agreement, tense, aspect and mood
morphology is also characteristic of heritage speakers' grammars. However, because
morphological errors by heritage speakers are more frequent in written than in oral tasks,
unlike the pattern found in second language learners, the Missing Surface Inflection
Hypothesis does not apply to heritage speakers. Montrul concludes that experience with
the language and mode of acquisition (through predominantly written input in second
language acquisition vs. oral input in heritage language acquisition) contribute to how the
language is processed and represented differently in the two types of learners. While
second language learners seem to do better on explicit tasks that maximize metalinguistic
knowledge, heritage speakers seem to do better on implicit tasks that minimize
metalinguistic knowledge. This issue is further developed and confirmed by Bowles (in
press) who used a battery of tests, ranging from more to less metalinguistic. Results
showed that second language learners of Spanish scored highest on a metalinguistic
knowledge test, whereas the heritage language learners scored the lowest on the
metalinguistic knowledge test and highest on the oral narration test (a less metalinguistic
task). Bowles' results confirm that heritage language learners seem to have more implicit
than explicit knowledge of their language by virtue of having acquired the language early
in childhood and in a naturalistic setting.
Keating, Jegerski and VanPatten (in press) investigated this same question with
structures subsumed under the syntax-discourse interface. They asked whether adult
Spanish heritage speakers and adult second language learners of Spanish utilize the same
antecedent assignment strategies as monolingually raised Spanish speakers when
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 75
processing overt vs. null subject pronouns and whether early exposure to and use of
Spanish confers advantages to Spanish heritage speakers relative to second language
learners. Spanish speakers raised without English contact, Spanish heritage speakers, and
second language learners of Spanish completed an off-line questionnaire comprised of
complex sentences such as Juan vio a Carlos mientras pro/él caminaba en la playa 'John
saw Charles while pro/he was walking on the beach'. Comprehension questions probed
participants' preferences regarding the antecedent of null and overt pronouns. The results
indicated that the monolingually raised Spanish speakers showed an antecedent bias, but
the heritage speakers and the second language learners did not. Furthermore, the two
experimental groups differed from the control group in different ways: the heritage
speakers displayed a stronger subject bias for the overt pronoun, whereas the second
language learners did not exhibit any clear antecedent biases. Keating et al.'s results
confirmed that heritage speakers differ from native speakers in their representation of
overt/null subjects in Spanish.
7 Broader implications of heritage language research
that heritage languages raise for linguistics and
In addition to all the questions
language acquisition, research on heritage speakers can be fruitful for other fields.
Heritage speakers form a significant student group in many colleges and universities.
Many of these students, particularly those at the lower proficiency level, sign up to study
their heritage language in a classroom setting. Some do it because of the sentiment to
maintain a connection with the parents’ cultural and, in some cases, religious heritage.
Others do it because some heritage languages are spoken in countries that are prominent
on the international political and economic scene, and students may see career
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 76
opportunities in maintaining and deepening their knowledge of their heritage languages15.
However, many colleges and universities have found themselves unprepared to deal with
this student group. These students usually enroll in the regular foreign language program
and find themselves in the same classroom with non-heritage learners who in many cases
may not have had prior exposure to the target language. This can create problems in the
classroom because the instructors need to find a way to accommodate different learners
with different backgrounds and abilities in relation to the target language.
Following the trend set by Spanish in the United States in the 1970s, some
colleges and universities have also started special tracks for heritage learners, although
the advantages or disadvantages of having different programs have not yet been
evaluated. This effort is the most advanced in the context of Spanish, though other
languages such as Arabic, Chinese, Russian and Vietnamese have also recently witnessed
efforts in that direction. However, proposals of different tracks for L2 learners and
heritage language learners are based on the underlying assumption that L2 learners and
heritage language learners are different linguistically, have different linguistic needs, or
may approach the language learning process in the classroom differently (Bowles, in
press; Montrul, in press). These are all empirical questions that are currently being
addressed in research, and whose answers will benefit tremendously and guide the
justification for and creation of adequate evidence-based language programs.
15 There are obviously heritage students who
may study the heritage language because they prefer not to contend with a completely new
language. Our experience indicates that this is not the dominant reason. Maintaining the
connection to their cultural heritage and career opportunities seem to be more important factors
(cf. Carreira & Kagan in press).
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 77
These emerging programs for heritage language learners, particularly for
languages other than Spanish, currently face tremendous challenges fueled by the scarcity
of instructional materials grounded in research on the linguistic profile of the heritage
learners. Instructors of heritage students usually provide more in-depth and culturally
richer materials at an accelerated pace, but the fact as far as we know is that there are no
materials that are based on research on the linguistic needs of heritage learners. This is
due to the fact that this research is in its infancy. Hopefully, more research on more
heritage learners will be carried out so that language instructors who are faced with the
difficult task of teaching heritage learners can approach their task with at least a better
understanding of this student population.
In addition to an understanding of potential differences and similarities between
heritage and non-heritage learners, instructors need to have a keen awareness and
appreciation of the vast linguistic diversity that characterize particular groups of heritage
language learners. Many heritage speakers enroll in heritage classes where the target
language is not their heritage language but a closely related language or dialect. For
example, in Standard Arabic classes it is typical to find a heritage speaker of an Arabic
dialect, say Egyptian or Iraqi, since the dialects are the heritage languages that they are
exposed to in the home. Any student of Arabic knows that there are significant
differences at the phonological, lexical, morphological and syntactic levels between
standard Arabic and the colloquial dialects, which has given rise to the diglossic situation
in the Arabic speaking world. In many ways, learning Standard Arabic is like learning a
third language for Arabic heritage speakers of different colloquial varieties. It would be
important to see whether non-heritage Arabic learners perform differently from heritage
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 78
Arabic learners and in which areas. As far as we know this research has not yet been
carried out. The same issues arise for Cantonese speakers who enroll in heritage Chinese
courses that teach Mandarin Chinese as the target language. This situation is typical in
heritage language programs, which tend to teach the standard variety spoken by a
particular subgroup of a given population.
