MANAGING VOLUNTEERING BEHAVIOUR: THE DRIVERS OF DONATIONS PRACTICES IN RELIGIOUS AND SECULAR ORGANISATIONS Madalena Eça de Abreu Instituto Universitário de Lisboa (ISCTE-IUL), UNIDE, Lisboa, Portugal (mabreu@iscac.pt) Raul Laureano Instituto Universitário de Lisboa (ISCTE-IUL), UNIDE, Lisboa, Portugal (raul.laureano@iscte.pt) Sharifah Faridah Syed Alwi (correspondence author) Brunel University London, UK (Sharifah.Alwi@brunel.ac.uk) Rui Vinhas da Silva Instituto Universitário de Lisboa (ISCTE-IUL), UNIDE, Lisboa, Portugal (Rui.Vinhas.Silva@iscte.pt) Pedro Dionísio Instituto Universitário de Lisboa (ISCTE-IUL), UNIDE, Lisboa, Portugal (pedro.dionisio@iscte.pt) 1 MANAGING VOLUNTEERISM: THE DRIVERS OF DONATIONS PRACTICES IN RELIGIOUS AND SECULAR ORGANISATIONS ABSTRACT The present research deals with donations practices and the extent to which drivers of donations practices contribute to volunteerism. The paper aims to deepen the understanding of the relationship between volunteerism and key drivers in donating behavior and donations practices, thus allowing charities to pursue more efficient ways in which to elicit volunteer work as well as better manage fundraising practices. It is argued that gender, age, religious affiliation, compassion, altruism, egoism, and religiosity impact on the level of volunteerism of the donor. A sample of 612 Portuguese donors was selected from within a population of donors that donate regularly to charitable institutions. The key findings of the current study are grounded on the idea of interaction between the volunteering behavior of a donor and both his level of religiosity and his religious affiliation. With regard to other drivers of donations practices, it was found that both altruism and compassion are positively correlated with donor volunteerism. KEYWORDS Volunteerism management, volunteerism, donations practices, pro-social behavior, religiosity 2 INTRODUCTION Much attention has been paid to questions addressing volunteering as a vital behavior in a society (Allison et al., 2002, Fisher and Ackerman, 1998, Wilson, 2000, Briggs et al., 2009, Becsi and Balasubramanian, 2008, Burns et al., 2006), as an honorable tradition of community service (Horn, 2012, Cornelis et al., 2013, Eckstein, 2001). Since early history, volunteering behavior has been a powerful driving force in the betterment of humanity, a key contributor to the solving of the problems of mankind. Overall, different types of institutions and organizations have always depended on volunteering work as a vital resource for the accomplishment of their objectives. Moreover, amongst other issues that are of the utmost interest to charitable organizations, including how much people are willing to donate, what are the profiles of donors, and which charitable causes donors tend and want to support, charitable donations and donations behavior in general has been thoroughly researched (Bennett and Sargeant, 2003, Ranganathan and Henley, 2008, Webb et al., 2000). Charities rely heavily on donations as sources of income: volunteerism and donations, or time and money are highly appreciated. Current research claims that time and money go hand in hand (Clain and Zech, 1999, Bekkers and Theo, 2008, Lee and Chang, 2007), and that they are complementary from the viewpoint of charities and their resource management strategies (Bekkers, 2010). It is believed that time and money, are therefore required to be considered in tandem by charitable institutions. Truth be told, recruiting volunteers and obtaining donations are seen nowadays as vital activities for every organization pursuing its mission in a responsible way. Thus fundraising management (Hoge et al., 1999) and volunteer management (Penner et al., 2005), demanding such a solid strategy by general management. This study has considered different aspects when researching two types of pro-social behavior: volunteerism and donations practices (Abreu, 2012). Volunteerism and donations practices are behaviors that emanate from citizens in general and have been under deep scrutiny from the academic literature for some time, with a recurrent emphasis on the origin and the exploration of different drivers that can work as explanatory schemata for the occurrence of these behaviors. 3 Nevertheless, and although there is ample literature on donations practices, studies centered on the impact of different drivers of pro-social behavior on volunteerism can be considered to be few and far between. Such key issues as the relationship between people’s volunteerism and donations practices combined with their level of religiosity or altruism is drawing more attention nowadays in the literature (Becsi and Balasubramanian, 2008), which prompts increasing necessary responses. This paper aims to be just one such contribution. A deeper understanding of volunteers’ life choices, attitudes, interests, pro-social behaviors and motivations is indeed required in order to attain a better and more useful knowledge that better aids volunteering causes and contributes to the survival of charitable institutions. Better and more in-depth knowledge of donations practices and donor behavior can aid the charitable organization with obvious gains to society at large. This paper looks into volunteerism and donations practices: an analysis of the impact of the donor’s volunteerism into donations’ practices is conducted, which aims to uncover the relationship between volunteerism in different donors and their donations practices. On this issue different studies have attempted to illustrate this relationship (Peloza et al., 2009, Lee and Chang, 2007, Gregory, 2006a) and its usefulness for organizations (Abreu, 2012, Becsi and Balasubramanian, 2008). The key research question that is elicited here addresses the impact of different drivers of pro-social behavior on volunteerism. Other questions that are worthy of investigation are: Is it advisable to consider the joint impact of different aspects whilst observing the level of voluntarism? Can we use different characteristics of the donor