The effects of individual differences and visual instructional aids on disorientation, learning performance and attitudes in a Hypermedia Learning System Abstract Research suggests that certain visual instructional aids can reduce levels of disorientation and increase learning performance in, and positive attitudes towards, HLS for learners with specific individual differences. However, existing studies have looked at only one or two individual differences at a time, and/or considered only a small number of visual instructional aids. No study has considered the impact of the three most commonly studied individual differences – cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience – on learning performance, disorientation and attitudes in a HLS incorporating a full range of visual instructional aids. The study reported here addresses this shortcoming, examining the effects of, and between, these three individual differences in relation to learning performance, disorientation and attitudes in two HLS versions: one that incorporated a full set of visual instructional aids and one that did not. Significant effects were found between the three individual differences with respect to disorientation, learning performance and attitudes in the HLS that provided no instructional aids, whereas no such effects were found for the other HLS version. Analysis of the results led to a set of HLS design guidelines, presented in the paper, and the development of an agenda for future research. Limitations of the study and their implications for the generalizability of the findings are also presented. Keywords: Hypermedia Learning; Individual Differences; Visual Support; Disorientation, Learning Performance; Attitudes 1. Introduction Hypermedia Learning Systems (HLS) are being increasingly used in Higher Education [55] to support students’ access to learning material in a flexible way. A defining feature of HLS is that the learning material is presented using a non-linear structure [29], allowing students to determine their own path through the material [4]. Allowing learners to decide the sequence in which they encounter 1 the learning content has been suggested to offer improvements in learning and cognitive flexibility [53]. However, some users have difficulty in navigating through HLS to find the information that they need and, as a result, experience disorientation [6]. A consequence of disorientation is that learners can miss at least some of the relevant content in the system, which may hinder their learning performance [36]. Performing less well may lead to these learners having a negative attitude towards HLS and also to have less interest in learning using these types of learning system [22]. Research findings suggest that not all learners are comfortable using, satisfied with or learn effectively from HLS, implying that the value of HLS varies depending on the individual and may be influenced by various characteristics of the learner [26]. This means that the individual differences that these characteristics represent become important when designing and developing HLS. A range of studies have looked at the relationship between individual differences and student learning in HLS, with the results tending to show that individual differences, particularly cognitive style (most often examined in terms of Field Dependence/Field Independence), domain knowledge and computer experience, influence learners’ levels of disorientation, learning performance and attitudes in HLS. Though widely studied individually or in pairs, the influence of, and relationships between, all of these individual differences in relation to disorientation, learning performance and attitude have not been looked in a single study. It is argued that supporting individual differences in HLS to reduce learners’ disorientation would be helpful, as it can improve learning performance and increase learning satisfaction [51]. A common way to reduce disorientation is to provide instructional guidance, in the form of visual navigational aids (e.g., maps) and a set of visual cues (e.g., breadcrumbs, highlighting of context, pagination and so on), within the HLS. The relationship between individual differences and such instructional guidance has been explored in many studies, yet there are no studies that have integrated a map and these visual cues in a single HLS. This paper reports a study that seeks to address these gaps in the research literature through by examining the effects of three individual differences (cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience) on disorientation, learning performance and attitudes in HLS with and without instructional guidance in the form of visual navigational aids and a set of visual cues. The paper begins by reviewing relevant literature related to individual difference and HLS use, in order to identify and to frame three research questions that will address these gaps. The paper then sets out the methodological 2 approach to exploring the research questions, introducing the empirical study, its research design, sample, the range of materials and instruments used, and the detailed experimental procedure. The paper then presents the results of the study and analyses the resulting data in order to provide answers to the three research questions. The paper ends by framing a set of design guidelines from the analysis of the data and by setting out directions for future work. 2. Background As the World Wide Web (WWW) becomes ever more widely used as an educational platform [55], HLS are gaining increased attention from researchers [5, 31]. One of the major reasons for moving from traditional classroom-based learning to offering instruction through the use of HLS is that the latter can present learning material in a non-linear structure [10]. Such non-linearity affords learners greater flexibility in navigating the learning content and allows them to choose their own paths through it to meet their learning goals [4]. Additionally, non-linearity allows learners to access and sequence information in accordance with their individual needs [25]. Furthermore, allowing learners to have control over their learning may also make them motivated to learn, improving their learning performance and cognitive flexibility [53]. However, the flexibility offered by HLS may cause problems for some users. It has been argued that not all users can ‘develop’ their own learning paths effectively to achieve their learning goals when using HLS [27], and many studies have shown that users may experience disorientation – reflected as questions of ‘where am I?’, ‘where have I been?’ and ‘where can I go next?’ – when seeking to navigate through HLS [6]. Some studies have suggested that learners who encounter higher levels of disorientation may, in turn, perform less well in learning tasks [36]. Separately and in combination, disorientation and poorer performance in learning tasks may have an impact on learners’ attitudes towards HLS. Dringus [22], for example, argues that when learners experience disorientation in HLS, and are hindered in their learning performance, there have an increased chance of showing negative attitudes towards the nonlinear learning environment. As a result, such learners may feel less motivation to learn using HLS. The range of reactions to being given freedom in terms of navigation may be explained by the different characteristics that learners possess, meaning that the individual differences that these characteristics represent are critical for effective HLS’ design. It has been suggested that individual 3 differences such as cognitive style [15], domain knowledge [8], and computer experience [55] are the most commonly studied in research related to student learning and HLS use. Each of these individual differences will be briefly introduced in the context of research into HLS. The ways in which an individual thinks, memorizes, perceives, organizes, processes and presents information is often referred to as cognitive style [47]. Among the different dimensions of cognitive style that have been studied to date, Field Dependence (FD) and Field Independence (FI) are often argued to be of interest, especially with respect to research that is related to HLS [12]. FI learners tend to rely on internal references, adopt an active approach to learning and process information using an analytical approach. Conversely, FD learners tend to rely on external references, adopt a passive approach to learning and accept information in exactly the way it is presented to them [17, 58]. Witkins et al. [58] Group Embedded Figure Test (GEFT) and Riding’s [46] Cognitive Style Analysis (CSA) are two common instruments used to identify a learner’s cognitive style. The research related to cognitive style and HLS suggests that FI learners: prefer being given high levels of freedom of navigation; experience less disorientation in HLS; perform well in learning tasks; and have a positive attitude towards HLS compared to FD learners [16, 32, 54]. One explanation of FI learners’ performance in HLS is that they are able to follow a restructuring approach more easily because they are internally directed and tend to adopt an active approach to learning; and they can extract relevant items from within the complex context offered within complex content, such as that offered in HLS, because they are more analytical. In contrast, FD learners prefer to follow the structure of the learning material because they are externally-directed and tend to adopt a passive approach to learning; they also have difficulties extracting the relevant items within the complex context because they are less analytical [17, 59]. Research into domain knowledge, mostly suggests that domain knowledge influences the degree of disorientation and learning performance in HLS, with low domain knowledge (novice) learners experiencing higher levels of disorientation and performing less well in HLS than high domain knowledge (expert) learners [7, 23, 35, 57]. A general conclusion drawn in studies in this area to explain this finding is that, novices are unfamiliar with the subject content, which makes it difficult for them to impose a meaningful conceptual structure on the content compared to experts [13]. In terms of the research related to computer experience, studies suggest that compared with those with lower levels of computer experience, learners with high levels of computer experience: prefer the 4 non-linear pathways that are normally offered in HLS; navigate effectively; take fewer steps to reach the information they need in the tutorial; browse more pages and are able to reach more detailed levels of the subject content; enhanced their time efficacy; and overall, perform well in learning tasks in the HLS [30, 37, 52, 55]. It is argued that this is because a well-developed understanding of different computer applications enables these learners to better navigate through the HLS to achieve their learning goals. In contrast, the lack of skills related to the use of computers and their applications makes it difficult for those with low levels of computer experience to successfully navigate through HLS to find the information that they