GENDER AWARENESS AND PREPARATION IN CALIFORNIA TEACHING CREDENTIAL PROGRAMS Rachael Eliza Browne B.S., Brigham Young University, 2007 THESIS Submitted in partial satisfaction of The requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in SOCIOLOGY at CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, SACRAMENTO SUMMER 2011 GENDER AWARENESS AND PREPARATION IN CALIFORNIA TEACHING CREDENTIAL PROGRAMS A Thesis by Rachael Eliza Browne Approved by: __________________________________, Committee Chair Todd Migliaccio, Ph.D. __________________________________, Second Reader Cid Martinez, Ph.D. ____________________________ Date ii Student:Rachael Eliza Browne I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. __________________________, Department Chair Judson Landis, Ph.D. Department of Sociology iii ___________________ Date Abstract of GENDER AWARENESS AND PREPARATION IN CALIFORNIA TEACHING CREDENTIAL PROGRAMS by Rachael Eliza Browne Although girls have slowly gained opportunity and access to public education, college, and a growing variety of occupations, access remains limited and different from those experienced by boys. Sex discrimination has become much more subtle in current society. This form of discrimination has become so institutionalized that it is blindly and tacitly accepted; consequently, relatively little is either planned or actually done to combat it. This study attempts to address and analyze the lack of gender awareness and preparation in our education institutions today through the use of content analysis and interview within California multiple subject teacher credential programs. Results indicate thatthrough a combination of program structure and individual agency, Californiateacher preparation programs need to be updated to include conscious, specific interventions that will arm future teachers with the knowledge and will to recognize, confront, and then ameliorate gender discrimination. _______________________, Committee Chair Todd Migliaccio, Ph.D. _______________________ Date iv DEDICATION This thesis is dedicated to my Grandmother, Geneve Leonard, a gender activist before her time. May this work stand as not only an accreditation to my graduate studies, but to hers as well. v ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to express great appreciation for the time and continual guidance of Dr Cid Martinez and especially, my thesis advisor, Dr. Todd Migliaccio, whose many suggestions undoubtedly made this a better study than it would have been otherwise. Similarly, I am forever thankful to my father, Dr Roger Ekins, for introducing me to Sociology, and for showing me, though example, the importance of good writing and dedicated teaching. Lastly, I thank my husband, Jarrod Browne, for his patience, love, and support. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Dedication……………………………………………………………………………………. v Acknowledgment…………………………………………………………………………….. vi List of Tables………………………………………………………………………………… ix List of Figures………………………………………………………………………………... x Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION……………………………………………………………………….. 1 Statement of the Problem............................................................................................. 3 2. LITERATURE REVIEW...................................................................................................10 Title IX.........................................................................................................................11 Curricula and Textbooks..............................................................................................12 Gender Differences Observed in the Classroom..........................................................18 Testing Vs. Grades.......................................................................................................23 The Girls’ or Boys’ Crisis?..........................................................................................31 Later in the Adult Job World....................................................................................... 39 Single-Sex Education: A Solution?............................................................................. 45 Looking to Teaching Credential Programs..................................................................49 Summary......................................................................................................................55 3. METHODS.........................................................................................................................57 Content Analysis..........................................................................................................59 Interviews.....................................................................................................................61 Sample..........................................................................................................................62 Limitations....................................................................................................................65 4. FINDINGS..........................................................................................................................68 Content Analysis Findings...........................................................................................68 Interview Findings....................................................................................................... 76 Defining Gender.......................................................................................................... 77 vii Teacher Impact.............................................................................................................81 Training........................................................................................................................88 Division of the Sexes................................................................................................... 96 5. DISCUSSION.................................................................................................................. 107 Where Do We Go From Here?.................................................................................. 113 Conclusion…………………………………………………………………………. 114 Bibliography……………………………………………………………………………….. 118 viii LIST OF TABLES Page 1. Table 1: Program Content Analysis Scores………………………………………….69 2. Table 2: Content Analysis Measurement Percentages……………………………… 75 ix LIST OF FIGURES Page 1. Figure 1: Program Content Analysis Scores………………………………………….70 x 1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION While the record of racial injustice is at the forefront of our national conscience, history books still do not tell the story of profound sexism at school. Few people realize that today’s girls continue a three-hundred-year old struggle for full participation in America’s education system (Sadker and Sadker 1995: 15). Throughout the years, girls have slowly gained opportunity and access to public education, college, and a growing variety of occupations. Regardless, access remains limited and different from those experienced by boys. In 1972, Congress passed Title IX, which stated that sex discrimination in schools is illegal with respect to athletics, career counseling, medical services, financial aid, admissions practices, and the treatment of students. The government even went so far as to say that those who did not comply with Title IX would lose their federal funding. Nonetheless, from 1972 to 1991 not one school lost federal funding due to sex discrimination (Sadker and Sadker 1995). Though there is still discrimination found within gender stereotyped courses and high stakes testing for college admission, improvement has been made. Before the creation of Title IX, female students were often not even allowed the option of taking courses such as auto mechanics or criminal justice. Likewise, male students were also often barred from enrolling in home economics. Women seeking admission to the New York State College of Agriculture were required to score 30 to 40 points higher than their male peers (Zittleman 2007). 2 While the implementation of Title IX has impacted the access to college degrees and the resources affiliated with Universities for women, it did not completely erase sex discrimination in schools. While it would be nice to imagine that sex discrimination in our schools simply did not occur over that first nineteen year period that Title IX had been initiated, history significantly remembers those years differently. Despite the fact that a law was put into place, many simply did not take it seriously—whether on the compliance or the enforcement side. For example, after Title IX, pregnancy was still grounds for expelling teenage mothers, but not fathers. Many schools spent twenty times as much on boys’ sports than on girls’ sports. One college even awarded ten times as many scholarships to males as to equally qualified females. While complaints were made concerning these rather egregious cases of discrimination, the paperwork was merely piled up and ignored (Sadker and Sadker 1995). Though today we rarely hear of such obvious acts of sex discrimination in our schools, we would be just as misinformed to think that it simply does not occur today, just as it “did not” from 1972 to 1991. Sex discrimination is much more subtle today. In general, it seems that this discrimination does not occur because society does not take it seriously or care to promote equity, but possibly it is because this form of discrimination has become so institutionalized that it is blindly and tacitly accepted; consequently, relatively little is either planned or actually done to combat it. While our general consciousness with respect to gender discrimination has likely been raised, our personal and collective commitment to do something about it may not have been. This study 3 attempts to address and analyze the lack of gender awareness and preparation in our education institutions today through the use of teacher credential programs. Statement of the Problem Gender discrimination researched by Smith and Hung (2008) has found that much of society still holds tightly to the belief that females are not as capable as males within the areas of mathematics and science. In general, very few individuals today come out and blatantly state this sexist belief. However, when women who are just as equally qualified and committed to their occupation as men are in these fields experience a higher turnover rate due to dissatisfaction with research support, advancement opportunities, and free expression of ideas, there is strong indication that sexism persists (Xu 2008). Many women, who are equally qualified as their male counterparts, are given fewer opportunities for promotion and leadership. This differential treatment and discrimination based on gender, however, did not start at the adult career level – it began in the classroom. Smith and Hung (2008) found that the reason we see gender differences in mathematical achievement is due to cultural influences in the classroom and not ability. Though this information is already known and accepted by many researchers, it is still not common opinion in schools and mainstream society. Taking for granted that we have “come a long way,” far too many have their heads buried in the sand when it comes to gender discrimination. Because these discriminatory practices are still embraced by many, children are being given subtle lessons about their gender and abilities. Girls who absorb this 4 message will often have low expectations for their success in math, coupled with the misperception that their chances for success in the science fields are limited (Smith and Hung 2008). Accepting these myths can easily confirm sex-based stereotypes, which can account for the low test scores and low number of women we see participating in these fields later in life, all of which only feeds the cycle of lower expectations for women. However, if the opportunity is seized in the classroom, teachers have the ability to break down these myths and close the gender gap in education by educating themselves through proper certification programs, promoting learning techniques that work for all students, and promoting equity through their daily equal treatment and encouragement to all students in all subjects. School classrooms are an arena perfect for harvesting a societal change because they already serve as a place for learning and developing one’s identity that is carried on throughout life. Also by becoming more aware of gender discrimination, we will be able to better understand all other forms of discrimination that we face today (Connolly 1994). Besides sex discrimination based on ability and opportunities in math and science, Sadker and Sadker (1995) have found classroom discrimination practices ranging from boys being called on more than girls, boys receiving better and more instruction from their teachers, girls having things done for them rather than shown to them when help is needed, and gender biased classroom activities and curricula. The one area in which these researchers found girls were recognized more than boys in the classroom concerned their appearance, which of course only further feeds gender-based stereotypes. 5 Through participant observation research, such as that administered by Sadker and Sadker, accounts for sex discrimination enacted by teachers in the classroom, it is not the intent of this paper to in any way claim that teachers use differential gender treatment intentionally, or that they hold anything less than the success of their students at heart. The purpose is, however, to point to the fact that though researchers are aware of much of the sex discrimination occurring in our classrooms today, most educators and administrators do not share that same awareness of subtle, subconscious, teacher bias and influence created through differential treatment based on gender, and how that influence can perpetuate stereotypes and negatively influence a student for life (Sadker and Sadker 1995; Sadker et al. 2009; Hamilton, Blumenfeld, Akoh and Miura 1991; Fang 1996; Shepardson and Pizzini 1992). While some teachers may be aware of some arguments concerning gender differences in academic performance concerning math, science, and reading, very rarely are they aware of the differential treatment they give concerning student interaction, feedback, or often why we see those differences in academic performance. Though some teachers are aware differential treatment occurs, they are not educated on how to change it, much less identify it when it occurs. It is not enough to merely know that there are differences in performance without knowing why or how they could be changed. The question, then, is how can we both identify and combat the subtle and subconscious sex discrimination in our classrooms today? To many, it may seem, by the lack of adequate solutions proposed and the continuation of discrimination, that perhaps 6 the problems of gender discrimination are too well fused into the other aspects of society, and that if they did not occur in the classroom, they would occur somewhere else. While many have suggested that teachers need to become more aware of the problems, they do not provide a solid plan for how to heighten awareness among all teachers (Stein, Stuen, Carnine, and Long 2001; Entwisle, Alexander, and Olson 2007; Shepardson and Pizzini 1992; Fang 1996; Smith and Hung 2008). Nonetheless, I argue that even subtle and subconscious sex discrimination can be significantly reduced by teachers becoming educated about the issue at hand. Sadker and Sadker (1994) observed that hidden lessons of gender discrimination are rooted in elementary school, exacerbated in high school, and are then fully enabled in the college classroom and work force. Though certainly schools are not the first place gender discrimination lessons are learned, the educational environment is a strong enforcer that shapes and socializes the adult-gendered world. While it would be impossible, or at least very difficult and certainly undesirable, to go into every home and demand that parents raise their children a particular way, it is nonetheless possible for us to more assertively shape the great enabler and twelve year (or more) enforcer: the public school system. From studies such as Rosenthal and Jacobson’s (1992, originally published in 1968) famous Oak School Experiment, we have seen how greatly the knowledge given to a teacher can potentially influence a student’s success or failure. Rosenthal and Jacobson divulged to school teachers the names of a number of students who were expected to excel in their classrooms that year based on IQ testing. Eight months later the accuracy 7 of their assessment was completely confirmed. Those students who were reported to excel scored much higher than they had on their previous IQ scores, while other students, who were expected to score about the same as their first test, did just that. But these expectations were a fraud; those students marked to excel in the classroom were not actually based on their IQ scores, but were drawn at random by the researchers. Because the teachers believed these students were to excel, they were more likely to encourage, give attention to, and push those students academically. Just as these children were influenced by their teachers’ academic expectations, children today are influenced by their teachers’ gendered expectations. Expectations, whether they be false or founded, can influence reality. Just as Rosenthal and Jacobson gave what the teachers believed was information about which students were going to excel, teaching credential programs can in fact truly inform tomorrow’s teachers about ways to help their students excel and become equals in education and society. Teaching credential programs are the foundation from which classroom etiquette and methods are born. As seen in the Oak School Experiment, teachers can makes students thrive with the information they are given. Likewise, teaching credential programs today can provide tomorrow’s teachers with the knowledge that all of their students “show potential to excel” and that they should be treated as such. One area that few researchers have provided as a source for a solution is in teaching credential programs. All elementary and secondary level teachers are required to go through such a program (California Commission on Teacher Credentialing 2007; 8 2009). Recent efforts have been made in many credential programs to include courses and curricula concerning equity and avoiding differential treatment based on race (Katsarou 2009; Smith 2009). Similarly, many budding teachers are being trained to employ greater sensitivity toward English as a Second Language students (ESL), and those students coming from low income families. In a five year study done by Oliveira and Athanases (2007), they explored the impact a California State University English Language Learners (ELL) focused teaching credential program had on its graduates. Through a combination of surveys and focus groups, the researchers received a very high response rate from these new teachers (1-3 years teaching after completion of the program) who felt that their program had prepared them very well to teach ELLs and promote equity both inside and outside of their classrooms more frequently and effectively than many of their colleagues. Participants in this study Reported advocating for ELLs in classrooms by creating and maintaining safe environments for English-language use and development, differentiating instruction and designing interventions for ELLs, and responding to sociopolitical issues related to race, language, and class (2007:213). Although the CSU program from this study did an excellent job of preparing its future teachers to combat issues relating to language, race, and class, it was missing another vital inequality that is also found in our classrooms today: gender. My hypothesis is that few credential programs place a concerted effort on identifying and effectively combating gender discrimination and that a similar focus in these programs could produce positive 9 results like those found in Oliveira and Athanases’ study. If teachers were given the proper education about gender discrimination, including its history in education, and— most importantly—methods for breaking down this discrimination in their future classrooms, every member of society would potentially benefit. Though the fight for true gender equity in the classroom has persisted for hundreds of years, and researchers have been addressing the topic for decades, this discrimination still exists and it cannot continue to go unnoticed and unaccounted for by society and by our schools if we desire a society that truly provides equal opportunity for all. Girls and boys, men and women, all suffer in one way or another from these discriminatory practices. Just as we have seen teaching credential programs used as a learning tool and forum for race, class, and ESL student awareness, we should be able to harness these programs towards gender awareness as well. Though researchers are aware of much of the sex discrimination occurring in our classrooms today, most educators and administrators do not share that same awareness of subtle, subconscious, teacher bias and influence created through differential treatment based on gender, and how that influence can perpetuate stereotypes and negatively influence a student for life. Let us now raise this awareness and understanding. If the opportunity is seized in the classroom, teachers will have the ability to break down these myths and close the gender gap in education by educating themselves through proper certification programs, promoting learning techniques that work for all students, and promoting equity through their daily equal treatment and encouragement to all students in all subjects. 10 Chapter 2 LITERATURE REVIEW The source for gender discrimination in education is often argued as being either biological, institutionalized, or socialized. While some (Buss 1998; Ichino and Moretti 2009) would argue that gender is more of a biological matter, for many other scholars and researchers gender goes greatly beyond the natural world and into the socially constructed. It is argued that everything from the social structure of tests and books used in the classroom, to the messages provided at home and via multi-media, can teach gender discrimination (Risman 2004; Warmoth 1998). And while it is true that these institutionalized facets have great impact on gender discrimination, institutionalization theories are missing one key factor: agency. Although always influenced by institutionalism, humans are thinking actors who are capable of making active decisions outside of those set by the main institution (Markstrom and Hallden 2008). Though institutions exist, we can be socialized in how we will react to them and if we will choose to accept their messages; it is in this particular cannon of thought that I will focus. Through socialization we can attempt to dismantle the stagnant view of gender in society if utilized correctly; however, socialization can also serve as a vice to only exacerbate discrimination as well. Just as racism has been created through prior institutions from earlier history, modern socialization and programs to promote diversity and understanding have diminished racism in society (Smith 2009; Katsarou 2009). 11 Similarly, I argue that positive socialization through teacher preparation programs can help diminish gender discrimination in education. Title IX As explained above, Title IX was passed by Congress in 1972 in an attempt to make sex discrimination in schools illegal. Punishment for violating the law was loss of federal funding. However, though many schools are in violation with Title IX, not one of them has lost a penny of funding from governmental investigations. Not even one case was brought against a school until Franklin v. Gwinnett County Public Schools in 1992, where it was determined that an institution could be held liable for its individuals who participated in sexual discrimination, and that plaintiffs could sue for monetary damages. As a result of this ruling, some schools have been required to pay damages and attorney fees for cases brought to court by individuals (Zittleman 2007), but none has lost federal funding directly as the result of Title IX violations. According to Zittleman (2007), congress completely eliminated the funds under Title IV of the Civil Rights Act of 1964, which supported having state Title IX coordinators and statewide gender equity training and that only about half of the states in the U.S. still have a designated employee to coordinate efforts to comply with Title IX. Zittleman also found that the Women’s Education Equity Act, which was the only federal program that focused specifically on increasing educational opportunities for women, has had its funding completely eliminated. 