STUDENT DIFFERENCES IN ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT: A CLOSER LOOK AT PARENTAL RULE SETTING AND FAMILY STRUCTURE A Thesis Presented to the faculty of the Department of Sociology California State University Sacramento Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in Sociology by Tyler Michael Elston FALL 2013 STUDENT DIFFERENCES IN ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT: A CLOSER LOOK AT PARENTAL RULE SETTING AND FAMILTY STRUCTURE A Thesis by Tyler Michael Elston Approved by: _______________________________ Committee Chair Charles Varano _______________________________ Second Reader Jackie Carrigan _____________________ Date ii Student: Tyler Michael Elston I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. ___________________________, Graduate Coordinator Amy Liu Department of Sociology iii _____________________ Date Abstract of STUDENT DIFFERENCES IN ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT: A CLOSER LOOK AT PARENTAL RULE SETTING AND FAMILTY STRUCTURE By Tyler Michael Elston The purpose of this study is to investigate the relationship the family has with a child’s academic achievement. Inequality extant in school provides obstacles that cannot be explained only by economic or class issues and requires a focus on inequality that persists through cultural and parenting practices. Differences that arise in academic achievement call into question the types of skills, values, and knowledge that are validated in school. This exposes the structural disadvantage many children experience when going to school. I find that family type and parental involvement via rule structuring has significant effects on academic achievement. Although patterns do not persist in all family types, communication and rule setting is significant to a child’s academic success. Research shows that two parent families with extensive communication and rule setting provide children with a stable platform for success. I argue that the expectations and involvement parents provide have significant mediating factors on a child’s academic success. iv Recommendations include a greater awareness made to parents of their importance and the need for support of school in the home, as well as, the need to place a greater value on working-class and minority culture. _______________________, Committee Chair Charles Varano _______________________ Date v TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapter Page 1. STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM ............................................................................ 1 2. LITERATURE REVIEW ............................................................................................. 7 Functional Paradigm ................................................................................... 7 Conflict Paradigm ..................................................................................... 11 James Coleman ......................................................................................... 14 Linguistics and Reproduction in the Family ............................................. 16 The Family and Parental Involvement ...................................................... 21 Parental Expectations ................................................................................ 26 Family Structure........................................................................................ 27 Summary ................................................................................................... 31 3. METHODS ................................................................................................................. 33 Data and Sample ....................................................................................... 33 Variables ................................................................................................... 33 Control Variables ...................................................................................... 36 Summary ................................................................................................... 37 4. FINDINGS .................................................................................................................. 40 Descriptive Analyses ................................................................................ 41 Biological Mother and Father ................................................................... 42 Biological Mother-other-father ................................................................. 44 vi Biological Father-other-mother ................................................................ 45 Biological Mother ..................................................................................... 45 Biological Father ....................................................................................... 46 Adoptive Parents ....................................................................................... 47 Guardians .................................................................................................. 47 Summary ................................................................................................... 47 5. CONCLUSION ........................................................................................................... 49 Two Biological Parents Findings .............................................................. 50 One Biological-Other Parent Findings...................................................... 52 Single Biological Parent Findings ............................................................ 54 Appendix ..................................................................................................................... 58 References ................................................................................................................... 67 vii 1 Chapter 1 STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM The United States is a country viewed around the world for providing opportunities to people regardless of their social background. Many people spend long periods of their life in school because of how critical it is to success in the future. The level of success that can be accomplished through education is a driving force in the lives of most individuals. Long term success in terms of obtaining an education has generated an insatiable demand for school (Anderson 1961). With the growth of educational institutions nationwide the opportunity to attend school has increased. However, widespread education has not equally advantaged all parts of the population (Bowles and Gintis 1976; MacLeod 2009; Entwisle and Alexander 1996; Kozol 2005). There are difficulties attending and being successful in school for groups historically disadvantaged based upon existing social inequalities. Studies showing educational inequalities based upon social origin are found in all Western countries, although the strength of this association varies and has declined over time (Ballarino et al. 2009; Breen et al. 2009; Esping-Anderson 2004; Goldthorpe 1996). Although there are a number of options for success in the U.S., obtaining access to education is highly reflective of an individual’s class and race (Gould 1996). Inequalities that exist in the larger society have consequences for children across the country and can be seen in longitudinal research where black-white test scores have narrowed over the past 30 years, but at the current rate of change would take an additional 50 years or more to close the gap (Orr 2003; Hedges and Nowell 1998). Class 2 is a major factor in discussing education because it limits access to various forms of resources. An example of restricted access to resources can be seen in the amount of wealth various ethnic groups possess. For example, Merida (1995) found middle-class blacks have only 15 percent of the wealth held by middle-class whites which has significant effects on social class (Orr 2003). Resources vary between tangible forms such as financial support, school infrastructure and technology, and less tangible forms such as norms, encouragement, and information gained from relationships and social networks (Oseguera et al. 2011). The relationship between family structure and academic achievement has consequences for the racial gap, especially due to black students being nearly twice as likely as white students to live in non-traditional households (Roscigno 1995, 1998). Family background is important to educational opportunities because of its influence on educational access to schools. Family type has implications for the type and quality of schools a student attends (Bowles and Levin 1968; Coleman et al. 1996; Roscigno 1998), the academic