need [40, 52]. It is suggested that reducing the levels of disorientation experienced by learners with different characteristics may improve their learning performance and, in turn, may show more interest in learning using HLS [12, 13]. To reduce learners’ disorientation, the use of instructional aids, such as maps which support visual navigational, has been suggested [5, 8, 13, 14], and a number of studies have examined the effects of both maps and individual differences (such as those considered in this paper) on learners’ levels of disorientation, learning performance and attitudes in HLS [3, 8, 33, 34, 45]. The results from these studies suggest that when maps are provided in HLS, learners’ levels of disorientation are decreased and learning performance and positive attitude are increased. However, these studies have not considered the effects of all of the three individual differences – cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience – in combination with maps in HLS. Rather, they have tended to examine the effects of only one or two of the individual differences presented in this study. Studies which examine the effect of maps in relation to a wider set of individual differences may help to determine whether maps reduce disorientation and increase learning performance and learning satisfaction for all users with different combinations of the individual differences, or whether maps create problems for specific groups. In addition to maps, previous studies suggest that visual cues – such as breadcrumbs [1, 44], graphic visualizations [39, 42], history-based mechanisms [24], context highlighting [20], page labels [18], different colors link [42] and link annotations [9] – can also reduce disorientation and enhance efficiency of learning performance and satisfaction in HLS [19, 49]. Despite the fact that these studies have explored the same set of individual differences considered in this paper and have considered a range of these visual cues, an important gap remains. There are no studies where cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience have been considered 5 together in relation to all of the visual cues identified earlier within a single HLS. This implies that the combined effects of these three individual differences and all of these visual cues on learners’ disorientation, learning performance and attitudes in HLS have not been fully examined. Though methodologically complex in terms of study design and subsequent analysis, addressing this gap may provide results which can help designers and developers to gain a better understanding of the relationships between HLS instructional aids (in the form of visual cues), individual differences and learner disorientation, performance and attitude. This leads to the framing of the three research questions addressed in the study reported in the remainder of this paper: Research Question 1: What are the effects of and between cognitive style (FD/FI), domain knowledge, and computer experience on learners’ levels of disorientation when using a HLS that includes a map and the defined set of visual cues and when using a HLS without any instructional aids? Research Question 2: What are the effects of and between cognitive style (FD/FI), domain knowledge, and computer experience on learners’ learning performance when using a HLS that includes a map and the defined set of visual cues and when using a HLS without any instructional aids? Research Question 3: What are the effects or and between cognitive style (FD/FI), domain knowledge, and computer experience on learners’ attitudes when using a HLS that includes a map and the defined set of visual cues and when using a HLS without any instructional aids? 3. Methodology This section describes the research methodology that was used to address the three research questions. An explanation of the research design is given, followed by a description of the sample, the materials and instruments used in the study, the experimental procedure and the approach to data analysis. 6 3.1 Research Design To answer the three research questions that were proposed, this study adopted an experimental research approach [48], in which a set of independent variables and dependent variables were identified and used. The independent variables were the HLS and the users’ individual differences (cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience). With respect to the HLS, two versions, one without instructional aids and one with visual instructional aids (in the form of a map and a set of visual cues) were required. The dependent variables in this experimental study were learning performance, disorientation and attitudes towards the HLS. A between-subjects design was used in this study, with one half of the sample using the HLS that provided no instructional aids and the other half of the sample using the HLS containing the set of visual instructional aids defined above. It is acknowledged that this research design is complex and has a wider range of variables than have been used in other studies in the field. This brings significant issues in terms of analysis of the data and the implications that can be drawn from them. These issues are discussed, and reflected on, in later sections of the paper. This study also aimed to gather detailed user information on individual differences, learners’ attitudes, feelings and preferences, their experience of disorientation, and their interaction behavior with respect to the HLS that they used. To support the experimental study, a descriptive study was also employed (using a qualitative approach), in which learners were observed, surveyed and interviewed [41, 48]. However, for reasons of space, this paper will consider only the experimental study and its results. 3.2 Sample The sample was drawn from university students across London, UK. University students were considered to be suitable participants because, as mentioned in sections 1 and 2, this study was concerned with research related to students and HLS in Higher Education. Recruitment of the sample was also supported by the existence of channels through which we were able to contact a large number of students at universities in the London region. Though the choice of university students as participants can be seen as part of a purposive sampling strategy, the restriction of the recruitment of participants to those from the London region introduce a ‘convenience’ characteristic to the sample 7 which is important to acknowledge, and which will be returned to in section 7 when the implications of the findings with respect to the study’s limitations, are discussed. 192 participants took part in the study: split equally between the HLS that provided no instructional aids and the HLS that contained the visual instructional aids. The participants were at undergraduate, postgraduate taught and postgraduate research levels and were registered on a range of courses at universities in the London region. They were informed about the experimental study through a discussion forum, notice boards in the Students’ Unions of universities across London and by word of mouth. Participants were also told that taking part in this study would help them to learn about, and to build their own web site, either for personal or business use, and that small incentives in the form of soft drinks, sweets and snacks would also be offered at the end of the practical study. Participants were tested for cognitive style (in stage 2, see section 3.4) and their levels of domain knowledge and computer experience were assessed (in stage 3, see section 3.4). This allowed the identification of participants with appropriate cognitive style types and experience profiles. Having secured the overall sample, participants were then assigned to one of the two HLS conditions (with and without instructional aids). The assignment of each individual to the HLS versions was random, but it was ensured that the sample was balanced in terms of the different experience profiles represented under each HLS treatment. There was an equal chance of an individual being assigned to each experimental condition (represented by the two versions of HLS), thus increasing the internal validity of this study. Table 1 presents a summary of the characteristics of the participants in the two versions of the HLS. In terms of gender, in the HLS that provided no instructional aids (version 1), 42 were male and 54 were female; in the HLS that provided visual instructional aids (version 2), 50 were male and 46 were female. With regards to age, the majority of the participants were between 18 and 23: 60 in version 1; and 71 in version 2. There were 19 participants aged between 24 and 29 (19 used version 1 and 14 used version 2); 20 were aged between 30 and 35 (12 used version 1; and eight used version 2); and eight were aged 36 or over (five used version 1 and three used version 2). <<Insert Table 1 here>> 8 Data on level of education were also gathered. For version 1, 52 participants were undergraduates, 27 were taught postgraduates (Masters) and 17 were research postgraduates (PhD). For version 2, 60 participants were undergraduates, 24 were taught postgraduates and 12 were research postgraduates. The participants were studying different subjects: business (18 in version 1; 11 in version 2); computing and mathematics (45 in version 1; 43 in version 2); engineering and design (7 in version 1; 14 in version 2); health sciences (10 in version 1; 10 in version 2); law (five in version 1;10 in version 2); and social sciences (11 in version 1; eight in version 2). A number of the participants in the study were registered as having dyslexia (10 who used version 1 and 14 who used version 2). Participants’ levels of domain knowledge, computer experience and cognitive style were also determined. In terms of domain knowledge, 96 participants were novice (defined as having low levels of domain knowledge) and 96 were expert (high levels of domain knowledge). With regards to computer experience, 96 had low levels of experience and 96 had high levels. Finally, in terms of cognitive style, 96 participants were FD and 96 were FI. The different experience profiles were assigned to the two HLS conditions to give a balanced sample in terms of these three individual difference types. 3.3 Materials and Data Collection Instruments To conduct the experimental study a range of materials and data collection instruments were used. The remainder of this section describes each of the materials and instruments. 3.3.1 The subject content of the HLS The two versions of the HLS were developed by the researchers for the sole purposes of this study and contained the same subject-based information as the learning content of the tutorial. The learning content covered core aspects of the Extensible Hypertext Mark-up Language (XHTML) and was structured into seven sections: (i) introducing XHTML; (ii) how to create, save and view XHTML documents; (iii) basic XHTML formatting: (iv) creating lists; (v) how to use images; (vi) how to insert links within a page; and (vii) how to create tables. 