12 To make matters worse, funds for programs that support equity in education have also been dramatically cut. Once the Reagan-Bush administration took office, “they conducted systematic search-and-destroy missions on all federal programs that encouraged gender equity” because encouraging women into more careers and education was seen as a threat to the family (Sadker and Sadker 1995:37). Since their publication of Failing at Fairness in 1995 the Sadkers updated their book Still Failing at Fairness in 2000 and added researcher Zittleman to their team. The Sadkers, as well as other researchers who have drawn on their ideas, continued to observe classrooms and unfortunately found that all the same gender discriminatory trends persisted. With over 15 continuous years of published research, Sadker and Sadker have become the primary, and most comprehensive study concerning the topic of gender discrimination in the world of education, and as a result, will be heavily cited throughout this literature review. As will be seen throughout this chapter, their findings, and the findings of other researchers, are of particular relevance to those entering the teaching profession. Curricula and Textbooks One controversial area in which administrators, teachers, and parents all have concern is in the curricula and textbooks implemented in the classroom. Whether it be a debate about teaching creationism vs. Darwinism, abstinence vs. sex education, or traditional vs. modern views on sexual orientation, all parties seem to be aware that these teachings impact how children are socialized to view themselves and the world around 13 them. Students roughly spend between 75 to 90 percent of classroom time using textbooks, and teachers make the majority of their instructional decisions based upon these texts (Farr, Tulley, and Powell 1987; Tyson and Woodward 1989; Sadker et al. 2009). Similarly, when children learn and read about people in non-traditional gender roles and occupations, this positive socialization makes it much easier for them to not limit their own futures to traditional and stereotypical roles and occupations (Crocco and Libresco 2007). When history books include the accomplishments beyond ‘old white men’ to those of women and minorities, children not only learn a more true and complete history, they also learn to respect women and minorities and see the potential in any person to invent, birth a political movement, or stand in a place of great leadership. “Even elementary students can exchange their reliance on a single narrative of history for a prism containing a variety of perspectives to arrive at a more fully realized vision of historical truth” (Crocco and Libresco 2007:129). To provide children with a more accurate and inclusive understanding of history that offer a greater variation in the perspectives for both men and women, it is therefore essential that schools and teachers become more selective in choosing textbooks. As very young children, so many of us dreamed of becoming astronauts, scientists, or even perhaps the President of our country, but how many of us abandoned those dreams? Of course over time our interests do change, but sometimes those interests are changed due to influences in the classroom and often un-intended discouragement from role models. 14 A study by Women on Words and Images (1975) examined over 2,700 stories from over 130 elementary level textbooks. This study included looking not only at the stories themselves, but also at the pictures and language used to describe the characters. The researchers alarmingly found that ratios of boy-centered stories to girl-centered stories were 5:2, adult male characters to adult female characters were 3:1, male biographies to female biographies were 6:1, and male fairy-tale stories to female fairytale stories were 4:1. Males overwhelmingly outnumbered females in every category. “When girls and women were included, they were typecast . . . While men were involved in 150 different jobs, women were housewives . . . [and those rare women who were shown as having a career] worked outside the home only as teachers and nurses” (Sadker et al. 2009:88). Publishers took notice of these findings and genuinely made an effort to improve children’s textbooks. Guidelines for removing racism and sexism were drawn up, focusing on how to include a more fair portrayal of human ability and potential. In her study looking at children’s social studies textbooks Bradford (2008) found that Despite the good intentions of curriculum and textbook writers to increase the content about women in elementary social studies knowledge, the failure to reconceptualize the purposes for social studies and the concomitant topics and perspectives in social studies results in a reification of taken-for-granted gender asymmetries, the valorization of men and their contributions, and the denigration of women and dismissal of their contributions (2008:6). 15 Although this study was done in Canada, it is still very applicable to the United States, as well as demonstrates how far reaching this issue still is. Though textbook writers and editors made an attempt to decrease sexism by including more females, they did not take into account that how the females were being portrayed also played a key role in perpetuating sexism. Specifically concerning the subject of history, females are still being left out of today’s classrooms. As Sadker et al. (2009) paraphrased Winston Churchill, “history is written by those in power, and women are not in power” (2009:89). These researchers have spoken to many different classes about gender issues in education. One exercise they would often have students participate in was their “famous men and women” challenge. Students were asked to write down as many famous men and women as they could think of, not including athletes, entertainers, or just by adding "Mrs." to the last name of a president. On average, students would list 12 men and 5 women. When the Sadkers (1995) originally began their research 15 years earlier, the average was 11 men and 3 women. Clearly, despite the passage of fifteen years, not much had changed. The highest number of males ever listed was 34 and nine for females. Even though President Jimmy Carter originally designated the week of March 8th as National Women’s History Week in 1980, and later by request of the National Women’s History Project, in 1987 it was expanded to be a full month long recognition, it seems that it has not been as widely celebrated and used as an education tool for special educational events based on race or ethnicity, such as Black History Month every 16 February (The U.S. National Archives and Records Administration). Such promotions we have seen by the education system to include Black History Month could explain the jump in student identification in the famous women exercise. While most of the research that has been done on sexism in textbooks has focused on how females are represented or under-represented, it is important to note that sexism applies to both females AND males. Evans and Davies (2000) examined first, third, and fifth grade literature books. Not only did they find that Title IX did not block all female stereotypes, but it also failed to block males being stereotyped as being aggressive, argumentative, and competitive. Evans and Davies argue that although there is still room for much progress, portrayals of females have become more frequent, positive, and less stereotyped, but that males are rarely portrayed with anything other than male stereotypes. Evans and Davies (2000) reviewed 132 characters in 82 children's stories. They found that 54% of the characters were male and 46% were female, which is a much better split than that found by Weitzman in 1975. Nonetheless, the percentile ratios of males to females in the following categories suggest a continuing proclivity for gender stereotyping: being aggressive 24:5, being argumentative 21:7, and being competitive 36:11. Male characters were also significantly portrayed less as affectionate (18:33), emotionally expressive (14:33), passive (8:30), and tender (7:20) than were the female characters. Not only is it damaging to represent female characters so often as passive actors, but it is also damaging not to show positive examples of male characters 17 expressing affection and emotion. People, no matter what gender, should not be stereotyped. While this study showed that males are depicted with both masculine and feminine traits, it is still clear that the masculine traits continue to outnumber those which are feminine. In the end, Evans and Davies felt that there was not enough of a representation of feminine traits for male characters to truly push the stereotyped boundaries of masculinity that our society has created. Researchers Hamilton, Anderson, Broaddus, and Young (2006) also explored the idea of sexism in children’s books. In their study, 200 distinguished children’s books published between 1995 and 2001 were examined. Unfortunately, they discovered that the books told twice as many male-centered as female-centered stories. Illustrations also depicted males 50 percent more than females. While happily female characters were shown as lawyers, doctors, and scientists, they were still mainly seen as passive characters, watching their more active male peers and family members at work and play. Similar to Evans and Davies' (2000) findings that male characters were shown lacking feminine traits, Hamilton and Anderson (2006) saw father characters in their stories depicted in less than half of the books. When such figures were presented, they were shown as hands-off parents, rarely showing emotion or care towards their children. While textbook publishers and editors seem to be trying to diminish sexism by increasing the number of female characters and also portraying those characters with some masculine traits and occupations, it is also important to incorporate feminine traits in male characters and not relegate female characters to predominately passive roles. 18 While we see female characters are being given a greater range of behaviors in stories, when they are predominantly the only ones in passive roles, this only perpetuates the stereotype that females are passive, and males are aggressive. Feminine and masculine traits and female and male contributions throughout history should be equally recognized and valued. One of the best ways to increase that recognition and value is through depictions of role models displaying this balance to children and by teaching a more inclusive history in the classroom. In order to bring such inclusiveness and balance into the classroom, teachers will need to become more selective about their textbook choices. While a great extent of how textbooks are chosen for classroom use is dictated by the institutions of the state and school district, teachers are allowed voice in the selection process (Farr et al. 1987). In order to make educated decisions, teachers themselves must first be socialized and educated about what is important to look for and avoid in their next textbook edition. Likewise, teachers can be prepared with supplemental lessons to counteract any negative socialization brought into the classroom by institutionalized textbooks. Researchers such as Stein, Stuen, Carnine, and Long (2001) believe that textbook adoption committees can be implemented and successful, but only if such members have been appropriately trained and are given sufficient time to actually review the different materials; however, they do not provide any recommendation for how such proper training should be obtained. 19 Gender Differences Observed in the Classroom As pointed out by Allen, Cantor, Grady, and Hill (1997), it is often understood that whoever talks in the classroom is the person with the power. This firstly would include the teacher, and then those students who are most engaged and active. Through their research and experience in the classroom, Allen et al. found that far too often the classroom becomes an arena for a competition of who can answer the quickest. While they found that often these quick answers are sufficient and correct, they also found that this learning style does not permit room for critical thinking and more thoughtful students to ever have a chance at raising their hands and participating in their own educations. Other students observed by the Sadkers (1995) became shy and some teachers chose to only call on those students who seemed to want to talk. They found that once these trends are set into place, children hold onto their pattern for participation, or lack of participation, often into their college years. Sadker et al. (2009) found significant gender differences in this classroom participation: Boys call out significantly more often than girls. Sometimes what they say has little or nothing to do with the teacher’s questions. Whether male comments are insightful or irrelevant, teachers respond to them. However, when girls call out, there is a fascinating phenomenon. Perhaps the teacher sees this as a warning sign and suddenly remembers the rule about raising your hand before you talk. 20 And those not as assertive as the animated male students are deftly and swiftly put back in their place (2009:66). Instances like this could serve as a quick, unintentional, gender socialization lesson for all: Boys and girls are not treated the same in the classroom and boys are allowed to be more assertive and even raucous and girls are expected to be more passive and "proper;" therefore, girls and boys may also be treated differently outside of the classroom and held to different standards. In many of the classrooms that the Sadkers (1995 and 2009) observed, time and time again they saw boys flinging their arms in the air to get attention while girls would sit quietly with their hands simply raised. The observed teacher, wanting to help release some of the energy of the boys, would inevitably call on them more often than the girls. After trying to participate without being called on, the Sadkers observed that girls would slowly lose interest and begin to not even raise their hands for the rest of the lesson. The Sadkers discovered that white male students were the most likely to receive teacher attention, followed by males of color, white females, and then lastly females of color. At the end of the classroom observations the Sadkers would meet with the teachers and often show them through video or notes how they neglected the female students immensely more than the male students. The teachers were subsequently embarrassed and always explained how shocked they were and how unaware they were of their own actions and that they meant well and were only trying to keep the classroom atmosphere more calm and in control. Teachers further explained that they felt the male 21 students simply required more one-on-one help and felt that since the girls were getting good grades or simply behaving, that they did not need as much help. Researchers Hamilton, Blumenfeld, Akoh and Miura (1991) also found through classroom observation that teachers pay more attention, particularly negative attention, to male students. However, one main difference with their findings was that they did not feel that the observed behavior of the male and female students varied significantly for this particular study. But it should be noted that this was in comparison to classrooms in Japan, where the difference in behavior between male and female students was very significant. Hamilton et al. also found that there was no difference in student behavior when the teacher was male or female. Sadker and Sadker (1995) found that in addition to boys being called on more than girls, boys also received better and more instruction from their teachers. With boys, teachers would provide constructive criticism and point out problems in their work for the boys to solve themselves. With girls, teachers usually would do things for the students when they were having trouble rather than showing them how to do it or encourage them to problem solve further on their own. "Boys were more likely to be praised, corrected, helped, and criticized – all reactions that foster student achievement. Girls received the more superficial 'Okay’ and boys gained clear feedback” (1995:55). When the Sadkers would confront the teachers about the differences in the depth of their feedback, teachers--especially male--would often respond that they do not like to tell a girl that anything is wrong with her work because they were fearful that they would 22 upset her and that if she broke into tears they would have no idea how to handle that situation. Though these teachers have well-meaning intentions, they simply are not doing their female students any favors with their sugar-coating. If students do not know that they did something wrong or specifically what they did wrong, it is nearly impossible for them to learn from those mistakes. The one area that the Sadkers found girls were recognized more than boys in the classroom concerned their appearance, which of course only further feeds gender-based stereotypes. Although much research points to girls not receiving enough feedback and encouragement in the classroom, researcher Gagnon (1998) found that girls were getting their push for success from somewhere else. Gagnon found evidence that girls are often encouraged more to succeed in school than their male peers. This encouragement comes from their mothers and also from themselves, in what Gagnon refers to as a form of early feminism where girls are wanting to prove themselves to their male peers. One of the main things girls do to succeed in school more than boys is to learn to adapt and conform to their classroom environments by following the set rules and behaviors that are expected by teachers. Though it is wonderful that girls are being encouraged in their educational endeavors, encouragement in good behavior, though it results in good grades, is not the long-term educational encouragement that is needed for either boys or girls, as conformity is not always a desideratum. MacMillian, Gresham, Lopez and Bocian (1996) also found that behavior was possibly being incorrectly linked to academic success. In this study they explored a 23 program called Student Study Teams (SST) for children in grades two through four. This program was completely based on teacher referrals of students that they felt were at risk for school failure. Not only did the researchers find trends regarding race, they were also found concerning gender. There was an over representation of males in SST. Females placed in the program were there only for poor achievement, while males were placed for some poor achievement combined heavily with behavioral issues. These findings clearly show that teachers were basing their determinations heavily on behavior for males rather than just their academic achievements. Researchers found that the teachers rated female students higher on overall academic competence. The above studies further suggest the importance of teacher education. If future teachers have not been trained to recognize how inappropriate gender differences are unwittingly perpetuated and sometimes even encouraged within classroom socialization, little will be done to ameliorate those differences. As will be seen in the next section, these differences can have major impact on how student performance is judged. Testing Vs. Grades Trends have been showing for years that males are scoring higher on high-stakes tests, but are receiving lower grades in the classroom than females (Jackson 1998). This makes one wonder which is a better tool--test scores or grades--for measuring a student’s academic understanding and performance. Today, due to programs such as No Child Left Behind (NCLB), many would argue that test scores hold more value than grades. In addition to NCLB, colleges place great importance on high-stakes tests such as the SAT, 24 ACT, and GRE. It is argued that such high-stakes testing is a solid predictor for one’s expected first year college grades; however, studies show that on average most males receive a lower GPA than predicted and females receive a higher GPA than predicted from their testing scores (Sadker et al. 2009). Hoffman and Lowitzki (2005) found that high schools grades are actually a stronger predictor for success than standardized testing scores, especially for minority students. But regardless of these facts, collegiate institutions continue to hold testing scores in high regard and male/female patterns continue. Even during the early years of education, students are scrutinized through standardized testing; one of the best known is the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP). The NAEP is administered to students in grades four, eight, and twelve. Gender patterns from this test have long been established. In the earlier years, females achieve the leading scores in reading, writing, math, and civics, while tying with males in history scores, and falling behind males in science scores. However, by the time testing is done in the twelfth grade, the results are very different. Females continue to have an advantage in the verbal subjects, but males take over math, science, and social science scores (National Assessment of Educational Progress 2007). Though the differences in scores between the sexes is relatively small, and the NAEP generally reports close to equal scores for males and females, females are still receiving higher grades on their report cards. 25 Though the score gaps of the NAEP are quite small, those found in higher-stake tests such as the Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT), American College Testing Program (ACT), and Graduate Record Exam (GRE) are much larger. Concerning the SAT In 1967, boys scored 10 points higher than girls in mathematics; by 1987, the boys’ lead grew to 24 points; between 1987 and 2006, the boys’ math lead grew again, to between 33 and 41 points. As with the PSAT, boys also outscored girls on the verbal section, although by a much smaller margin. Boys outperform girls on both the SAT verbal (now called critical reading) as well as math sections, and have since 1972 (Sadker et al. 2009:183). As explained earlier, the purpose of such exams is to predict a student’s performance for the first year of college, and it does this quite falsely time and time again. In 2005 attempts were made to modify both the PSAT and SAT to include a new writing section to help raise female testing scores since writing is generally an area in which females are seen to outperform males. And though females outscored males on average 11 points higher on the new writing section, males still outscored females by a few points in the critical reading section and more than 30 points higher in the mathematics section, thus continuing to leave a large overall scoring gap (College Board 2007). Though the SAT is the dominant test for college admittance, the ACT is another test recognized by many colleges. The main difference between the two exams is that the ACT’s sections are based around more classroom subjects: English, mathematics, science, reading, and an optional writing test. “In 2007, the average male score was 21.2, 26 and the average female score was 21.0 . . .” which is a significantly smaller gap on a much smaller scale than that of the SAT (Sadker et al. 2009:185). Besides the ACT proving more variety in its subject matter, there is no penalty for guessing at questions when one is unsure of the answer. While girls generally outperform males on written sections of tests, males completely dominate multiple-choice sections (which make up the majority of high-stakes tests). Simply put, males are more likely to use risk and guess answers than females. Many multiple-choice high-stakes tests such as the NAEP and SAT give a penalty for wrong answers, so if one can narrow down the answer to two options, guessing is worthwhile, but if one had little or no idea (or lack of confidence), guessing is out of the question, and a student would be wise to mark the option “I don’t know.” Significantly more females than males answer a question they are unsure of with “I don’t know” (Sadker et al. 2009:190-191). Though the results from the ACT continue to incorrectly predict first year college performance, females, if given a choice, would be wise to take the ACT over the SAT. Similar problems are found in other institutionalized high-stakes tests such as the Graduate Management Admission Test (GMAT), (MCAT), and (LSAT). From interviews with females taking such admissions tests the Sadkers discovered that first, testing scores were not similar to how grades are awarded in the classroom; some women’s experience was that if you worked hard enough, you could get the grade. Second, that these testing scores, which do not predict what they claim, are keeping many deserving women from entering top schools, and later top jobs. And without female role 27 models in the top jobs, how do we expect young girls to see themselves in such occupations later in life? How can we expect that this socialized subtle gender lesson is not being soaked up by younger students? It’s no wonder why, when young elementary school children are asked to draw a doctor, almost every picture displays a man (Sadker and Sadker 1995). They are simply producing what society is showing them. So what could account for the clear gender gaps seen in testing scores? Sadker, Sadker, and Zittleman suggest four ways in which they believe tests could be lowering female scores. The first is found in testing question content. They found that males perform better on questions with male characters, while girls achieve more on questions with female characters or an equal number of males and females. They also found that some researchers believe that even testing questions without male or female characters still often portray topics which are more familiar to boys than girls, thus creating an often un-fair advantage. Carol Dwyer (1976) found that when females achieved higher scores on the SAT verbal section, ETS responded by making an effort to help boys by adding questions pertaining to boy-friendly topics such as politics, business, and sports. It worked. Males’ scores rose and have continued to surpass females on the verbal section. However, Dwyer then points to the fact that no similar efforts were made to aid females with their lower scores in the math section. It seems that when females are outperforming males the testing organizations find it necessary to “re-balance” scores, but when males are outperforming females it is merely accepted, ignored, or met with little effort for change. 