track a student is placed on (Dauber, Alexander, and Entwisle 1996; Gamoron and Berends 1987; Oakes 1985), and the expectations a teacher places on a student (Alexander et al. 1987; Rist 1970). The finding that children of different racial groups and socioeconomic backgrounds begin their formal educational careers with disparities in skills on standardized tests (Lee and Burkham 2002) and that these inequalities persist through primary and secondary school (Phillips, Crouse, and Ralph 1998) is well established (Cheadle 2008). Although parenting practices play an important role in the development 3 of differences in early childhood achievement (Guo 1998; J. R. Smith, Brooksgun, and Klebarov 1997), less is known about the role that families play in disparities in achievement once children enter elementary school (Cheadle 2008). Reports find disadvantaged children such as those whose parents are less educated, lower class, and/or poor can potentially benefit more from pre-school attendance than their advantaged counterparts (Manguson et al. 2004). On the other hand, arguments have been made that differences in academic achievement vary due to IQ (Hernstein 1973; Jensen 1973) while others repeatedly refute it (Bowles and Gintis 1976, 2000, 2002; Bowles et al. 2001). Obviously, the family plays an integral role in a child’s educational development as it provides experiences and socialization intended to prepare them to interact with others and learn new things. Those with supportive families often have increased chances for success and are situated to achieve goals out of reach for those whose parents are too busy or unable to help. A parent’s inability to help their child with education varies between difficulties such as long work hours, lack of educational resources, and low levels of education. Obstacles to a cohesive relationship between school and the home lie in a parent’s education, occupation, and ability to create an academic environment reflective of school standards. It is because of the obstacles impeding education that my research is focused on the family. It is important to look at and examine deeper the ways parent’s direct effort varies based upon available resources and experiences. A concept that will be examined in this study is concerted cultivation which is the conscious attempt by parent(s) to customize their children’s lives to better prepare them for school through 4 activities and experiences (Lareau 1989). A central focus will involve the various levels of concerted cultivation and the different outcomes it has on academic achievement. The development of large school systems has not done away with inequality and continues to be affected by social disparities which reduce access to educational opportunities. This lack of equal access works against under-represented groups such as women, non-whites, and the poor. Because there is a lack of representation and equal access to opportunities for all groups it becomes important to understand the effect of family. There is not a singular entity which prepares a child to be successful later in life, but a multitude which a child relies upon to deal with and understand experiences over time. For those able to communicate with others the transition into adulthood is less difficult. This ability is critical as children get older and change teachers, schools, and friends. The family is a place of practice for these skills where children learn the necessary tools to be successful in school. Addressing the nation’s inequalities will support educational institutions that are more equitable and receptive of diverse backgrounds. The current study looks at variance in academic achievement based upon parental involvement via rule setting and family structure and asks the question: How does a child’s level of parental involvement via rule setting and family structure affect their academic achievement? Parental involvement’s main points include: setting rules related to completing homework, watching television and obeying curfew; restricting privileges for bad behavior, and interaction between parent and child about the school day and plans for the future. For family structure there are various family types examined: single-parent 5 mother or father and two-parent family types, as well as guardian(s) and adoptive parent(s). The data set I will be using is the Kindergarten Class 1998-99 ECLS-K, a longitudinal study collecting data in waves beginning with Kindergarten and returning during the first, third, fifth, and finally the eighth grade year. I focus on the final wave and will use it to test two hypotheses. The first hypothesis examines the effect family type or types of parents in the home have on a child’s academic achievement. The second hypothesis examines parental involvement through rule setting related to completing homework, watching television and obeying curfew, restricting privileges for bad behavior, and interaction between parent and child about their day at school and future plans as contributing to a child’s academic success. The sociological significance of this research lies in the area of improving society’s understanding of inequality in children’s education. This study relies heavily upon the previous work done by those asking a similar question about academic achievement: why do some succeed while others do not? Education is viewed as the great equalizer (Hurn 1993) specifically because it provides opportunities for success which have direct implications for a happy life. The inequality that persists in America calls into question the revolutionary impetus of schooling. My study focuses on the family and its relation with school in working toward a child’s future. It is important to continue challenging and questioning the status quo as patterns continue to disadvantage specific groups such as African Americans, Asian Americans, and Latina(o)s. Questions still exist about fixing the current system and working toward greater equality of opportunity and 6 conditions. This is the reason it is important to bring together the many works of authors with multiple perspectives and different biases and interests. This creates the opportunity to use their perspectives to better shape our understanding of children’s education and the family. The intention of this study is to add more peer-reviewed data into a field focusing its attention on improving the lives of young people unable to enact the kinds of changes necessary to improve their future. I look at research from several perspectives in order to gain better understanding of how to improve the lives of children in school and make the family a cohesive, helpful, and educationally significant space. 7 Chapter 2 LITERATURE REVIEW A large number of low-income children attend schools in the United States but do not experience the same opportunities for success as do their high-income counterparts. Part of the reduced success among minorities is related to the frequency of parental rule setting and family type as well as material resources available in the home. School provides opportunities for children to develop skills they are expected to have been previously exposed to such as reading, working independently, and communicating with adults. Inequality persists and differential academic achievement between whites and minorities continue to disadvantage low-income students. Theoretical explanations of inequality are constructed through frameworks that have overlooked the role family plays. The functional framework begins the discussion of inequality and illustrates my study’s focus on educational inequality reproduced through social structures. Explanations for inequality in school can be developed through understanding the way family intersects with schooling. In the next section I consider how the functional paradigm questions the role family plays in preparing children for school. The focus will be on how society and the family are intended to work together for the betterment of the child while ignoring the effects of cultural practices and structural inequality. Functional Paradigm The functional paradigm defines school as a social institution providing three primary functions for society. First, school allows for efficient and rational sorting of 8 talented peoples for the most important status positions. Schools create a meritocratic society valuing individual ability over family background. Second, schools teach cognitive skills and norms that are necessary for success in an increasingly knowledgebased economy. Transmission of knowledge is the key to success in school where grades and standardized tests are used to gauge academic achievement. The third and final aspect views society