If heritage language learners are placed in special language tracks because they
are assumed to be different from non-heritage language learners in terms of the linguistic
knowledge of the heritage language they already bring to class, an important research
question that arises in this context concerns the issue of transfer: Which language would
the heritage learner transfer from, English in the case of heritage learners in the US or the
heritage language spoken in the home? The first impression based on preliminary
research on Arabic at the University of Illinois seems to indicate that heritage learners do
not transfer from their spoken dialects when learning Standard Arabic but rather seem to
transfer from English. If this transfer is confirmed by further research, the inevitable
question is whether this is because English is psycholinguistically more dominant than
the heritage language; even though the latter is closely related to the target language, it is
not the most accessible target for transfer. However, the lay impression is also that
heritage speakers do better on the acquisition of a third language that is related to the
heritage language, which in turn suggests that the heritage grammar is accessible as a
source of knowledge. In short, this would be a very valuable topic to research in order to
better understand the interplay between the heritage language and the dominant language
and also to refine our understanding of the issue and process of language transfer.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 79
The communities where heritage languages are spoken are also increasingly
interested in promoting the teaching of their languages as a way to maintain connections
between the immigrant generations and their offspring. Furthermore, there is now a
prevalent sense that knowing a foreign language well can open additional career
opportunities for these children of immigrants and immigrant children. By developing
heritage language programs, colleges and universities can provide outreach sources to
establish connections with population groups with whom they may not have had any
significant contact.
It is unrealistic to expect research in this area to salvage heritage languages from
eventual disappearance beyond 3rdth and 4 generations of immigrants, because this is,
unfortunately, the natural course of immigrant languages and of language change more
generally. Nevertheless, we believe that research on this population still offers invaluable
social benefits. These include theoretically-informed and methodologically rigorous
studies that will educate the interested stake-holders (e.g., educational institutions and the
relevant communities) and will form the basis for evidence-based educational practices
and language policies.
There is also a whole untested area of psychological issues experienced by
heritage speakers. Just a generation or two ago, maintaining one’s heritage language was
not what was expected or considered important—popular movies such as Hester Street or
Zoot Suit, or even the more recent My Big Fat Greek Wedding, provide clear examples.
With ambivalence toward “otherness” always being very close to the surface, it is not
difficult to imagine that a bilingual child or adolescent becomes so eager to integrate into
the target culture that their own language or culture becomes a source of embarrassment,
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 80
or just gets ignored (Tse 1998; Cho et al. 2004). Even though bilingualism is much more
accepted and celebrated in the current social climate, otherness remains an issue.
Accepting bilingualism is easy and nowadays fashionable; practicing it is hard.
Many heritage speakers are happy to be considered bilingual and less happy to put more
effort in becoming bilinguals. Moreover, for many of them, especially the ones who excel
academically, it is painful to admit that they are less than excellent in their home
language, but that’s what they often hear. Family members and educators alike are much
too eager to emphasize the deficiencies of heritage speakers; constant criticism may
create a perception that one’s language is never good enough and that is not a great
motivation to keep it up. A remark by a heritage speaker is telling: "It is frustrating when
I'm speaking to my parents and can’t fully comprehend what we're trying to say to each
other. I hate it when I eat dinner with my parents [and] they always carry on their own
conversation that I can only half understand. Yet, they complain that we don't eat as a
family enough. I hate having something to say, but not being able to say it." (Cho et al.
2004: 7).
None of us is an expert on the psychological issues of high- or low-achieving
bilinguals, but we hope that this discussion may motivate further research at the
boundaries of linguistics and social psychology.
Conclusions
Research on heritage languages brings together several related fields of inquiry
that are regrettably not in the habit of talking to each other: theoretical linguistics, with its
emphasis on universal principles of language structure; experimental linguistics,
especially the study of comprehension, which stands to gain quite a bit from working
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 81
with not always responsive, but readily available, populations; first language acquisition,
which can compare happy and arrested development; and second language acquisition,
which allows us to compare heritage languages with both first and second languages.
Although we are only beginning to understand how heritage languages are
structured, the emerging patterns point to interesting differences between complete and
incomplete first language acquisition, as well as second language acquisition by heritage
speakers and foreign language learners. The defining characteristic of heritage speakers is
exposure to the heritage language in childhood, typically in the home and heritage
community context. From a language acquisition perspective, this means that heritage
speakers are usually exposed to the language during the critical period, unlike late L2
learners who also display variability in ultimate attainment but are exposed to the second
language after puberty. The standard assumption is that exposure to natural language
during the critical period (before puberty) should allow one to develop native-like
competence, but, as we have seen, heritage speakers do not develop uniform native-like
competence in all modules of the grammar. If there is a continuum of native-like
attainment, with L1 speakers on one end and L2 speakers on the other, heritage speakers
are in between, as schematically shown in Figure 4.
native
heritage
L1
speakers
speakers
Figure 4. Hypothetical continuum of native-speaker ability.
non-native
late L2 speakers
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 82
On a number of occasions throughout this paper, we emphasized that heritage
languages are still unchartered territory, but we would like to conclude on an optimistic
note, underscoring how much these languages have to offer linguistic theory. A parallel
that immediately comes to mind is with the study of creoles. Some forty years ago,
creoles were the domain of specific language study or sociolinguistics, and no respectable
linguist worth his/her stripes would go near them. As soon as linguists recognized that
creole phenomena speak directly to Plato’s problem in language16, creoles gained
visibility in linguistic theorizing. Heritage languages add yet another piece to the puzzle
of how a grammar can be acquired under minimal input.
Heritage languages also hold another attraction. Since the 1990s, linguists have
become increasingly aware that the study of language should no longer be solely the
prerogative of introspective investigation. Instead, language is something that can be
measured using standard experimental methods, and modeled on the basis of rigorously
established data. Nowhere is this paradigm shift more apparent than in studies of those
who can barely produce language: children, aphasics, and aging populations. Several
times in this survey article we have mentioned the fact that heritage speakers often have
problems with spontaneous production, which calls for the development of new methods
16 In the Socratic dialogue Meno, Socrates is
talking with an uneducated servant and shows that the servant knows the Pythagorean theorem
though he has never been explicitly taught any geometry. How could that be possible? Plato
suggests that people have innate knowledge of a number of concepts. In relation to language,
Plato’s Problem amounts to explaining how a child acquires language without explicit instruction
and through limited input. In heritage language acquisition (as in creole acquisition), the input is
particularly limited, so the shape of the resulting grammar is of special interest.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 83
to discover their linguistic knowledge. Unlike children or patients with speech disorders,
heritage speakers are easy to find, they are motivated and cooperative, and they may even
become active participants, not just experimental subjects, in the study of the extent of
their linguistic knowledge.