9 3.3.2 HLS with visual instructional aids A HLS incorporating visual instructional aids was designed and developed. The visual instructional aids incorporated into the HLS took the form of: a structural map; different colors link; breadcrumbs; pagination; page labels; annotation of links; graphic visualization; highlighting context; and history based mechanism. The ‘map’ (see Figure 1) helped the user to navigate through the tutorial to find the information that they needed in order to achieve their learning goals in three ways: it presented a global overview of the Extensible Hypertext Mark-up language (XHTML) learning content; it offered a representation of the relationships between the different information topics presented in the HLS; and it offered a document structure of the information presented in the HLS. The ‘different colors’ link (as shown in Figure 1) informed users about the pages that they had visited. This approach prevented them from repeatedly opening the same page they had visited earlier, hence, a save of time. <<Insert Figure 1 here>> The ‘link annotation’ cue (as shown in Figure 2) provided users with information about each link in relation to the XHTML tutorial. The ‘breadcrumb’ visual orientation cue (see Figure 2) helped users to identify their current location in the HLS and the path that had led them there. The ‘pagination’ visual cue (as shown in Figure 2) allowed users to know how many pages there were in the section, the page that they were currently on; the number of pages that they had viewed in the section; and the number of pages left to view. The ‘page labels’ visual cue, in the form of headings and sub-headings (see Figure 2), helped users to locate particular information in the tutorial. <<Insert Figure 2 here>> The ‘graphic visualization’ cue (as shown in Figure 3) showed a global overview of the information that was presented in the HLS, and offered a conceptual structure of the information presented in the HLS. The ‘highlighting context’ visual cue that was provided in the graphic visualization (as shown in Figure 3) showed disabled links as differently colored nodes, with the accompanying text provided in different font sizes, styles and colors. Using the ‘highlighting context’ approach allowed users to be 10 alerted to: (i) their current position in the HLS; and (ii) their position in relation to the overall system structure. <<Insert Figure 3 here>> The ‘history-based mechanism’ (as shown in Figure 4) allowed users to view and access the last two visited pages. With the use of this visual cue, the chance of unintentionally opening previously visited nodes was reduced. <<Insert Figure 4 here>> 3.3.3 HLS without instructional aids As part of this experimental study, a HLS without instructional aids was also designed and developed, offering a non-linear structure that could be flexibly navigated, allowing users to set their own learning paths in order to meet their learning goals (see Figure 5). In this version of the system only one navigation support mechanism was provided – an index (as shown in Figure 6) which showed an alphabetic list of the system’s links in relation to the XHTML content. The users were able to access the links in any order. The same learning content – a tutorial on Extensible Hypertext Mark-up Language (XHTML) – was used for both versions of the HLS. The tutorial comprised seven lessons, each with a minimum of three and a maximum of seven sections. <<Insert Figure 5 here>> <<Insert Figure 6 here>> 3.3.4 Cognitive Style Analysis (CSA) To identify learners’ cognitive styles, Riding’s [42] Cognitive Styles Analysis (CSA) was employed. The CSA includes two sub-tests. In the first sub-test, learners were presented with items containing pairs of complex geometrical figures from which they had judge whether the figures were similar or different. In the second sub-test, learners had to indicate whether a simple figure was contained within a complex one. The first sub-test required field-dependent capacity whereas the second sub-test required field independence capacity. Following Riding’s [42] classification, FM (Field Mixed), FD and FI users were identified with the following scores: those who scored less than 1.02 were classed as FD and those who scored 1.36 and above were classed as FI. Those falling 11 between 1.03 and 1.35 were classed as Field Mixed (FM); only FI and FD users were included in the analysis. 3.3.5 Pre-test and Post-test Questions, and Practical Task Sheet To determine the effects of the HLS’ visual instructional aids on participants’ learning performance (in the form of declarative and procedural knowledge), a set of instruments were used. In line with the approach taken in other studies in the field (for example, [11]; [23]; [36]; [37]; [56]), pre-test and posttest instruments were respectively applied to measure the participants’ declarative knowledge of the learning material (XHTML) prior to and after the use of the prescribed HLS. Given the specific learning material addressed in the HLS, the pre- and post-test instruments had to be developed specifically for this study. Each test contained 20 multiple-choice questions, with each question having four possible answers and a “I do not know” option. Participants were asked to circle the answer that they thought was correct. The answers to all of the questions were available in the tutorial. Ensuring that the pre-test and post-test questions were comparable was achieved by either rewriting the question and providing the possible answers in a different order or, where appropriate, by substituting different numbers or variables into the questions. To assess procedural knowledge, a practical task sheet was designed and developed, again specifically for this study to address the XHTML learning content. The task sheet comprised five questions related to the construction of web pages. The key areas addressed by the questions related to the insertion of images and links, building tables, formatting text, creating lists, and creating, saving and viewing a document in the browser. 3.3.6 Questionnaires Finally, three closed-question questionnaires were used in this study. The first collected information on: gender; age; course area and level; and disability. Participants’ levels of computer experience, domain knowledge in relation the tutorial’s subject content (XHTML) and familiarity with other programming languages (such as Hypertext Markup Language (HTML), Visual Basic, C++ and Java) was also gathered, using a three-point Likert scale (1 representing “novice”, and 3 representing “expert”). 12 The second questionnaire presented closed choice questions to measure participants’ levels of the four types of disorientation: (i) I know my current location in the HLS; (ii) being on the current page, I know where I was previously in the HLS; (iii) being on the current page, I know where to go next in the HLS; and (iv) I know how to reach my desired location in the HLS. The third questionnaire presented closed choice questions to rate attitude towards different aspects of the HLS: structure; navigation; overall level of disorientation; dependency on and distraction by the visual instructional aids; and overall satisfaction. A Likert scale was used for all questions in the second and third questionnaire, with the scale items comprising 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Participants were required to circle the response that most closely reflected their answer to each question. This use of closed choice questionnaires is evident in a number of other studies in the field (for example, [2]; [21], in relation to disorientation; and [12]; [11]; [36]; [37]; [38]; [56], in relation to attitudes and perception). Though there are several questionnaires which gather data on disorientation, and attitudes and perception, this study had to develop its own instantiations in order to gather data about participants’ use and perceptions of the specific HLS used in this study. 3.4 Procedure The experimental study was conducted over a number of sessions in a computer usability laboratory at Brunel University. Each session contained a small group of participants, each working individually on a PC. The experimental study was divided into nine stages: 1. Participants were given a brief explanation of the study; 2. Participants took the CSA test to determine their level of field dependence (classified into FD, FM and FI) according to their CSA score. Participants identified as being FM did not undertake the subsequent stages; 3. Participants completed the questionnaire to determine their level of computer expertise, experience of XHTML and other programming languages and to gather demographic information; 4. Participants were given a maximum of 15 minutes to complete the pre-test (marked out of 20) to determine their prior knowledge of XHTML; 13 5. Participants were then randomly assigned to use one of the two HLS (with visual instructional aids; and without instructional aids) and used the assigned HLS to complete the online instructional content of the XHTML tutorial; 6. Participants undertook the practical task to measure their learning performance (in the form of procedural knowledge). They were allowed to use the HLS to find the answers and the task was marked out of 50; 7. Participants were given a maximum of 15 minutes to complete the post-test (again, marked out of 20); 8. The disorientation questionnaire was administered to measure participants’ levels of the four types of disorientation; 9. The attitude questionnaire was administered to record participants’ attitudes towards the HLS. 3.5 Data Analysis As already noted, each participant’s cognitive style was determined using the CSA instrument, categorizing participants as Field Dependent (FD) or Field Independent (FI) (see stage 2, above). The ‘domain knowledge’ variable was categorized as taking one of two values: novice – signifying low domain knowledge; or expert – signifying high domain knowledge. Similarly, the ‘computer experience’ variable was categorized as taking one of two values: novice – low computer experience; or expert – high computer experience (see stage 3, above). Thus each participant had a profile of individual differences – with one of two cognitive style types, one of two levels of domain knowledge, and one of two levels of computer experience – from an overall set of eight possible profiles. These profiles were used within the grouping strategy for the three-way ANOVA [28, 43] which was used to determine whether there was a significant effect between the three individual differences (cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience) on learners’ disorientation, learning performance and attitudes when using the two versions of the HLS: one without instructional aids and with visual instructional aids. In addition to the ANOVA test, the Newman Keuls post-hoc test was used to identify whether or not there were significant differences between the sub-groups. The data gathered from the disorientation questionnaires (in stage 8), attitude questionnaires (in stage 9) and achievement 14 tests/practical tasks (stages 4, 6 and 7) were analyzed using the Statistical Package for Social Science [50]. The level of significance was set at p < 0.05. 