as democratic and moving toward humane goals such as social justice, a fulfilling life for all, and acceptance of diversity. This paradigm finds educational sorting to be functional for society, but ignores the development and continuance of inequality (Hurn 1993). Functionalism addresses education and the family in an attempt to understand the role each plays in constructing society. The role of family in socializing and educating children has changed over time as industrialization drastically shifted the child away from the home and into the school. This is not viewed in a negative light by structural functionalists who understand that family has the new role of preparing children for the experience they will encounter once they enter school. Due to this, failures within the school are not attributed to poor teachers or administrators but rather a lack of preparation by the parents. Randall Collins engages the discussion of credentials and the workforce at the point where schools transmit cognitive abilities and skills to individuals (Collins 1971; Jensen 1973). Collins argues that a technical-functional perspective views the United States as an ever-changing industrial society where formal education provides training for highly skilled jobs and educational requirements constantly rise leading to large 9 proportions of the population spending longer periods in school. His analysis questions the link between increased technological complexities on the job and accessibility to those jobs which have high status and require educational degrees which work as cultural currency. Employers value cultural currency for its supposed indication of a good worker and the belief that those with more cultural currency will do a better job while also increasing the job’s status. The value placed on cultural currency creates pressure to obtain an education, and there has been an inflated worth attached to degrees pushing levels of education up higher. Collins argues that society is becoming largely dependent on credentialed individuals who are over qualified for many jobs. Many people finishing secondary school are able to directly enter the job market filling the open positions. Collins explains how educational expansion has out-paced the number of highly skilled and professional jobs, a conclusion which largely refutes the technical-functional theory. Jobs today are not sufficiently more complicated or require significantly more technical skills. There is little evidence that proves those with degrees are more productive than those who are trained on the job. Collins finds schools are “ineffective in producing cognitive skills and they have more to do with teaching conventional ideologies and traits which reproduce the status quo” (Collins 1971:1014). Largely missing from Collins’ analysis is the role of family in society. He highlights the ways in which schools fail to transmit cognitive abilities and skills, focusing instead on dominant traits and values, but neglects to include the moderating effect of family. Meritocracy is a concept Collins refutes with evidence from the 10 workforce and higher-education institutions but he misses where family comes in. Family plays the primary role of socializing and is the first group the child experiences crucial life events with. One of the crucial factors in obtaining employment to Collins is getting trained on the job. The ability to interact with new people and take orders is a technique developed over the course of the early years. Another aspect of functionalist models of education is Becker’s (1964) notion of human capital. Human capital theory views education as an investment in an individual that increases their knowledge or expertise in the form of experiences, language, and readiness which yield higher earnings in the future (Dore 1976). This perspective views the costs of investment as educational expenses but also the income that is forgone to be able to attend school. The value attached to human capital is that investment into education increases overall production and economic growth (Hurn 1993). While this investment is developed in the school it is initially established by the family. As can be seen so far from structural functionalist theories, structure is largely accepted as permanent and the family is viewed as having a small role to play. There are some difficulties with Becker’s human capital theory specifically in terms of evaluating the role of the family. The role of the family is largely unaccounted for because Becker’s focus is on future success rather than questioning the ways human capital is developed in the family. In a later section on James Coleman the role the family plays in creating human capital will be more fully developed. Finally, status attainment theory explains student differences in school achievement using social backgrounds and the length of time students remain in school 11 and college. Status attainment research finds significant differences in eventual educational attainment based upon different social backgrounds with those students having higher-status being more likely to enter college. There are two findings from status attainment research in relation to educational outcomes based upon social background that are significant. First, a child’s ability and competence in school generates success that is informed by social capital rather than race. Second, students from high-status families frequently have higher aspirations for educational achievement. The opposite can be said for low-status students who tend to have lower aspirations. In a move toward more acknowledgement of the role family plays, status attainment theory only hints at the need for family to have social capital and encourage children’s aspirations. Social capital, when applied successfully, develops social relations which aid parent’s encouragement of children and works to inform them of other children’s development. This gives the parent a better understanding of student progress and a sense of when help is needed and if any further educational decisions need to be made. Conflict Paradigm Conflict theorists find little evidence to support the meritocratic framework put forth by functional theorists. Instead they argue it is the class-related values and attitudes that schools transmit to their students leaving social, human, and cultural capital to be developed by the parents in home. To better understand the problems experienced at school, conflict theorists assert that schools appeal to the demands of elites through the transmission of docility and compliance instead of cognitive skills (Bowles and Gintis 1976). In what follows, various conflict theorists will highlight the inaccuracies with the 12 functional paradigm and assert new ways for considering educational inequality and social reproduction. Beginning with the notion of meritocracy, Bowles and Gintis (1976) illustrate the ways that schooling is not based upon personal skills, but tracking of individuals into the necessary social location for capitalist economies. Bowles and Gintis stated that changes in the structure of education are associated historically with changes in the social organization of economic production. These changes have seen socialization by the family reduced with an increased reliance on school to prepare children for the workforce. They argue that there is a correspondence between the social relations of education and the social relations of production. The education system mimicked the relationship between the work process and product that children will later encounter in the workplace. Moreover, the forms of consciousness encouraged and rewarded in the education system are those that would best prepare children to accept the situation they are subsequently subjected to work in. Through their historical account of the education system Bowles and Gintis show how children of different social classes are prepared for their respective places in the production process. This preparation for the types of activities experienced at work is not available in the home, and when considering the correspondence theory school has become the only institution which can prepare a child for future employment. Moving into the home, the focus of Pierre Bourdieu’s (1977) work on social inequality informed Jay MacLeod’s (2009) study which sought to explain the failings of 13 low-income children in school due to difficulties faced in the family. MacLeod used his knowledge of intersections between the family and school to develop a deeper understanding of the ways early-life influences and experiences shape people’s life aspirations and opportunities. Difficulties encountered by youth in MacLeod’s