Finally, not only do heritage language speakers present us with a wonderful
linguistic challenge, they are also an untapped national resource. The globalization of the
world’s economy and, even more so, the political turbulence of the early twenty-first
century have brought new urgency to the need for corporate government and NGO
employees fluent in the languages and customs of the countries with which our nation has
political and economic ties. The knowledge possessed by heritage speakers puts them
years ahead of anyone studying the language from scratch. Thus it only makes sense to
give them the opportunity to develop further the abilities they already have. The
introduction of heritage language courses reflects the acknowledgment that heritage
language speakers are a very special group, and in some sense a severely underutilized
national resource; with proper instruction, they are much more likely than any second
language learner to achieve near-native linguistic and socio-cultural fluency.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 84
References Albirini, A., E. Benmamoun, and E. Saadah. In press. Grammatical features of
Egyptian and Palestinian Arabic heritage speakers’ oral production. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition 45. Aldridge, E. 2008. Generative approaches to syntactic ergativity. Language and
Linguistics Compass 2/5, 966–995. Anderson, R. 1999. Noun phrase gender agreement in
language attrition. Preliminary results. Bilingual Research Journal 23, 318–337. Anderson, R.
2001. Lexical morphology and verb use in child first language loss. A preliminary case study
investigation. International Journal of Bilingualism 5, 377–401. Aoun, J., E. Benmamoun, and
L. Choueiri. 2010. The syntax of Arabic. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Arkadiev, P.,
A. Letuchiy, N. Sumbatova, and Ya. Testelets (eds.). 2009. Aspekty polisintetizma: Ocerki po
grammtike adygejskogo jazyka. Moscow: Russian University for the Humanities. Au, T., L.
Knightly, S. Jun, and J. Oh. 2002. Overhearing a language during childhood. Psychological
Science 13, 238–243. Au, T., L. Knightly, S. Jun, J. Oh, and L. Romo. 2008. Salvaging a
childhood language. Journal of Memory and Language 58, 998-1011. Au, T. and J. Oh. 2005.
Leraning Spanish as a heritage language: The role of sociocultural background variables.
Language, Culture, and Curriculum 18, 229241. Baker, M. 2003. Lexical categories: Nouns,
verbs, and adjectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bar-Shalom, E. and E. Zaretsky. 2008. Selective attrition in Russian-English bilingual children:
Preservation of grammatical aspect. International Journal of Bilingualism 12, 281-302. Barton,
D. In preparation. Genericity in adult bilingual speakers of German and French. Bates, E., V.
Marchman, D. Thal, L. Fenson, P. Dale, J. S. Reznick, J. Reilly, and J. Hartung. 1994.
Developmental and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary. Journal of Child
Language 21, 85-124. Benmamoun, E. 1993. The status of agreement and the agreement
projection in Arabic. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences 23, 61–71. Benmamoun, E. 2000. The
feature structure of functional categories. New York: Oxford University Press. Benmamoun,
A., S. Montrul, A. Albirini, and E. Saddah. 2008. Agreement and plural features in Arabic
heritage speakers. Paper presented at the Second Summer Heritage Languages Institute, Harvard
University, June 22-27, 2008. Bhatt, R. 2005. Long-distance agreement in Hindi-Urdu. Natural
Language and Linguistic Theory 23, 757-807.
Bialystok, E., K. McBride-Chang, and C. Luk. 2005. Bilingualism, language proficiency, and
learning to read in two writing systems. Journal of Educational Psychology 97, 580-590.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 85
Birdsong, D. and M. Molis. 2001. On the evidence for maturational constraints in second
language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language 44, 235-249. Blake, R. 1983. Mood
selection among Spanish-speaking children, ages 4 to 12. The Bilingual Review 10, 21–32.
Bobaljik, J.D. 2008. Where's Phi? Agreement as a post-syntactic operation. In D. Harbour, D.
Adger and S. Béjar (eds.). Phi-Theory: Phi features across interfaces and modules, 295-328.
Oxford: Oxford University Press. Bobaljik, J.D. and P. Branigan. 2006. Eccentric agreement and
multiple case-checking. In A. Johns, D. Massam and J. Ndayiragije (eds.). Ergativity: Emerging
issues, 4778. Springer: Dordrecht. Bolonyai, A. 2007. (In)vulnerable agreement in incomplete
bilingual L1 learners. The International Journal of Bilingualism 11, 3–21.
Borer, H. and K. Wexler. 1987. The maturation of syntax. In T. Roeper and E. Williams (eds.).
Parameter setting, 123-172. Dordrecht: Reidel.
Borer, H. and K. Wexler. 1992. Bi-unique relations and the maturation of grammatical
principles. Natural language and Linguistic Theory 10, 147-189. Bowers, J. 1993. The syntax
of predication. Linguistic Inquiry 24, 591-656. Brehmer, B. and A. Czachor. 2010. A case
study of two groups of Polish-German
bilinguals. Paper presented at the Workshop on Incomplete Acquisition and
Language Attrition. Hamburg University, July 2010.
Broschart, J. 1997. Why Tongan does it differently: Categorical distinctions in a language without
nouns and verbs. Linguistic Typology 1, 123-166.
Campbell, R. and E. Sais. 1995. Accelerated metalinguistic (phonological) awareness in
bilingual children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 13, 61-68.
Carreira, M. and O. Kagan. In press. The results of the National Heritage Language Survey:
Implications for teaching, curriculum design, and professional development. Foreign Language
Annals. Carstens, V. 2001. Multiple agreement and case-deletion: Against F-(in)completeness.
Syntax 4, 147-163. Cho, G. and S. Krashen. 2000. The role of voluntary factors in heritage
language development: How speakers can develop the heritage language on their own. ITL:
Review of applied linguistics, 127-140.