28 The second way in which Sadker et al. (2009) believed tests could be lowering female scores was through the massive use of multiple-choice in tests, with penalties being given for incorrectly guessing, as explained earlier. If more balance were given between written and multiple-choice sections, and more tests followed the ACT’s example of eliminating penalties for wrong answers, there is good reason to believe that female testing scores would rise and balance more with those of males. A third way in which testing designs can be held responsible for lower female scores is found in timed testing. Sadker et al. (2009) found the males perform better on timed tests, such as the SAT or GRE, while females are more likely to succeed when no time limit is given. As was observed with guessing patterns on multiple-choice questions, we see that females are more cautious and thoughtful in their answers, being less likely to take a risk or quickly jump to a conclusion. Females have been found more likely to completely work out a problem, consider more than one answer, and to then check their answers than males. Though these skills are taught, encouraged, and rewarded in the classroom institution, they quickly soak up a students’ valuable time during a test. They also found that when tests are not timed, female scores greatly improve, and male scores either remain the same or slightly increase. Though having a time limit lifted from high-stakes tests could admittedly result in costing testing centers more money to pay test administrators to wait for students to finish, removing the time constraints would benefit many females as well as some males. 29 Though females fall short when it comes to high-stakes tests, they dominate in achieving high grades in the classroom, which suggests that in some respects, boys, too, may be treated unfairly. One would expect that testing scores and grades would reflect similar academic performance, but nothing could be farther from the truth. As explored above, we have seen how high-stakes tests can be biased towards males, so it is only fair to examine grading practices and how they may also contain bias towards females. Sadker et al. (2009) interviewed high schools students and teachers to get their opinions on the issue of differential treatment or favoritism playing a role is how teachers give out grades. One male student recounted a teacher recently returning an assignment in which most students received a low grade. Some of the females who were marked down got upset almost to the point of tears and the male teacher then let the females re-do the assignment and then received the points back. Some of the male students who were also marked down felt that they would not be allowed to re-do their assignment like the females had because they didn’t react emotionally to the situation, but instead handled it in a more socially acceptable frustrated and confrontational masculinized manner. When a group of English teachers was asked to explain how they assigned grades, the consensus was that a high grade was achieved through “hard work, good attendance, handing in assignments on time, classroom tests, quality materials prepared outside of class, positive attitudes and conforming to prescribed style, including good handwriting” (Sadker et al. 2009:195). Many of the teachers agreed that good behavior had become blended into academic achievement when it came to assigning grades. With such logic, it 30 quickly becomes clear how females could be outperforming males in the classroom. Following the rules, good handwriting, and conforming to the socialization of the classroom are all things that females typically have less difficulty fulfilling than males. One teacher specifically recounted how she knew which students would be at the top of the class before the first day had even ended. She explained how she knew one girl was a “top student” because “She does everything neatly and on time and obeys rules and sets a good example. She reads beautifully, [and] gets along with everybody” (Sadker et al. 2009:196). She described another student as an immediate trouble maker and lower achieving student because the girl was always showing up to class in jeans that were too big, with messy hair. The teacher explained that if the student could not maintain herself in a proper way, that she would likewise not be able to maintain her education. By giving high grades to females based heavily on things beyond actual academic performance, teachers are only doing an injustice to these students. From the earlier discussed interviews the Sadkers held with female college students, it was made clear that many girls had expected that their high grades would get them into the college of their dreams; however, when it came time to take their high-stakes test, neat handwriting, being well dressed, and sitting quietly in their seat would have no impact on raising their scores to what their teachers had always made them believe was their performance level. Clearly, teachers need to be aware of the extent to which we are holding males and females accountable to two different grading standards, and the harm it can do. They must be socialized and educated first within their own preparation programs about the 31 potential shortfalls in standardized testing with respect to gender equity and armed with strategies that will help them ameliorate those problems. Though it is the institution, and not teachers that writes the standardized tests, teachers can choose to present information and techniques on the side in their classrooms to help socialize females to be able to navigate standardized testing more successfully. Similarly, teachers can choose to utilize a more uniform standard for all grading, but of course such information can only be provided and utilized if teachers have first been made aware of the issues and socialized in how to combat them. The Girls’ or Boys’ Crisis? The media, some researchers, and even some of the greater public have become caught up with pointing fingers and trying to view gender education issues as male versus female, and not as an overall issue for them both. Why? Because creating sides, blame, and opponents sells and is easy. The truth is that differential treatment and socialized stereotyped expectations hurt everyone. In the 1990s researchers birthed the Boy Crisis by announcing that boys are alarmingly less motivated, less focused on academics, less likely to do their homework, less positive about school, more likely to get into trouble, more likely to be left back a grade and to even drop out (Van Houtte 2004). It was also brought to the public’s attention that females had become the majority of college students, and that they were also receiving more degrees. Efforts were then made to help “rescue” boys from the feminine damages that the educational system was inflicting. Programs were created especially for males to re-instate masculinity, such as the Boy 32 Scouts, founded in 1910 (Sadker et al. 2009). But, as researcher Kimmel (2000) explains, “It is not the school experience that “feminizes” boys, but rather the ideology of traditional masculinity that keeps boys from wanting to succeed” (2000:7). Kimmel went on to explain that boys are not engaging in some of their school work because they view it as “girls’ work” and “not real work.” The real problem, therefore, is with society’s definition of masculinity and the strict lines created for males to not cross. Kimmel continued to explain that while many girls do undervalue their abilities and lack confidence, boys are suffering from a “false voice of bravado” that causes them to overvalue their abilities, often resulting in lower grades and achievement. In the classroom, teachers can help to de-construct such harmful socialized guidelines for masculinity by reinforcing the importance of all education and by not bolstering boys’ self-conceptions of ability beyond their actual proven skill. Some of the public became worried by how many female teachers were beginning to replace male teachers in lower division classrooms. But more strong than their worry of feminization of their young boys was their grip on their pocket books. Simply put, female teachers were paid less than male teachers, so it saved the educational system and tax payers significant amounts of money. Because attributes such as good behavior, verbal skills, and neat handwriting, stereotypically seen as those portrayed by girls, were promoted as part of the sought after standard in the classroom, some, such as Sexton (1969) believed that boys were being held up to something that was much more difficult for boys to meet than girls. Sexton 33 explained that boys were being forced to choose between feminization and academic failure. In 2006, in reference to current classroom etiquette and evaluation, a psychologist claimed that “Girl behavior becomes the gold standard. [And] boys are treated like defective girls” (Sadker et al. 2009:200). With female traits being valued in the classroom and females running them, some believed that their boys would have to choose between being “feminized” or failed. In 2006, a male student, with the help of his father, sued his entire school district “for boy-bashing and routinely discriminating against him.” The student argued that his B-minus average was by no means his own fault, but was “the result of sex discrimination: teachers made him follow orders, stay seated, and do what he was told, [which were] clearly unnatural and unhealthy behaviors for a boy.” He went on in his claim to explain that it is simply “in a boy’s nature to rebel, and schools did not get it [and that] . . . all boys’ grades should be raised retroactively” for this terrible injustice (Sadker et al. 2009:201). Some believe that single-sex classrooms can help in repairing the damages they feel were done to boys by the feminist movement, though the struggles of boys in academics were apparent long before the feminist movement even emerged (Sadker et al. 2009). One might expect this claim and others similar to it to have been met with jeers and laughter from on-looking media and the public, but the response was quite the opposite. Countless articles in mainstream magazines and newspapers have been and are still being published, making claims of the all-boy or all-girl crisis, or how our education 34 system is emasculating males. Such messages have been produced by the media for years, only giving one perspective of the complex and misunderstood dilemma. In truth, there is an education crisis today. There are boys that are severely struggling academically, but they are fewer now than ever. “According to the nonpartisan Education Sector and the American Council of Education, most boys are doing better today than they were a decade or two ago” (Sadker et al. 2009:201). While it is true that we still see females outperforming males in reading at all grade levels, it is not new information. “Girls have outperformed boys in reading and writing since 1969,” but in recent years boys have been reducing the reading gap measured by the NAEP from 13 points to now only five points difference (Sadker et al. 2009:202). While it is true that the achievement gap in reading overall for boys is being closed, research by Entwisle, Alexander, and Olson (2007) found that in the early years (starting with first grade) those boys who received meal-subsidies showed lower reading performance than girls, while those boys who were not receiving meal-subsidies matched the reading performance of girls. From their study they concluded that being disadvantaged and coming from homes where there was less socialization of educational encouragement and expectation for boys to succeed in school, coupled with the expectations of their teachers for their behavior to keep them behind, explained in greater depth the gendered reading gap that has been discussed and researched for so many years. It is not so much that this is a crisis for all boys, but only for certain boys and by coming 35 to better understand who those boys are we can learn how to close the true gaps in gender achievement that still exist. Besides closing the overall gap in reading, males have also greatly closed the gap in college enrollment. Over the past 25 years there has been a 10 percent increase in male college enrollment directly out of high school. While degrees earned by minority females has tripled since 1976, we have also seen a great increase in degrees earned by minority males (though not quite as drastic). And though we often hear that there are more females enrolled in colleges, this fact is rarely coupled with the similarly important fact that men make up half or more of the enrollment in the much more prestigious universities, which lead to the more prestigious and higher paying jobs (Sadker et al. 2009). Boys and girls each have their own strengths and weaknesses in our current educational system. While it is true the girls have often outperformed boys in reading and language, it should be noted that boys have outperformed girls at all grade levels in math and generally have a slight lead in geography and science (Sadker et al. 2009:202). In a study done by Hall, Davis, Bolen, and Chia (1999) they found that amongst fifth and eighth grade students, gender differences in math scored on the California Achievement Test had significantly declined to near extinction. They also found that parents’ beliefs and attitudes towards math often greatly impact their children’s performance. “The parents' most advanced math course and their education level were negatively correlated with their negative attitudes toward math. The parents with more advanced math courses 36 and higher education levels tended to have less negative feelings about their own math abilities. That attitude seemed also to influence their children's opinions of their own selfefficacy (belief about one's own performance capabilities) in mathematics” (1999:680). Hyde, Fennema, and Lamon (1990) as cited by Hall et al. (1999) also did not find what they considered to be significant differences in math achievement between boys and girls from elementary through junior high school; however, once in high school into college they found a steady upward trend showing males achieving higher than females. Sadker and Sadker (1995) found that as students progressed into higher education that males were outperforming females and also taking more advanced math and science courses. In Sax’s 1994 study, the idea is developed that girls’ math achievement is often heavily a result of both her self-confidence and exposure to the subject. This is similar to the idea presented by Hall, et al. (1999) for the partial explanation for why some students do not perform well in mathematics. The gap we see in math could very likely not be due to one’s born sex or actual skill, but to something as simple as self-esteem and confidence, which is influenced, to some extent, by the teachers that can provide support and positive socialization for their students. One must then wonder where boys are either getting their extra boost in selfconfidence in mathematics, of where girls are getting their sense of pre-mature failure in the subject. One potential suspect could be linked back to textbooks and curricula. “In a study of elementary mathematics software only 12 percent of the characters were female” (Sadker et al. 2009:89). In addition to biased text books and curricula, numerous studies 37 such as those done by Fang (1996) and Shepardson and Pizzini (1992) have found that both parents and teachers irrationally underestimate the intelligence and academic capabilities of female students. Sadker et al. (2009) also found that such biases are continuing today, explaining that many teachers begin at the start of elementary school believing that their male students are inherently smarter in math than their female students. Ironically, this happens to be the time that we see girls getting better grades as well as near equal score on their standardized math tests as their male peers. This just shows how deeply some aspects of society and negative socialization are rooted into traditional, and often incorrect, assumptions towards gender in education. Awareness through positive socialization has obviously not been raised sufficiently if this perception of females is still being produced in the classroom. Hanna (2003) found that studies by the International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement (IEA) . . . provided convincing evidence that gender differences in achievement vary widely from country to country, with the degree and direction of variation depending greatly on topic and grade level . . . [such findings] indicate, first of all, that some educational systems do provide, wittingly or unwittingly, educational conditions that work to prevent an achievement gap between males and females in mathematics, Second, in showing that gender differences in mathematics achievement vary in magnitude and direction from country to county, the IEA findings call into question the validity of the claim made by a number of 38 researchers that there are innate differences between males and females in mathematical ability (P. 206). These findings from the IEA are significant for the fight for gender equity in education. If one country can learn how to raise awareness and combat mathematical bias then all should be able to follow suit and similarly apply those concepts to other subjects where either sex is apparently falling behind. Similar patterns of females not having confidence, but later participating in more advanced classes have been found in the field of science as well. Mulkey (1988) found that when literature was made available to girls at a young age that depicted female scientists it not only provided girls with a positive attitude towards female scientists, but that because of this early intervention there was a correlation to later recruitment into a science career. Unfortunately, from earlier discussions in this chapter it is known that textbooks and curricula promoting female scientists are often scarce. Research on the relationship between science and gender has found a variety of variables that affect who succeeds and who becomes discouraged from participating and further pursuing a science career. Some such variable range from “Parental attitudes, the media, stereotyping, textbooks, clothing, peers, toys, teacher bias, social influences, discrimination, curricula and classroom dynamics . . . [just to name a few]” (Koch 2007:207). Koch found that recently researchers have been finding some success with programs that include gaining participation of parents and highlighting role models in science fields that are both male and female, white and of color. 39 Though not the only influencing factor, socialization of positive attitudes towards academics from both parents and teachers greatly impacts the success of their children and students. And while it would be impossible to control the socialization that parents provide in their own homes, it is fully ascertainable for teachers to become socialized to promote such positive attitudes, encouragement, and academic techniques to students when they interact with them at school. While new teachers need to be aware of the above concerns, in a very real sense, much of the above can rightly be perceived as merely an "academic" concern. For most people today what really matters is not so much how one scores on a given test, what kinds of grades one receives, or even what kind of college one is admitted to. What really matters, in the end, is how successful one is in his or her chosen profession. In the next section we'll examine that question specifically. Later in the Adult Job World To the extent that gender stereotyping exists, as suggested by the previous discussion, one then asks how those biases influence career opportunities for both men and women and how well both sexes succeed in their chosen professions. Levy, Sadovsky, and Troseth (2000) found when interviewing children from pre-school and primary level education that students already have a strong sense of the gendered adult job world. Interviews indicated that the children, especially boys, had been socialized to believe that men were more competent in "masculine occupations," and that women likewise were more competent in "feminine occupations." As children grew older, they 40 came to believe that men were more competent in general when compared to women, regardless of occupation. Similarly, most older children believed that men made more money than women in masculine jobs, and concomitantly, women made more money than men in feminine jobs; younger children believed men made more in any type of occupation. From such findings it is reasonable to expect that such understandings of male and female competence would later translate into children's perceptions of their own competence and options for future careers. Women are currently holding 98 percent of the low-income feminine occupations and fewer than 15 percent of the board seats as major corporations (Sadker et al. 2009). According to the U.S. Department of Labor, in 2006, female surgeons and physicians are earning 38 percent less than their same level male colleagues, female college and university teachers are earning 25 percent less than male colleagues, and female lawyers earn 30 percent less than male lawyers. With such extreme differences in pay, one would wonder if men and women are aware of such monetary inequalities. In 2006 the case of Ledbetter v. Goodyear Tire & Rubber Co. was brought to the Supreme Court. Lilly Ledbetter had worked for the Goodyear Company for over 19 years and was considered one of the best employees compared to both men and women. In 1998 someone placed a memo in Ledbetter’s box that provided her base salary ($44,000) as well as that of three of her male colleagues who were all of equal or lesser tenure as well as title. Salaries listed for these men ranged from $53,000 to $62,000. Understandably upset about her new realization, Ledbetter 41 sued the company and was about to be awarded $300,000 for damages. Goodyear then appealed the case to the Supreme Court, which found that according to law, Ledbetter would have had to sue the company within six months of being hired to collect any damages for this outrageous action. Of course Ledbetter would have sued the company earlier, but she had not been made aware of the situation until over a decade of work. Ledbetter was left with no award but had her court costs paid for by Goodyear. Still feeling disgusted with her treatment, Ledbetter than made it her personal vendetta to change the law. In 2007 the U.S. House approved a bill that would hold employers accountable for the most recent discriminatory paycheck, not just those from the first 6 months of employment; however, the act failed to gain a filibuster-proof majority in the Senate and the bill died (Cullen 2008). As discussed earlier, Smith and Hung (2008) have found that much of society has been socialized to still hold tightly to the belief that females are not as capable as males within the areas of mathematics and science. In general, no one today comes out and blatantly states this sexist belief, but it is subtly suggested when we see women who are just as equally qualified and committed to their occupation as men are in these fields experiencing a stronger turnover rate due to dissatisfaction with research support, advancement opportunities, and free expression of ideas (Xu 2008:607). Xu’s study specifically concerned women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM). As Xu found, many women who are equally qualified as their male counterparts are given fewer opportunities for promotion and leadership. In addition to 42 these disadvantages, Xu found evidence that women in the STEM were also generally paid less than their male colleagues. This differential treatment and discrimination based on gender, however, did not start at the adult career level – as we have seen, it often begins in the classroom, during the early years of the socialization process. Though it is often difficult, there are women in predominantly male careers who find great success. Tindall (2007) investigated what factors lead to some women’s success in science and engineering fields. Four major themes were found throughout the experiences of the various women: (1) they did not perceive any academic limitations during the elementary years, (2) they did not experience any other challenges during the elementary years that could have limited their academic focus, (3) they had no gendered expectations in their families, and (4) they did not experience any expectations of discrimination in graduate school that was gender related. Many of the women indicated that either during high school or their undergraduate work they made some large academic achievement that was related to their field. Participants indicated that they felt their success was due to having a supportive family, having their father as a mentor or role model growing up, having a strong mentor in their graduate work, and having participated in collaborative research in their field. Unfortunately in her study Tindall did not interview any women who had tried to enter these fields with failure. Such research would be highly valuable to better understand how we can help women to succeed in male dominated fields; however, what Tindall does help demonstrate is how important 43 the roles of parents and the classroom are in shaping or diminishing gender stereotyped walls and boundaries. And this, of course, holds as true for men as it does for women. While women experience difficulty when taking on jobs in predominantly traditional male positions, it is important to note that the door swings in both directions. Studies such as that done by Jacobson (2008) discuss the experiences of men in teacher education programs, which for many years has been a predominantly traditional career for women, especially at the younger educational level. In her study, Jacobson specifically was looking to see if the teacher education programs themselves had a gender biased curriculum. The study found that the programs were very gender organized, with a constant—albeit unintentional—implication that it is appropriate for women to teach young children and that this is not so much the case for men. Jacobson felt that such programs easily created or sustained men’s feelings of incompetence when working with children. If teacher education programs would first remove the negative subtle gender socialization lessons that persist within their own curricula, it would lessen the chances of their future teachers from carrying those same subtle lessons and gender divisions into classrooms of their own. As some of the