work center on habitus and its reproduction of the status quo. The concept of habitus was developed by Pierre Bourdieu and refers to the particular environment which produces a system of expectations and dispositions in an individual. This is influenced by the family’s structure and the effects familial interactions have on individuals. Perceptions, tastes, behavior, etc. are closely linked to the experience and interactions occurring in the home. Because the theory closely examines the home environment, Bourdieu’s theory allows for a better understanding of the role of family and its effect on academic achievement. Whereas Bowles and Gintis used over-deterministic theories to explain the capitalistic effect on the school system through tracking, Jay MacLeod’s (2009) book Ain’t No Makin’ It uses habitus to establish the family as a factor in the difficulties related to academic achievement. MacLeod went into the homes of two groups of boys documenting their everyday actions and behaviors trying to understand the effects of their family life on their education. The Hangers are exposed to familial disappointments in the form of occupational and educational failures which negatively affect their aspirations. The Brothers on the other hand experienced support from their parents and were actively motivated through parental expectations, yet they too struggled in school given their devalued cultural capital. 14 The Hangers aspirations were frequently “leveled” by others’ failures, including the family, and directly affected the motivation of the youth in the classroom. In effect, their habitus dictated their educational opportunities through learned patterns and experiences in the family. Because the family has this effect we see different perspectives and motivations between the Brothers and Hangers. MacLeod finds failures in the home, and difficulties in school, make for high drop-out rates and low levels of efficacy. He displays the ways rejection at school is weakly correlated to the curriculum and highly correlative with the home environment and experiences with failure and success in the family. MacLeod demonstrated the way in which the place one lives and their experiences have a direct impact on future aspirations. James Coleman James Coleman (1988) argues that social capital is necessary for success in school and the work place. Social capital is a social resource that garners support and success through awareness of processes going on at school such as getting help on a difficult subject, knowing when and how to apply to colleges, or getting information on where to find a job after school. Coleman produced a study designed to assess high dropout rates utilizing the concept of social capital. He details three forms of social capital: obligations and expectations, information channels, and social norms. Sanctions and closure work as behavior modifiers by society to reproduce the status quo. Obligations and expectations are a form of social capital which depends upon two factors: “trustworthiness of the social environment (meaning obligations will be repaid) and the actual extent of 15 obligations held” (Coleman 1988: S102). Information channels are the pathways in which individuals exert agency in affecting their social location. Coleman illustrated this relationship in a general sense: A does something for B and trusts B to reciprocate in the future, this establishes an expectation in A and an obligation on the part of B. This obligation can be conceived as a credit slip held by A for performance by B. If A holds a large number of these credit slips, for a number of persons with whom A has relations, then the analogy to financial capital is direct. These credit slips constitute a large body of credit that A can call in if necessary-unless, of course, the placement of trust has been unwise, and these are bad debts that will not be repaid. (Coleman 1988:S102) These three forms of social capital empower individuals to be successful in school due to increased opportunities to acquire resources. Crucial to these types of social capital is their transmutable form, and more specifically their usefulness to multiple other social and economic situations. Social capital permits “taking resources and combining them with other resources such as income, education, or occupation to produce different system-level behavior or, in other cases, different outcomes for individuals” (Coleman 1988:S101). Coleman found that no matter the source, “individuals in social structures with high levels of obligations outstanding at any time have more social capital on which they can draw” (Coleman 1988:S103). Information channels are important to social capital because of the potential for information that is part of social relations. Acquiring information costs something, be it time or services. Maintaining relationships for other purposes may also serve as a tool for acquiring information. Coleman noted the importance and transmutable form of social capital with intergenerational closure or the relationship between parent and child A with parent and child B. Communication and interaction in relation to their children would 16 permit an opportunity for the parents A and B to discuss other topics and potentially inform one another about areas unrelated to children’s education. Two parents who interact on the basis that their children know each other are able to discuss types of acceptable activities, establish standards, and discuss sanctions for their children. Each parent reinforces the other in sanctioning her child’s actions and furthermore each parent constitutes a monitor for both children. The existence of “intergenerational closure provides a quantity of social capital available to each parent in raising their children, not only in matters related to school but in other matters as well” (S107). The role family plays in advantaging children in school rests on levels of social capital held by the parents. As Coleman indicated social, human, and cultural capital are significantly related to success in school because information channels, skills, and experiences prepare children for various situations they will face. Different forms of capital can garner more resources, generate help from others, and develop knowledge that will advantage children in school. The development of these forms of capital are significantly related to the ability of children to adapt to and succeed in school. Linguistics and Reproduction in the Family Because the family affects children’s lived experiences, those from the lower and upper class begin to exhibit distinct speech codes and cognitive functioning based upon the common-sense understandings they hold founded in linguistics. Basil Bernstein (1977) looks at social reproduction through sociolinguistic codes which “point to the social structuring of meanings and to their diverse but related contextual linguistic realizations” (Bernstein 1977:474). He attempts to investigate how 17 symbolic systems are realizations and regulators of the structure of social relationships. There are certain contextual constraints upon speech due to a child's exposure to different types of socialization which transform the biological into a specific cultural being. This process of socialization which depends on speech, activates a specific cognitive and affective awareness in the child and assigns a specific form and content (Bernstein 1977). This develops in the child a common-sense understanding that things are inevitable and limits other options or choices. The family is the most fundamental agent of socialization and “it is through the interaction of the family, school, and work that the orderings of society are made manifest” (Bernstein 1977:477). With this in mind Bernstein asks the question: what sociological factors affect linguistic performances within the family that are critical to the process of socialization and educational success? Social class is a significant mediator of linguistic patterns and from its influence on the family, work, and school it penetrates the structure of life experiences within the family. The family reinforces and practices linguistic patterns which children utilize in school. If the family practices linguistic patterns significantly different from those in school problems will arise due to different ways of understanding. There are two types of meanings which speech codes are based on: universalistic and particularistic. Universalistic meanings are those in which principles and operations are made linguistically explicit. Particularistic orders of meaning are meanings in which principles and operations are relatively linguistically implicit. When orders of meaning are