Cho, G., F. Shin, and S. Krashen. 2004. What do we know about heritage languages? What do
we need to learn about them? Multicultural Education. Choi, H.-W. 2003. Paradigm leveling in
American Korean. Language Research 39, 183204. Chomsky, N. 1959 . Review of “Verbal
Behavior” by B.F. Skinner. Language 35, 26-58 Chomsky, N. 1965. Aspects of the theory of
syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Chomsky, N. 1980: Rules and Representations. New York:
Columbia University Press. Chomsky, N. 1981. Lectures on Government and Binding.
Dordrecht: Foris. Chomsky, N. 1986. Barriers. Camridge, MA: MIT Press. Chomsky, N. 1993.
A Minimalist Program for linguistic theory. In K. Hale and S. J. Keyser (eds.).The View from
Building 20,1-49. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Chomsky, N. 1995. The Minimalist Program.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 86
Chomsky, N. 2000. Minimalist inquiries: The framework. In R. Martin, D. Michaels, and J.
Uriagereka (eds.). Step by step: Essays on minimalist syntax in honor of Howard Lasnik, 89-155.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Chomsky, N. 2008. On phases. In R. Freidin, C. Otero, and M.-L.
Zubizarreta (eds.). Foundational issues in linguistic theory: Essays in honor of Jean-Roger
Vergnaud, 133-166. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Cho, G., Cho, K.S., and L. Tse. 1997. Why ethnic minorities need to develop their heritage
language: The case of Korean Americans. Language, Culture and Curriculum, 10, 106-112.
Clark, E. 2003 First language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Comrie,
B., G. Stone, and M. Polinsky. 1996. The Russian language in the twentieth century. Oxford:
Blackwell. Cook, V. 2003. The effects of the second language on the first. Clevedon, UK:
Multilingual Matters. Cook, V. and M. Newson. 2007. Chomsky's Universal Grammar: An
introduction. Oxford: Blackwell. Corbett, G. 1991. Gender. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press. Crain, S, and Nakayama, M. 1987. Structure dependence in children's language.
Language 62, 522-543. Crain, S. and R. Thornton. 1998. Investigations in Universal
Grammar. A guide to experiments on the acquisition of syntax and semantics. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Cummins, J. 2005. A proposal for action: Strategies for recognizing heritage language
competence as a learning resource within the mainstream classroom. The Modern Language
Journal 89, 585-592. Dabrowska, E. 1997. The LAD goes to school: A cautionary tale for
nativists. Linguistics 35, 735-766. Dabrowska, E. 2010. Naïve v. expert intuitions: An empirical
study of acceptability judgments. The Linguistic Review 27, 1-23. Davies, A. 2003. The native
speaker. Myth and reality. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters. de Groot, C. 2005. The grammars
of Hungarian outside Hungary from a linguistictypological perspective. In A. Fenyvesi (ed.).
Hungarian language contact outside Hungary, 351-370. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
de Leeuw, E., M. Schmid, and I. Mennen. 2010. The effects of contact on native language
pronunciation in a migrant context. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 13, 33-40. Delgado,
M. R. 2009. Spanish heritage language socialization practices of a family of Mexican origin.
Ph. D. Dissertation, University of Arizona. Demonte, V. 1995. Dative alternation in Spanish.
Probus 7, 5-30. Demuth, K and M. Kline. 2006. The distribution of passives in spoken Sesotho.
Southern African Linguistics and Applied Language Studies, 24, 377-388. Diessel, H. and
M.Tomasello. 2000. The development of relative clauses in spontaneous child speech. Cognitive
Linguistics 11, 131-151. Dorian, N. 1981. Language death: The life cycle of a Scottish Gaelic
dialect. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 87
Dorian, N. (ed.). 1989. Investigating obsolescence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Drai, D. and Y. Grodzinsky. 2006. A new empirical angle on the variability debate: quantitative
neurosyntactic analyses of a large data set from Broca's aphasia. Brain and Language 96, 117-28.
Dubisz, S. 1997. Jȩzyk polski poza granicami kraju—proba charakteristiki kontrastowej. In S.
Dubisz (ed.). Jȩzyk polski poza granicami kraju, 324-376. Opole: Uniwersytet Opolski. Dubisz,
S. 2001. Jȩzyk polski poza granicami kraju. In S. Gajda (ed.). Jȩzyk polski, 492514. Opole:
Uniwersytet Opolski. Elman, J. and E. Bates. 1996. Rethinking innateness. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press. Espiritu, Y. L. and D. Wolf. 2000. The paradox of assimilation: Children of Filipino
immigrants in San Diego. In R. Rumbaut and A. Portes (eds.). Ethnicities: Children of
immigrants in America, 157-186. Berkeley: University of California Press. Fassi Fehri, A. 1993.
Issues in the structure of Arabic clauses and words. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Fenson, L., Bates, E., Dale, P., Goodman, J., Reznick, J.S., and D. Thal. 2000. Measuring
variability in early child language: Don't shoot the messenger. Child Development 71, 323-328.
Fenyvesi, A. 2000. The affectedness of the verbal complex in American Hungarian. In A.
Fenyvesi and K. Sándor (eds.) Language contact and the verbal complex of Dutch and
Hungarian: Working papers from the 1st Bilingual Language Use Theme Meeting of the Study
Centre on Language Contact, November 11-13, 1999, 94107. Szeged, Hungary. Szeged: JGyTF
Press. Fenyvesi, A. (ed.). 2005. Hungarian language contact outside Hungary: Studies in
Hungarian as a minority language. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Ferguson, C.
1959. Diglossia. Word 15, 325-340. Fernandez-Soriano, O. 1999. Two types of impersonal
sentences in Spanish: Locative and dative subjects. Syntax 2, 101-140. Fitch, E. 2010. The
evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Fox, D. and Y. Grodzinsky.