previous studies have shown, while women may often be found within a given workplace, they are all too often kept at a certain level while seeing their male co-workers ride the “glass escalator” up in administrative positions. Surprisingly, Jacobson (2008) found that within the field of teacher education, both males and females expressed little interest in moving up into administrative positions. Their passion was 44 instead to teach. Though this may be true for those in elementary teaching, it would not be fair to then assume that women and men in other occupations do not wish for administrative promotions. And while many male public school teachers may have been expressing no interest in moving up to administration, most do make that move, often due to societal pressure from those who feel it is inappropriate to others that they teach to young children. Through her interviews, Jacobson also found that even though both academic advisors and professors in teacher education expressed interest in integrating more men into elementary school teaching, little was reported showing effort to meet that goal. One might rightly wonder to what extent these attitudes inform or favor existing teacher education curricula. Similar to Xu’s (2008) findings that women became dissatisfied in male dominated occupations, Cognard-Black (1997) found that men who work at the elementary level as teachers are significantly more likely than their female colleagues to leave that occupation due to social pressures affiliated with being part of the gender minority. Clearly, there are many factors militating against success when one chooses a career path typically filled by those belonging to a different gender. But one's own aspirations, as conditioned in childhood, are often the main problem. And who, besides one’s own parents, is more likely to influence conditioning than one’s K-12 teachers? Teachers can learn to promote socialization that accepts males and females in any career they choose. Simple lesson plans and classroom decorations portraying men and women in non-stereotypical jobs could later on impact the career paths of students and 45 how they learn to view the job world around them. And though encouragement for breaking down gender barriers is important, as shown by Tindall (2007), success for some can sometimes be dependent on the mere absence of negative messages alone. Both learning to deteriorate negative messages and promote positive ones can all be part of teachers’ preparation programs. Single-Sex Education: A Solution? Throughout this literature review we have examined several studies that might be used to support gender-equity awareness as an integral part of a teacher education program. But one alternative that some educators and researchers point to as a place to eliminate gender issues in education is single-sex classes and schools. Though a trend of the past, single-sex education has exploded over recent years. “While only five public single-sex high schools were still operating in 1996, a decade later that number had jumped to more than thirty” (Sadker et al. 2009:259). Some supporters such as Hughes (2007) believe that single-sex classrooms help students to focus more on their academics and less on the distractions created by their hormones (though it should be noted that this observation would only hold true for heterosexuals). Reports of the validity of this notion are mixed. Some believe that while females feel less pressure to act in certain ways to receive approval from males when those males are not present, males are often more rambunctious and crude in their behavior and sexual discussions when they do not have females around to overhear them. 46 Many books and articles have emphasized biological differences in male and female brains, promoting the hypothesis that separate teaching strategies and even buildings are necessary to meet their separate needs. For example, Sax (2005) opined that boys need an intense competitive environment and that yelling is often an effective technique for teaching boys, while girls need a more relaxed environment, where tests are not timed and they can relax. Though much is still unknown and debated, we can agree that there are some differences between the sexes in how they learn, test take, and behave in the classroom. Single-sex education has predominately been found in private schools due to the restrictions put in place by Title IX. However, in 2004, President George W. Bush made an alteration to the law. This change made it legal to segregate students within coed public schools into different classes by one’s sex, even allowing for classes to be created for one sex and not for the other. Now all that is required to make these acts legal in a public school is a reason provided by a school administrator. This reason could be based on the fact that parents show a great interest in single-sex classes, or, unfortunately, just based off of one’s own personal biases. Bush’s administration funded research looking into the effectiveness of single-sex education. The results from their research were mixed and faulty, with no significant findings to support single-sex education as a better alternative. Despite the fact that his administration's research did not overwhelmingly support it and a meager 100 citizens supported the change, what is more noteworthy is the fact that well over 5,000 citizens opposed the changes and that Bush still went ahead 47 with the alteration (Sadker et al. 2009:262). Because of Bush’s changes to Title IX, the law no longer guarantees the equal treatment it once promised to both sexes. While it is true that many all-female schools report that their students have higher self-esteems, speak up more in the classroom, and that they participate and show interest in more non-traditional subjects, much of the success of such schools is found in the mentors it provides (Sadker et al. 2009:253-254). Such schools are found having more females in non-traditional fields of math and science than in coed schools. Having such female role models in coed schools would likely also result in similar improvements for female students. In addition to strong female mentors, these all female schools provided small classes and curricula and classroom wall displays which reflected the contributions of both men and women. Cable and Spradlin (2008) also point out in their overview of single-sex education that the success they have identified is likely due to the fact that in such schools, only teachers who are highly motivated, trained, and socialized for this type of education take the job. In addition to having better trained and gender aware teachers, Cable and Spradlin point out that if the students themselves are voluntarily signing up for single-sex courses with permission from their parents, there is a great chance that either the students, parents, or both parties are already demonstrating higher levels of interest to be involved in such a program. There are far too many outside variables that can account for the successes found within single-sex education, especially when there are also cases where success was not found, to accept single-sex education as a universal panacea. 48 Evidence of the success in all male schools is much more lacking due to the limited number of such schools compared to those provided for females and also due to the fact that those few schools which exist are generally private and wary of letting researchers into their classrooms. David and Myra Sadker were able to speak with male alums from all male schools who reported that sexual stereotypes faded more when attending these schools and that males were more free to participate in non-traditional fields such as the arts and language (Sadker et al. 2009:254). With so much being gained by females and some males in this single-sex education setup, one may wonder why our society ever left single-sex education in the first place. The answer is simple: it was cheaper. And with the current state of the U.S. economy, that reason alone will militate against the overthrow of coed public schooling. Taxpayers are not interested in supporting both a school for boys and one for girls in their community. In addition to money being a factor, there are other negative aspects which singlesex schools bring. Segregating students by sex is not a long-term solution. Eventually males and females need to interact and feel comfortable working together, and if kept apart with the belief that they are so different they need to be separated, it would be fairly difficult and awkward to socialize and form relationships. Besides feeling comfortable interacting in the everyday world, we will be expecting men and women to interact freely in the job market, but how will they take one another seriously after such separation and differences in teaching styles? Many of the students from single-sex schools that David 49 and Myra Sadker (2009) spoke with expressed concerns of not knowing how to speak with or interact with the opposite sex. Some expressed loneliness due to the fact that in younger grades their best friends were of the opposite sex and now in their middle year single-sex school they feel like an outcast. Coming to a firm conclusion of the effectiveness of single-sex education would be difficult due to mixed and limited results. As Cable and Spradlin (2008) exclaimed, “The only consistent finding on single-sex classrooms is that the findings are not consistent” (2008:10). But despite mixed conclusions, it would be fair to argue that most of the benefits that are seen in these schools could be found in coed schools with proper actions taken by well-prepared mentor figures. Just as many female teachers in single-sex schools were shown to bring out great success for their students due to their awareness and focused preparation for handling gender issues in education, public teacher preparation programs adding such focus could potentially bring about similar success. Looking to Teaching Credential Programs As has been pointed out through previous discussion here, today’s educational world is still shaped and socialized by gendered stereotypes, misconceptions, and myths. Laws that have been put into place to deflect discriminatory practices have been changed, weakened, and left without enforcers. While a great effort has been made to improve textbooks and curricula, much work is still needed so that males and females alike are seen embracing both active and passive traditional roles. In the classroom teachers are unconsciously giving their students different educational experiences and subtle gender 50 socialization lessons. The grading and testing systems’ goals are not the same, leaving many female students with high grades, yet confused when they find great difficulty in meeting the standardized test score requirements for their dream University. Incorporating new methods such as non-timed testing could benefit both female and some male students. We have seen that both males and females suffer from the differential treatment and that the educational crisis does not belong to only one side. The relationship between some students’ lack of self-confidence and support for crossing gendered subject boundaries often cuts off potential job opportunities as adults. And when those boundaries are surpassed, one is often met with more differential and discriminatory treatment. So how can we overcome these disparities? Efforts have been made to de-genderize education by literally separating the sexes into different classes and schools; however, it is not yet apparent that such practices have resulted in consistent success; the results are mixed and un-clear. Overall trends tend to show that same-sex education is a more useful approach for females than males, but should we not seek for a solution that benefits all students? In addition it seems that there are other variables linked with the success found in some all-girl schools, such as teachers that are more aware of gender issues and how they relate to education, as well as serving the role as a strong mentor to their students. Gender awareness and mentorship are certainly both factors that could be similarly incorporated into any co-ed education. Researchers such as Krafchick (2007) have found that programs such as Fairness for All Individuals through Respect (FAIR) help to promote the understanding of social 51 injustice in the classroom. Specifically FAIR is aimed at helping students to identify social injustices concerning gender, race, and class. The program was developed by Colorado State University’s Department of Human Development and Family Studies. Krafchick found that fourth, fifth, and sixth graders were the ideal age group for the FAIR program to impact. Educators felt that the program was helpful in their own classrooms, but that some of the program curricula was more appropriate at later ages and developmental stages. Though the FAIR curricula helps to get both students and teachers thinking about and learning to identify social injustices, the program only consists of five classroom activities that do not go into any great depth. Nonetheless, it is a start. And though many educators (at least in Colorado) have chosen to become involved in FAIR, the majority are not, nor are they required to do such. Currently, according to the Commission on Teacher Credentialing, there is no required training concerning gender and the role it has played and continues to play in California teaching credential programs. While we have seen great progress in that multicultural courses have become required, such instruction tends to focus more on race, class, and ESL students than on gender (Smith 2009; Katsarou 2009). Katsarou (2009) explained that teaching credential program today are being asked to step away from some of their traditional approaches and to re-evaluate their curricula and textbooks in order to better understand and combat racism in the classroom. While it seems clear that our country has become more aware of racism, gender discrimination is far less detected. If we saw that in classrooms children of color were being called on far less than white 52 students, or that they were being treated and talked to differently it is reasonable to assume that many people would much more quickly take notice and express outrage over such behavior. So why should we not be equally outraged when there is such strong evidence of discrimination with respect to gender? As deduced by Connolly (1994), after completing his study concerning racism and masculinity at an elementary school level, without a solid understanding of other forms of inequality such as class and gender, strategies to combat racism cannot reach their full potential. And this notion should likewise be applied to every battle against any form of inequality. As explained by Smith (2009) when referring to multicultural awareness, the success or failure of this education is dependent upon the effective preparation of teachers and administrators. A similar argument can be made that the solution for creating more gender awareness in education is also determined by level of teacher preparation and understanding. Though certainly schools are not the first place gender discrimination lessons and socialization are learned, the educational environment is a strong enforcer that shapes and socializes the adult-gendered world. While it would be impossible, or at least very difficult and certainly undesirable, to go into every home and demand that parents raise their children a particular way, it is nonetheless possible for us to more assertively shape the great enabler and twelve year (or more) enforcer: the public school system. Numerous studies used in earlier discussions have pointed out that there is a problem, but few have provided real solutions to those problems. I believe the solution lies within teaching credential programs. Those who go into the teaching profession desire nothing 53 more than the best for their students, but without a more complete education themselves, how can we expect the best possible education for the next generation? Some studies have come very close by also looking to teaching credential programs such as that discussed earlier by Oliveira and Athanases (2007). In their study they explored the impact a California State University English Language Learners (ELL) focused teaching credential program had on its graduates. Although the CSU program from this study did an excellent job of preparing its future teachers to combat issues relating to language, race, and class, it was missing another vital inequality that is also found in our classrooms today. If courses concerning the role of gender in education would have also been added to their program, it is reasonable to expect that the interview participants would have likewise reported that they felt more prepared for handling gender issues inside and outside their classrooms and that they felt more prepared than their colleagues who had other training, just as they explained concerning ELLs. In another study by Birdsell and Suckow (2009), they compared teacher preparation programs in Gujarat, India and California. While both programs included a more recent focus on English language learners (ELLs) and Special Education accommodations, both also showed a lack of focus on any gender awareness and its relationship to education. And though the researchers were aware that “Education is not just for students and schools; it is for the entire community”, they failed to see in all the ways their education programs do impact communities (2009:9). Similar to Oliveira and Athanases (2007), this cross-cultural examination found that programs from both 54 countries saw that their new emphasis for ELLs and Special Education made a great impact on the level of education teachers were then able to provide in their own classrooms after completion of their preparation programs, and even before during their student teaching. And no study has come closer to pointing to teaching credential programs as a solution than that done by Athanases and Martin (2006). In their study, the researchers conducted a five year investigation of a California teaching credential program. Athanases and Martin wanted to find out how successfully a program claiming to prepare teachers to advocate for educational equity was in application. Data was collected through focus groups with graduates from the program. Focal points of the program and conversations ranged from promotion for ELLs, cross-cultural awareness and sensitivity, community outreach, and application beyond the classroom. Though the program and researchers were completely focused on equity in the classroom, the role that gender equity plays in education was completely overlooked. This shows that often even those who are fully aware of educational equity can still be unaware of the subtle gender lessons and negative socialization going on in classrooms today. Though the fight for true gender equity in the classroom has persisted for hundreds of years, and as shown above, researchers have been addressing the topic for decades, this discrimination still exists and it cannot continue to go unnoticed and unaccounted for by society and by our schools if we desire a society that truly provides equal opportunity for all. The fact is that girls and boys, men and women, all suffer in 55 one way or another from these discriminatory practices. We must continue to better ourselves and our systems by using all the resources and education that we possibly can. Athanases and Martin found that “teacher education coursework emerged as profoundly influential in preparing teachers to advocate for equity,” so why should we not attempt to harness this resource to help eliminate gender discrimination in our classrooms? (2006:640). Summary As demonstrated through the above review of the research relevant to the challenges of sex discrimination in our society, many of the noted problems are associated with the manner in which both boys and girls are acculturated within their K12 education. Many of these problems persist well into adulthood. Despite efforts to combat sex discrimination, such as Title IX, issues related to curricula and textbook selection, differential gender expectations in the classroom, an emphasis on testing over grading, and pitting males against females all contribute to persisting inequities in our society. And while it may be useful in some instances, single sex education is not a sufficient solution. So long as teachers continue to lack awareness of the problems noted, and do not benefit from specific interventions designed to help them combat sex discrimination, that discrimination and negative gendered messages will continue to be transmitted unwittingly into classrooms. Regardless of how well-meaning they might be, teachers 56 who are not educated about inappropriate gender differences will likely continue to perpetuate them. On the other hand, we know how effective teacher training programs can be. As noted above, specific efforts to make future teachers more aware of problems related to such issues as race and ELLs have resulted in more effective teaching with respect to those topics. As teachers are socialized and taught how to better educate, they consistently rise to the challenge. My hypothesis is that few credential programs place a concerted effort on identifying and effectively combating gender discrimination and that a similar focus in these programs could produce positive results like those found in Oliveira and Athanases’ (2007) and Birdsell and Suckow’s (2009) studies. If teachers were given the proper education about gender discrimination, its history in education, and methods for breaking down this discrimination and negative socialization in their future classrooms, every member of society would potentially benefit. Teaching credential programs are the foundation from which classroom etiquette and methods are born and we must harness that function for the best possible outcome. 57 Chapter 3 METHODS Throughout the years, girls have slowly gained opportunity and access to public education, college, and a growing variety of occupations. Regardless, access remains limited and different from those experienced by boys. Sex discrimination is much more subtle today. In general, it seems that this discrimination does not occur because society does not take it seriously or care to promote equity, but possibly it is because the vast majority of us is unaware that it still happens and/or is oblivious to what can be done to truly combat it, necessitating analysis of the persistence of gender inequity in education. While some studies have solely focused on content analysis of teaching curricula and materials (Farr, Tulley, and Powell 1987; Tyson and Woodward 1989; Women on Words and Images 1975; Bradford 2008; Evans and Davies 2000; Hamilton, Anderson, Broaddus, and Young 2006; Stein, Stuen, Carnine, and Long 2001), classroom observation (Allen, Cantor, Grady, and Hill 1997; Hamilton, Blumenfeld, Akoh and Miura 1991; Sadker and Sadker 1995; Sadker et. al. 2009), or general focus groups about educational equity (Athanases and Martin 2006) one area that few researchers have 58 provided as a source for a solution is in teaching credential programs. All elementary and secondary level teachers are required to go through such a program. Recent efforts have been seen in many credential programs to include courses and curricula concerning equity and avoiding differential treatment based on race (Smith 2009; Katsarou 2009). Similarly, many budding teachers are being trained to employ greater sensitivity toward English as a Second Language students, and those students coming from low income families (Oliveira and Athanases 2007). But, my hypothesis is that few credential programs place a concerted effort on identifying and effectively combating gender discrimination. If teachers were given the proper education about gender discrimination, its history in education, and methods for breaking down this discrimination in their future classrooms, every member of society would greatly benefit. Girls and boys, men and women, all suffer in one way or another from these discriminatory practices. While the previously discussed methods for researching gender awareness in education were both insightful and useful, they failed to collaborate information and provide a solution for all aspects of the issue at hand. In order to fully study California teaching credential programs I felt it was necessary to employ a multi-method approach including a pilot content analysis, content analysis, and interviews. I chose to approach this study qualitatively through the use of interviews because this method allows for flexibility as new insights may emerge, the ability to probe more deeply by not having standardized inquiries for all observations, and the ability to also analyze the personal inflections that are produced during an interview answer rather than a static written 59 response (Babbie 1986). Qualitative analysis allows for more in depth exploration of what is occurring in credential program and what is being learned in them. Conducting a content analysis is a useful method when the unit of analysis is an organization and the researcher is incorporating a broad range of conditions. Also, by using specific codes for analysis, it makes it easier for other researchers to replicate the study themselves (Jauch, Osborn, and Martin 1980). Rourke and Anderson (2004) also explain that by systematically identifying and categorizing the objects of study through content analysis, the analysis leaves little room for counter interpretation and is especially valuable when it concerns new educational phenomena. A pilot study was implemented in order to conduct a more interpretive and exploratory analysis of teacher education programs. By investigating other out of state programs, this helped to create a structured format that allowed for more quantitative coding and comparison of the programs to help identify patterns for gender awareness and to also insure that the programs involved represent varying degrees within the awareness spectrum. Content Analysis Previously a content analysis pilot was completed analyzing teacher education programs throughout the United States (excluding California). Information was gathered for the pilot from program websites and related online materials such as offered courses, website imagery, ratio of male to female faculty, gender of administrative heads, number and gender of faculty with gender studies interests, gender related website language, and 60 other gender related programs connected with that program’s University. The pilot study showed overwhelmingly that most schools do not provide any courses that consider the topic of gender in education. Those programs which had courses with some nod towards a gender discussion were programs already heavily focused on other forms of discrimination such as race, or were taught by a professor who specifically had a personal interest in gender and inequalities studies. Relying on the previously conducted pilot, a content analysis of California teaching credential programs was analyzedaccording to a more structured coding process similar to the criteria as the pilot study, scoring the number of courses that discuss gender focus/awareness, the ratio of depictions of males to females in program website imagery, ratio and professional level of male to female faculty, gender of administrative heads, number and gender of faculty with gender studies interests, counting the occurrence of gender related website language, and other gender related programs connected with them in their University. After being analyzed for the above, programs were then grouped into two categories: those that earn a high content analysis score of gender awareness and those that earn a low score. Two program examples, one from the top 10 percent and one from the bottom 10 percent were then selected and contacted for interviews. Subjects being interviewed were comprised of both students and faculty from teaching credential programs in the state of California. It was expected that, similar to the pilot study, the vast majority of California teaching credential programs would not show strong gender awareness and preparation 61 and that common patterns would be found in those that do i.e., having professors in the program that have already adopted personal interest in gender and inequalities studies and having co-partner programs in their University that focus on gender and inequalities. It was also expected that even within those programs that show some awareness and preparation for gender in education, gender would be a subtopic of the course, and it would often be an elective, rather than a required course. These hypotheses were based on results from the prior pilot study. Interviews This study consisted of a half hour to one hour interview concerning participants’ definitions and concepts of gender, the origin(s) of gender identity, the training that their program may offer concerning the role of gender in education, knowledge of Title IX, knowledge and opinion of sex segregated education, knowledge of gender achievement gaps, and awareness of gender stereotyping in textbooks and curricula. In addition to the interview, a basic demographic survey was sent along with the consent form to be completed and returned prior to the interview to establish the participants’ University, sex, race/ethnicity, age, current level of education, number of completed semesters in their program (for students only), and any special interests or foci participants had adopted. Due to location of the participating schools, all volunteers were interviewed over the phone at their scheduled convenience. With consent, interviews were recorded. 