4. Findings This section presents and considers the findings that were gathered from the experimental study, the aim of which was to address the three research questions. To this end, data gathered from the pre- and post-tests tests, practical tasks and closed questionnaires were analyzed. 4.1 Description of participants 192 university students participated in the study: 96 of them used the HLS that provided no instructional aids; the remainder used the HLS that incorporated visual instructional aids. Since this study used a three-way ANOVA, providing a focus on three individual differences – cognitive style (FD or FI), domain knowledge (high or low DK) and computer experience (high or low CE) (as discussed in section 3.5), a minimum of 12 participants were needed in each group to ensure a sample that had the potential for revealing significant results (see Table 2), giving a total sample comprising 192 participants. Table 2: Distribution of participants according to their cognitive style, DK and CE HLS that incorporated visual instructional HLS that provided no instructional aids aids FI FD Total FI FD Total Low DK and low CE 12 12 24 Low DK and low CE 12 12 24 Low DK and high CE 12 12 24 Low DK and high CE 12 12 24 High DK and low CE 12 12 24 High DK and low CE 12 12 24 High DK and high CE 12 12 24 High DK and high CE 12 12 24 Total 48 48 96 Total 48 48 96 Total number of participants = 192 15 4.2 Results related to disorientation With regards to the HLS that provided no instructional aids, the three-way ANOVA revealed a significant effect between cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience in relation to participants’ levels of the four types of disorientation in the HLS. The analysis of the Newman Keuls post-hoc tests also revealed some significant differences between the groups. The summary results of the ANOVA and analysis of the post-hoc tests are presented in Table 2. For the group of participants that had low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, or high DK and low CE, cognitive style had a significant impact. FD users with one of these experience profiles experienced higher levels of all four types of disorientation than did FI users with the same experience profile. This is argued to be a major finding of the study, though there are important issues relating to interpretation of this finding which will be discussed in section 7. <<Insert Table 2 here>> However, for the group of participants that had high DK and high CE, cognitive style did not have a significant effect: neither FD nor FI users in this group experienced higher levels of any of the four types of disorientation in the HLS. In the version of the HLS that incorporated visual instructional aids, the three-way ANOVA identified no significant effect in relation to cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience on the levels of the four types of disorientation. With regards to all four experience profiles ((i) low DK and low CE; (ii) low DK and high CE; (iii) high DK and low CE; and (iv) high DK and high CE), cognitive style did not seem to have an impact: FD and FI users with the same experience profile tended to agree equally with the statement that they did not experience higher levels of disorientation in the HLS in relation to the four types. Having considered disorientation, the next section reports the learning performance results in relation to both versions of the HLS. 4.3 Results related to learning performance For the HLS that provided no instructional aids, a three-way ANOVA revealed a significant effect in relation to cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience on learning performance. 16 This was reinforced by the results of post hoc tests. Tables 3 summarizes the results. For the group of participants that had either low DK and low CE, or low DK and high CE, cognitive style showed a significant impact: FD learners with one of these experience profiles scored lower in the post-test and in the practical task, achieved lower test gain scores (the difference between pre-test and post-test scores), and took more time completing the tutorial and the practical task compared to FI learners with the same experience profile. <<Insert Table 3 here>> However, for the group of participants that had either high DK and low CE, or high DK and high CE, cognitive style showed no significant impact: both FD and FI learners with the same experience profile performed equally well in the post-test and in the practical task, achieved lower gain scores, and took almost the same amount of time completing the tutorial and the practical task. With regards to the HLS that incorporated visual instructional aids, a three-way ANOVA revealed no significant effect between cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience on learning performance. For all four experience profile groups ((i) low DK and low CE; (ii) low DK and high CE; (iii) high DK and low CE; and (iv) high DK and high CE), both FD and FI users with the same experience profile performed well in the post-test and practical task, and took broadly the same amount of time to complete the tutorial. Additionally, for the group of participants that had either low DK and low CE, or low DK and high CE, both FD and FI users with the same experience profile achieved higher gain scores. For the group of participants that had either high DK and low CE, or high DK and high CE, both FD and FI users with the same experience profile achieved lower gain scores. However, with respect to the length of time taken to complete the practical task, the results from the three-way ANOVA revealed a significant result when considering cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience. Analysis of the post-hoc tests identified some significant differences between the groups (see Table 4 for summary results). For the group of participants that had either low DK and low CE, or low DK and high CE, cognitive style had a significant impact: FD users with one of these experience profiles spent more time completing the practical task than FI users with the same experience profile. However, for the group of participants that had either high DK and low CE, or high DK and high CE, cognitive style did not have a significant impact, with both FD and FI users with the 17 same experience profile spending almost the same amount of time to complete the practical task as each other. <<Insert Table 4 here>> Having considered the results of the ANOVA in relation to learning performance, the next section reports the findings gathered from the attitudes questionnaires. 4.4 Results related to attitude For the HLS that provided no instructional aids, the three-way ANOVA identified a significant effect between cognitive style domain knowledge and computer experience on attitudes towards structure, navigation, overall experience of levels of disorientation, and overall satisfaction. Analysis of the post-hoc tests also showed significant differences between the groups. Table 5 shows a summary of the ANOVA and post-hoc tests results. <<Insert Table 5 here>> For participants with experience profiles of low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, and high DK and low CE, cognitive style had a significant impact: FD users with one of these experience profiles showed more negative attitudes towards the structure, had more difficulties navigating through the HLS and disliked the high levels of freedom of navigation that were offered in the HLS, reported much higher levels of disorientation in the HLS, and showed greater dissatisfaction learning in the HLS compared to FI users with the same experience profile. However, with regards to those users with high DK and high CE, cognitive style showed no significant impact. Both FD and FI users with this experience profile showed a positive attitude towards the structure, had no difficulties navigating through the HLS and enjoyed the higher levels of freedom of navigation offered in the HLS, did not experience disorientation, and reported being satisfied learning in the HLS. In the version of the HLS that incorporated visual instructional aids, the three-way ANOVA revealed no significant effects in relation to cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience on participants’ attitudes towards the HLS in terms of their views of structure, navigation (and navigation efficacy), levels of disorientation, distraction by the visual instructional aids, and 18 overall satisfaction. With regards to participants in all four experience profile groups ((i) low DK and low CE; (ii) low DK and high CE; (iii) high DK and low CE; and (iv) high DK and high CE), cognitive style had no significant impact: all participants showed a positive attitude towards the structure; reported that they did not experience difficulties navigating through the HLS; tended to disagree equally strongly with the statement that they experienced high levels of disorientation in the HLS; responded that they were not distracted by the visual instructional aids when performing their learning in the HLS; and reported being satisfied learning in the HLS. However, the three-way ANOVA revealed a significant effect on the views related to dependency on the visual instructional aids. Additionally, the analysis from the post-hoc tests identified some significant differences between the groups. The summary results of the ANOVA and post-hoc tests in relation to this issue are presented in Table 6. For the groups of participants that had low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, or high DK and low CE, cognitive style had a significant impact: FD learners with one of these experience profiles depended more on the visual instructional aids to reduce their levels of disorientation in the HLS than did FI learners with the same experience profile. For the group of participants that had high DK and high CE, cognitive style showed no significant impact, with both FD and FI learners with this experience profile responding that they did not depend on the visual instructional aids to reduce disorientation in the HLS. <<Insert Table 6 here>> The analysis of the three-way ANOVA also found a significant effect between cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience on the attitudes towards the levels of freedom of navigation in the HLS. Analysis of the post-hoc tests identified significant differences between the groups. The summary results of the ANOVA and the post-hoc tests in relation to this issue are presented in Table 7. Similar results gathered in the version of the HLS that provided no instructional aids were identified. <<Insert Table 7 here>> 19 5. Discussion This study aimed to examine the effects of and between cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience in relation to learners’ disorientation, learning performance and attitudes when using a HLS that provided no instructional aids and when using a HLS that incorporated visual instructional aids (see research questions 1-3). Before moving on, it is important to restate the complexity of the findings and the limitations in inferences that can be drawn from them, given the number of variables being considered. Drawing on the preceding data analysis, this