as proxies for determining who is more likely to be a volunteer? With all of this in mind, this paper aims to make salient these gaps in the literature, put together some relevant propositions and subsequently proceed to offer the results of a survey, its analyses and discussion as well as presenting some worthwhile conclusions derived from empirical scrutiny. VOLUNTEERISM AND PRO-SOCIAL BEHAVIOUR Volunteerism is considered a form of pro-social behavior (Wilson, 2000), as well as a widely acclaimed key aspect within pro-social behavior, i.e., an intentional and voluntary 4 behavior valued by society or a significant segment of it (Eisenberg, 1982, Penner et al., 2005, Piliavin and Charng, 1990). Pro-social behavior is considered as a broad sort of activities that benefit other people such as helping, sharing, altruistic behavior and sympathy, cooperation, giving, donating, and being sensitive and responsive, regardless of personal motivation that would justify that behavior (Eisenberg, 1982, Penner et al., 2005, De Concillis, 1993). Within this framework, volunteerism can be defined as a behavior involving a pro-social action in an organizational context, which is planned and that continues for an extended period (Penner et al., 2005). Moreover, volunteerism, giving, philanthropy or humanitarianism, have been applied more or less interchangeably, all terms mirroring the progression of the noble human tradition of helping the less fortunate though the work of organizations. The ones that are worried with social problems tend to be associated more with volunteering activities, and are the ones that engaged more regularly in civic compromise. And all sorts of people (Plagnol and Huppert, 2010) are contributing their time and energy in assisting others in a variety of ways. Not surprisingly, volunteering has also been recognized as a source of well being to the volunteer himself (Binder and Freytag, 2013), and is believed to have a positive impact on the lowering of the incidence of serious human conditions including morbidity and mortality (Poulin, 2013). Moreover, volunteerism is increasingly gaining value at the organizational context. Likewise, the so-called ‘employer-supported volunteering’ (Booth et al., 2009) makes a positive contribution to the relationships between employee and employer, both gaining with regards to the volunteering activity. This situation fosters benefits both at an individual level, for example increasing job meaningfulness (Rodell, 2013), and at an organization level (Jessica, 2013). Corporate volunteerism is indeed an advantage for the corporation due to its image and reputation improvement and to its effect on employer motivation (Booth et al., 2009). The array of benefits of volunteerism for society has been acknowledged by different sources (McCallum et al., 2013, Binder and Freytag, 2013, Becsi and Balasubramanian, 2008), and is an important part of today´s economies, like the leveraging of social values by volunteerism activities (Agostinho and Paço, 2012). On this issue, both organizations and society show large gains within volunteerism. CRS initiatives with regards to the 5 trendy ‘employee skill-based volunteerism’ generates economic value for the company itself indeed, while at the same time, it originates a large benefit for the society (McCallum et al., 2013). It is also important to refer that, nowadays, there has been an expansion in the number of people volunteering for services in different charities, and also volunteering in the socalled third world, especially young people (Cornelis et al., 2013) and elderly retired people at a more local level. There is also a vivid discussion around whether an action can be considered volunteering, if and when it is remunerated in some form, and also if volunteering has a pre-determined intention, or even whether volunteering can be solely defined as an output (Wilson, 2000). DRIVERS OF VOLUNTEERISM Various disciplines have been actively engaged with charities in their volunteering activities and an understanding of motivation of volunteerism is an important debate (Bales, 1996, Withers et al., 2013, Bekkers and Theo, 2008, Gregory, 2006a, Clary and Snyder, 1999, Burns et al., 2006). That is to say, different studies have attempted to explain the reasons why people engage in volunteering activities (Cornelis et al., 2013, Clary and Snyder, 1995, Clary and Snyder, 1999, Agostinho and Paço, 2012, Gregory, 2006a). In fact, within the volunteerism and pro-social behavior literature, identifying and understanding the drivers behind these types of activities has been one of the most debated topics (Eisenberg, 1982): drivers for volunteerism are extremely important in uncovering volunteering behavior, understanding volunteers and attracting and retaining them sustaining organizations. In particular, one can say that the drivers of a specific behaviour are the set of processes and mechanisms that precede an action. The previous literature has offered different sets on how the types of drivers can be differentiated for this specific human behaviour (Allison et al., 2002). For instance, the main motivations for volunteering can be grouped into individual attributes and social influences (Wilson, 2000). As alluded to earlier, the literature has been overwhelming in explaining and classifying the drivers for volunteerism. Demographic attributes have been used as explanatory variables. Moreover demographic variables have been highly debated in volunteering 6 (Plagnol and Huppert, 2010). Age (Krause and Hayward, 2012, Knowles et al., 2013, Principia et al., 2012), for instance, has been under inspection, as volunteerism is often associated with older people (Taghian et al., 2012). Gender is another variable under scrutiny, but with various outcomes in relation to volunteerism (Wilson, 2000). Allison et al. (2002) have identified such motives as career, esteem, protection, social esteem or understanding as drivers for volunteerism. Within pro-social behavior at large, various terms have been debated, including