1998. Children's passive: a view from the by-phrase. Linguistic Inquiry 29, 311-332. Franck, J.,
Lassi, G., Frauenfelder, U., and L. Rizzi. 2006. Agreement and movement: A syntactic analysis of
attraction. Cognition 101: 173-216. Friedmann, N. 2006. Speech production in Broca's
agrammatic aphasia: Syntactic tree pruning. In Y. Grodzinsky and K. Amunts (eds.), Broca’s
region, 63-82. New York: Oxford University Press. Galambos, S. and K. Hakuta. 1988.
Subject-specific and task-specific characteristics of metalinguistic awareness in bilingual children.
Applied Psycholinguistics 9, 141162. Gass, S. and L. Selinker. 1992. Language transfer in
language learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Gil, D. 2000. Syntactic categories, cross-linguistic variation, and Universal Grammar. In P.
Vogel and B. Comrie (eds.). Approaches to the typology of word classes, 173216. Berlin:
Mouton de Gruyter.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 88
Godson, L. 2003. Phonetics of language attrition: Vowel production and articulatory setting in
the speech of Western Armenian heritage speakers. Ph.D. dissertation, UCSD. Godson, L. 2004.
Vowel production in the speech of Western Armenian heritage speakers. Heritage Language
Journal 2. http://www.heritagelanguages.org. Goodluck, H., E. Guilfoyle, and S. Harrington.
2006. Merge and binding in child relative clauses: The case of Irish. Journal of Linguistics 42,
629-661. Green, G. and J. Morgan. 2005. Why verb agreement is not the poster child for any
formal principle. In S. Mufwene, E, Francis, and R. Wheeler (eds.). Polymorphous linguistics:
Jim McCawley's Legacy, 455-478. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. Guasti, M. T. 2002. Language
acquisition. The growth of grammar. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Guasti, M. T. and A. Cardinaletti. 2003. Relative clause formation in Romance child's
production. Probus 15, 47-89. Gupol, O. 2009. The acquisition of Russian verbal
morphosyntax in Russian-Hebrew bilingual children. Ph.D. dissertation, Bar-Ilan University.
Gürel, A. 2004. Selectivity in L2 induced L1 attrition. A psycholinguistic account. Journal
of Neurolinguistics 17, 53–78.
Gürel, A. 2007. (Psycho)linguistic determinants of L1 attrition. In B. Köpke, M. Schmid, M.
Keijzer, and S. Dosterst (eds.). Language attrition. Theoretical perspectives, 99–120.
Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Gvozdev, A.N. 1961. Voprosy izucenija detskoj reci. Moscow:
Nauka. Håkansson, G. 1995. Syntax and morphology in language attrition. A study of five
bilingual expatriate Swedes. International Journal of Applied Linguistics 5, 153– 171. Haugen, E.
1987. Blessings of Babel: Bilingualism and language planning. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Hayashibe, H. 1975. Word order and particles: A developmental study in Japanese. Descriptive
and Applied Linguistics 8, 1- 18. Holmberg, A. and C. Platzack. 1995. The role of inflection in
Scandinavian syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Hopp, H. and M. Schmid. Submitted.
Perceived foreign accent in L1 attrition and L2 acquisition: the impact of age of acquisition and
bilingualism. Hornstein, N. 2009. A theory of syntax: Minimal operations and Universal
Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hulsen, M. 2000. Language loss and
language processing. Three generations of Dutch migrants in New Zealand. Ph.D. dissertation,
University of Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
Hyltenstam, K., and N. Abrahamsson. 2003. Maturational constraints in SLA. In C. Doughty
and M. Long (eds.). The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition, 539–588. Oxford:
Blackwell. Iatridou, S. 1990. About Agr(P). Linguistic Inquiry 21, 551-577. Jarvis, S. 1998.
Conceptual transfer in the interlingual lexicon. Bloomington, IN: IULC Publications.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 89
Keating, G., Jegerski, J., and VanPatten, B. In press. Who was walking on the beach?
Anaphora resolution in Spanish heritage speakers and adult second language learners. Studies
in Second Language Acquisition. Khattab, G. 2002. VOT Production in English and Arabic
bilingual and monolingual children. In D. Parkinson and E. Benmamoun (eds.). Perspectives on
Arabic linguistics, 1-38. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Kim, J.-H. 2007. Binding interpretations
in Korean heritage speakers and L2 learners. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Illinois at
Urbana-Champaign.
Kim, J-H, Montrul, S. and Yoon, J. 2009. Binding interpretation of anaphors in Korean heritage
speakers. Language Acquisition 16, 1, 3-35.
Kim, J-H., Montrul, S. and Yoon, J. 2010. Dominant language influence in acquisition and
attrition of binding: Interpretation of the Korean reflexive caki. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition 13, 73-84. Knightly, L., S. Jun, J. Oh, and T. Au. 2003. Production benefits of
childhood overhearing. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 114, 465–474. Kondo-Brown,
K. 2009. Heritage background, motivation, and reading ability of upperlevel postsecondary
students of Chinese, Japanese, and Korean. Reading in a Foreign Language 21, 179-197.
Koopman, H. 2008. Samoan ergativity as double passivization. Ms., UCLA.
http://ling.auf.net/lingBuzz/000768 Koopman, H. and D. Sportiche. 1991. The position of
subjects. Lingua 85, 211-258. Kwon, N. and M. Polinsky. 2005. Heritage language retention:
A quantitative study.
Paper presented at the Symposium “Heritage Learners: Overcoming Curricular Challenges”, 25th
South Asian Language Analysis (SALA 25) Roundtable, University of Illinois,
Urbana-Champaign, IL. http://www3.ntu.edu.sg/home/nayoung/papers.htm Kupisch, T. and C.
Pierantozzi. 2010. Interpreting definite plural subjects: A comparison thof German and Italian
monolingual and bilingual children. In K. Franich et al. (eds.). 34 Boston University Conference
on Language development, 245-254. Boston: Boston University Press.
Laleko, O. 2008. Compositional telicity and Heritage Russian aspect. In M. Grosvald and D.
Soares (eds.). Proceedings of the Thirty-Eighth Western Conference on Linguistics (WECOL).