62 After interviews took place, full transcriptions were created. Those interviews which were recorded were properly destroyed after the transcriptions were made. After the transcription process was completed, patterns in participants’ responses were then grouped together concerning each of the interview questions. The proposal for this study was passed by The Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects at California State University, Sacramento (protocol number 112). Participants were only asked about the structure of their program and opinions about materials not affiliated with their program. Though participants may have had concern that their responses and identities would be reported to their program, no such action would take place. Participants did not need to worry that their responses would result in academic or workplace retribution of any sense because all participants were kept strictly confidential. Also, participants were allowed to refrain from answering any questions that they desired not to discuss. Privacy was protected by the use of pseudonyms for individuals and all other identifying information, creating no way to connect contacts to their interview responses. All records were only made available to the researcher. All files were password protected. Only the researcher was able to view the protected list of subjects’ names, contact information, and interview transcriptions. Sample Due to the fact that the majority of public University of California (UC) multiple subject teaching credential programs are combined with a Master’s degree and the great 63 variations in program type, content, and lack of generalizability, UC programs were not included in the sample for this study. Only California State Universities (CSUs) were used for findings and interview selection. From there, only 22 of the 23 CSU approved programs listed by the California Commission on Teacher Credentialing were included. The one excluded CSU program was web-based only. This program had a completely different structure that was tied to a number of CSU regional centers. The main reason it was not included was because it did not have similar content available that was being measured in the other programs. After being scored and analyzed, programs were then grouped into two categories: those that earned a high content analysis score of gender awareness and preparedness (6 through 10) and those that earn a low score (1 through 5). Two program examples, one from the top 10 percent and one from the bottom 10 percent were then selected and contacted for interviews. Subjects being interviewed were comprised of both students and faculty from teaching credential programs in the state of California. Eight students and six faculty from two programs were asked to participate: 28 subjects in total. Programs selected for the interview process were contacted via e-mail and phone first with the department chair for initial consent for participation. Pending the initial chair approval, e-mails containing the consent letter and form were then sent to faculty asking for volunteers to participate in an interview. E-mails asking for student volunteers to participate in interviews were sent by the department chair or program coordinator 64 when available since access to student contact information is confidential. Those who wanted to be involved were then e-mailed a consent form and a simple demographics survey to print out, sign, and mail back before the interview commences. All faculty participants from the bottom 10 percent were female, completed a PhD as their highest level of education, ranged from ages 36 to 56+, and ranged from 2 to 25 years working in that program. These participants were made up of Caucasian, Hispanic/Latino, and Asian ethic/racial groups. Faculty expressed special interest or focus in topics such as literacy and bilingual education, teacher education, special needs populations, and Latino and diverse populations. Student participants from the bottom 10 percent were made up of three females and one male, all completed a high school diploma as their highest level of education, and all ranged from ages 20 to 25. These participants were made up of Caucasian and Hispanic Latino ethnic/racial groups. Students expressed special interest or focus in math. Faculty participants form the top 10 percent were made up of two males and one female, complete a PhD as their highest level of education, ranged from ages 46 to 56+, ranged from 7 to 21 years working in that program, and were all Caucasian. Faculty expressed special interest or focus in topics such as developmental and educational foundations of education, gender and sexuality studies, human rights education, clinical teacher preparation, educational psychology, and child development. Student participants from the top 10 percent were made up of three females and one 65 male and ranged from ages 20 to 50+. Concerning highest level of education, one student completed a high school diploma, one student completed an associate’s degree, and two students completed bachelor’s degrees. These participants were made up of Caucasian and Hispanic/Latino ethnic/racial groups. None of these students specified any special interest or focus topics. Limitations While these findings do not represent the general public, they are still useful for understanding teaching credential programs for the state of California, and can serve as a stepping stone for future research to further these methods in more regions and programs across the country. By using a content analysis, it will be easy for future researchers to replicate the study for the teacher education programs in their respective states or countries as well. It should be noted that the participants from each program were limited to volunteers who may already have a bias and interest in the topic of gender awareness. The purpose of this study was to perform exploratory research and to first gain insight from those who have an interest and are involved in the programs first hand. Future studies may choose to engage those who are likely less interested and focused on gender awareness education to help determine a broader possible impact that programs are having through use of random sampling. Similarly, while the sample size of participants was limited to only two programs, this sample was derived from the content analysis of all public California State 66 Universities. Because public credential programs, on average, graduate over two and a half times as many teachers as private credential programs, only public programs were analyzed for this exploratory study (California Commission on Teacher Credentialing 2007-2008). Future studies may choose to employ this study to include all California approved teaching credential programs for a more full representation and application. Since it was not possible to meet in person will volunteers due to the location of the participants, all interview data was limited to being conducted over the phone. While face to face interviews would have allowed for the additional variable of analysis of body language, those interviews conducted over the phone were limited. Besides location restrictions, for this exploratory analysis it was more important to make sure volunteers were comfortable in the setting they chose so that the most accurate information could be obtained with greater depth and detail. Because the topic of interests involves gender, it was important to make sure that the sample captured the viewpoints and experiences of both men and women. Unfortunately, it was expected that the male viewpoint would be in the minority since teaching credential programs tend to be dominated by females. Nonetheless, because the aim of this study was to determine the awareness that teaching credential programs are creating for their students/future teachers—regardless of the gender of those teachers— the preponderance of females in the teaching field was not the overriding concern in this study; however, measuring accurate accounts from those who were in the teacher credential programs was. 67 While the results from the content analysis could not answer the question of whether programs are in fact educated about gender or not, it was able to identify whether gender was emphasized in a program’s online content or not. While the potential for gender education may be present in a program, the content analysis also could not identify if students were actually learning about it and accepting such messages. For this reason, interviews were also needed to establish the range of gender awareness of both faculty and students. 68 Chapter 4 FINDINGS This chapter contains the findings and general analysis from the website content analysis and faculty and student interviews. A more in depth overall analysis will be provided in chapter five. Content Analysis Findings It was found from the content analysis, that on a possible score from one to ten (1 being lowest and 10 being highest), over 50 percent of California multiple subject teaching credential programs scored below a five. Only nine percent (2 programs) scored between a seven and ten (see Table and Figure 1 for details). As hypothesized, the vast majority of California multiple subject teaching credential programs did not show strong, or sometimes any, gender awareness and preparation. The two top programs scoring an eight and a ten had well organized web-based information. They both made a thorough effort to include gender awareness in multiple aspects of their programs, including the provision of required courses that highlighted gender and its relation to education, a fairly balanced ratio of male to female faculty, and website language utilizing gender. The program scoring an eight also collaborated with another program that concerned gender and education. The program scoring a ten also had a faculty member who was specifically interested in gender related studies. The only measured variable in which both top scoring programs lacked was website imagery 69 portraying a balance of male and female images. Part of this failure is due to the fact that the program scoring an eight did not have any website images with human subject matter whatsoever. It was not so much a case of having an unequal program or replacement with an unequal image depiction, but just a lack of this as a venue for portraying the equality of their institutionalized values which are still warranted through the other measured factors. It is important to note that all scoring is solely based on the materials made available on each school/program’s website. A program earning a low score from the content analysis could be due in part to a lack of web based information, though by far the majority of program websites provided ample information. The only program to earn a bottom ranking score of one had the most disorganized website, lacked the most information, and was also run as a half web-based program and half based at small onsite site local facilities. For these reasons, this bottom ranking school was not chosen to participate in interviews. Table 1: Program Content Analysis Scores Program Score 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Percentage of Programs * 4.5% 18.2% 9.1% 22.7% 22.7% 13.6% 0% 4.5% 0% 4.5% Number of Programs 1 4 2 5 5 3 0 1 0 1 70 *Rounded to the nearest tenth Figure 1: Program Content Analysis Scores Percentage of Programs 25.00% 20.00% 3 4 6 5 3 10.00% 0.00% 2 2 15.00% 5.00% 1 4 5 6 1 8 10 7 8 7 9 9 10 Program Score All program content analysis measurement factors are displayed in Table 2 below. As depicted in Table 2, a total of 31.8 percent of all measured programs (7 programs) offered a required gender related course, while over double, 68.2 percent (15 programs) offered a required course relating to diversity, social justice, and/or equity topics. In only a few instances were there programs which offered a non-required gender related course and such courses were often aimed at undergraduate students preparing for the credential program. Of the seven programs that offered a required gender related course, three were from high scoring programs (6-10). Though one may argue that it is notable that the majority of programs that offered the gender course were actually from low scoring programs (1-5), it is critical to acknowledge that these numbers translate into 60 percent 71 of all high scoring programs and only 24 percent of all low scoring programs. Clearly, there is still a strong pattern that having a required gender related course is correlated with a higher score for showing gender awareness and preparedness. The fact that twice as many programs have required courses relating to diversity, social justice, and/or equity topics supports the idea that teaching credential programs have been socialized and institutionally mandated to place a more concerted effort and focus on other forms of diversity excluding gender. Eight of the programs that included general diversity courses did not include any courses that dealt specifically with gender diversity. Some program websites may not have listed gender as part of their online course descriptions, even though the subject might be briefly discussed in one or more courses. Nonetheless, the online representation depicts the larger ideology of that program to the community and any such representation that excludes specific mention of gender education awareness or preparation certainly suggests that this is not a high priority for that program, or at least not part of their larger ideology for educating teachers. Once again we see that the majority of programs that offered courses relating to diversity, social justice, and/or equity topics were from low scoring programs (1-5), but this is no surprise since the majority of programs received low scores. It was found that 100 percent of all high scoring programs and 83 percent of all lower scoring programs included general diversity courses. While it is reassuring to see that the majority of programs, both high and low scoring, are placing a concerted effort on general diversity 72 awareness and preparedness, it seems that having a focus on general diversity has no correlating pattern to gender diversity inclusion. Such an inclusion of general diversity topics is likely due to evolving state program mandates and the general greater push for general diversity awareness as discussed earlier rather than the actual ideology of these programs. Ironically, exactly 50 percent of programs were found to have an equal ratio of male to female faculty at similar rank. It was found that 27.3 percent of faculty (6 programs) listed that they had a specific interest in gender related studies and that 36.4 percent (8 programs) had interest in general equity and diversity topics. The majority of those faculty members who listed any interest in the general equity and diversity topics were also interested in gender studies. While it was hypothesized that one of the factors that may contribute to a program showing greater gender awareness and preparedness would be having a faculty member who was specifically interested in gender studies, results proved no solid relationship. Programs scored for having a faculty member with a gender related interest were found ranked at the two, four, six and ten levels and the surprising majority of those programs received scores of five or less. While for some specific examples it may have had an impact on why their program had more gender awareness and preparedness, it does not seem that this is a clear causal relationship. This idea will be expanded further in the interviews discussed below. 73 On occasion, programs will link themselves with or collaborate with other programs within their same university. When looking at such collaborative programs, it was found that only 13.6 percent of credential programs (3 programs) were linked to an outside program concerning gender focus and 22.7 percent (5 programs) were linked to an outside program concerning diversity, social justice, and/or equity topics. The large majority of programs that had a collaborative program concerning gender, diversity, social justice, and/or equity topics also ranked overall scores in the top half (6-10). It is not surprising that those programs that are willing to put in the extra effort to include gender awareness and preparedness into their program socialization, are likewise more willing to take the extra initiative to find and collaborate with other beneficial programs for education. Similarly, a program displaying a gender inclusive ideology is more likely to want to consciously share this ideology with other departments and programs within their university. As discussed earlier, collaborative programs such as Fairness for All Individuals through Respect (FAIR) have helped to promote the understanding of social injustice in the classroom (Krafchick 2007). It is similarly expected that such programs at a university level would also help to promote a better educated and prepared credential program. School/program website images depicted a roughly equal ratio of males to females 45.5 percent of the time (10 programs). It could possibly be argued that this is evidence that overall, credential programs may be more visually aware of surface gender equity than they are aware at a deeper level of gender equity within their curricula and 74 teaching methods. Such a revitalization of visual gender awareness could be due in part to the greater push credential programs have made over the years concerning race (Katsarou 2009; Smith 2009). Since one’s race is a visual variable, teachers may have learned to train themselves for avoiding differential treatment based on initial sight. As shown in Table 2, website language used to describe the programs utilized gender terms 22.7 percent of the time (5 programs) and utilized more broad terms of diversity, social justice, and/or equity 81.8 percent of the time (18 programs). Interestingly four of the five programs that used gender terms in their website language describing their program were low scoring (1-5). Three of the four low scoring programs that discussed gender terms in their program overviews or mission statements provided no other indicators that they have gender awareness and preparation in their program at all. Though the content analysis is solely based on web-based content, it seems that some programs may “talk the talk, but not walk the walk.” In contrast, the limited used of gender terminology by the other program websites, especially those that scored higher on the gender scale, may be more of an institutionalized decision rather than one based on the ideology of those programs, much less the agency of the individuals who organize them. As the United States has progressed more over the years towards acceptance of racial and cultural diversity, it seems that the more general “buzz” words like diversity, social justice, and equity are being used. While we have seen great progress in that multicultural courses have become required and institutionalized, such instruction tends to focus more on race, class, and ELL students than on gender (Smith 2009; 75 Katsarou2009). Such increased focus on multiculturalism could account for the high percentage of programs using the general buzz words in attempts to openly show their compliance; however, since gender courses are currently not required by California, there is not nearly as much open discussion of the subject. Table 2: Content Analysis Measurement Percentages Content Analysis Measurements of Programs Percentage from Low (1-5) Programs 13.6% 45.5% Required gender related course Required course relating to diversity, social justice, and/or equity Roughly equal ratio of male to female 31.8% faculty by position Faculty with gender related interest 18.2% Faculty with general interest in 22.8% diversity and equity Related program concerning gender 4.5% focus Related program concerning diversity, 13.6% social justice, and/or equity Images showed roughly equal ratio of 36.4% males to females Website program language and 18.2% description relating to gender Website program language and 59.1% description relating to diversity, social justice, and/or equity *Rounded to the nearest tenth Percentage from High (6-10) Programs 18.2% 22.7% Total Percentage of Programs * 31.8% 68.2% 18.2% 50% 9.1% 13.6% 27.3% 36.4% 9.1% 13.6% 9.1% 22.7% 9.1% 45.5% 4.5% 22.7% 22.7% 81.8% As depicted through the quantitative content analysis, the hypothesis that few credential programs place a concerted effort on identifying and effectively combating gender discrimination was very clearly validated. We have seen that only five (22.7 76 percent) of all CSU multiple subject credential programs were able to score above a five on a scale from one to ten for meeting measurements of program gender awareness and preparedness. Only seven programs (31.8 percent) claimed to have even one required course that partially discussed the subject of gender and how it relates to education. If a program does not even discuss gender in its curricula, it is still failing if only their mission statements claim they do, that their websites may show equal numbers of males and females, that they employ an equal number of males and female, and so on. One cannot fight subtle gender discrimination with subtle preparation. While the content analysis was able to identify the degree to which a program’s online content emphasized gender, it was not able to identify or measure if faculty and students were actually learning or accepting a gender awareness ideology. For this information we now turn to the interview portion of the study. Interview Findings Several low scoring programs (in the bottom 10 percent) that were contacted for interview requests often not only lacked some information on their website from the content analysis, but seemed to lack overall program organization. As noted above in the content analysis, the lower scoring programs’ websites tended to be poorly organized, referencing less information, and even displaying incorrect information. One school website incorrectly identified contact phone numbers and a handful of other program websites listed classes, but offered no explanation or description of the classes. Even more telling of the lack of structure and organization, was that it was difficult to discern 77 who taught in which credential program, their affiliation with the program, and who the contact person was for the program; such was the case with one program that was contacted for possible interviews. Most faculty members did not respond, and the few who did were not fully aware of whom to contact or a process to follow about the possibility of such a study. Some messages to department chairs or program advisors never received a response. With such lack of basic organization and commitment it is not then surprising that some of these low scoring programs would also be lacking in putting extra effort to include awareness and preparation on gender issues in education as part of their curricula. In complete contrast, the program selected from the top 10 percent for gender awareness was very well organized and helpful. I received an immediate response from a faculty member who knew exactly with whom I should speak about the possibility of the study. That contact person also responded directly to me and was very willing to pass the information on to his program’s students and also to collaborate a list of student volunteers with contact information for me. Many of the students contacted me directly, expressing excitement to participate rather than doing so out of a sense of obligation. The program contact had a structured plan to ask for faculty volunteers as well. In a short time the volunteers were gathered and scheduled for interviews. Unlike the search for a program from the bottom 10 percent to volunteer, I did not have to continue contacting various programs. The first top 10 percent program volunteered without hesitation. 