compassion, implying feelings, like caring and loving, resulting sometimes in actions such as sharing, giving, serving, and supporting (Callahan, 1992). Compassion has been related with benefiting others (Shariff and Norenzayan, 2007, Batson et al., 1979, Callahan, 1992), and thus, associated with volunteerism. Altruism is conceptualized as a motivational state with the ultimate goal of increasing another’s welfare (Batson and Shaw, 1991) and a socalled type of pro-social behavior (Clary and Snyder, 1995, Hibbert and Horne, 1996, Hur, 2006, Andreoni, 1990, Magat, 1989, Bruce, 2005, Smith, 2003). Also, altruism has long been established as one of the motivations for giving to others and also heavily debated and correlated with volunteerism (Cooper et al., 2013). Moreover, egoism can also be tested for explaining levels of volunteerism (Horn, 2012). Lately, the understanding of the origin of altruistic motives to give has been taken up by different schools such as evolutionary theory and social psychology (Penner et al., 2005) or socio-biology (Pope, 1994). What is more, empathy has been probably the psychological mechanism most used in explaining altruistic motivation (Bennett and Sargeant, 2003, Webb et al., 2000), hence, a source of donations practices and volunteerism. Batson (1987) further advances the empathy-altruism hypothesis for investigating this phenomenon. The empathy-altruism hypothesis identifies three different paths to helping (Batson and Shaw, 1991): two egoistic paths and one altruistic path, being it incorporated into the present research design. The idea of people helping other people also as part of their religious duties and obligations has been historically acknowledged, and religious topics have been identified as the most important drivers for volunteering (Tienen et al., 2010, Bekkers and Theo, 2008, Cnaan and Boddie, 2001, Krause and Hayward, 2012, Saroglou et al., 2005). Different constructs have been used with regards to this topic including religious 7 affiliation, religiousness, or religiosity. Surprisingly, or not, diverse studies have reported to some extent different results. Religiosity is been found having a positive relationships with formal volunteering (Parboteeah et al., 2004). Conversely, Tienen et al. (2010) concluded that volunteering can be enhanced via a so-called informal spirituality, whilst religious practice and church attendance are more related with formal volunteering. Einolf (2013) claims that spirituality is significant as a predictor of volunteerism, irrespective of attendance of a particular church. But Bekkers and Theo (2008) have found that church attendance is a key indicator for volunteerism, especially for volunteerism inside the church. Currently some authors argue for a deeper analysis of the direct or indirect influence that religion has on people’s volunteering behavior, especially in European countries that are characterised by a strong decline in religious participation (Bekkers and Theo, 2008). DRIVERS OF DONATIONS PRACTICES In simple terms, one can say that the ‘donation’ is the monetary gift by individual donors to an organization (Reistma, 2007, Sargeant and Woodliffe, 2007) and often characterized as a sub-type of pro-social behavior (Burnett and Wood, 1988). Moreover, the marketing literature has been focusing mainly on two issues around donations practices: the value or level of giving and the likelihood of giving (Sargeant and Woodliffe, 2007). Donations practices are extensively covered by the literature of the marketing discipline (Sargeant &Woodliffe, 2010). Nevertheless donations research has also been approached through other areas such as the social psychological literature (Bekkers & Wiepking, 2010) or the exchange paradigm (Belk and Coon, 1993). Donations, analyzed as a process, have also been examined under a wider literature including social exchange theory, symbolic interaction, equity theory and resource exchange theory (Burnett and Wood, 1988). Addressing the organizational side, donations practices have been extensively under scrutiny in the funding of charities (Hibbert et al., 2007, Bekkers and Wiepking, 2010, Bennett and Sargeant, 2003) and, like this, a vast amount of studies and reports around this 8 topic have emerged from the organizations´ side, and supplemented by academic insights (Bendapudi et al., 1996). A vast literature has also covered the drivers of donations practices (Abreu, 2012). Sargeant (1999) distinguishes between intrinsic and extrinsic determinants. Bekkers and Wiepking (2010) establish a set of eight categories determining donation behavior, e.g.,: (a) awareness of need, (b) solicitation, (c) costs and benefits, (d) altruism, (e) reputation, (f) psychological benefits, (g) values, and (h) efficacy. The explanation of the drivers of this behavior can also be found in philological schema as motivation, values and attitudes (Gregory, 2006a, Bekkers and Wiepking, 2010). The main reasoned influence, commonly referred to as the “reason why”, lies in the motivation (Gregory, 2006b). Apart from the motivation, donation behavior can be studied through other dimensions of reasoned influences (Watt and Maio, 2001), the most common being researched were attitudes and values. Therefore, reasoned influences (e.g., attitudes, values, involvement and motives), unreasoned influences (e.g., habitual giving), and situational influences (e.g., income, education, etc.) were all capable of explaining donations practices (Belk and Coon, 1993). In summary, there has been a myriad of studies attempting to understand the reason why people donate (Watt and Maio, 2001, Ribar and Wilhelm, 2002, Clary and Snyder, 1995). However, limited research captures the impact of the drivers of donation practices and volunteerism, which will be further explained below. GAPS AND CONCEPTUAL MODEL Consistent with the literature, the gaps identified are: i) the pattern involving volunteerism and religion stays complex and controversial; ii) there is a need for a deeper understanding of altruism and egoism motives as predictors of donations practices and their relationship with volunteerism; and iii) little is known about how the three types of donors, religious, religious but non-church goer, and secular, are similar or different with respect to their donation practices, considering their level of volunteerism. 