universalistic, then the meanings are less tied to a given context giving individuals access to change their experiences. For particularistic meanings a higher level of context-bound 18 meaning is tied into local relationships and to a local social structure. This renders meaning as dependent upon context and it may be restricted to those who share a similar contextual history. The argument holds that forms of socialization orient the child towards speech codes which control access to meanings. Therefore “elaborated codes orient their users towards universalistic meanings, whereas restricted codes orient, sensitize, their users to particularistic meanings” (Bernstein 1977:477). Hess et al. (1965) use Bernstein's (1973) description of two types of speech codes: restricted and elaborate. By its nature, restricted speech codes require limited speech for a child to understand the ideas being put forth relying more on context. Elaborate speech codes require extra cognitive work to figure out what a parent says to a child because the parent uses concepts to clearly explain a situation and does not rely solely on context. The limited experience for children in families reproducing restricted speech codes impedes the development of different linguistic styles and limits their linguistic abilities. During their research Hess et al. (1965) conducted a study testing the motherchild interaction. The objective of this study was to discover how teaching styles of the mothers induce and shape learning styles and information processing strategies in the children. They examined linguistic patterns to assess the cognitive processes going on and the way in which a task could be more or less difficult based on the speech code utilized. They found that elaborate speech codes, or explanatory and helpful verbal and non-verbal gestures, aided the child in finishing the task. When the task was administered to subjects using restricted speech codes the mother and child struggled to finish the task. The significant variance between classes was the parents’ use of planning and the verbal 19 and cognitive environments which they presented (Hess et al. 1965). Mothers utilizing a restricted code socialized their children into passive learners while mothers implementing an elaborate speech code socialized their children into active learners making tasks like the one in the study or in school achievable. The family is the place where children practice their speech codes and develop an understanding of object and person relationships. This is significant because later when they are in school and attempting to communicate with teachers their understanding of object and person relationships will affect how well they can re-tell a story, explain a problem they have, or seek help when it is needed. Cognitive functioning is reliant upon linguistic patterns and establishes thought patterns which can inhibit communication (Hess et al. 1965). It is in the family that children develop a set of inter-related contexts which have implications to the orderings of a culture or subculture made through substantive forms of its linguistic realizations. To better situate the application of socialization through speech codes it is important to look at the family communication structures and the ways they can be cognitively limiting (Hess et al. 1965). The linguistic understandings children hold are based on their interactions within the family and it is there that they establish the patterns used in understanding and communicating language. In response to the degradation of education during the 20th century the federal government created the Office of Economic Opportunity to institute laws to help the disadvantaged. They used this approach because quality education and reaching children when they are young are crucial to achieving equitable academic success. Historically a 20 lack of motivation in the family has been linked to cultural deprivation and the poor performance of children with this behavior leading to social, educational, and economic poverty. This is especially evident in the family where “cultural deprivation's effects are centered on a lack of cognitive meaning in the mother-child communication system” (Hess et al. 1965:221). Crucial to the mother-child communication system is the family structure which inhibits or enables different language patterns shaping thought and cognitive styles of problem solving. With this foundation of speech codes established, another study which illustrates difficulties for students in school based on linguistic patterns learned from the family is Shirley Brice Heath's conducted in 1983. Heath engages a problem extant in most classrooms in America: teachers and students coming from different cultural backgrounds struggling to communicate. She focuses on why some children did not or could not respond as did other children by analyzing the role of questioning in language and socialization in the family. In the classroom students from Trackton, a lower income neighborhood, would not engage with teachers through questions and responses. To better understand this problem, Heath interacted with Trackton families to investigate the different assumptions and norms of linguistic patterns impeding the student/teacher interaction. The significant difference between teachers and Trackton students was that in the family children were not viewed as conversational partners until they were seen as realistic sources of information and competent partners in talk (Heath 1983). 21 Because the students from Trackton were inexperienced with the teacher’s linguistic patterns, “school questions were unfamiliar in their frequency, purposes and types, and in the domains of content knowledge and skills display they assumed on the part of students” (Heath 1983:123). It is clear that the problems faced by the Trackton students came from restricted speech codes being reproduced by the family and the teacher’s inability to successfully communicate with students. The Family and Parental Involvement Research in the area of academic achievement has outlined the ways structure reinforces inequality (Collins 1971) and the types of capital necessary to be successful in school (Coleman 1988). The following section will illustrate the way parental involvement through rule setting and family structure has direct implications for a child’s academic achievement. There are many ways to define parental involvement in a child’s education. Parental home-based involvement has been studied by Annette Lareau (1989) through observing parent-child interactions and contact with the child’s school. What is important to parental involvement is the transmission of specific knowledge and skills in developing a child’s cultural capital. When applied with social capital, cultural capital makes parental involvement a positive academic experience overall. There is extensive literature detailing the ways in which a child’s academic achievement is positively correlated with increased parental involvement when controlling for income and social background and I highlight some of the central research below. 22 Social ties and parental networks are extensively detailed by Lareau in her ethnography of primary school children of working-class and middle-class backgrounds. Lareau interviews and observes parents, teachers, and administrators to understand differences in academic achievement. She focuses on parental involvement and family structure, but she also investigates the relationship with teachers. Her use of the concept cultural capital is grounded in social capital, obligations and expectations, information channels and social norms, a concept used by Coleman (1988) and created by Bourdieu (1977). Cultural capital is defined by Lareau as cultural resources which influence social selection. This holds that possessing key cultural resources provides social advantages which are important to academic success (Lareau 1989). Lareau uses this term to discuss the differences within families and the consequences of certain parenting practices. Middle-class children are advantaged when their parents reproduce cultural capital because it is valued in school. Working-class children without these resources face difficulties adapting to new experiences where their culture is not present. Although cultural capital is important it alone does not advantage children. The resources must be activated and simply possessing them does not advantage children. This is part of the reason cultural capital when activated is often accompanied by social and human capital, all three working to help children succeed. This advantage generates gaps in achievement with many working-class children struggling in