Vol. 19, 150-160. Laleko, O. 2010. The syntax-pragmatics interface in language loss: Covert
restructuring of aspect in Heritage Russian. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Minnesota. Lander,
Yu. And Ya. Testelets. 2006. Nouniness and specificity. Paper presented at “Universality and
particularity of part-of-speech systems”, University of Amsterdam. Lardiere, D. 2007. The
fossilized steady state in second language acquisition. Mawhaw, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Laskowski, R. 2009. Jezyk w zagroz eniu. Przyswajanie jezyka polskiego w warunkach
polsko-szwedzkiego bilingwizmu. Warsaw: Universitas.
Lee, J.-S. 2008. Korean heritage language education in the United States: The current state,
opportunities, and possibilities. Heritage Language Journal 6: 2.
Lee, S.-H., C. Gomez Gallo and M. Polinsky. In preparation. Lexical categories in heritage
Korean.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 90
Legate, J. 2008. Morphological and abstract case. Linguistic Inquiry 39, 55-101. Legate, J. and C.
Yang. 2002. Empirical re-assessment of stimulus poverty arguments. Linguistic Review 19,
151-162. Lipski, J. 1993. Creoloid phenomena in the Spanish of transitional bilinguals. In A.
Roca and J. Lipski (eds.). Spanish in the United States, 155–173. Berlin: Mouton. Lynch, A.
1999. The subjunctive in Miami Cuban Spanish. Bilingualism, contact and language variability.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota. Mack, M. 1997. The monolingual
native speaker. Not a norm, but still a necessity. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences 27, 113–146.
Mahajan, G. 2009. Heritage-Schmeritage: It’s their language. Paper presented at the
Second Language Acquisition Conference, San Diego, April 2009. Major, R.C. 1992.
Losing English as a first language. Modern Language Journal 76, 190208. Marantz, A. 2005. Generative linguistics within the cognitive neuroscience of
language. The Linguistic Review 22, 429-445. Martínez Mira, M. I. 2009. Spanish heritage
speakers in the southwest: Factors contributing to the maintenance of the subjunctive in
concessive clauses. Spanish in Context 6(1), 105-126. McCarthy, J. 1979. Formal problems in
Semitic phonology and morphology. Ph.D. Dissertation, MIT. McWhorter, J. 2007. Language
interrupted. Signs of non-native acquisition in standard language grammars. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Meisel, J. 1997. The acquisition of the syntax of negation in French and German. Contrasting
first and second language development. Second Language Research 13, 227–263. Mikulski, A.
M. 2010. Receptive volitional subjunctive abilities in heritage and traditional foreign language
learners of Spanish. The Modern Language Journal 94 (2), 217-233.
Montrul, S. 2002. Incomplete acquisition and attrition of Spanish tense/aspect distinctions in
adult bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 5, 39–68.
Montrul, S. 2004. Subject and object expression in Spanish heritage speakers. A case of
morpho-syntactic convergence. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 7, 125– 142. Montrul,
S. 2005. Second language acquisition and first language loss in adult early bilinguals, Exploring
some differences and similarities. Second Language Research 21, 199–249.
Montrul, S. 2006. On the bilingual competence of Spanish heritage speakers. Syntax,
lexical-semantics and processing. International Journal of Bilingualism 10, 37–69.
Montrul, S. 2007. Interpreting Mood distinctions in Spanish as a heritage language. In K.
Potowski and R. Cameron (eds.), Spanish Contact. Policy, Social and Linguistic Inquiries,
23–40. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Montrul, S. 2008. Incomplete acquisition in bilingualism.
Re-examining the age factor. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Montrul, S. 2009. Incomplete
acquisition of Tense-Aspect and Mood in Spanish heritage speakers. The International Journal
of Bilingualism 13, 239-269.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 91
Montrul, S. 2010. How similar are L2 learners and heritage speakers? Spanish clitics and word
order. Applied Psycholinguistics 31, 167-207.
Montrul, S., Bhatt, R., and Benmamoun, A. 2010. Morphological errors in Hindi, Spanish and
Arabic Heritage speakers. Invited colloquium on the linguistic competence of heritage speakers.
Second Language Research Forum, University of Maryland, October, 14-17, 2010.
Montrul, S. and Bowles, M. 2009. Back to basics: Differential Object Marking under incomplete
acquisition in Spanish heritage speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 3, 363–383.
Montrul, S. and M. Bowles. 2010. Is grammar instruction beneficial for heritage language
learners? Dative case marking in Spanish. Heritage Language Journal 7, 1, 4773.
http://www.heritagelanguages.org/
thMontrul, S., R. Foote, and S. Perpiñán. 2008a. Knowledge of wh-movement in Spanish L2
learners and heritage speakers. In M. Almazán, J. Bruhn de Garavito and E. Valenzuela (eds.),
Selected Papers from the 8 Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Montrul, S., R. Foote, and S. Perpiñan. 2008b. Gender agreement in adult second language
learners and Spanish heritage speakers: The effects of age and context of acquisition. Language
Learning 58, 3-54. Montrul, S. and T. Ionin. 2010. Transfer effects in the interpretation of definite
articles by Spanish heritage speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 449-473.
Montrul, S. and T. Ionin. In press. Dominant language transfer in Spanish heritage
speakers
and L2 learners in the interpretation of definite articles. The Modern Language Journal.
Montrul, S. and S. Perpiñán. In press. Assessing differences and similarities between instructed
L2 learners and heritage language learners in their knowledge of Spanish Tense-Aspect and Mood
(TAM) Morphology. Heritage Language Journal.
Munro, M. J., and T. M. Derwing. 1999. Foreign accent, comprehensibility, and intelligibility in
the speech of second language learners. Language Learning: A Journal of Research in
Language Studies 49, 285-310. Murasugi, K. and T. Kawamura. 2004. On the Acquisition of
Scrambling in Japanese. Language and Linguistics 5, 131-151. Myers-Scotton, C. 1993.
Duelling languages: Grammatical structure in code mixing. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Odlin, T.
1989. Language transfer: Cross-linguistic influence in language learning. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press. O’Grady, W. 1997. Syntactic development. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press. O’Grady, W. 2009. Assessing heritage language competence. Paper presented at
the Third Heritage Language Institute, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, June 2009.
www.nhlrc.ucla.edu/2010summer/readings/ogr ady.pdf O’Grady, W., M. Lee, and M. Choo.