9 The gaps that have been identified lead to an inquiry about the influence and interaction of different drivers on volunteerism, including age and gender, religious affiliation, religiosity, compassion, egoism, and altruism. The scrutiny of the literature has revealed the importance of volunteerism for charities and the importance of identifying donations drivers that along with some of the characteristics of individuals tend to be related to volunteerism(Abreu, 2012, Lee and Chang, 2007, Bekkers, 2010). Moreover, the literature is not unanimous about the role of religiosity regarding the explanation of volunteerism and, consequently, donations practices and religious behavior (Batson, 1976, Ji et al., 2006, Reistma et al., 2006). In this context this study aims to explore and test the role of religiosity and the relationship between the characteristics of individuals and their volunteerism level, leading to the following conceptual model (Figure 1) and propositions: Proposition 1: There is a significant effect of religiosity on the level of volunteerism. Proposition 2: There is a significant effect of gender and religiosity on the level of volunteerism. Proposition 3: There is a significant effect of age and religiosity on the level of volunteerism. Proposition 4: There is a significant effect of religious affiliation and religiosity on the level of volunteerism. Proposition 5: There is a significant effect of compassion and religiosity on the level of volunteerism. Proposition 6: There is a significant effect of egoism and religiosity on the level of volunteerism. Proposition 7: There is a significant effect of altruism and religiosity on the level of volunteerism. 10 Figure 1. Conceptual Model Gender Religiosity Age Religious affiliation Volunteerism Compassion Egoism Altruism METHODOLOGY Data was collected for the purposes of the current research by means of a survey instrument conducted next to donors into Portuguese charities. At the beginning, all Portuguese residents were considered as the target population as it was thought that all of them (respondents) have the will and are able to contribute to a charitable donation. Moreover, these respondents are accessible as they could be easily accessed by the charities as charities keep the records of their donors. Later, we found out that the database of the Portuguese donors were nonexistent, hence, it is not possible to draw a fair or representative sample of this population. Due to this limitation the option seen as fair was sending the questionnaires through the charities, and asking them to send the questionnaire to the donors on their databases, so as to maximize the respondents and reaching the individual donor. Thus, our respondents for this study were the individual donors registered in charity databases. The charities chosen for the current study were based on five groups following Franco´s (2005) characterization of the Portuguese charities landscape: (1) Foundations; (2) Holy houses of Mercy (3) Nongovernmental organizations of cooperation for development; (4) Associations plus Private Institutions of Social 11 Solidarity (IPSS); (5) Museums. Moreover, this sample was considered sufficiently large since the list of charities obtained was considered to be a quasi total of the total number of charities in Portugal with an e-mail account. Therefore, a large scale questionnaire was delivered to these participating organizations on the 3rd of February of 2010, and it stayed on-line until the 9th of April of the same year. This approach was taken with a view to capturing a sample that would be representative of the average donor and also to avoid the possible biases that could possibly arise from directly surveying donors. Six hundred and twelve donors to Portuguese charities participated in this study, 66.3% of them female and aged between15 and 77 years old (mean 36.8 years old with a standard deviation of 11.2 years). Taking into consideration religious affiliation, the sample comprises 51.4% religious, 28.3% non-church goer and 20.3% secular donors. Thus, despite the sample selection not being random, it is possible to conclude that the diversity of the population´s characteristics has been incorporated into the study sample. To measure volunteerism De Concillis´ (1993) framework of validated items was used. A 5-point Likert scale was used with extreme points ranging from “totally disagree” to “totally agree”, the uni-dimensionality of the scale was tested with Principal Components Analyses (PCA) and a Cronbach’s Alpha of 0.824 was obtained. The scale scores that were obtained were the item averages. The same procedure was followed for religiosity (Reistma, 2007), compassion (De Concillis, 1993), altruism and egoism (Batson and Shaw, 1991) scales. Table 1 presents all the scales Cronbach’s Alpha, suggesting a good level of reliability. For a better interpretation of the results these scales were re-coded in two categories: low for a value equal or less than 3 (center point of the scale) and high for values above 3. Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the scales used Scale Volunteerism Religiosity Compassion Egoism Altruism Cronbach’s Alpha 0.824 0.971 0.723 0.860 0.837 The seven propositions were analyzed with 2-way ANOVA, a statistical procedure to analyze the influence of two categorical independent variables (the drivers of 12 volunteerism) on one continuous level dependent variable (the volunteerism level). This statistical procedure has three assumptions (Laureano, 2012): i) the samples must be independent; ii) the populations from which the samples were obtained must be normally or approximately normally distributed; and iii) the variances of the populations must be equal. All of them were verified, except for the analysis of the interaction between compassion and religiosity where the variances are significantly different (Levene(4;608)=4.931;p=0.002). Each proposition from 2 to 7 is corroborated if the differences in the mean level of volunteerism for two levels of one volunteerism driver are not the same across all levels of the second volunteerism driver. In this case, the interaction between the two drivers is significant. RESULTS Tables 1 presents the descriptive statistics for the scales used, namely, volunteerism (as dependent variable), religiosity as moderator factor, and compassion, egoism and altruism as independent variables. Volunteerism (mean 2.49) is lower as a type of pro-social behavior rather than compassion (mean 3.92). Religiosity displaces high values. The mean level of altruism (3.37) is much higher than for egoism (1.73) suggesting altruism as a much stronger driver for donation practices. Table 2. Descriptive statistics of the scales used Scale Number Mean Standard Deviation Minimum Median Maximum Volunteerism 612 2.49 0.81 1.00 2.40 5.00 Religiosity 612 3.50 1.28 1.00 3.90 5.00 Compassion 612 3.92 0.74 1.33 4.00 5.00 Egoism 612 1.73 0.51 1.00 1.67 3.89 Altruism 612 3.37 0.92 1.00 3.50 5.00 13 The perception of religiosity is related with the level of perceived volunteerism, with the mean level of volunteerism higher when the donor has higher religiosity (mean=2.60; standard deviation =0.82) then when he or she has low religiosity (mean=2.24; standard deviation=0.74), and this difference is significant for 0.05 level (t(610)=5.052; p<0.001). This conclusion is also supported by all 2-way ANOVA tests where religiosity, as main effect, is found significant in all of them. The results of the different 2-way ANOVA performed are presented in the six profile plot (interaction plot) where the points represent the volunteerism mean values for each combination of two levels of the two drivers plotted (see Figures 2 to 7). The results allowed for the conclusion that the interaction between the drivers religious affiliation and religiosity is significant (F(2;606)=2.726; p=0.066, η2=0.009). In fact, secular donors vary from low to moderate voluntarism level depending on which level of religiosity they have. Seculars with low level of religiosity have, on average, a volunteerism level of 2.23 (standard deviation=0.72) but if their religiosity is high than the mean level of volunteerism is higher (mean=3.02; standard deviation=0.96). With previous results we can state that propositions 1 and 4 are corroborated and the remaining five are not. Although, despite just one interaction effect was found, three significant main effects were significant, not including the religiosity main effect as just analyzed: a) the effect of religious affiliation (F(2;606)=3.341; p=0.036, η2=0.011) as religious donors have higher perceived volunteerism level (mean=2.64; standard deviation=0.81) than those that are non church goer (mean=2.33; standard deviation=0.79) or secular (mean=2.30; standard deviation=0.78), being the differences between these two groups not significant as showed by the Scheffé post hoc test (p=0.946); b) the effect of altruism (F(1;608)=30.492; p<0.001, η2=0.048) as donors with high level of altruism have higher perceived volunteerism level (mean=2.63; standard deviation=0.81) than those that have low level of altruism (mean=2.33; standard deviation=0.75); c) compassion effect (assumptions of the test are not verified) as donors with high level of compassion have higher perceived volunteerism level (mean=2.59; standard 14 deviation=0.81) than those that have low level of compassion (mean=1.91; standard deviation=0.58). Figure 2. Perceptions of volunteerism Figure 3. Perceptions of volunteerism under two levels of religiosity and gender under two levels of religiosity and age Figure 4. Perceptions of volunteerism Figure 5. Perceptions of volunteerism under two levels of religiosity and three under two levels of religiosity and two levels of religious affiliation levels of perceived compassion 15 Figure 6. Perceptions of volunteerism Figure 7. Perceptions of volunteerism under two levels of religiosity and two under two levels of religiosity and two levels of perceived egoism levels of perceived altruism DISCUSSION The values for volunteerism (see Table 2) are low, with the exception of the value for the secular donors, when their religiosity is also high. In contrast, the values for compassion, the other type of pro-social behavior, are much higher. This probably occurs because volunteering costs time for the donor, it is not a remunerated task (Wilson, 2000), and is therefore a more difficult type of pro-social behavior to be administered by a charity. Religiosity, both by its main effect, and while combined with religious affiliation (interaction effect), does have a positive relation with the level of volunteerism of the donors. This is also in line with other studies (Bekkers and Theo, 2008) although for the purposes of this paper volunteering was only applied as a general construct: Tienen et al. (2010) found that spirituality positively impacts on informal volunteering, and religious attendance impacts on related formal volunteering. The demographic characteristics at stake, gender and age, are not associated with voluntarism, and the interaction effect of religiosity is not found, and thus propositions 2 and 3 are not corroborated. As a matter of fact, different outcomes have been found for the relationship between gender and voluntarism, or no relationship whatsoever (Winterich et 16 al., 2013) ; on the other hand, getting older has been highly appraised as a condition for volunteerism (Principia et al., 2012, Plagnol and Huppert, 2010, Krause and Hayward, 2012, Knowles et al., 2013). This study showed that there is a slightly tendency for older people displacing higher values of volunteerism, but this difference is not significant. Compassion displays a direct effect on volunteerism, a result also found in previous studies (e.g. Shariff