school because their knowledge and experiences are limited. The level of involvement the family has in a child’s education frequently determines the level of success and expectations they experience. Middle-class parents 23 develop a “two person single career” where they would “intervene in the child’s school experience and compensating at home for omissions in the school’s program” (Lareau 1989:82). Teachers found the parents of middle-class children to be open to customizing their child’s school life. She found considerably different patterns with working classclass parents who turned over responsibility for education to the school deferring to the notion of professional expertise. This lack of parental involvement and customization of a child’s daily life carried into the family leaving the child unprepared and lacking practice in school valued skills, knowledge, and experience. Because of the working-class parents reduced level of involvement their children experienced “single educational careers” where their success was based upon their individual ability, diligence, and overall performance in the classroom (Lareau 1989). Lareau’s findings point to the significance of the family and the role parental involvement has on academic success. When the family defers all responsibility to teachers and there are no school expectations reinforced in the home through an educational atmosphere children struggle academically. Removing the family from a child’s academic path leaves them uninformed and disadvantaged significantly in relation to the families promoting a two-person single career. She emphasized the importance of customizing a child’s education so as to help them in the ways they need to achieve the highest level of academic success. Lareau conducted a similar ethnography in 2003 that analyzed the role of the family in a child’s academic success, with a focus on the influences of different parenting practices. She wanted to more fully understand the cultural practices within the family 24 and the intersections with school. Investigating the relationships in the family Lareau determined there were two forms of parenting at play in the home: concerted cultivation and the accomplishment of natural growth. Concerted cultivation is the direct attempt by parent(s) to create a customized daily life for their child. The intention is to provide opportunities to gain experience interacting with other children as well as adults generating various forms of capital. The accomplishment of natural growth focuses on feeding, clothing, and housing the child and allowing more autonomy with less direct attempt to keep them busy with activities. Both parenting practices attempt to support the child and create discipline, but only concerted cultivation reinforces in the child the standards expected in school, such as rules for homework, limiting how late watch television, or setting curfews. Concerted cultivation is an important socializing factor due to its development of greater verbal ability, comfort with authority figures, and familiarity with abstract concepts and discipline, all of which are traits important to academic achievement (Lareau 2003). When a family is more concerned with providing the basics for their child the obstacles limiting their ability to provide support and be involved in the child’s education become apparent. Families practicing the accomplishment of natural growth are not able to provide a discipline in terms of education because there is often a deprivation of resources and fluctuations in family structure. Past research documenting the significance rule setting for homework, television, and eating meals together has on academic achievement may explain some of the difficulties faced by children living in homes where accomplishment of natural growth 25 exists. It has negative effects on their schooling because children need guidance and assistance from adults, but many parents lack significant experience with education making involvement more difficult to enact. The significant difference between the types of discipline socialized into children is that families practicing concerted cultivation are developing in their child a sense of self-actualization while the accomplishment of natural growth promotes deference to authority (Hess et al. 1965). It is the role of the family to support and aid the child, but also to recognize when outside help is needed. This is where social, human, and cultural capital becomes significantly related to academic achievement due to the times when families need support (Lareau 2003). The awareness and ability of the family to act are significantly related to success in school because it is the successful activation of resources that promotes academic achievement. Other researchers have re-tested the quantitative aspect of Lareau’s work and they support her theory that concerted cultivation has multiple dimensions of parenting practices which support my argument that concerted cultivation has various forms (Cheadle and Amato 2010). When there are fewer parents in the home there is a reduced rate of concerted cultivation as is the case with step-parents and a high number of siblings or children in the home. Consistent with Lareau’s work is the need for parents to possess and develop social capital that enhances academic achievement. McNeal Jr. (1999) similarly identified the advantage parents experience when they communicate and interact with parents of other children gaining them access to information about their child’s school. This also creates potential for help if needed in the future as Coleman established in his development of obligations and expectations. 26 McNeal Jr. details three aspects of parental involvement that build one’s social capital: having dyadic relationships with other parents, positive relationship between parent and child, and income. The support from these resources allows for positive parental involvement in the child’s education improving academic achievement. McNeal Jr. found higher levels of academic success among children with higher levels of SES regardless of parental involvement. Causes for this may lie in types of after-school childcare they are provided with or the level of familial changes in the home. Reduced parental involvement can be moderated by aunts, uncles, or cousins who frequently interact with and or read to children. This moderating effect can advantage working-class children and improve their chances for high academic achievement. The role family plays in providing interactions, teaching opportunities, and educational experiences have a significant effect on academic achievement. Parental Expectations Parental expectations can be positive for a child and the arenas in which children operate to feel safe are crucial to academic success. One of the main ways to create such an environment is through eating meals together as a family and talking about school and future aspirations. The regularity of shared meals creates a routine allowing parents to accurately “impart important values to children” (Roche and Ghazarian 2011: 875). This process plays a part in developing the competence of youth, and parents report decreased levels of externalizing negative behavior. It is important to note that transition and change in a child’s life can have negative effects on their schooling. Roche and Ghazarian develop their idea of arenas of comfort; situated relationships that help protect or ease the 27 struggle of a youth in other arenas of their life such as school. The family provides opportunities for eating meals together which is a predictable beginning and end to the day and no matter the interactions beyond those meals, a child can count on their parent(s) to sit down with them and give them attention. When controlling for SES or parent occupation there is a reduced rate of eating meals together amongst students who regularly underperform. Part of the difficulty in eating meals together lies in the time available for the parent(s) to both prepare and be available to eat with youth, and increased nonstandard work schedules impede this process (Han and Fox 2011). The family suffers due to difficult work hours and the amount of interacting with the child is directly affected. Nonstandard work hours when combined with stress at work create a problematic environment for communication where children are aware of difficulties in the family based upon parent-child interactions. The Ecological System perspective improves our understanding of difficulties experienced by children through focusing on stress which impacts a child’s surrounding environment and affect their well-being (Bronfenbrenner and Morris 1998). In a supportive