section seeks to identify the effects of the individual differences when using different navigational support mechanisms, rather than trying to prove that specific combinations of individual differences caused the effects. This may seem a semantic point, but it is important. The focus is on gaining insights from the data that can have practical value for designing systems which better support learners. As such, after the discussion, the paper will draw on the findings to frame support for designers of such systems, with evidence that they will be helpful to specific user groups while not hindering others. This section will be structured in relation to the three research questions posed in section 2. A summary of the overall findings are presented in Table 8, for the version of the HLS without visual aids, and Table 9, for the version with visual aids, for ease of reference. <<Insert Table 8 here>> <<Insert Table 9 here>> 5.1 Research question 1 With regards to research question 1 and in relation to the HLS that provided no instructional aids, the analysis of the disorientation questionnaires mostly suggested that there was a significant effect between cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience on disorientation. Cognitive style had a significant impact on learners’ disorientation for the groups of participants that had low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, or high DK and low CE. FD users with one of these experience profiles experienced higher levels of disorientation in the HLS than did FI users with the same experience profile, making it a relevant issue in HLS design. However, care must be exercised in 20 relation to this finding as there are alternative interpretations (other than there being an ordinal interaction) which could be valid. This issue will be returned to in section 7. Moving on to the remaining group, cognitive style did not have a significant impact for those users with an experience profile of high DK and high CE, with both FD and FI users in this group disagreeing with the statement that they experienced disorientation problems in the HLS. It is argued that this is because their prior knowledge of the learning content of ‘XHTML’ and their expertise in using computers supported them in navigating effectively through the HLS. Thus, when considered in relation to other individual differences such as domain knowledge and computer experience, cognitive style does not always influence disorientation. In the version of the HLS that incorporated visual instructional aids – the map and set of visual cues introduced earlier in the paper – the analysis of the disorientation questionnaires revealed no significant effects between cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience in relation to disorientation. For all four experience profile groups, cognitive style had no significant impact on disorientation in the HLS, with neither FD nor FI users with the same experience profile agreeing with the statement that they experienced high levels of disorientation. However, the analysis of the attitudes questionnaires suggested that cognitive style had a significant effect on the use of the visual instructional aids to reduce disorientation in the HLS. For the group of participants that had low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, or high DK and low CE, FD users with one of these experience profiles depended more on the visual instructional aids to reduce disorientation in the HLS than did FI users with the same experience profile. The study’s results suggest that while FD users with one of these experience profiles are totally dependent on the visual instructional aids, this is not the case for FI users with the same experience profile – that is, they are not totally dependent on these visual instructional aids to reduce disorientation problems in the HLS. For those users with an experience profile of high DK and high CE, cognitive style did not have a significant impact on the use of the visual instructional aids in order to reduce disorientation problems in the HLS, with neither FD nor FI users in this group being dependent on them. This study has shown that when considering cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience together, it is not always the case that cognitive style will influence the use and impact of visual instructional aids to reduce disorientation problems. 21 5.2 Research question 2 With regards to research question 2, and in relation to the HLS that provided no instructional aids, the analysis of the achievement tests (post-test scores, test gain scores, practical task scores and time efficacy) suggested that there was a significant effect between cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience in relation to learning performance. For participants with experience profiles of either low DK and low CE, or low DK and high CE, cognitive style had a significant impact, with FD users with one of these experience profiles performing less well in the post-test and in the practical task, achieving lower gain scores, and taking more time to complete the tutorial and the practical task than FI users with the same experience profile. However, care should be exercised in relation to this finding as the extent to which experience level and/or prior domain knowledge accounted for the variance may mean that there is too little left for analysis of other interaction effects. This issue, effectively one of statistical power, will be returned to in section 7. If, though, cognitive style is significant in this context, it raises a concern in relation to learning performance for FD users in these experience profile groups. With regards to those users with experience profiles of either high DK and low CE, or high DK and high CE, cognitive style revealed no significant effects, with both FD and FI users with the same experience profile performing equally well in the learning tasks. This is to be expected given their high domain knowledge prior to the use of the tutorial, as reflected in their higher pre-test scores. This study has shown that when domain knowledge is high, irrespective of cognitive style, computer experience and the mode of learning system used, learning performance will not be hindered. This insight may help designers to gain an improved understanding of when and why cognitive style will not influence learning performance. In terms of the HLS that incorporated the visual instructional aids, for the group of participants that had low DK and low CE, or low DK and high CE, cognitive style showed no significant effects on learning performance in terms of post-test scores, test gain scores, practical task scores and time efficacy (time taken to complete the tutorial), with both FD and FI users with the same experience profile performing equally well in the post-test and in the practical task, achieving higher gain scores and taking broadly the same amount of time to complete the tutorial. However, cognitive style had a significant impact on time efficacy (the time taken to complete the practical task) for the learners who had one of these two experience profiles (that is, low DK and low CE, or low DK and high CE): FD 22 learners with one of these experience profiles spent more time completing the practical task than FI learners with the same experience profile. The study’s results suggest that, in general, the visual instructional aids seemed to have supported the FD users with one of these two experience profiles in reducing their higher levels of disorientation. This, in turn, led them to learn as effectively as FI users with the same experience profile, in contrast to the findings for the same experience profile groups using the version of the HLS that provided no instructional aids. These results suggest that the relationship between HLS, visual instructional aids, individual differences and learning performance needs to be carefully considered in the design of HLS. Finally, for the participants that had either high DK or low CE, or high DK and high CE, similar findings were identified in both versions of the HLS (i.e., with and without instructional aids) in relation to cognitive style (as discussed earlier in this section). These results suggest that when considering cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience together, cognitive style will not always influence the way that visual instructional aids are used to enhance a user’s learning performance in the HLS. 5.3 Research question 3 With respect to research question 3 and in relation to the HLS that provided no instructional aids, the analysis of the attitude questionnaires suggested a significant effect between cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience in relation to learners’ attitudes. For the groups of participants that had low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, or high DK and low CE, cognitive style showed a significant impact on attitudes: FD users with one of these experience profiles showed more negative attitudes towards the HLS in terms of their views of structure, navigation and overall satisfaction than did FI users with the same experience profile. This finding is supported by the earlier discussion in relation to research questions 1 and 2, which suggested that FD users with one of these three experience profiles were less comfortable learning in the version of the HLS that provided no instructional aids than were FI users with the same experience profile. It seems that, unlike FI users with one of these experience profiles, the FD users with same experience profile have difficulties imposing a structure on the learning content or mapping a mental representation of the information that is presented in the HLS to enhance their learning. This raises a concern in relation to attitudes for FD users in these groups, and is an issue when seeking to develop guidance for HLS design. Finally, with 23 regards to those users with an experience profile of high DK and high CE, cognitive style did not have a significant impact on attitudes, with both FD and FI users in this group showing a positive attitude in terms of their views of the structure of, navigation in, and overall satisfaction with the HLS. It is argued that this could be because their prior knowledge of the learning content of ‘XHTML’ and of using computers permitted the users in this group, irrespective of their cognitive style, to be comfortable learning in a non-linear learning environment. This study has shown that when considering other individual differences, for example domain knowledge and computer experience, cognitive style will not always influence attitudes in HLS, with FD and FI users with an experience profile of high DK and high CE showing the same preference for non-linearity features in the HLS. This makes it an important area to consider in the HLS design. With regards to the HLS that incorporated visual instructional aids, for participants in all four experience profile groups, cognitive style did not have a significant impact on attitudes: both FD and FI users with the same experience profile showed a positive attitude towards the HLS, responded that they were