2001. The acquisition of relative clauses by heritage and non-heritage learners of Korean as a
second language. A comparative study. Journal of Korean Language Education 12, 283–294.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 92
O’Grady, W., Kwak, H-K, Lee, M. and Lee, O-S. (in press). An Emergentist perspective on
partial language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition.
Oh, J., S. Jun, L. Knightly, and T. Au. 2003. Holding on to childhood language memory.
Cognition 86, B53-B64. Oller, D. and R. Eilers (eds.). 2002. Language and literacy in bilingual
children. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. Otsu, Y. 1992. Case marking and phrase structure. In B.
Lust, M. Suner, and J. Whitman (eds.). Syntactic theory and first language acquisition:
Cross-linguistic perspectives, 159-169. Mahwah, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum. Oyama, S. 1976. A
sensitive period for the acquisition of a non-native phonological system. Journal of
Psycholinguistic Research 5, 261–283. Paikeday, T. M. 1985. The native speaker is dead!
Toronto—New York: Paikeday. Paradis, M. 2004. A neurolinguistic theory of bilingualism.
Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Patowsky, M. 1980. The sensitivity period for the acquisition of a
second language in syntax. Language Learning 30, 449–472. Pavlenko, A. and S. Jarvis. 2002.
Bidirectional transfer. Applied Linguistics 23, 190-214. Pereltsvaig, A. 2005. Aspect lost, aspect
regained. In R. Slabakova and P. Kempchinski (eds.). Aspectual inquiries, 369–395. Dordrecht:
Kluwer. Pesetsky, D. 1999. Linguistic universals and Universal Grammar. In R. A. Wilson and F.
C. Keil (eds.). The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Pinker, S. 1994. The Language instinct: How the mind creates language. New York: Harper
Collins. Polinsky, M. 1997. American Russian: Language loss meets language acquisition. In W.
Browne et al. (eds.). Formal Approaches to Slavic Linguistics, 370-407. Ann Arbor, MI:
Michigan Slavic Publications. Polinsky, M. 2000. The composite linguistic profile of dpeakers of
Russian in the US. In O. Kagan and B. Rifkin (eds.). The learning and teaching of Slavic
languages and cultures, 437-465. Bloomington, IN: Slavica. Polinsky, M. 2005.. Word class
distinctions in an incomplete grammar. In D. Ravid and H. Bat-Zeev Shyldkrot (eds.).
Perspectives on language and language development, 419-436. Dordrecht: Kluwer. Polinsky, M.
2006. Incomplete acquisition: American Russian. Journal of Slavic Linguistics 14, 191-262.
Polinsky, M. 2008a. Heritage language narratives. In D. Brinton, O. Kagan and S. Bauckus
(eds.). Heritage language education. A new field emerging, 149–164. New York: Routledge.
Polinsky, M. 2008b. Russian gender under incomplete acquisition. Heritage Language Journal
6, 1.
http://www.heritagelanguages.org/ Polinsky, M. 2008c. Without aspect. In G. Corbett and M.
Noonan (eds.). Case and grammatical relations, 263-282. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Polinsky, M. In press. Reanalysis in adult heritage language: A case for attrition. Studies in
Second Language Acquisition. Polinsky, M. and O. Kagan. 2007. Heritage languages: In the
‘wild’ and in the classroom. Language and Linguistics Compass 1/5, 368-95.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 93
Polinsky, M. and E. Potsdam. 2001. Long-distance agreement and topic in Tsez. Natural
Language and Linguistic Theory 19, 583-646. Pollock, J.-Y. 1989. Verb movement, UG and the
structure of IP. Linguistic Inquiry 20, 365–424.
Potowski, K., Jegerski, J. and K. Morgan-Short. 2009. The effects of instruction on linguistic
development in Spanish heritage language speakers. Language Learning 59, 537-579. Prévost,
P. and L. White. 2000. Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language
acquisition? Evidence from Tense and Agreement. Second Language Research 16, 110–133.
Ravid, D. and R. Farah. 1999. Learning about noun plurals in early Palestinian Arabic. First
Language 19, 187-206.
Richards, M. 2007. On phases, phase heads and functional categories. Ms., U of Cambridge.
Rizzi, L. 1986. Null objects in Italian and the theory of pro. Linguistic Inquiry 17, 501557.
Rothman, J. 2007. Heritage speaker competence differences, language change, and input type:
Inflected infinitives in heritage Brazilian Portuguese. The International Journal of Bilingualism
11, 359–389. Rothman, J. 2009. Understanding the nature and outcomes of early bilingualism:
Romance languages as heritage languages. International Journal of Bilingualism 13, 211-238.
Saadah, E. 2010. Gender differences in VOT production of Arabic/English bilingual
children. Ms. University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
thSano, T., M. Endo, and K. Yamakoshi 2001. Developmental issues in the acquisition of
Japanese unaccusatives and passives. Proceedings of the 25 Boston University Conference on
Language Development, 668-683. Sasse, H.-J. 1991. Arvanitika: Die albanischen Sprachreste
in Griechenland. Wiesbaden: Harassowitz. Sasse, H.-J. 1992. Theory of language death. In M.
Brenzinger (ed.) Language death: Factual and theoretical explorations with special reference to
East Africa, 7-30. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Schachter, J. 1990. On the issue of completeness
in second language acquisition. Second Language Research 6, 93–124. Schmid, M. 2002. First
language attrition, use and maintenance: The case of German Jews in anglophone countries.
Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Schmid, M., B. Köpcke, M. Keijzer, and L. Weilemar (eds.).
2004. First language attrition: Interdisciplinary perspectives on methodological issues.
Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Schmidt, A. 1985. Young people's Dyirbal: An example of
language death from Australia. Cambridge--New York: Cambridge University Press. Schütze,
C. 1996. The empirical base of linguistics: Grammaticality judgments and linguistic
methodology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Schwartz, B. 2004. Why child L2
acquisition? In J. Van Kampen and S. Baauw (eds.). Proceedings of Generative Approaches to
Language Acquisition 2003, 47– 66.Utrecht, The Netherlands: LOT Occasional Series.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 94
Schwartz, B. and R. Sprouse. 1996. L2 cognitive states and the full transfer/full access
hypothesis. Second Language Research 12, 40–72.