home environment parental expectations of homework and eating meals together frequently create an environment similar to the arena of comfort explained by Roche and Ghazarian (2011). Family Structure There are a number of defining events that take place in the home on a regular basis which aid in a child’s academic success. Eating meals together, talking about the future and their day at school are just a few examples of the ways in which parents are 28 able to create a living situation which stimulates growth and learning. The basic components of family structure involve the types of parents in the home the child interacts with on a day-to-day basis. For the child to receive attention and be successful in school there needs to be consistency and high levels of interaction. When mothers and fathers constantly move in and out of the home, complications arise in relation to math and reading, as well as overall academic achievement. Fluctuations in parent numbers and or living situations cause disturbances in a child’s education. Single-parent families frequently involve mothers as head of household but Lee, Kushner, and Cho (2007) studied the effects of single-parent households on male and female children when headed by either mother or father. Social learning theory posits gendered behavior transmits from parent to child through same-gender parents (Rossi 1984). Although other research has led to various conclusions on academic performance in same-gender households, Lee, Kushner, and Cho (2007) found no significant difference between academic achievements in mixed-gender single-parent households. Lee and Kushner (2008) followed up on their research and confirmed their previous findings that gender does not matter for child or parent; rather the key to academic success is involvement. A difficulty to same-gender households is the friend relationship that potentially develops. The authority held by the parent is reduced through over identification with same gender parent or a lack of engagement with cross-gender parent (Lee, Kishner, and Cho 2007: 155). Further research would need to investigate the positives of the friend relationship, but under these circumstances the parent-child 29 relationship is fundamentally more important to academic success because of the need to enact expectations and restrictions when necessary to spur development. Entwisle and Alexander (1996) found reason to suggest family structure is relevant to academic achievement as long as the child has an adult available to regulate television and check completion of homework. When a parent and their child interact together the parent is practicing concerted cultivation which transmits cultural capital and prepares the child for school. Difficulties arise in homes where long work hours interrupt the interactions in the family. The family is unable to provide youth with the important skills and cultural capital necessary for participation in school. Two-parent homes are not required to achieve success if there is adequate time spent teaching and interacting with the child whether it is with the mother, father, or a relative. Part of the reason Entwisle and Alexander found African American children in single-mother families performing better in reading than their counterparts in mother only or mother-father families is related to other adult family members around the home who read to and interact with them. Entwisle and Alexander highlight the importance of adult-child interactions and the elevated importance of parental involvement over socioeconomic status. In families with parents at home but low levels of involvement and support, children underachieve similarly to single-parent families starved of resources. Singleparent families possess fewer educational materials at home and cannot afford activities that stimulate children cognitively like museums and plays. Musick and Meier (2009) find the leading reasons for children’s academic success are resources and parental expectations. Musick and Meier find single-mothers expect less of their children on their 30 first marks in math than two-parent families where involvement is increased due to an additional parent in the home. Expectations by parents motivate children and qualify as involvement in a child’s education. Pong, Dronkers, and Hampden-Thompson (2003) questioned the importance of family structure in relation to social safety nets, and suspect family structure is relevant with respect to the types of socialization the child experiences. As noted above, access to adults is crucial to children in single-parent families where a mother’s or father’s time with their children is often constricted by work. Entwisle and Alexander (1996) looked at the lack of access to human capital due to restricted time spent interacting with mothers and found it to negatively affect a child’s academic achievement. Children in families with mothers who have never been married are more at risk of suffering academically than those in divorced families. Research in the area of father-headed households experiencing similar circumstances would develop the concept further. Entiwisle and Alexander (1995) found summer to be a major deficit in educational growth for students not attending summer school, but they found families with higher SES were able to compensate for the lack of cognitive stimulation from school through engaging with their child academically and creating an academic environment such as required reading, camps, and other activities generating cognitive stimulation. When educational levels of parents are equitable between working and middle-class parents, differences between children’s cognitive development and achievement disappear leading to speculation that a child’s socialization is determined by 31 the characteristics or social, human, and cultural capital of the parent(s) attained in part through schooling (Ricciuti 2004). Additional research on childcare providers as developers of a child’s human capital and cultural capital would inform this research. Summary The literature on children’s education illustrates the ways parental involvement is fundamental to the educational process. Both the functional and conflict paradigms are weak in their understanding of the intersections between school and the family. Research, such as this one, will help expand our understanding of the causal factors leading to disparate levels of academic achievement. Structural influences on academic achievement are illuminated by the works of Randall Collins (1971) and Samuel Bowles and Herbert Gintis (1976), but the family is conceptually absent as in many works reviewing structure. Jay MacLeod (2009) sought to conceptualize the family and structure together, and he revealed new findings connecting a positive family life with increased opportunities for success in school. With the development of social and cultural capital (Coleman 1988; Lareau 1989, 2003; Bourdieu 1977) parental involvement has become more important in the educational process throughout primary and secondary school. When parents practice concerted cultivation and attempt to customize their child’s education, opportunities for academic success are significantly improved. These are the types of parenting practices that would work towards validating my hypotheses. Differences between working-class and middle-class families have been attributed to cultural differences, economic disparities, and overall perception of the importance their culture places upon education. 32 The literature has developed the idea that two parent families are not the only way for a child to be academically successful, but that a child needs adult interaction and expectations that both support and inspire them to achieve success. This support can come from adults other than a child’s mother or father. The ways that rule setting and parental involvement improves academic success gives support to the hypotheses that children in two parent families will do better academically than their single-parent counterparts that do not have extended kin or other adults in the home to interact with. Based on the literature no single variable is the source of success or failure, but the intersections of many variables lead to educational achievement. From the research addressed above, I have developed two hypotheses: 1. Two parent families lead to increased academic achievement for their children. 2. Parental involvement through communicating and rule setting produces increased academic achievement for their children. 