not distracted by the visual instructional aids, and reported being satisfied learning in the HLS. However, for the group of participants that had low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, or high DK and low CE, cognitive style showed a significant effect on the use of the visual instructional aids to reduce levels of disorientation in the HLS. FD users with one of these experience profiles were more dependent on the visual instructional aids to reduce their levels of disorientation in the HLS than were FI users with the same experience profile. The visual instructional aids may have assisted the FD users in these experience profile groups in reducing their levels of disorientation, and in turn, to learn effectively in the HLS. This, in turn, may have led them to show a positive attitude towards this version of HLS. The study’s results suggest that FD users in these experience profile groups must be provided with guidance in the form of visual instructional aids, for example maps and visual orientation cues in the HLS, otherwise they may show less interest learning in this non-linear learning environment. For those users with high DK and high CE, cognitive style did not have a significant effect, with neither FD nor FI users with this experience profile being dependent on the visual instructional aids to reduce disorientation in the HLS. 6. Development of HLS design guidelines In order for these findings to be useful in informing the design of HLS, it is important that they are captured in a way that makes them accessible. As such, this section will present design guidelines that 24 draw on the findings and seek to capture the issues in a form that is useful to, and usable by, HLS designers. Particular attention is paid to identifying issues which, on the basis of the findings and their analysis, offer scope to support particular groups of users while not hindering other groups – in other words they identify factors that can positively support some users while being benign for others. As such, the guidelines seek to be generally applicable to HLS. Based on the discussions of the findings from this study (as presented in preceding section), designers need to be aware that while some users (for example FI users with low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, or high DK and low CE; and FD/FI users with high DK and high CE) have no difficulties learning in HLS, others (for example FD users with low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, or high DK and low CE) struggle to learn effectively and, in turn, demonstrate negative attitudes towards non-linear learning environments. The FD users with one of these experience profiles (low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, high DK and low CE) are largely dependent on learning guidance in the form of visual instructional aids to perform their learning in HLS. However, the results of this experimental study suggest that including these visual instructional aids will not have detrimental effects for those users who are less dependent on them or who do not need them at all to perform their learning in HLS (for example, FI with low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, or high DK and low CE; FD/FI with high DK and high CE). The remainder of this section will suggest ways in which HLS may be designed to support FD users with one of the following experience profiles – low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, or high DK and low CE – to learn effectively in HLS but without disturbing the other users (FI with low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, or high DK and low CE; FD/FI with high DK and high CE) who do not require additional support in using HLS. 6.1 Provide conceptual support in the form of graphical maps FD users with experience profiles of low DK and low CE, low CE and high CE, or high CE and low CE, experience higher levels of disorientation in HLS because they: have difficulty imposing a structure on the learning content that is presented in the HLS; or have difficulty mapping a mental representation of all the information that is presented in the HLS. In order to help FD users to reduce these problems in HLS, designers need to provide visual instructional aids in the form of a graphical map on every page. The graphical map will help the learners to see a global overview of the information (in the form of nodes/links) that is represented in the HLS. Additionally, it will help the 25 learners to gain a representation of the relationships between the different pieces of information presented in the HLS. The conceptual structure of the information presented by the graphical map can further assist them in navigating through the HLS to reach the information that they need in relation to their learning goals. Referring to the graphical map will make these FD users confident that they are successfully completing all of the learning materials in the HLS which are needed to achieve their learning goals. This, in turn, can help them not only to complete all of the topics, but also to integrate their knowledge so as to complete the tutorial effectively. The map can also support FD users with high DK and low CE to navigate the tutorial to access content in accordance with their information needs and navigation preferences, but without them being disorientated in the non-linear environment. However, care does need to be taken when presenting the graphical map on the same page where the learning content is presented as this can create two issues: first, the graphical map may take up a lot of space on the page, which may prevent the learning content from being clearly visible on the screen; and second, the graphical map may disturb other users who do not need it (for example – FI users with low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, or high DK and low CE; FD/FI users with high DK and high CE ). In order to reduce the impact of these two issues, zoom-in and zoom-out features can be added to the visual representation. FD users with low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, or high CE and low CE may still encounter higher levels of disorientation if they do not know the following when they navigate through the HLS: (1) where are they?; (2) what have they visited?; (3) where have they been (including repeatedly visited pages) and how can they go back to these visited pages?; and (4) where to go next? These issues may be particularly acute when high levels of freedom of navigation have been provided, and when the tutorial consists of a large number of main sections, sub-sections and even sub-sub sections. Maps may not reduce all of these four types of disorientation, making it an issue in relation to the design of the HLS. It is therefore imperative to present and consider other methods that can be applied to reduce these four types of disorientation problems in the design of the HLS. 6.2 Provide ‘breadcrumbs’ and ‘highlighting context’ visual cues to reduce the first type of disorientation – ‘where are they’? With respect to the first type of disorientation (‘where are they?’), FD users with experience profiles of (i) low DK and low CE, (ii) low DK and high CE, or (iii) high DK and low CE) sometimes 26 have difficulties finding: (a) how far they have navigated in the HLS to their current location in the HLS; and (b) their location within a specific section in the tutorial. In terms of the first issue, ‘breadcrumbs’ can be used as a visual cue to show these FD users how they reached their current location in the HLS. For this to be effective, the link showing the current page should be disabled and presented in a different color text. Placing the ‘breadcrumbs’ above the page title can clearly attract the attention of these FD users. Additionally, a different background color can be used to differentiate the ‘breadcrumb’ visual cue from the learning content on the page. With regards to the second issue, one solution is to clearly identify the learner’s current position on the graphical map – for example by disabling and differently coloring the ‘current’ node and using a ‘hand’ icon with the accompanying text ‘you are here’ to point to the node on the graphical map. 6.3 Use ‘check mark’ or ‘different link colors’ visual cues to reduce the second type of disorientation – ‘what have they visited?’ The second type of disorientation – ‘what have they visited?’ – is mostly experienced by FD users with low DK and low CE, or low DK and high CE, who find it difficult to impose a structure on the learning content in the HLS. A consequence of this disorientation problem is that the FD users in these experience profile groups may open unnecessary nodes or fail to complete effectively all of the learning materials needed to achieve their overall learning goals. One remedy for this issue is to provide a check mark to indicate the pages that the user has visited in the HLS. Such a check mark (in the form of a ‘tick’ symbol, for example) can be displayed inside the nodes (which represent the information that is presented in the HLS) in the graphical map. Additionally, different colors could be used for the tick symbols to differentiate the latest page from the previous pages visited. The tick symbol should be of reasonable size, so that it is clearly visible inside the nodes in the graphical map. If this approach is adopted, designers need to tell users, especially those with low CE, what the tick symbol/colors refer to by, for example, annotating the symbols (activated when the mouse is rolled over them). Providing different link colors is another way of informing the FD users in these experience profile groups of the pages that they have already visited. 6.4 Use the ‘history-based mechanism’ cue to reduce the third type of disorientation – ‘where have they been and how can they go back to these destinations?’ 27 The third type of disorientation is mostly experienced by FD users with low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, or high DK and low CE. FD users with one of these experience profiles have difficulties finding where they have recently been in the HLS (including the pages that they have opened more than once), especially on the rare occasions that they use a non-linear approach in relation to their learning goals. This makes it harder for them to go back to these previously visited pages if they need to. One way to reduce this problem is to provide a history-based mechanism. Through the use of this visual technique, the FD users in these experience profile groups will be able to view a list of the pages that they have visited in the HLS. Additionally, links could be provided to the visited pages to help the FD users to access them directly should they need in order to accomplish their learning goals. Designers will need to be aware, however, that displaying all of the visited pages (including those that have been visited repeatedly) on one page may lead to space and scrolling problems. A compromise may be to display, say, only the last 10 visited pages. Even if this compromise approach is adopted, designers will need to be cautious about the historybased mechanism being displayed on the same page as the learning content because it may cause two issues: first, the learning content may not be clearly visible, which may prevent some users from learning effectively; and second, it may irritate other users, for example FD/FI users with high DK and high CE, and FI users with low DK and low CE, low DK and high CE, or high DK and low CE. In order to reduce these two issues, the history-based mechanism cue could be opened in a new pop-up window, or zoomed in and out on the same page where the learning content is displayed. 