78 Defining Gender Concerning a basic definition of what gender is, for Program F (from the bottom 10 percent), there does not seem to be a consistent perception. Half of the faculty from Program F agreed that gender is solely a biological factor of whether someone is male or female. In complete opposition, faculty member from Program F, interview number one (FF1), stated that gender was completely a social construct. And finally, faculty member from Program F, interview number four (FF4), provided a more individualistic and psychologically driven definition in that gender is how one chooses to identify oneself. While it is notable that those faculty members who believed gender could only be controlled through biology presented a limited viewpoint, what is more critical is that fostering three distinct viewpoints within one program shows a lack of cohesion and structure. While offering a more cohesive perspective, all of the students from Program F agreed that gender was strictly a biological factor of being male or female, thus displaying a limited education concerning the diversity of perspectives about gender. Though the student from Program F, interview number two (SF2), believed gender was biological, she also believed that gender roles were not, and that they differ by one’s culture. In a sense, even this student, who had an awareness of social dynamics not expressed by other students, articulated an inadequate perspective, inasmuch as her explanation did not supersede the biological construct of gender. 79 In contrast, all faculty members from Program A (from the top 10 percent) agreed that gender is predominantly a social construction, emphasizing a broader awareness of gender within academia, and society in general. While half of the students from Program A identified gender as a biological factor, they also included the additional category— beyond male and female—of transgender. Though these students saw gender as biological, the inclusion of transgender shows more awareness into the subjectivity of gender and perhaps more openness to a more fluid approach to gender. The other half of the students from Program A explained gender as different from one’s sex in that gender is socially constructed and sex is biological. When referring to gender, student interview number four from Program A (SA4) stated “I think it is more state of mind rather than your body. I’ve known people or heard and read about people that say in their mind they are a male and were born a female. Sometimes that happens and so your mind is more powerful than the physical.” Again, this shows a more fluid approach to gender than students from Program F, and shows awareness of gender situations outside the status quo, which tends to emphasize biology. When faculty and students were probed specifically if they felt that gender were biological, socially constructed/learned, or some of both, some participants became more aware of the potential variables and their responses adjusted. For example, all the faculty members from Program F agreed, to at least some degree, that gender was both biological and socially constructed, although faculty member from Program F interview number three (FF3) articulated that biology still took precedence. Students from Program F 80 similarly changed their responses, with the exception of one student (SF4), who believed that gender was only biological and that it did not matter if one felt differently on the inside. To this student, the only way one could alter gender would be to undergo surgery. In Program A, two faculty members (FA1 and FA2) slightly altered their responses once presented with more specifics. The two faculty members that changed their answers explained that gender could be some of both biology and social construction in that gender is based on physiological sex difference, but that it is more heavily a social construction. The third faculty member (FF3) did not change his answer and explained that gender is a social construction in all situations. As with the faculty, once presented with more specifics, all the students from Program A only adjusted their perceptions, agreeing that gender could be seen as both biological and socially constructed, but again some students explained that social construction was the larger factor that determined gender. When viewing responses after participants were specifically asked if gender were biological, socially learned/constructed, or some of both, there was a strong pattern of difference between faculty from Program F and Program A. While some faculty in Program F changed their response after the further probing, all faculty in Program A were able to agree that social constructionism plays the strongest role in the definition of gender both before and after the probing. This is an example of the organizational and structural differences between the two programs in having an understood, shared ideology, and even more importantly, an example of the difference in awareness about 81 gender. Faculty members from Program F were not aware of the variables that could explain gender until someone else presented them directly to them. The students from Program A—similar to students from Program F—were initially split down the middle, with half stating gender was biological and half stating it was socially constructed; however, the difference with Program A was that those students who felt gender was biological provided more fluid explanations, including examples of transgendered and transsexual individuals, thereby identifying an awareness of larger social issues and context for gender, even if they did not define it as socially constructed. Faculty and students from Program F had mixed opinions about whether gender was biological or socially constructed and to what degree these factors played a role even after being prompted further with the second question. In Program A, however, despite some differing opinions among students with the initial question, after being prompted with question number two, there was a general consensus among both faculty and students that gender was more heavily influenced by social construct. It could be argued that because the faculty in Program A had adopted a similar gender ideology, this more evenly transferred to the students of Program A. Again, we see that a solid structure and focus of a program is a vital factor in the gender awareness and preparation of a program. While having an understanding of gender is vital to gender awareness and preparation, applying that understanding and utilizing it through aspects of the education world have much greater potential impact for improving gender equity in our classrooms. Teacher Impact 82 Knowledge of teacher impact on student learning is important concerning gender awareness, as it can help focus teachers to be aware of gender when they engage with students. The majority of the faculty from Program F agreed that teachers play a role of reinforcement rather than development when it comes to a student’s gender identity, and that any differential treatment was either warranted, subconscious, or a combination of the two. Faculty members from Program F also agreed that teachers come to the classroom with their own pre-conceived notions and biases, but that more or less, this is to be expected. Such faculty members were only aware of differential treatment on a surface level and failed to see the potential negative outcomes that could result from such treatment and complacency towards combating its perpetuation. FF3 stated that “A lot of teachers think that girls have more heightened communication skills. Often they will then cater those types of lesson towards a female stereotype . . . Teachers often want boys to fit into boxes and act how girls do and they are often punished for having natural male behaviors.” FF3 had a tone of disdain when discussing punishment for “natural” male behaviors. This is not unlike the common debate concerning the girls’ vs. the boys’ crisis. As discussed earlier, differential treatment and socialized stereotyped expectations hurt everyone, not just boys or girls. Once again we see that some of the ideologies held by faculty from Program F contain potentially negative messages. As researcher Kimmel (2000) explains, “It is not the school experience that “feminizes” boys, but rather the ideology of traditional masculinity that keeps boys from wanting to succeed” (2000:7). Credential programs and 83 faculty members that are not caught up in the girls’ vs. boys’ crisis can help to deconstruct such harmful socialized guidelines for masculinity and femininity by reinforcing the importance of all education. FF4 stated that she made attempts to briefly introduce equitable discourse ideology into her courses, and that it is up to the teacher to develop such. Unfortunately this ideology was not encouraged by the rest of the program structure. While professors from Program F were able to identify some potentially harmful differential treatment in the classroom, it was often not realized as harmful and shrugged off as understandable, subconscious, and as something that can only be reinforced and not changed at the classroom level. Such a lax ideology could potentially perpetuate these problems through the students who graduate from Program F. Half the students from Program F agreed with the faculty from Program F in their belief that teachers only reinforce gender identity, whereas the other half of the students felt that teachers actually play a large factor in helping to develop gender identity from the beginning. Students who felt that teachers could only reinforce gender identity explained that the family is the factor that develops a child’s actual gender identity. Students who felt that teachers actually develop gender identity within their classrooms explained that teachers help to construct an understanding of what is acceptable for boys and girls. These students strongly highlighted how teachers are viewed as role models that help shape what children think about the world around them. 84 Concerning differential treatment based on observed gender, most students from Program F viewed such treatment as acceptable, non-harmful, and sometimes even a manifestation of common sense. Three of the four students from Program F were able to identify ways that children are treated differently based on their observed gender. The fourth student from Program F interview number one (SF1) did not feel any differential treatment occurred in the classroom. Students from Program F explained that differential treatment happens, but for “good” reason. Students explained that teachers speak to girls more sweetly because they are afraid of hurting their feelings, and that though boys are given more hands on tasks, teachers only do such because the boys need it to help them pay attention. They continued by explaining that girls were lucky to not get as many tasks and that they are allowed a greater amount of independence in the elementary school system as a result. Only one specific example was given by the fourth student interviewed from Program F (SF4), who felt there was some differential treatment that could be viewed as negative. She explained that teachers will sometimes only call on girls or boys depending on what type of question is being asked. For example, in math, a teacher she had observed called on the boys the majority of the time even though girls would also be raising their hands to answer. Though the majority of students from Program F were able to identify differential treatment in the classroom, they justified this behavior by claiming such treatments were either part of the learning environment or that they had a positive impact on students. 85 Either way, students from Program F did not recognize how such treatment could potentially be damaging, relying instead on past stereotypes or common sense assumptions to understand the experience rather than having engaged the topic in an academic environment to understand its social context. Responses from Program A faculty members showed a deeper understanding of the roles teachers can play concerning gender development and the impacts that differential treatment can have on students. Faculty member from Program A interview number one (FA1) explained that teachers can reinforce already existing gender stereotypes or they can challenge them. He explained that it all depends on the structure of the classroom, program, and school. Furthermore, faculty member from Program A interview number three (FA3) expressed that classrooms and schools both influence what it means to be male and female, and how to express that significance. He explained that they not only teach gender roles, they also teach attitudes and ideologies that are applied into other aspects of society. Concerning differential treatment in the classroom, faculty from Program A seemed to agree that teachers are generally not aware of what they are doing or the impacts their actions can have. All the faculty members were also able to identify numerous examples of such treatment without prompting. FA3 summed up sentiments that all faculty members from Program A discussed when he said the following: I think that by and large, teachers typically still socialize boys and girls into traditional gender roles in many ways. I think that just as the teacher performs 86 gender, the teacher also draws out particular performances on the part of children as well. They engage gender in literature and classroom management or even sitting boys and girls on different sides of the room, what behaviors they expect, encourage, or overlook in students. Great forms of aggression are more permitting amongst boys than girls. Teachers do, in fact, still reinforce gender stereotypes and gender ideologies. I think teachers worry about students who are gender transgressant—students who don’t fit into the mold. Quite honestly, from my experience I think it is more than worry, but they actually make the students uncomfortable. Responses from the students in Program A similarly reflected a deeper level of understanding demonstrated by their faculty, as opposed to students and faculty from Program F, in that they discussed an awareness of the unintended implications and possible harms that could come from teachers influence in gender identity development and treatment in the classroom. Program A students explained that unfortunately, some teachers still encourage boys and girls to stick to stereotypical activities only, which impacts a child’s sense of gender identity. Students expressed the idea that teachers can either reinforce or critique stereotypical behaviors and activities, similar to the message produced by the faculty in their program. Students in program A witnessed differential treatment during their classroom observations concerning gender in a range of areas, including playground equipment choice, acceptability of dominant behavior, color of paper allowed for art 87 activities, acceptability of crying, what curricula teachers push students towards, and how bullying conflicts were addressed. Student from Program A interview number two (SA2) mentioned a classroom where a child who was female biologically dressed like a tom boy or male. The child wanted to go by her middle name, which was more gender neutral, but the teacher insisted on calling her by her first, more feminine, name. SA2 also described a situation in a different classroom she had observed, there was a biological boy who was very effeminate. This boy wanted to be treated like one of the girls and do their activities, but the teacher kept pushing him to be involved in stereotypical male activities. Though the boy acted out his gender as more effeminate, everyone in the class treated him as though his gender identity was masculine simply due to his observed biological gender. The examples discussed by students in Program A depicted the awareness students in the program have about the impact teachers can have, including how many teachers will fight against a student’s personal desires in order to maintain traditional roles and his/her own biases, without taking into account how frustrating or damaging this could be to that student. The large contrasts between faculty and students from Program F and Program A were that those from Program F viewed teachers as only having the ability to reinforce the potentially damaging status quo that has already been set in place by society and the family, whereas Program A viewed teachers as agents with the ability to choose to either reinforce or challenge the status quo. Members of Program F were able to identify differential treatment based on observed gender, but viewed most treatment as 88 acceptable, understandable, and generally un-harmful. The few times differential treatment was deemed as negative at all, it was dismissed as being subconscious and therefore, understandable. Members of Program A identified and analyzed differential treatment as possible sources for negative impact that could be forcing children to abide by gender rules that may not suit their given identity. Again, we see that the ideology of faculty is often strongly transferred to their students. Though one faculty member from Program A (FA3) felt that his students did not grasp the impact of gender in the classroom as much as he would have liked, it seems that due to the necessity his program felt for making them aware of this impact, the message did, in fact, get through to the majority of students. Students from Program A were able to identify and analyze how pushing children to engage in more traditional gendered behaviors could stifle a child’s academic interests and overall behavior. They were also able to identify that allowing boys to dominate in the classroom left girls silenced, unlike students from Program F, who saw that girls being left alone was a privilege, fostering independence. As faculty members from Program A explained, most students and teachers are not aware of how gender role development and treatment can impact academic performance and all other aspects of society, and that such awareness and change are left up to the structure of programs, classrooms, and schools. Training While much of how a teacher organizes and conducts a classroom is derived from personal preference, bias, and experience over time, the training and preparation of 89 credential programs can be considered one of the strongest influencing factors for classroom structure. Concerning education on bias in the classroom, in general or specific to gender, all the Program F faculty members identified that they had courses that discussed topics such as teacher attitudes, prejudices, stereotypes, multiculturalism, diversity in the classroom, religion, and especially race. All of the faculty members believed that the topic of gender was briefly mentioned in some of their course work, but that their program did not spend focused time on gender diversity. For example, FF4 explained that in one of her courses she discusses gender and GLBT studies briefly, but, like the rest of her colleagues, the majority of her focus is on racial diversity. Professors explained that gender discussion mainly happened in the undergraduate courses that are available to those who are doing a combination Bachelor’s and credential program, but not someone who had just come to their school for only the credential program. Students from Program F agreed that cultural and ethnic differences were the main focus in the coursework. Half of the students did not feel that there was any discussion of gender in any of their courses, highlighting that even if they received education in the undergraduate coursework, it did not have an impact student awareness. Students from Program F explained that when gender was mentioned in their graduate courses, it was usually a simple comment by the professor that was not reflected in the course work and curricula of their program. SF1 explained that the only section on gender she remembered was relating to culture, in that there are different cultural norms and that in some cultures men are held to higher educational expectations than women 90 and that they, as future teachers, should respect such a norm. Though this student understood the importance of respecting cultures, once again she did not seem to be aware of the harm that could also come from sending such a message to her future students: a common pattern seen throughout Program F. And though some faculty members may have put small, subtle messages about gender into their courses, in the end, the students did not feel that they had taken away any training concerning the subject when it was provided almost parenthetically. All Program A faculty included gender when prompted with the first question about courses in their program that instruct on avoiding general bias in education, whereas with Program F, faculty only mentioned gender once they were specifically asked about the subject. Gender ideology was not even a conscious aspect of Program F. Faculty members from Program A explained that they have courses on diversity that discuss culture, race/ethnicity, gender, socio-economic status and sexual orientation. Specifically, faculty mentioned their psychology of education, child development, and diversity courses solidly contain gender related course content and that all three are required courses. Though the entire faculty believed that their program had a strong focus on gender, none of the faculty members felt they had completed any significant educational training on gender that would have created such a focus. In addition to the enabling structure of the program itself, the knowledge that faculty members in Program A had concerning gender in education was accredited to their personal agency in 91 conducting studies or reading on such subjects, and sharing that knowledge with their colleagues. Half of the students from Program A felt gender was a recognized part of their coursework training while the other half did not feel there was a significant contribution. Regardless, unlike students in Program F, who felt they had to search to even find a brief mention of gender in their program, all of the students from Program A recognized that they did have some gender training. Even more interestingly, students from Program A not only received more training, but sought out more information on their own, likely because of the initial awareness that their program had created. For example, SA2 expressed a desire for her program to include even more gender awareness, by making their university’s introduction to lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender studies (LGBT) a part of their program. The student explained that she took this course on her own and that it broadened her overall educational insight. Student from Program A, interview number 3 (SA3) explained that many of the teachers in his program are very passionate in emphasizing the need to avoid bias in the classroom. He mentioned specifically that in a math methods course they discussed how society has been taught for years that boys are better at math than girls and that we need to overcome this ideology. This student explained that though he has not had any gender training outside of his program, he felt that he has gained a lot of insight into it through life experience as a homosexual. And though both of the students who indicated gender awareness in their program had outside personal connection or interest to the subject 92 matter, they were also both able to identify specific examples of how the subject was discussed inside their program. Furthermore, unlike students in Program F, who were not even aware of gender discussions in the classroom, students form Program A pushed and wanted more because they had an awareness of the importance of the concepts to their field. Though there was disagreement concerning how much Program A focused on gender, all the students from Program A were able to agree that their coursework solidly focused on other forms of educational bias. Students related that they felt there was a larger focus on topics of race, socio-economic status, and culture. Responses from faculty and students in both programs indicated that there truly is a greater focus on multiculturalism and race in teaching credential programs today than gender. Though gender was not completely ignored by Program F, there was a much stronger awareness and attempt to include gender education preparation into Program A. It is clear that even if the faculty of a credential program share a structured ideology and incorporate it into their curricula, students are still left to their own agency when it comes to how fully they will accept that ideology and if they additionally want to research the topic further. And just as it can be argued that faculty in Program A used their agency to include gender into their ideology, it can similarly be argued that faculty in Program F used their agency not to; passivism is still an action. Though many of the students in both programs felt that they had not received enough training on gender, it is encouraging to see that some students from Program A used their agency to research the topic further on their own to 93 become better informed future teachers. It is also possible that those students who identified their programs as conveying information about gender in education were already biased towards the subject with their own prior interests; however, when backed up by information from the faculty and content analysis, the finding cannot be overlooked. Though the content of credential programs has the potential to either preserve damaging status quos or make a large impact on gender discrimination awareness and combative techniques, classroom materials can also have a significant impact. When discussing gender stereotyping in the materials and curricula faculty and students are given or can use in the classroom, all of the faculty members from Program F agreed that none of the materials they used in their own program courses contained any such stereotyping. While all faculty members agreed that their program was free of such gender stereotyping, they all also agreed that some, though to a small degree, could potentially exist out in the education world outside their credential program. And though FF4 expressed her desire for teachers to instruct students to monitor the messages in their textbooks, none of the faculty was able to specifically identify in what ways or forms they believed gender stereotyping is or could be an issue in materials and curricula today. It seems that without being able to identify such stereotyping and bias on a faculty level, it would be impossible, or at least very difficult, to expect their students to be able to identify and avoid such content. 