Seliger, H. 1996. Primary language attrition in the context of bilingualism. In W. Ritchie and T.
Bhatia (eds.). Handbook of second language acquisition, 605-625. New York: Academic Press.
Seliger, H. and R. Vago (eds.). 1991. First language attrition. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press. Serratrice, L., Sorace, A., Filiaci, F. and Baldo, M. 2009. Bilingual children's sensitivity to
specificity and genericity: evidence from metalinguistic awareness. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition 12: 1-19.
Sheldon, A. 1974. The role of parallel function in the acquisition of relative clause in English.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 13, 272-81. Shlonsky, U. 1997.Clause structure
and word order in Hebrew and Arabic: An essay in comparative Semitic syntax. New York:
Oxford University Press Silva-Corvalán, C. 1994. Language contact and change: Spanish in Los
Angeles. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Silva-Corvalán, C. 2003. Linguistic consequences of reduced input in bilingual first language
acquisition. In S. Montrul and F. Ordóñez (eds.). Linguistic theory and language development in
Hispanic languages, 375–397. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. Slobin, D. I. (ed.). 1997. The
crosslinguistic study of language acquisition: Vol. 4. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Song, M., W. O’Grady, S. Cho, and M. Lee. 1997. The learning and teaching of Korean in
community schools. In Y.-H. Kim (ed.). Korean Language in America 2, 111– 127. American
Association of Teachers of Korean. thSorace, A. 2000. Differential effects of attrition in the L1
syntax of near-native L2 speakers. Proceedings of the 24 Boston University Conference on
Language Development, 719–725. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Sorace, A. 2004. Native language attrition and developmental instability at the syntaxdiscourse
interface: Data, interpretations, and methods. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 7, 143-145.
Sorace, A. 2005. Selective optionality in language development. In I. Cornips and K. Corrigan
(eds.). Syntax and variation: Reconciling the biological and the social, 55-80. Amsterdam: John
Benjamins.
Sorace, A. and Serratrice, L. 2009. Internal and external interfaces in bilingual language
development: Beyond structural overlap. International Journal of Bilingualism 13, 195-210.
Sprouse, J., and D. Almeida. 2010. A quantitative defense of linguistic methodology. Ms. UC
Irvine. Stowell, T. 1995. Remarks on clause structure. In A. Cardinaletti and M.T.Guasti (eds.).
Syntax and semantics, vol. 28: Small clauses, 271-286. San Diego: Academic Press.
Tavakolian, S. 1978. The conjoined-clause analysis of relative clauses and other structures. In
H. Goodluck and L. Solan (eds.). Papers in the Structure and Development of Child Language,
37–83. Amherst, MA: UMass Graduate Linguistic Student Association.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 95
Taylor, W.L. 1953. Cloze procedure: A new tool for measuring readability. Journalism
Quarterly 30, 415–433.
Thal, D., E. Bates, J. Goodman, and J. Jahn-Samilo. 1997. Continuity of language abilities in
late- and early-talking toddlers. Developmental Neuropsychology 13. 239-273. Thal, D., E.
Bates, M. J. Zappia, and M. Oroz. 1996. Ties between lexical and grammatical development:
Evidence from early-talkers. Journal of Child Language 23. 349-368. Tomasello, M. 2003.
Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Tse, L. 1998. Ethnic identity formation and its implications for heritage language development. In
Krashen et al. (eds.). Heritage language development, 15-29. Culver City: Language Education
Associates. Tsimpli, I. and M. Dimitrakopoulou. 2007. The interpretability hypothesis: evidence
from wh-interrogatives in second language acquisition. Second Language Language Research 23,
215–242.
Ullman, M. 2001. The neural basis of lexicon and grammar in first and second language: The
declarative/procedural model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 4, 105– 122. Unsworth, S.
2005. Child L2, adult L2, child L1: Differences and similarities. A study on the acquisition of
direct object scrambling in Dutch. Ph.D. Dissertation, Utrecht University. Valdés, G. 2000.
Introduction. Spanish for Native Speakers, Volume I. AATSP Professional Development Series
Handbook for teachers K-16. New York, NY: Harcourt College. de Villiers, J. G., Tager
Flusberg, H. B., Hakuta, K., and M. Cohen. 1979. Children’s comprehension of relative clauses.
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 8, 499518.
Wang, X. (ed.) 1996. A view from within: A case study of Chinese heritage community language
schools in the United States. Washington, DC: The National Foreign Language Center. Watson,
J. 2007. The phonology and morphology of Arabic. New York: Oxford University Press.
Weger-Guntharp. 2006. Voices from the margin: Developing a profile of Chinese heritage
language learners in the FL classroom. Heritage Language Journal 4 (1). White, L. 1989.
Universal Grammar and second language acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. White, L.
2003. Second language acquisition and Universal Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press. White, L. and F. Genesee. 1996. How native is near-native? The issue of ultimate
attainment in adult second language acquisition. Second Language Research 12, 238–265.
Williams, E. 1980. Predication. Linguistic Inquiry 11, 203-238. Woolford, E. 2006. Lexical case,
inherent case and argument structure. Linguistic Inquiry
37, 111–130.
HERITAGE LINGUISTICS 96
Xiao, Y. 2006. Heritage learners in the Chinese language classroom: Home background.
Heritage Language Journal 4.
http://www.heritagelanguages.org/ Zaliznjak A.A. 1968. Russkoe imennoe slovoizmenenie.
Moscow: Nauka. Zapata, G., L. Sánchez, and A. J. Toribio. 2005. Contact and contracting
Spanish. International Journal of Bilingualism 9, 377–395. Zeller, J. 2006. Agreement and the
EPP in Kinyarwanda applicatives. In P. Brandt and E. Fuss (eds.). Form, structure, and
grammar, 275-295. Berlin: Akademie. Zhou, M. 2000. Straddling different worlds: The
acculturation of Vietnamese refugee
children. In In R. Rumbaut and A. Portes (eds.). Ethnicities: Children of
immigrants in America, 187-227. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Download