6.5 Use ‘page labels’ and the ‘direct guidance’ visual cue to reduce the fourth type of disorientation – ‘where to go next?’ The fourth type of disorientation –‘where to go next in the HLS’ – is a common problem that faced by some FD users, especially those with low DK and low CE, or low DK and high CE. One way to help these FD users is to provide page labels in the form of headings and sub-headings on a page. This can help these learners to take coherent paths in order to successfully complete their current learning goals. Using different font sizes, styles and colors for the text that is used for the page labels will help these users to differentiate the learning content from the page label visual cues. Additionally, the font size that is used for the labels should not be too large otherwise the labels may take up too much space on the page, causing a disturbance to other users who do not use them. 28 Another approach to alleviating the fourth type of disorientation is to provide direct guidance. A pagination facility (which shows the current page) can be displayed at the bottom of the learning content along with two arrow links: the backward arrow link to its left, and the forward arrow link to its right. The backward arrow link will direct these FD users one page back from the current one, whereas the forward arrow link will direct users one page ahead. Using this mechanisms to allow navigation only one page either ‘side’ of the current page makes sure that these users will be opening appropriate pages in relation to their current learning goals and should help alleviate disorientation. 7. Limitations of the study and future research In attempting to answer the three research questions posed in this paper, certain issues may have limited the generalizability of this study’s findings. The first limitation relates to the sample used in the study, which was made up of university students. They were considered to be a suitable sample group because the research relates to HLS and Higher Education and because most existing studies in the area have also used university students as their subjects. It is important to note that university students may be more educated than the general population, so the findings related to the use of the two versions of the HLS may have been different had the sample been drawn from the general population. To overcome this shortcoming, the study could be replicated using a sample drawn from this wider group. This may encourage researchers to consider the effects of an even wider range of individual differences alongside cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience. Other factors such as age, educational level, and ability to use computers and their applications may prove worthwhile to study. Within the choice of university students (as part of the study’s purposive sampling approach), the decision to recruit participants from only the London region may be seen as a limitation associated with the sample. It is not necessarily the case that students from this region are representative of university students nationally, although there is no specific bias of which we are aware in taking such a regional recruitment approach. Since the study’s focus was on, and classified the participants in terms of, cognitive style, prior knowledge and experience, the limiting of the sample to participants from the London region should not have made a meaningful difference as the three different variables – and levels within them – were appropriately represented in the sample. Conducting further studies with 29 university students from across the country would, however, address any criticism in this respect and aid confidence in the generalizability of the findings to the wider UK HE student population. It may also be useful to conduct comparable studies using different learning content in the HLSs. Although the content in the tutorial reported in this study covered, for example, conceptual and procedural information and we see no immediate reason why the choice of content should limit the applicability of the findings to other subject domains, studies that attempt to replicate the findings with other subject content structured and presented in the same way as for this study’s tutorial may be useful to demonstrate generalizability. A second limitation of this research is that, in both versions of the HLS, the XHTML learning content was presented and explained using text and images. Some of the participants in this study were registered as having dyslexia, and their presentation of the learning content may have hindered their learning performance and may have caused them to show less interest in the HLS. The findings related to learning performance and attitudes may have been different had an alternative approach to overcome presenting aspects of the learning content (for example, the use of audio) been employed. To overcome this limitation, further studies could be designed to examine the effects of audio on disorientation, learning performance and attitudes in relation to the same set of individual differences considered in this study, but with a sample of learners who have dyslexia. The results may help to identify whether using audio supports these learners in overcoming the issues associated with their dyslexia. It is also important to note the complexities in interpreting ordinal interactions identified in studies such as the one reported in this paper. The interpretation of an identified ordinal interaction in a study of individual differences is always complex and challenging, because the counterfactual is not clear, making any conclusion drawn vulnerable to alternative interpretations. In this study, the suggested ordinal interaction between cognitive style and disorientation in the HLS with no instructional aids for three of the four experience profile groups (and the mitigation of the disorientation effects seen for the same groups in the HLS when instructional aids were provided) may be down to factors other than cognitive style. However, as noted in section 3.1, we undertook additional qualitative data collection and analysis as part of the study and the findings here supported the interpretation of the ordinal interaction. Whilst. these data cannot be fully reported given the space constraints of this paper, the 30 support from the qualitative data analysis gives us confidence in the interpretation of the data as showing an ordinal interaction. A further issue, relevant to the findings related to cognitive style and learning performance reported in section 4.3 and discussed in section 5.2, is the strength of evidence and the threats to the validity of inferences drawn from the data analysis. Since third order interaction was being explored in the study, the issue of research power is exacerbated and it is fair to question whether the size of the sample was large enough to give weight to the finding with respect to learning performance. Repeating the study with a larger sample would be one way to address this concern about whether the study was ‘underpowered’ and to improve confidence in the validity of the findings. Strength of evidence issues are ones that, given the methodological limitations – and overall complexity – of designing and running studies of this kind, must be acknowledged. The findings presented in this paper must, as a result, be viewed with caution, as the discussion in this section has stressed. An associated, wider limitation of the study relates to the complexity of the issues that it seeks to explore. Experimental studies of three individual differences as independent variables in a single study are rare. This is not surprising as the complexity of the design and analysis – and the care and caution with which any results should be interpreted, and the scope for them to be criticized – perhaps makes such studies unappealing to researchers. However, studies that concentrate only on a single individual difference (often without controlling for others that have been argued to be important) also run the risk of generating findings that have limited value or are open to strong challenge. We recognize the methodological complexity and associated analytical difficulties associated with this study, and the associated criticisms that can be levelled in relation to the strength of evidence produced, but feel that seeking to overly simplify studies of this type also creates associated issues such as those noted above. Providing full details of the approach that we have taken allows researchers to assign weight to the findings based on their analysis of the study’s design and execution. We hope that it encourages others to undertake similarly complex studies and add to the corpus of evidence in the field. 8. Conclusion This paper has examined the effects of and between cognitive style, domain knowledge and computer experience in relation to disorientation, learning performance and attitudes in two versions of 31 HLS: one that provided no instructional aids and one that incorporated visual instructional aids (in the form of a map and a set of visual cues). Based on the analysis of the data gathered in the study, this paper has framed a set of guidelines for the use of visual instructional aids in the design of the HLS that aim to support designers in building systems that reduce disorientation, support effective learning and enhance learning satisfaction, but do not have detrimental effects on those users who do not depend on the additional support of visual aids in using HLS. Additionally, the identified limitations of this study – and the caution raised over the interpretation and generalizability of the findings – are argued to be useful for identifying areas of future research. References [1] Aery, Sean C. Breadcrumb Navigation Deployment in Retail Web Sites. A Master’s Paper for the M.S. in I.S. Degree. July, 2007. [2] Ahuja, J.S. and Webster, J. Perceived disorientation: an examination of a new measure to assess web design effectiveness. Interacting with Computers, 2001. 14(1): p. 15-29. [3] Alomyan, H. and Au, W. Exploration of instructional strategies and individual difference within the context of web-based learning, International Education Journal, 4 (4), (2004), 86-92. [4] Amadieu, F., Tricot, A., and Mariné, C. Individual differences in learning from hypermedia: Learners’ characteristics to consider to design effective hypermedia. 