94 All of the students in Program F agreed that there are no issues concerning gender stereotyping in materials and curricula. Students identified that gender stereotyping could occur in the classroom due to an individual teacher’s bias, but not at all from any materials or curricula. When specifically prompted, these students agreed that the education world and credential programs have addressed multiculturalism more in the last few years to avoid bias, but did not feel that this has been similarly addressed regarding gender. Similar to Program F, all the faculty from Program A agreed that there is still gender stereotyping in some educational materials and curricula, though they do not use such biased materials in their credential program. All faculty members were also able to provide various specific examples of such gender stereotyping that still exists and methods for combating it within their own program. FA3 explained that textbooks about child development that are used in programs do not address gender issues significantly enough and that silence is always biased towards preserving the status quo. He explained that it is for this reason that he brings in outside sources and engages his students in critical discussions about the topics. FA3 explained that he cannot trust his textbooks to do this for him. He described most textbooks as “safe” and “vanilla,” but not critically engaging the issue. They are “safe” in that they will not engage, not that they cannot do harm. FA3, in particular, stated that the textbooks written for teacher education students devotes remarkably little space to gender issues, and that unless their teacher supplements 95 their textbook with other materials students will not get any more understanding of gender issues than what they came in with. Not only were all the faculty in Program A aware that gender stereotyping and bias still exist in teaching materials and curricula, but where faculty from Program F failed, they were able to provide specific examples and explain how they combat and instruct about it in their program. While rarely a professor from Program F would express a desire for new solutions or more awareness, they showed no signs of looking for those solutions themselves. In contrast, faculty members in Program A have already found supplemental materials and are staying current on the research topic by reading the work of Sadker and Sadker. The majority of students in Program A did not feel that gender stereotyping is an issue. All of the students explained that they based this decision from the classrooms they have observed as part of their credential program. Students who did not observe gender stereotyping in their classrooms still displayed a working knowledge of what to look for by explaining that the materials on the classroom walls were gender neutral and mainly student work, that boys and girls read the same books, and that boys are not discouraged from effeminate behavior. In contrast to the experiences of her peers, SA2 explained how furious she has been by some of the examples of gender stereotyping she has seen during her classroom observations within materials and curricula. The student explained that it is not always blatant, but that stereotyping does exist in the textbooks and testing materials used today. 96 She then recounted a 6th grade benchmark test that asked how the student would feel at a tea party and the correct word to choose was "uncomfortable." Another example she provided concerned a math problem that involved two men and two women. The questions asked the student to solve how many possibilities there were to create a couple. The teacher then explained to the class that they should "of course" assume that only a man could be with a woman and that a man and another man do not go together. The teacher explained this in a joking tone and all the students then laughed. Though not all the students from Program A felt that gender stereotyping exists in teacher materials and curricula, they did identify things that would be considered as such if handled differently. This suggests that the students from Program A have been trained more proficiently to identify gender stereotyping and bias in their classrooms. Once again we see that having a collective and fully constructed ideology among faculty members results in a greater chance for students to similarly use their agency to adopt this structured ideology. Division of the Sexes All faculty members from Program F said they were familiar with Title IX; however, when queried about what Title IX was, the main focus they discussed concerned equity between gender and sports rather than the wide range of items that Title IX concerns. Only one of the four faculty participants (FF4) identified that Title IX also concerns “sexual harassment and any other discrimination based on sex.” When asked if they felt that Title IX was being enforced, all faculty members agreed that it was enforced, but that 97 it is not perfectly enforced. Faculty members attributed their knowledge of enforcement to everyday life observations, rather than to any research or published work. FF3 explained that she felt it was enforced because she often hears her husband, an avid sports fan, complain that men’s sports are having to fund women’s sports because legally, both the male and female teams have to exist, but only the men’s teams bring in money because people are only interested in watching the men’s teams. Once again, such a simple message from a Program F faculty member of bias, forced tolerance, and hierarchy of sports by gender was not recognized as potentially harmful if passed on to credential students. Though the entire faculty from Program F was familiar with Title IX on at least a surface level, this knowledge was not translated to the program’s students. Only one of four students from Program F had ever heard of Title IX. The one student (SF4) who was familiar only understood it to concern gender equity for sports. This student went on to explain that the only reason she knew about Title IX was due to an incident when she was in high school and not from her teaching credential program. She explained that some at her school were trying to eliminate their gymnastics team to make more room for their football team. The student went on to explain that the school utilized Title IX to keep the gymnastics team for her high school. Not surprisingly, with Program A, it was found that all faculty members were familiar with Title IX, but unlike Program F, all faculty from Program A were also aware that Title IX not only concerns sex equity for sports, but concerning equal access to 98 everything concerning education and its programs. All faculty members also agreed that Title IX has not been, and is still not being fully enforced today. While the knowledge from faculty in Program F did not transfer to any of their students, the case was different for Program A. Only one of the four students from Program A (SA4) was not at all familiar with Title IX. Two of the students (SA1 and SA2) who were familiar only understood Title IX in its relation to sex equity and sports. The third student (SA3) had a full understanding of Title IX, which he accredited to his teaching credential program. Once again Program A students display not only a working knowledge of further gender issues, but also the ability to apply them further and analyze the influences such issues can have on a large scale. Clearly, the understanding of Title IX was much more developed among faculty and students in Program A than F. Again this could be due to the fact that all faculty members seemed to have a structured, shared understanding of Title IX and how it relates to their program’s ideology concerning sex and gender studies. It is common sense that if faculty members are teaching different or conflicting ideas, the students from that program will learn, or at least be exposed to, those different or conflicting ideas, thus finding it more difficult to apply in their education and training. Though more common before the implementation of Title IX, some educators and researchers are re-exploring sex segregated classrooms and/or schools. All the Program F faculty members seemed to have an understanding that sex segregation is generally implemented to control behavior better and also to promote math and science to girls. 99 None of the faculty members from Program F was in full support of sex segregated learning environments and one was completely against it. Those who saw some good in it, but did not necessarily promote the idea, explained that their understanding was that it has been helpful for some girls, but not as much for boys. They felt that if it did not work for everyone, it was not a solution. FF3 spoke of a school district she was in where a pilot of gender segregated classrooms was implemented. She explained that this was difficult mostly because there was not enough training for teachers on how instruction may be different and that hardly anyone wanted to teach an all boy classroom. Two faculty members explained that they were completely against the idea because "separate is never equal." Despite their feelings on this, they both then promoted the idea of how well African American boys have been doing in segregated charter schools. Once again, this shows how much stronger Program F’s concern was for racial issues over those of gender. FF1 explained that she wished there were alternative strategies beyond sex segregation and that if there is something that really works, it would mean that everything has to be re-revaluated. The majority of students from Program F seemed to only view sex segregated schools as being a way to avoid hormones distracting students from their work. None of the students from Program F was in favor of sex segregation. Student from Program F interview number three (SF3) felt that sex segregated schools were only for females and that in such schools the only things discussed are women’s studies, gender studies, and that everything is biased and only presented from the female perspective. SF1 explained 100 that one of the schools she observed through her program had been changed to sex segregated classrooms. Unfortunately, she only observed in the elective courses which were still integrated. She explained that in speaking with the teachers of the segregated classrooms, they felt girls benefited more and that the boys just “goofed off” more. Faculty members from Program A were open to the possibility of sex segregation, but felt there was still a lack of research to support it as the ultimate answer. All the faculty mentioned the benefits of sex segregation in depth—particularly to females concerning math, science, and participation in the classroom. They also discussed the potential negative impact of sacrificed social learning that could come as a result of sex integration. Faculty members in this program expressed a desire for constant experimentation with and evaluation of such matters. The majority of the faculty, echoing some of the faculty in Program F, also mentioned the research that is currently being conducted with charter schools that segregate African American males. Faculty member from Program A interview number two (FA2) mentioned that her program is currently exploring the idea of doing field work in such a charter school, but that her overall feeling is that some segregated classes are a much better solution than an entirely segregated school for all classes. FA3 explained that though the jury is still out for how successful sex segregated education is, one cannot deny the necessity for females to be able to interact more in their classes. He explained that in these segregated environments, the females are not competing with males who 101 have already been socialized to be more aggressive in the classroom, and competing with teachers who are used to hearing mainly male voices. Though the faculty from Program A had a solid understanding of what current research says about sex segregation and why it is often promoted or not, this knowledge and understanding did not appear to be a subject that was passed on to their students. The majority of students felt that they did not have insight concerning sex segregated education. SA1 explained that in elementary school she was in a sex segregated language arts class, but did not know if it benefited anyone. None of the students felt that sex segregation would be significantly beneficial, and one student even felt that it could be detrimental. SA2 explained that by segregating by sex, you are also forcing students into a gender to associate with even more than what is done in an average classroom. She explained that some boys may be more comfortable having friends who are girls and participating in stereotypical girl activities, but if such a boy were placed into an all boy school he might not fit in, not have any friends, have greater difficulty with his gender identity, and even suffer academically due to this frustration. While faculty in Program F seemed aware of the surface idea that sex segregation was to promote comfort in the classroom and encouragement for females into math and science, their students seemed completely unaware of these concepts. Though some of these faculty members were aware of the need for alternative strategies and inclusion of new ideas, they were completely unaware of the gender teaching resources that currently exist. 102 Similarly, faculty from Program A expressed a desire to have more teacher education concerning gender issues, but unlike their colleagues in Program F, Program A faculty were already in the practice of seeking out those additional resources to supplement their existing program. Program A faculty also showed a deeper understanding of why sex segregation is often successful for females. They also displayed more interest in taking action through experimentation and field work to try to resolve some of the educational issues of our day, rather than simply noting that it would be nice if something existed, waiting for someone else to do the work and bring it to their attention. Students from neither program appeared to have a vast knowledge concerning sex segregated education. Still, there was a distinctly different perspective about sex segregation among the programs. For example, students in program F, similar to their faculty, relied on opinion and personal experience in their understanding. In one explicit example, SF3 referenced a strongly biased assumption that the curricula in sex segretated institutions was nothing more than harmful feminist propaganda, offering no citation or reference to any form of research to back up the claim. In contrast, students in program A tended to be more rational in their perception about gender segregation. Despite the fact that students from Program A did not feel like they had a solid understanding of sex segregation, SA2 expressed how she could apply her understanding of gender and gender identity relating to the classroom to issues of segregation. Her reasoning showed a more sophisticated understanding of the relationship between gender identity and academic 103 performance, relying on academic findings in general areas of gender to understand the issue. As discussed earlier, one of the main reasons some promote sex segregated education is due to the performance differences found between girls and boys. Studies have found that while girls tend to achieve higher grades on their report cards, boys tend to achieve higher scores on high stakes tests (Jackson 1998). Half the faculty members from Program F stated that they were not familiar with such a phenomenon and half stated that they were. Faculty members who claimed to be aware of the phenomenon explained that girls have a greater desire to please others through compliance in the classroom and by completing their homework, which teachers value and reward through grades. FF3 was the only faculty member to posit a reason for why boys would score higher on high stakes tests. She stated that “Teachers often underestimate how much boys are taking away without jumping through the hoops. They may master, but not be compliant. My son, husband, and boys in general have more of a memory for facts and figures and girls aren’t as socially constructed for that.” Though it is encouraging that at least one faculty member was able to determine that part of performance differences can be attributed to different social constructions, professor FF3 posited the differences in a deterministic manner that limits potential changes in the disparity of test scores. Simply, faculty from program F tend to promote the understanding that through limited awareness or passive acceptance, girls will continue to do poorly in standardized test scores. 104 Students from Program F followed a similar pattern as their faculty. Only one student from Program F (SF1) claimed to be aware of girls achieving higher grades and boys scoring higher on high stakes tests and a second student stated that she was only aware of girls receiving higher grades. Regardless, students in Program F relied on gender stereotypes to explain differences, i.e. that compliance is something highly valued by teachers, and that girls tend to be more concerned with compliance in academics than boys. For example, SF3, while trying to explain this disparity in greater detail, was still reliant on stereotypical assumed differences between boys and girls. As he stated, “Maybe men are just better at test taking in general. And boys can maybe try to figure out a potential solution to a problem they don’t know how to do and girls only know how to do to the specific types of questions they learned how to answer in the classroom, but boys can apply their knowledge to new problems.” Ultimately, students in Program F referenced limited knowledge about performances differences, relying on stereotypes to explain these differences, offering little depth in the explanation, much less any social context. Unlike Program F, all of the faculty members from Program A related their responses more strongly back to research they have reviewed, rather than to personal speculation. For example, FA2 explained that she was very aware of grade versus test score differentials but thought that while there are statistically significant differences in some academic performance of males versus females, such differences are not particularly consequential. 105 Though the majority of faculty members in Program A explained that they were not fully aware of the reported disparity between girls' grades and boys' test scores, it was clear that they had an awareness that classroom dynamics and our education system in general could account for such outcomes. FA3 explained that girls generally perform better on a daily basis inside the classroom, following instruction, and sitting quietly for the teacher, something he coined as “braids in school.” He then began to discuss patterns in standardized testing and noted that roughly twice as many girls take AP tests in English and foreign languages as boys do. FA3 explained that though both boys and girls take AP tests, the subjects they take them in are still very gender divided. He stated that differences in grades result from a combination of student self-concept and teacher expectation or bias. Concerning testing, he felt that differences in scores were a result of student attitudes towards testing itself, which are often socially ascribed by gender. He explained that there is often anxiety associated with testing that mainly hurts girls’ scores. While faculty members from Program F seemed unprepared, providing answers based on guesses, personal experiences, and brain storming at that very moment, responses from faculty in Program A reflected a working knowledge of research on the subject matter and the ability to apply it along with their own experiences. This is another example of the great awareness and preparation differences found between the two programs. Students from Program A—similarly to their faculty members—while providing mixed and varied answers, all displayed a working knowledge of some gender research in 106 their comments. Comments by SA2 offer a good summary about the differences in academics by sex as discussed by students in Program A. She explained how there is still often a “message” in education concerning specific subjects, i.e. boys do better in math and science and girls excel in reading and writing. She explained how teachers can produce these biased messages and reinforce them to students. She felt that sometimes girls will hear such a message about girls not doing well in math, or that it isn’t “cool” for girls to be good at math, science, or testing and as a result, they will pretend not to be good at the subject, or just not try at all. Furthermore, SA2 explained that boys can feel pressured to focus more on sports instead of their homework because school is not as "cool." Similarly, she suggested that there are societal messages to boys to tell them that they are not good in reading or writing. What is expressed through SA2’s comments is a perspective of teacher and school impact on student experience and education. In contrast, students in Program F tended to rely on these same “messages” in their explanation, but more so as an absolute, not as a social construction. . Overall, while faculty and students from both Program F and A presented a variety of answers concerning performance disparities between boys and girls, Program A faculty and students were more in touch with the explanations posited by the researchers who explored the phenomenon of grade/test disparity between the sexes, as well as applying a deeper analysis and awareness. This seems to fall in line with overall perspectives in Program A and F concerning gender issues, as faculty and students in Program A rely more on research and social context to explain phenomena, contrasting 107 with faculty and students in Program F, who tend to rely more on personal experience and social opinion. Chapter 5 DISCUSSION Based on the findings, subtle—yet potentially harmful—gender discrimination exists in California multiple subject teaching credential programs. With over 50 percent of programs identified as being weak in gender awareness and preparation, and only nine percent of the programs recognizedas being high in gender awareness and preparation, my hypothesis—that few credential programs place a concerted effort on identifying and effectively combating gender discrimination—was fully validated. Overall, while the agency of individual faculty is an important contributor towards gender ideology, without the support and encouragement of a structured and organized program, as evidenced in Program F, individuals can only have so much impact. Other research has stressed the importance of having a supported philosophy among teachers and administrators concerning race awareness and preparation (Smith 2009), which this study only further confirms with respect to gender. Even if the faculty members share a generally enlightened gender ideology, unless that understanding is systematically built into the structure and curriculum of the program in question, unintended gender bias is likely to be perpetuated. 