and Norenzayan, 2007, Batson et al., 1979). Egoism is not related with voluntarism, and, this relation has been discussed in other studies where egoistic values are shown as competing with pro-social values and behavior (Horn, 2012). The literature suggests that altruism can indeed make a difference in volunteerism (Batson and Shaw, 1991) and this study has also revealed that altruism does have a positive effect the other way around, that is the interaction effect of altruism and religiosity was not found. CONCLUSION The aim of this study was to conduct an analysis and develop an understanding of the relationship between different drivers of volunteerism, in line with a perceived gap in the literature. There was also an added purpose of the current research in analyzing the influence of religiosity on volunteerism. The present study looks at seven different types of drivers of volunteerism: age, gender, altruism, egoism, compassion, religious affiliation, and religiosity. As far as one can tell, research papers applying these particular drivers and mixing demographic traits, attitudinal intentions, and religious variables, are few and far between and to be more rigorous none were identified. Grounded on a large scale, this study indicates that religiosity, altruism, and compassion, can indeed enhance the volunteerism level of the donor. Besides, religiosity positively influences the volunteerism of a donor, in conjunction with the religious affiliation of the donor. This conjunction can make a valuable contribution to those practitioners involved in fundraising campaigns. Indeed, it was said that there is a large tendency, when an organization is looking for volunteers, like participating in a fundraising campaign, to address a group of religiously affiliated persons highly engaged within the community and with an active participation in the religious services. And also the organization can increase 17 the activities for their affiliates to be involved and, in this way, augment their incomes from donations, and also count these members as more likely volunteers and donors for future campaigns. In addition, charities can look at the joint impact of religious affiliation and religiosity: higher levels of religiosity for the ones who are not religious can increase their level of volunteerism. This outcome can also be useful when addressing people to volunteer specifically into secular organizations. Indeed, these results provide insights and a way for charities to obtain competitive advantages and achieving their mission and goals. Most importantly, it aids the mission’s aim to gain volunteers and to raise donations. The implications are important to fundraising managers as this study demonstrates that religiously affiliated donors can be approached both by religious and secular organizations. In relation to the fact that religious donors are willing to help different types of organizations, we advise these organizations to strengthen their ties and efforts and work more in synergy: the network of religious organizations can be an important issue to be managed. Moreover, both altruism and compassion are important drivers for volunteerism and organizations, in general, can tailor their messages in their campaigns by addressing messages or images focusing behaviours addressing these issues. As an example, making use of compassion can originate the intensifications of the drama reported in the news by the organization. Like most studies, the present study suffers from limitations. First of all, the sample was not random but rather purposive because of an absence of national databases of donors in Portugal. Also, the sample was one of Portuguese donors, and there could be different results in response to questions of religion or volunteering if the questionnaires were administered elsewhere in Europe; and moreover people that are volunteers and not donors have been inquired. Besides, future research should take into consideration the use of other measurement scales. In spite of the evidence found, the most striking contrast with the existing literature is that age as a demographic characteristic is no indicator of volunteerism. Perhaps this is due to cultural differences between Portugal and other European countries and the United States, where older people tend to be are the highest volunteering group, but this was not found to be the case in southern European countries (Plagnol and Huppert, 2010). This is 18 therefore an issue that can be the subject of further scrutiny in subsequent studies. Another opportunity for forthcoming research is to seek a deeper understanding of the intricate relationship between different sorts of volunteering (formal and informal), religiosity and religious affiliation. REFERENCES Abreu, M. E. d. (2012), ‘Drivers of Donations Practices: Altruism and Religiosity Revisited’, PhD, ISCTE – IUL (University Institute of Lisbon), Lisbon. Agostinho, D., & Paço, A. (2012), ‘Analysis of the motivations, generativity and demographics of the food bank volunteer’, International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing, Vol. 17, No. 3, pp. 249–261. Allison, L. D., Okun, M. A., & Dutridge, K. S. (2002), ‘Assessing Volunteer Motives: A Comparison of an Open-ended Probe and Likert Rating Scale’, Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, No. 12, pp. 243–255. Andreoni, J. (1990), ‘Impure Altruism and Donations to Public-goods - A Theory of Warm-glow Giving’, Economic Journal, Vol. 100, No. 401, pp. 464-477. Bales, K. (1996), ‘Measuring the Propensity to Volunteer’, Social Policy & Administration, Vol. 30, No. 3. Batson, C. D. (1976), ‘Religion as Prosocial: Agent or Double Agent?’ Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, Vol. 1, No. 15, pp. 29-45. Batson, C. D. (1987), ‘Pro-social motivation - Is it ever altruistic?’Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, No. 20, pp. 65-122. Batson, C. D., Harris, A. C., McCaul, K. D., Davis, M., & Schmidt, T. (1979), ‘Compassion and compliance - alternative dispositional attributions for ones helping behavior’, Social Psychology, Vol. 42, No. 4, pp. 405-409. Batson, C. D., & Shaw, L. L. (1991), ‘Evidence for Altruism: Toward a Pluralism Of Prosocial Motivation’, Psychological Inquiry, Vol. 2, No. 2, pp. 107-122. Becsi, T. P., & Balasubramanian, S. K. (2008), ‘Predicting Volunteering Behavior: Influences of Social Networks and Organizations’, Attitudes toward Society and Observations of Helping Behavior Paper presented at the Society for Marketing Advances Proceedings. 19 Bekkers, R. (2010), ‘Who gives what and when? A scenario study of intentions to give time and money’, Social Science Research, Vol. 39, No. 3, pp. 369-381. Bekkers, R., & Theo, S. (2008), ‘And who is your neighbor? Explaining denominational differences in charitable giving and volunteering in the Netherlands’, Review of Religious Research, Vol. 1, No. 50, pp. 74-96. Bekkers, R., & Wiepking, P. (2010), ‘A Literature Review of Empirical Studies of Philanthropy: Eight Mechanisms That Drive Charitable Giving’, Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly, Vol. 40, No. 5, 924-973. doi: 10.1177/0899764010380927 Belk, R. W., & Coon, G. S. (1993), Gift Giving as Agapic Love: An Alternative to the Exchange Paradigm Based on Dating Experiences, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 20, No. 3, pp. 393-417. Bendapudi, N., Singh, S. N., & Bendapudi, V. (1996), ‘Enhancing helping behavior: An integrative framework for promotion planning’, Journal of Marketing, Vol. 60, No. 3, pp. 33-49. Bennett, R., & Sargeant, A. (2003), ‘The nonprofit marketing landscape: guest editors' introduction to a special section’, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 58, No. 6, pp. 797805. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2003.12.001 Binder, M., & Freytag, A. (2013), ‘Volunteering, subjective well-being and public policy’, Journal of Economic Psychology, No. 34, pp. 97-119. Booth, J. E., Park, K. W., & Glomb, T. M. (2009), ‘Employer-supported volunteering benefits: Gift exchange among employers, employees, and volunteer organizations’, Human Resource Management, Vol. 48, No. 2, pp. 227–249. Briggs, E., Peterson, M., & Gregory, G. (2009), ‘Toward a Better Understanding of Volunteering for Nonprofit Organizations: Explaining Volunteers' Pro-Social Attitudes’, Journal of Macromarketing, Vol. 30, No. 1, pp. 61-76. doi: 10.1177/0276146709352220 Bruce, I. (2005), Charity Marketing - Meeting Need Through Customer Focus, London: ICSA Publishing Limited. Burnett, J. J., & Wood, V. R. (1988), ‘A proposed model of the donation decision process’, Research In Consumer Behavior, No. 3, pp. 1-47. Burns, D. J., Reid, J. S., Toncar, M. T., Fawcett , J., & Anderson, C. (2006), 2Motivations to volunteer: The role of altruism’, International Review on Public and Nonprofit Marketing, Vol. 3, No. 2, pp. 79-91. Callahan, K. L. (1992), Giving and Stewardship In an Effective Church. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 20 Clain, S. H., & Zech, C. E. (1999), ‘A household Production analysis of religious and charitable activity’, American Journal of economics and sociology, pp. 923-946. Clary, E. G., & Snyder, M. (1995), ‘Motivation for volunteering and giving: a functional approach’, In C. H. Hamilton & W. F. Ilchman (Eds.), How heritage, gender, wealth, and values influence philanthropy (Vol. Cultures of Giving II, pp. 111-123), California: JosseyBass Publishers. Clary, E. G., & Snyder, M. (1999), The motivations to volunteer: Theoretical and practical considerations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, Vol. 8, No. 5, 156-159. Cnaan, R. A., & Boddie, S. C. (2001), ‘Philadelphia census of congregations and their involvement in social service delivery’, Social Service Review, Vol. 75, No. 4, pp. 559580. Cooper, S. B., Cripps, J. H., & Reisman, J. I. (2013), ‘Service-Learning in Deaf Studies: Impact on the Development of Altruistic Behaviors and Social Justice Concern’, American Annals of the Deaf, Vol. 157, No. 5, pp. 413-427 Cornelis, I., Van Hiel, A., & De Cremer, D. (2013), ‘Volunteer work in youth organizations: predicting distinct aspects of volunteering behavior from self- and otheroriented motives’, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 43, No. 2, pp. 456-466. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2013.01029.x De Concillis, A. (1993), Individual Differences Correlate of Prosocial Behavior: A Comparison of Three Models. Ph.D. Thesis, Loyola College, Baltimore. MD. Eckstein, S. (2001), ‘Community as Gift-Giving: Collectivistic Roots of Volunteerism’, American Sociological Review, Vol. 66, No. 6 Einolf, C. J. (2013), ‘Daily Spiritual Experiences and Prosocial Behavior’, Social Indicators Research , No. 110, pp. 71-87. Eisenberg, N. (Ed.), (1982), The development of prosocial behavior. New York: Academic Press. Fisher, R. J., & Ackerman, D. (1998), ‘The Effects of Recognition and Group Need on Volunteerism: A Social Norm Perspective’, Journal of Consumer Research, No. 25, pp. 262-275. Franco, R. C., Sokolowski, W., Haire, E., & Salamon, L. M. (2005), ‘The Portuguese notfor-profit sector In comparative perspective’, In J. H. U. Universidade Católica Portuguesa (Ed.), Lisbon. Portugal. Gregory, G. (2006), ‘The Act of Giving: Involvement, Habitual Giving, and Motives of Volunteerism’, Advances in Consumer Research – Working papers, Vol. 33, No. 1, pp. 286-288. 21 Hibbert, S., & Horne, S. (1996), ‘Giving to charity: questioning the donor decision process’, Journal of Consumer Marketing, Vol. 13, No. 2, pp. 1-13. Hibbert, S., Smith, A., Davies, A., & Ireland, F. (2007), ‘Guilt appeals: Persuasion knowledge and charitable giving’, Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 24, No. 8, pp. 723-742. doi: 10.1002/mar.20181 Hoge, D. R., Zech, C., McNamara, P., & Donahue, M. J. (1999), ‘Giving In Five Denominations’, In M. Chaves & S. L. Miller (Eds.), Financing American Religion (Vol. 3), United States: Altamira Press. Horn, A. S. (2012), ‘The Cultivation of a Prosocial Value Orientation Through Community Service: An Examination of Organizational Context, Social Facilitation, and Duration’, Journal of Youth and Adolescence, Vol. 41, No. 7, pp. 948-968. Hur, M. H. (2006), ‘Exploring the Motivation Factors of Charitable Giving and their Value Structure: A Case Study of Seoul, Korea’, Social Behavior and Personality, Vol. 34, No. 6, pp. 661-680. Ji, C.