6th International Conference on Human System Learning - ICHSL, 14-16 may, Toulouse, (Toulouse: IEEE France Section), (2008), pp. 1-9. [5] Amadieu, F., Tricot, A., and Mariné, C. “Prior Knowledge in Learning from a Non-Linear Electronic Document: Disorientation and Coherence of the Reading Sequences.” Computers in Human Behavior 25: (2009a), 381-88. [6] Amadieu, F., Gog, F., Paas, F., Tricot, A., and Mariné. C. Effects of prior knowledge and conceptmap structure on disorientation, cognitive load, and learning. Learning and Instruction, 19 (2009b), pp. 376–386. [7] Amadieu, F., Tricot, A., and Mariné, C. Exploratory Study of Relations between Prior Knowledge, Comprehension, Disorientation and On-line Process in Hypertext. The Ergonomics Open Journal, 9, (2009c), pg 49-57. 32 [8] Amadieu, F., Tricot, A. and Mariné, C. Interaction Between Prior Knowledge And Concept-Map Structure on Hypertext Comprehension, Coherence of Reading Orders and Disorientation. Interacting with Computers, 22 (2), (2010), 88-97. [9] Brusilovsky, P., Sosnovsky, S. and Yudelson, M. Addictive links: The motivational value of adaptive link annotation in educational hypermedia. In: V. Wade, H. Ashman and B. Smyth (eds.) Proceedings of 4th International Conference on Adaptive Hypermedia and Adaptive Web-Based Systems (AH'2006), Dublin, Ireland, June 21-23, Springer Verlag, (2006), pp. 51-60. [10] Calcaterra, A., Antonietti, A., and Underwood, J. Cognitive style, hypermedia navigation and learning. Computers and Education. 44, 4, (2005), 441–457. [11] Chen, S. Y. Evaluating the learning effectiveness of using Web-based instruction: An individual differences approach. International Journal of Information & Communication Technology Education, 1(1), (2005), 69–82. [12] Chen S.Y. and Macredie R.D. Cognitive modelling of student learning in web-based instructional programmes. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction 17, (2004), 375–402. [13] Chen, S. Y., Fan, J. and Macredie, R. D. Navigation in Hypermedia Learning Systems: Experts vs. Novices. Computers in Human Behavior. 22(2), (2006), 251-266. [14] Chen, N.S., Kinshuk, Wei, C.W and Chen, H.J. Mining e-learning domain concept map from academic articles. Computers & Education, 50, 3, (2008), pp. 1009–1021. [15] Chen, S. Y. and X. Liu. Mining students' learning patterns and performance in Web-based instruction: a cognitive style approach, Interactive Learning Environments, 19:2, (2011), 179-192. [16] Chen L-H. Web-based learning programs: use by learners with various cognitive styles. Comput Educ 54: (2010), 1028–1035. [17] Chou, H. W. Influences of cognitive style and training method on training effectiveness. Computers and Education, 37, (2001), 11–25 [18] Chung, P. H. Changing the Interface with Minimal Disruption: The Roles of Layout and Labels. Doctoral dissertation, Rice University, Houston, TX, (2006). [19] Davidovic, A., Warren, J., Trichina, E. and Lakes, M. Learning benefits of structural examplebased adaptive tutoring systems, IEEE Trans Educ 46 (2003), 241_251. 33 [20] De La Passardiere, B. and Durfresne, A. Adaptive navigational tools for educational hypermedia [On-line]. Computer Assisted Learning. Available: http://mistral.ere.umontreal.ca/_dufresne/Publications/ical92.htm, (1992). [21] Demirbilek, M. Effects of interface windowing modes and individual differences on disorientation and cognitive load in a hypermedia learning environment. PhD. Dissertation, University of Florida, (2005). [22] Dringus, L.P. Towards active online learning: A dramatic shift in perspective for learners. The Internet and Higher Education, 2(4), (2002), 189-195. [23] Fan, J. P. Interface design for hypermedia learning systems: a study of individual differences and hypermedia system features. Ph.D. dissertation, Brunel University, West London, UK, (2005). [24] Fan, J.P. and Macredie, R.D. Gender Differences and Hypermedia Navigation: Principles for Adaptive Hypermedia Learning Systems, Idea Group Publishing, Hershey, PA, (2006). [25] Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K., Opfermann, M., Hesse, F. W. and Eysink, T. H. S. Learning with hypermedia: The influence of representational formats and different levels of learner control on performance and learning behavior. Computers in Human Behavior, 25, (2009), 360–367. [26] Graf, S., Liu, T.-C. and Kinshuk. Analysis of learners’ navigational behavior and their learning styles in an online course. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 26, (2010), 116-131. [27] Hua, Q. and Pei-Luen, P.R. Method for Reducing Disorientation in Hypermedia Educational systems. Tsinghua Science and Technology, Volume 14, Issues 5, (2009), 655-662. [28] Karpinski. Factorial ANOVA (chapter 8): Higher Order ANOVA, (2006). [29] Khalifa M. and Lam R. Web-based learning: effects on learning process and outcome. IEEE Transactions on Education, 45, (2002), 350-356. [30] Khosrowjerdi, M. and Iranshahi, M. Prior Knowledge and information-seeking behavior of PhD and MA. Library and Information Science Research, In Press, Corrected Proof, (2011). [31] Lajoie, S.P. and Azevedo, R. Teaching and learning in technology-rich environments, P. Alexander, P. Winne, Editors, Handbook of Educational Psychology, (2nd ed.), Erlbaum, NJ, (2006), pp. 803–821. 34 [32] Lee, H. M., Cheng, Y. W., Rai, S. and Depickere, A. What affect student cognitive style in the development of hypermedia learning system? Computers and Education, 45, (2005), 1–19. [33] Lin, H. and Chen, L. Discovering learning pattern in different cognitive style of learners. Proceedings of the Third International Conference on Convergence and Hybrid Information Technology, 2, (2008), 268–273. [34] Minetou, C. G., Chen, S. Y. and Liu, X. Investigation of the use of navigation tools in web-based learning: A data mining approach. International Journal of Human–Computer Interaction, 24(1), (2008), 48–67. [35] Mishra, P. and Yadav, A. Using hypermedia for learning complex concepts in chemistry: a qualitative study on the relationship between prior knowledge, beliefs, and motivation. Education and Information Technologies 11 (1), (2006), 33–69. [36] Mitchell, T. J. F., Chen, S. Y. and Macredie, R. D. Cognitive Styles and Adaptive Web-based Learning. Psychology of Education Review. 29(1), (2005a), 34-42. [37] Mitchell, T. J. F., Chen, S. Y. and Macredie, R. D. Hypermedia learning and prior knowledge: Domain expertise vs. system expertise. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 21(1), (2005b), 53–64. [38] Mitchell, T. J. F., Chen, S. Y. and Macredie, R. D. The Relationship between Web enjoyment and students’ perceptions and learning using a Web based tutorial. Learning, Media and Technology. 30(1), (2005c), 29-42. [39] Mueller-Kalthoff, T. and Moeller, J. The effects of graphical overviews, prior knowledge, and self-concept on hypertext disorientation and learning achievement. Journal of Educational Multimedia & Hypermedia, 12(2), (2003), 117-134. [40] Mustafa, K. Individual Learner Differences In Web-based Learning Environments: From Cognitive, Affective and Social-cultural Perspectives. Turkish Online Journal of Distance Education-TOJDE, Volume: 6 Number: 4 Article: 2, (2005). [41] Myers, M.D. Qualitative Research in Business & Management. Sage Publications, London, (2009). 35 [42] Nielsen, J. Designing web usability: The practice of simplicity. USA: New Rider Publishing, (2000). [43] Pallant, J. SPSS, Survival Manual, (2001). [44] Pardue, J.H., Landry, J.P. and Kyper, K. Look ahead and look-behind shortcuts in large item category hierarchies: The impact on search performance. Journal of Interaction with computers, 21, 4, (2009). [45] Parkinson, A., Redmond, J.A. and Walsh, C. Accommodating field-dependence: A cross-over study. In: The 9th Annual Conference on Innovation and Technology in Computer Science Education (ITiCSE 2004), Association for Computing Machinery, (2004), 72–76. [46] Riding, R. Cognitive styles analysis – CSA administration. Birmingham: Learning and Training Technology, (1991). [47] Riding, R., and Rayner, S.G. Cognitive styles and learning strategies. David Fulton Publisher, London, (1998). [48] Ross, S.M., Morrison, G.R., and Lowther, D.L. Educational Technology Research Part and Present: Balancing Rigor and Relevance to Impact School Learning. Contemporary education technology, 1(12), (2010), 17-35. [49] Spyridakis, J.H., Mobrand, K.A., Cuddihy, E., and Wei, C.Y. Using structural cues to guide readers on the Internet. Information Design Journal, 15(3), (2007), 242-259. [50] SPSS for Windows. Release 15.0: SPSS, (2001). [51] Su, Y. and Klein, J. D. Effects of navigation tools and computer confidence on performance and attitudes in hypermedia learning environment. J. Educ. Multimedia Hypermedia, 15(1), (2006), 87–106. [52] Thatcher, A. Web search strategies: The influence of web experience and task type. Information Processing and Management, 44(3), (2008), 1308–1329. [53] Triantafillou, E., Pomportsis, A. and Stavros, D. The design and formative evaluation of an adaptive educational system based on cognitive styles. Computers and Education, 41, (2003), pp. 87-103 36 [54] Umar, I. N. and Maswan, S. The effects of a web-based guided inquiry approach on students’ achievement. Journal of Computers, 2(5), (2007), 38–43. [55] Waniek, J, and Schafer, T. The role of domain and system knowledge on text comprehension an information search in hypermedia. Journal of Educational Multimedia and, Hypermedia v18 n2, (2009), p221-240. [56] Wang, A. The effects of varied instructional aids and field dependence and independence on learners’ structural knowledge in a hypermedia learning environment: Ph.D. dissertation, Chio University, (2007). [57] White, R. W., Dumais, S.T. and Teevan, J. Characterizing the influence of domain expertise on Web search. In Proceedings of WSDM, (2009), 132-141. [58] Witkin, H. A., Moore, C.A., Goodenough, D.R. and Cox, P.W. Field-dependent and field- independent cognitive styles and their educational implications. Review of Educational Research, 47(1), (1977), 1-64. [59] Witkin, H.A., and Goodenough, D.R. Cognitive Styles: Essence and Origins. New York: International Universities Press, Inc, (1981). Figure captions Figure 1: A screen from the HLS, showing the map and link color visual aids Figure 2: A screen from the HLS, showing the breadcrumbs, pagination, link annotation and page labels orientation cues Figure 3: An example of the graphic visualization and highlighting context visual cues Figure 4: An example of the history-based mechanism visual cue Figure 5: A screen from the HLS showing the non-linear structure 37 Figure 6: The HLS and its index navigation tool Table captions Table 1: Distribution of participants according to their cognitive style and levels of DK and CE Table 1: Characteristics of the participants and their assignment to the versions of the HLS Table 2: Analysis of variance of levels of disorientation Table 3: Learning performance results Table 4: Learning performance results related to the length of time taken to complete the practical task Table 5: Results related to attitudes towards the HLS Table 6: Analysis of variance on views related to dependency on the visual instructional aids Table 7: Analysis of variance on views of levels of freedom of navigation Table 8: High level findings in relation to the HLS that provided no instructional aids Table 9: High level findings in relation to the HLS that provided visual instructional aids 38