108 When faculty members are united through a structured program and a focused, informed ideology, students are more likely to both understand and reproduce that same ideology. Program A was structured to include gender education in its curricula and included faculty members who were individually committed to a shared gender ideology, thereby contributing to a more sophisticated understanding of gender issues among their students. For example, students from Program A were able to identify that allowing boys to dominate in the classroom left girls silenced. When that structure and united front are missing from a program, students are then left to call upon whatever gender lessons, observations, or biases they may have developed through their personal lives. Faculty members in Program F were unaware of the need for further inclusion of gender into the structure and curricula of their program, thereby unwittingly contributing to a less enlightened student cohort. As a result, there was a common pattern of students from Program F providing simple answers, and sometimes off hand guesses during interviews. For example, students from Program F saw that girls being left alone was a privilege to be seen as independence—a misplaced conclusion that far too many educators make (Sadker and Sadker 1995). In contrast, students from Program A based their responses more often on more fluid and thoughtful considerations of gender, often based on research and predominantly from their program training. Not only were students from Program A more aware of gender issues in the classroom, they were also more aware of the potential harm that could arise from such 109 subtle gender discrimination, bias, or lack of proper training. Observations from participants in Program A supported the findings of Smith and Hung (2008) in that the reason we see some gender differences in academic achievement is due to bias and cultural influences in the classroom and not ability. As observed in classrooms by Sadker and Sadker (1995), gender biases are more likely to be produced or perpetuated by teachers who are unaware of such biases and their potential negative impact. Similarly, it could be deducted that credential programs, administrators, and faculty members who are unaware of gender biases in education would also be more likely to produce and perpetuate such damaging stereotypes. As discussed earlier, most educators and administrators do not share the same understanding and awareness of subtle, subconscious, teacher bias and influence created through differential treatment based on gender, and how that influence can perpetuate stereotypes and negatively influence a student for life (Sadker and Sadker 1995; Sadker et al. 2009; Hamilton, Blumenfeld, Akoh and Miura 1991; Fang 1996; Shepardson and Pizzini 1992). However, in the case of Program A, teachers and administrators did share a common awareness about having a gender ideology. Program A, as a whole, made an intentional effort to include structured gender discussion into their curricula. In contrast, in Program F, faculty members were not united structurally concerning gender issues, and were subsequently left only to the capabilities of their personal agency and commitment to teach about gender. 110 With very few exceptions, due to these differences in program cohesion of structure and ideology, there were notable variations between Program F and Program A. For example, all the faculty members from Program A regularly displayed a working knowledge of current research concerning gender in education, whereas only one or two faculty members from Program F referred to current research and studies on occasion. As a result, faculty in Program A were generally able to reach and teach a consensus on their ideologies, whereas faculty in Program F were often in disagreement and sometimes even used potentially harmful ideologies as grounds for their classroom lessons. This is potentially due to the structural and organizational differences between the two programs. Even from the beginning, while faculty members in Program F were split on the very definition of gender, all faculty members in Program A were able to agree that social constructionism plays the strongest role in the definition of gender. This is an example of the organizational and structural differences between the two programs in having an understood shared ideology. If a program cannot even agree on a simple definition of gender, it is no wonder why it would meet great difficulty in combating gender discrimination. Time and time again, the study showed that when faculty members did not share an ideology, neither did their students. And though it was encouraging to hear that one faculty member from Program F (FF4) attempted to incorporate some gender discussion into her classes, her efforts were so small that they had no lasting impact on her students. 111 Students from Program A regularly reported that their faculty members were not only knowledgeable, but passionate about including gender in their credential program courses. Students seemed to care and be aware of gender discrimination because their faculty members and program did as well. Findings showed that having a collective and fully constructed ideology, or at least awareness among faculty members resulted in a greater likelihood for students to similarly use their agency to adopt this structured ideology. As explained by Smith (2009), the success or failure of education is dependent upon the effective preparation of teachers and administrators. Furthermore, the success or failure of those teachers and administrators is heavily dependent upon having a structured program that allows for and encourages gender awareness and preparation among its faculty. Faculty from Program A expressed a desire for all teacher education programs to include more training for gender issues. They understood the vital importance of promoting both further research and training for the benefit of all society. Faculty from Program A expressed their distress in the fact that most of their own students were quite unsophisticated concerning gender issues and had never considered the topic of gender in education until it was brought forward in their program. And even though Program A is leaps and bounds ahead of the majority of California teaching credential programs concerning gender awareness and preparation, the faculty in this program still expressed a desire to learn more for themselves as well. This yearning for more knowledge was also reflected in their students. 112 As discussed earlier, gender discrimination is greatly influenced by the institutionalization and socialization of various facets in our society, and in this case that facet is in the form of a teaching credential program. While some programs have been socialized through state mandates to include multiculturalism and more general diversity subject matter, no such institutionalized socialization on the behalf of gender diversity has taken place (California Commission on Teacher Credentialing 2007; 2009). When the larger institutionalized mainstream social ideologies are not challenged, we merely perpetuate the status quo, which in the case of Program F includes subtle gender discrimination. While it is true that institutionalized facets of socialization have a tremendous impact on gender discrimination, institutionalization theories are missing one key factor: agency. Although always influenced by institutionalism, humans are thinking actors who are capable of making active decisions outside of those set by the main institution (Markstrom and Hallden 2008). We see such agency being used through the programs that have chosen to structure gender awareness and preparation socialization into their ideologies. Put more simply, the best program in the world will only be partially effective if it is not staffed by faculty who are committed to progressive ideals. On the other hand, though the agency of individual faculty was found to be an important contributor towards gender ideology, without being coupled with a structured program, those individual faculty members had limited impact. For example, FF4 often suggested in her responses that gender awareness was important and that she attempted to 113 include limited information on the topic. Unfortunately, this ideology was not transferred to her colleagues. It is vital that a successful program not only have individuals who will use their agency to harbor positive gender ideologies, but also that the programs themselves have clear organization and goals that allow individual agency to have a greater impact. As important as individual agency is, it is the combination of the two that result is the greatest chance for success. While it was clear from the example of Program F that agency without structure is very limited, one must wonder if structure without agency would similarly be stunted in its potential. A study done by Athanases and Martin (2006) that reviewed California teaching credential program suggests that such programs have a greater potential for effectiveness in general educational equity awareness and preparation when program coherence and integration across courses, fieldwork, and ideology are all present. Though an institutionalized structure may present an idea, individuals must also evoke their agency to effectively introduce new ideas to their students. Where Do We Go From Here? While this study has identified a potentially significant attributor to the problem of gender discrimination in our classrooms, due to the small size of Program A and the lack of organization of Program F, it was difficult to obtain a large number of participants. Future studies with larger populations would result in more generalizable findings. Though this study only focused on California multiple subject teaching credential programs, it would be naïve to assume that such findings would not result 114 similarly in California single subject teaching credential programs, as well as programs from other states in the USA. Further research would be needed to verify such results and to determine the magnitude at which our nation is “blissfully” still unaware of the various issues related to gender bias. Aside from the unfeasible task of having faculty and administration from the top nine percent of schools provide mentoring to the remaining California programs, how do we start improving? Though changing state mandated requirements will likely be necessary down the road, such implementation takes time. Effort should also be made to reinstate much of the lost, forgotten, and sometimes purposefully ignored power of Title IX. Tremendous resources already exist that could easily be implemented into any teaching credential program. Anthologies such as Gender in the Classroom: Foundations, Skills, Methods, and Strategies Across the Curriculum (Sadker and Silber 2007) could serve as a very easy way to introduce the history of gender in education and the struggles that it still faces and how one can implement ways to improve these issues within one's own classroom. Each chapter is followed by numerous activities that teachers can include in their own lesson plans. Not only would using a broad resource such as this provide the what and make teachers more aware of the issue at hand, it could also provide them with the how to do something about it. 115 Conclusion In agreement with my content analysis findings, other researchers have found that recent efforts have been made in many credential programs to include courses and curricula concerning equity and avoiding differential treatment based on race (Katsarou 2009; Smith 2009). The majority of California multiple subject teaching credential programs, similar to Program F, have in fact made this a primary focus. Though race/ethnicity issues are certainly vital, their importance does not overshadow the necessity for awareness of other issues such as gender. By becoming more aware of gender discrimination, we will be able to better understand all other forms of discrimination that we face today (Connolly 1994). Just as racism has been created through prior institutions from earlier history, modern socialization and programs to promote diversity and understanding have diminished racism in society (Smith 2009; Katsarou 2009). Gender issues can—and should—be countered by emphasizing gender discrimination in our programs and that can best be accomplished through a well-focused and organized program (such as Program A), which is staffed by faculty who are personally committed to educating a new generation of students about the dangers of gender bias. Results from this study showed that though much improvement has been made concerning gender discrimination in the classroom concerning children’s reading books and classroom displays, subtle—potentially harmful—gender lessons still exist within teacher/student interaction, gender ascribed games and toys, curricula and testing 116 materials, and equal access to full participation in all aspects of the classroom. Though this finding may seem somewhat disheartening, regarding the current state of our education system, having identified the teaching credential program as a possible significant contributor to the problem of gender discrimination in our classrooms, clear, purposeful steps can and should now be taken to ameliorate the situation. As long as teachers in the majority of California teaching credential programs continue to lack awareness of the problems discussed above, and do not benefit from concrete interventions specifically designed to help them combat sex discrimination, that discrimination and negative gendered messages will continue to be transmitted unwittingly into future classrooms. Regardless of how well-meaning they might be, teachers who are not educated about inappropriate gender differences will likely continue to perpetuate them. Teacher education training is profoundly influential in preparing teachers to advocate for equity (Katsarou 2009; Anthanases and Martin 2006). Studies concerning race education have similarly suggested colleges of education have been complicit in maintaining educational inequalities for too long and that teacher preparation and curricula need to first eliminate damaging stereotypes and biases for change to happen in classrooms, which will in turn, influence communities and larger social movements (Katsarou 2009). If the perpetuation of stereotypes related to race and ethnicity is a problem (and it most certainly is), then shouldn't other forms of bias, including gender, be similarly confronted? 117 If we give teachers a proper education about gender discrimination, its history in education, and methods for breaking down this discrimination and negative socialization in their future classrooms, every member of society will potentially benefit. Teaching credential programs truly are the foundation from which classroom etiquette and methods are born and we must harness that function for the best possible outcome. While our society’s general consciousness with respect to gender discrimination has likely been raised, our personal and collective commitment to do something about it may not have been. It is precisely for this reason that our nation's teacher preparation programs should be updated to include conscious, specific interventions that will arm future teachers with the knowledge and will to recognize, confront, and then ameliorate gender discrimination. 118 BIBLIOGRAPHY Allen, Sara, Anne Cantor, Helen Grady and Pam Hill. 1997. "Classroom Talk: Coed Classes that Work for Girls." 20(1):41-46. Athanases, Steven Z. and Kathleen J. Martin. 2006. “Learning to advocate for education equity in a teacher credential program.” Teaching and Teacher Education 22: 627-646. Babbie, Earl. 1986. Observing Ourselves: Essays in Social Research. Long Grove, Illinois: Waveland Press Inc. Birdsell, Jo and Marjorie A. Suckow. 2009. “A Cross-Cultural Comparison: Teacher Preparation Programs in Gujarat, India and California, United States.” Delta Kappa Gamma Bulletin 75(4):5-10. Bradford, Kathy J. 2008. "An Androcentric Gauge: Portrayals of Gender Relations in Elementary Social Studies Knowledge." Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Sociology, The University of Western Ontario, Canada. Buss, David M. 1998. “Sexual Strategies Theory: Historical Origins and Current Status – The Use of Theory in Research and Scholarship on Sexuality.” The Journal of Sex Research 35(1):19-31. Cable, Kelly E. and Terry E. Spradlin. 2008. Single-Sex Education in the 21st Century. Education Policy Brief. 6(9):1-12. Center for Evaluation and Education Policy, Indiana University. 119 California Commission on Teacher Credentialing. 2009. “Single Subject Teaching Credential Requirements for Teachers Prepared in California.” California Commission on Teacher Credentialing, Retrieved April 20th 2009 (http://www.ctc.ca.gov/credentials/requirements.html). California Commission on Teacher Credentialing. 2007-2008. “Appendix A-1: Pass-Rate Data for Regular Teacher Preparation Programs.” Annual Report Card on California Teacher Preparation Programs. Retrieved May 3rd 2010 (http://www.ctc.ca.gov/reports/TitleII_2007-2008_AnnualRpt.pdf). California Commission on Teacher Credentialing. 2007. “Multiple Subject Teaching Credential Requirements for Teachers Prepared in California.” California Commission on Teacher Credentialing. Retrieved April 20th 2009 (http://www.ctc.ca.gov/credentials/requirements.html). Cognard-Black, Andrew J. 1997. "Will they Stay, Or Will they Go?: Occupational Mobility among Male Elementary School Teachers in the United States." American Sociological Association Conference Paper. College Board. 2006. “College Board Announces Scores for new SAT with Writing Section.” College Board Press Releases. Retrieved November 14th 2009 (http://www.collegebaord.com/press/releases/150054.html). Connolly, Paul. 1994. "All Lads Together?: Racism, Masculinity and Multicultural/AntiRacist Strategies in a Primary School." International Studies in Sociology of Education 4(2):191-211 120 Crocco, Margaret S. and Andrea S. Libresco. 2007. “Citizenship Education for the 21st Century—A Gender Inclusive Approach to Social Studies. Pp. 109-164 in Gender in the Classroom, edited by D. Sadker and E. S. Silber. Cullen, Lisa Takeuchi. “Show Us Our Money: Not knowing what your colleagues make can hurt you. A modest proposal for closing the paycheck gap.” Time Magazine May 12, 2008. Dwyer, Carol A. 1976. “Test Content and Sex Differences in Reading.” The Reading Teacher 29(8):753-757. Entwisle, Doris R., Karl L. Alexander, and Linda S. Olson. 2007. "Early Schooling: The Handicap of being Poor and Male." Sociology of Education 80(2):114-138. Evans, Lorraine and Kimberly Davies. 2000. "No Sissy Boys here: A Content Analysis of the Representation of Masculinity in Elementary School Reading Textbooks." Sex Roles 42(3-4):255-270. Fang, Zhihui, 1996. “A Review of Research on Teacher Beliefs and Practices.” Educational Research 38(1):47-65. Farr, Roger, Michael A. Tulley, and Deborah Powell. 1987. “The Evaluation and Selection of Basal Readers.” The Elementary School Journal 87(3):267-281. Gagnon, Claudette. 1998. "Dynamics of Academic Success of Primary-Level Females: A New Interactionist Approach." RecherchesFeministes (1):19-45. 121 Hall, Cathy W., Nichelle B. Davis, Larry M. Bolen, and Rosina Chia. 1999. "Gender and Racial Differences in Mathematical Performance." Journal of Social Psychology 139(6):677-689. Hamilton, Mykol, David A. Anderson, Michelle Broaddus, and Kate Young. “Gender stereotyping and under-representation of female characters in 200 popular children’s picture books: A twenty-first century update.” Sex Roles 55 (2006), pp. 757-65. Hamilton, V. L., Phyllis C. Blumenfeld, Hiroshi Akoh, and Kanae Miura. 1991. "Group and Gender in Japanese and American Elementary Classrooms." Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 22(3):317-346. Hannah, G. (2003). Reaching Gender Equity in Mathematics Education. Educational Forum, 67, 204-214. Hoffman, John L. and Katie E. Lowitzki. 2005. “Predicting College Success with High School Grades and Test Scores: Limitation for Minority Students.” The Review of Higher Education 28(4):455-474. Hughes, Teresa A. 2007. “The Advantages of Single-Sex Education.” National Forum of Educational Administration and Supervision Journal 23(2):1-10. Ichino, Andrea and Enrico Moretti. 2009. “Biological Gender Differences, Absenteeism, and the Earnings Gap.” American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, American Economic Association (1)1:183-218. 122 Jackson, D. 1998. “Breaking out of the Binary Trap: Boys’ Underachievement, Schooling, and Gender Realtions.” Chapter 5 in Failing Boys? Issues in Gender and Achievement, edited by D. Epstein, J. Elwood, V. Hey, and J. Maw. Florence, KY: Taylor & Francis Inc. Jacobson, Jennifer A. 2008. "The Gendered Processes of Hidden Curriculum and Cultural Capital within Two Teacher Preparation Programs." Ph.D dissertation, Department of Sociology, Arizona State University. Jaunch, Lawrence R., Richard N. Osborn, and Thomas N. Martin. 1980. “Structured Content Analysis of Cases: A Complementary Method for Organized Research.” Academy of Management Review 5(4): 517-525. Katsarou, Eleni. 2009. “Reading African-American Literature to Understand the World: Critical Race Praxis in Teaching Preparation.” Ethnicity and Education 12(2):253-266. Kimmel, Michael. 2000. “What About the Boys?” WEEA Digest. Newton, MA: Women’s Education Equity Act Resource Center. Koch, Janice. 2007. “A Gender Inclusive Approach to Science Education.” Pp. 205-223 in Gender in the Classroom, edited by D. Sadker and E. S. Silber. Krafchick, Jennifer L. 2007. "Teaching Social Justice: Developmental Considerations." Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Human Development And Family Studies, Colorado State University. 123 Levy, Gary D., Adrienne L. Sadovsky, and Georgene L. Troseth. 2000. "Aspects of Young Children's Perceptions of Gender-Typed Occupations." Sex Roles 42(1112):993-1006. MacMillan, Donald L., Frank M. Gresham, Maria F. Lopez, and Kathleen M. Bocian. 1996. "Comparison of Students Nominated for Prereferral Interventions by Ethnicity and Gender." The Journal of Special Education 30(2):133. Markstrom, Ann-Marie and GunillaHallden. 2008. “Children’s Strategies for Agency in Preschool.” Children & Society 23(2):112-122. Ma, Xin. 2008. “Within-School Gender Gaps in Reading, Mathematics, and Science Literacy.” Comparative Education Review 52(3): 437-460. Mulkey, Lynn M. 1988. "An Empirical Investigation of Attitudes Toward Women in Science." American Sociological Association Conference Paper. National Assessment of Educational Progress. 2007. “2007 Report Card.” Retreived April 19th 2010 (http://nationalreportscard.gov/). Oliveira, Luciana C. and Steven Z. Athanases. 2007. Graduates’ Reports of Advocating for English Language Learners.” Journal of Teacher Education 58: 202-215. Papastergiou, Marina. 2008. “Are Computer Science and Information Technology Still Masculine Fields? High School Students’ Participations and Career Choices.” Computer and Education 51(2): 594-608. 124 Ray, Susan A. 2008. “Standardized Tests as Measures of Achievement: Does the High School Assessment Program (HSAP) Measure Up?” Online Submission, Retrieved from ERIC database on April 19th 2010. Risman, Barbara J. 2004. “Gender as a Social Structure: Theory Wrestling with Activism.” Gender and Society 18(4):429-450. Rosenthal, Robert and Lenore Jacobson. 1992. Pygmalion in the Classroom: Teacher Expectation and Pupils’ Intellectual Development. Vermont: Crown House Publishing Limited. Rourke, Liam and Terry Anderson. 2004. “Validity in Quantitative Content Analysis.” Educational Technology Research and Development 52(1):5-18. Sadker, David, Myra Sadker, and Karen Zittleman 2009. Still Failing at Fairness: how Gender Bias Cheats Girls and Boys in School and What we can do about it. New York: Scribner. Sadker, Myra and David Sadker. 1995. Failing at Fairness: How our Schools Cheat Girls. New York: Touchstone. Sadker, David and Ellen S. Sibler. 2007. Gender in the Classroom: Foundations, Skills, Methods, and Strategies Across the Curriculum. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Sax, Leonard. 2005. Why Gender Matters: What Parents and Teachers Need to Know about the Emerging Science of Sex Differences. New York: Doubleday. 125 Sax, Linda. J. (1994). “Mathematical self-concept: How college reinforces the gender gap.” Research in Higher Education 35(2):141-166. Sexton, Patricia. 1969. The Feminized Male: Classrooms White Collars, and the Decline of Manliness. New York: Random House. Shepardson, Daniel and Edward Pizzini. (1992). “Gender Bias in Female Elementary Teachers’ Perceptions of the Scientific Ability of Students.” Science Education 76(2):147-153. Smith, Cary Stacy and Li-Ching Hung. 2008. “Stereotype Threat: Effects on Education.” Social Psychology of Education: An International Journal 11(3): 243-257. Smith, Earl B. 2009. “Approaches to Multicultural Education in Preservice Teacher Education: Philosophical Frameworks and Models for Teaching.” Multicultural Education 16(3):45-50. Stein, Marcy, Carol Stuen, Douglas Carnine, and Roger M. Long. 2001. “Textbook Evaluation and Adoption Practices.” Reading and Writing Quarterly 17:5-23. The U.S. National Archives and Records Administration. “Special Emphasis Observances: Women’s History Month.” The U.S. National Archives and Records Administration. Retrieved January (http://www.archives.gov/eeo/special-observances/#mar). 15th 2010 126 Tindall, Anna T. 2007. "Case Studies of Women in Academia: Challenges, Accomplishments, and Attributions to Success."Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Sociology and Social Work, Mississippi State University. Tyson, Harriet and Arthur Woodward. 1989. “Why Students aren’t Learning very much from Textbooks.” Education Leadership 47(3):14-17. U.S. Department of Labor, Bureau of Labor Statistics, Employment and Earnings. 2006. “Median Weekly Earnings of full-time Wage and Salary Workers by Detailed Occupation and Sex.” Table 39. Van Houtte, Mieke. 2004. “Why Boys Achieve less at School than Girls; The Differences Between Boys’ and Girls’ Academic Culture.” Educational Studies 30(2):159173. Warmoth, Arthur. 1998. “Education and the Collaborative Construction of Social Reality.” Working Draft, Department of Psychology, Sonoma State University. Women on Words and Images. 1975. Dick and Jane as Victims: Sex Stereotyping in Children’s Readers. New Jersey: Women on Words and Images. Xu, Yonghong Jade. 2008. “Gender Disparity in STEM Disciplines: A Study of Faculty Attrition and Turnover Intentions.” Research in Higher Education 49(7): 607624. Zittleman, Karen. 2007. “Teachers, Students, and Title IX: A Promise for Fairness.” Pp. 73-107 in Gender in the Classroom, edited by D. Sadker and E. S. Silber.