-H. C., Pendergraft, L., & Perry, M. (2006), ‘Religiosity, Altruism, and Altruistic Hypocrisy: Evidence from Prootestant Adolescentes’, Review of Religious Research, Vol. 48, No. 2, pp. 156-178. Knowles, S. R., Hyde, M. K., & White, K. M. (2013), ‘Predictors of Young People's Charitable Intentions to Donate Money: An Extended Theory of Planned Behavior Perspective’, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Vol. 42, No. 9, pp. 2096-2110. Krause, N., & Hayward, R. D. (2012), ‘Volunteer Work in the Church Among Older Mexican Americans’, Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, Vol. 18, No. 3, pp. 277–284. Laureano, R. M. S. (2012), Testes de hipóteses com o SPSS: o meu manual de consulta rápida. Lisboa: Edições Sílabo. Lee, Y.-K., & Chang, C.-T. (2007), ‘Who Gives what to charity? Characteristics affecting donation behavior’, Social Behavior and Personality, Vol. 35, No. 9, pp. 1173-1180. Magat, R. (1989), Philanthropic Giving. New York: Oxford University Press. McCallum, S., Schmid, M. A., & Price, L. (2013), ‘CSR: a case for employee skills-based volunteering’, Social Responsibility Journal, Vol. 9, No. 3, pp. 479-495. Parboteeah, K. P., Cullen, J. B., & Lim, L. (2004), ‘Formal volunteering: a cross-national test. Journal of World Business’, Vol. 39,No. 4, pp. 431-441. Peloza, J., Hudson, S., & Hassay, D. N. (2009), ‘The Marketing of Employee Volunteerism’, Journal of Business Ethics, Vol. 85, No. 2, pp. 371-386. 22 Penner, L. A., Dovidio, J. F., Piliavin, J. A., & Schroeder, D. A. (2005), ‘Prosocial Behaviour: Multilevel Perspectives’, Annual Review of Psychology, No. 56, pp. 365-392. Piliavin, J. A., & Charng, H.-W. (1990), ‘Altruism: A review of recent theory and research’, Annual Review of Sociology, No. 16, pp. 27-65. Plagnol, A. C., & Huppert, F. A. (2010), Happy to Help? Exploring the Factors Associated with Variations in Rates of Volunteering Across Europe. Soc Indic Res , No. 97, pp. 157– 176. Pope, S. J. (1994), The evolution of altruism and the ordering of love. Washington D.C.: Georgetown University Press. Poulin, M. J. (2013), ‘Volunteering Predicts Health Among Those Who Value Others: Two National Studies’, Health Psychology. Retrieved from doi:10.1037/a0031620 Principia, A., Chiattia, C., Lamuraa, G., & Frerichs, F. (2012), ‘The Engagement of Older People in Civil Society Organizations’, Educational Gerontology, Vol. 32, No. 2, pp. 83106. Ranganathan, S. K., & Henley, W. H. (2008), ‘Determinants of charitable donation intentions: a structural equation model’, International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing, Vol. 13, No. 1, pp. 1-11. doi: 10.1002/nvsm.297 Reistma, J. (2007), Religiosity and Solidarity - dimensions and relationships disentangled and tested. Published doctoral dissertation. Ph.D. Thesis, Radboud Universiteit, Nijmegem, The Netherlands. Reistma, J., Scheepers, P., & Te Grotenhuis, M. (2006), ‘Dimensions of individual religiosity and charity: Cross-national effect differences in European countries?’ Review of Religious Research, Vol. 47, No. 4, pp. 347-362. Ribar, D. C., & Wilhelm, M. O. (2002), ‘Altruistic and Joy-of-Giving Motivation in Charitable Behavior’, Journal of Political Economy, Vol. 110, No. 2, pp. 425- 457. Rodell, J. B. (2013), ‘Finding meaning through volunteering: Why do employees volunteer and what does it mean for their jobs?’ Academy of Management Journal, Vol. 56, No. 5, pp. 1274-1294. Sargeant, A. (1999), ‘Charitable Giving: Towards a Model of Donor Behaviour. Journal of Marketing Management’, Vol. 15, No. 4, pp. 215-238. doi: 10.1362/026725799784870351 Sargeant, A., & Woodliffe, L. (2007), ‘Gift giving: an interdisciplinary review’, International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing, Vol. 12, No. 4, pp. 275-307. doi: 10.1002/nvsm.308 23 Saroglou, V., Pichon, I., Trompette, L., Verschueren, M., & Dernelle, R. (2005), ‘Prosocial behavior and religion: new evidence based on projective measures and peer ratings’, Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, Vol. 44, No. 3, pp. 323-348. Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2007), ‘God id watching you’, Psychological Science, Vol. 18, No. 9, pp. 803-809. Smith, D. H. (2003), ‘Paved with good intentions’, New Directions for Philanthropic Fundraising, No. 42, pp. 109-116. Taghian, M., D’Souza, C., & Polonsky, M. (2012), ‘A Study of Older Australians’ Volunteering and Quality of Life: Empirical Evidence and Policy Implications’, Journal of Nonprofit & Public Sector Marketing, No. 24, pp. 101–122. Tienen, M., Scheepers, P., Reitsma, J., & Schilderman, H. (2010), ‘The Role of Religiosity for Formal and Informal Volunteering in the Netherlands’, VOLUNTAS: International Journal of Voluntary and Nonprofit Organizations, Vol. 22, No. 3, pp. 365-389. doi: 10.1007/s11266-010-9160-6 Watt, S. E., & Maio, G. R. (2001), Why people donate to charity: a social psychological perspective (C. U. UK, Trans.) Report for the National Council for Voluntary Organizations: Cardiff University. UK. Webb, D. J., Green, C. L., & Brashear, T. G. (2000), ‘Development and validation of scales to measure attitudes influencing monetary donations to charitable organizations’, Journal of the academy of marketing science, Vol. 28, No. 2, pp. 299-309. Wilson, J. (2000), Volunteering. Annual Journal Soc. No. 26, pp. 215–240. Winterich, K. P., Mittal, V., & Aquino, K. (2013), ‘When Does Recognition Increase Charitable Behavior? Toward a Moral Identity-Based Model’, Journal of Marketing, Vol. 77, No. 3, pp. 121-134. Withers, M., Browner, C. H., & Aghaloo, T. (2013), ‘Promoting Volunteerism in Global Health: Lessons from a Medical Mission in Northern Mexico’, Journal of Community Health, Vol. 38, No. 2, pp. 374-384. 24