33 Chapter 3 METHODS Data and Sample The data used in my analysis comes from the Educational Childhood Longitudinal Study-Kindergarten cohort (NCES 2005). This subset includes 8,809 parent respondents with children aged 13-14. There are 10,446 female and 10,950 male children covered in this data set and the racial distribution is 1,149 Asian, 2,937 Black or African American, 3,559 Hispanic, 11,308 White, and 1,077 Other which covers American Indian, Native Hawaiian, and more than one race. The researchers who conducted the ECLS-K 1998-99 administered an initial wave of interviews with the parent and proceeded to return every other year to acquire updated information. General demographic characteristics were compiled through school documents on file and composite measures taken by the researchers themselves. The interviews were in the home and involved whatever type of parent(s) (biological, step, adoptive, guardian) that was available, but if there was difficulty in recording both parent’s responses due to unavailability the researcher returned at a future time. The initial intent of the study was to get detailed longitudinal information on children to assess differences in academic achievement, and it is because of this initial intent that the current study is using the ECLS-K 1998-99. Variables The dependent variable in the analyses is P7SCHGRD, or grades children received in school, measured by parent’s responses to the question “now I would like to 34 ask you about {child}'s grades during this school year. Overall, across all subjects (he/she) takes at school does (he/she) get,” and the parent had the option of mostly a’s, mostly b’s, mostly c’s, mostly d’s, mostly f’s, child’s school does not give grades, refused, don’t know, and not ascertained (NCES 2005). The question was asked at the end of the school year of each parent. The variable was recoded into GradesAtSchool and “child’s school does not give grades” was moved to missing along with refused, don’t know, and not ascertained. To ease the analysis process an automatic recode was done to flip the answers making one equal to “mostly f’s” and five “mostly a’s.” The independent variables used in this study focus on family structure, parental involvement and rule setting. My hypotheses are developed through seven multiple regression models using family type as an independent variable as well as incorporating other variables to focus on parental involvement. Family type: P7HPARNT- types of parents in household has nine categories, biological mother and biological father, biological mother and other-father (step, adoptive, foster), biological father and other-mother (step, adoptive, foster), biological mother only, biological father only, two adoptive parents, single adoptive parent or adoptive parent and stepparent, related guardian(s), and unrelated guardian(s). The related guardian(s) category pertains to immediate family excluding the mother and father while unrelated guardian(s) is any adult that is not an adoptive parent or a blood related guardian(s). This variable was recoded into “types of parents” with biological mother and biological father assigned position 1, biological mother and Other-father (step, adoptive, foster) assigned position 2, biological father and other-mother (step, 35 adoptive, foster) assigned position 3, biological mother only assigned position 4, biological father only assigned position 5, two adoptive parents and single adoptive parent or adoptive parent and stepparent assigned position 6, and related guardian (s) and unrelated guardian(s) assigned position 7 (NCES 2005). Parental Involvement has four independent variables: family rules for homework, communication, curfew, and strict rules. The first independent variable, P7HWKRUL was recoded into P7HWKRUL2, and asks the question: “Are there family rules for {child} about any of the following…doing homework?” The parent(s) had the option of yes, no, na, refused, don’t know, and not ascertained.. The second independent variable, communication, is a computed variable combining P7OFTTLK, which asks the question: “now I would like to ask you about some things you might talk with {child} about. In the past month, how often have you talked with {child} about {his/her} day at school? Would you say not at all, a few times a month, a few times a week, or every day?” The parent(s) had the option of not at all, a few times a month, a few times a week, every day, refused, don’t know, and not ascertained; with P7TLKFUT which asks the question: “Now I would like to ask you about some things you might talk with {CHILD} about. In the past month, how often have you talked with {CHILD} about...] talked about (his/her) future?” The parent(s) had the option of not at all, a few times a month, a few times a week, every day, refused, don’t know, and not ascertained. In computing P7OFTTLK and P7TLKFUT the new variable communication produced eight valid categories (NCES 2005). 36 The third independent variable, curfew, was not changed from P7CURFEW but asked the question: “How often do you…make and enforce curfews for {child}?” The parent(s) had the option of never, rarely, sometimes, always, refused, don’t know, and not ascertained. Finally, strict rules is a computed variable comprised of P7HITPRV which asks the question: “[Most teenagers misbehave or get into trouble from time to time. When {child} misbehaves, what do you or {name of spouse/partner} typically do?] Take away a privilege.” The parent(s) had the option of yes, no refused, don’t know, not ascertained; and P7TVRUL2 which asks the question: “Are there family rules for {child} about any of the following…? How early or late {he/she} may watch television (NCES 2005). Control Variables Other important variables that were included in the analyses were child’s age, gender and race, as well as mother’s and father’s education, occupation and family income. There was an SES measure used to evaluate the education, occupation and household income of the family. The SES measure was collected during the initial wave of data collection and is composed of three categories: mother/guardian and father/guardian education and occupation as well as household income. To define poverty level, the Census poverty thresholds for 1998 is used. For households with only one parent available, not all components are defined. For these cases, the SES was computed averaging the available components (NCES 2005). The variable used for child’s race is WKRACETH (Child race from parent interview), which is stated above and recoded into three dummy variables, titled Black Child, Hispanic Child, and AsianOther. The variable 37 used for child’s gender is GENDER which provided two categories male and female and was recoded into Girl. The analysis uses multi-regression to compute one dependent variable, GradesAtSchool, and seven independent variables: P7HWKRUL recoded into P7HWKRUL2;P7HITPRV recoded into TakePrivilege and P7TVRUL2 recoded into LateWatchTV, which were combined to generate StrictRules; P7OFTTLK and P7TLKFUT were combined to generate Communication; P7CURFEW, and P7HPARNT recoded into seven different variables to isolate each individual family structure: biological mother and father, biological mother and other-father (step, adoptive, foster), biological father and other-mother (step, adoptive, foster), biological mother, biological father, Adoptive parent(two and single), and Guardian(s) (related and unrelated). There are three control variables: GENDER recoded into Girl; WKRACETH recoded into three categories: Black/Hispanic, AsianOther and White; W8SESQ5 which is socioeconomic status. Summary The data set used in this research has developed a channel of information to identify statistically significant factors in children’s academic achievement. The variables used highlight the types of parental involvement which correlate with success. Activities such as checking homework, enforcing a curfew, or setting rules for watching television all signify some form of concerted cultivation. Eating meals together, talking about school and the future are examples of parental involvement which develops children’s comfort with adults as well as themselves. All of these variables are connected and 38 require joint understanding to see how each affects the other. Family structure is used to isolate the effect different types of parental units have on various levels of concerted cultivation. There were control variables used to make the findings applicable to various racial groups, income categories, and levels of parental education. This study utilizes important variables that are limited by the size of the data set, yet they accurately conceptualize forms of concerted cultivation. Conceptualizations of parental involvement by researchers vary slightly from study to study and the case here is no different. I have taken concepts used repeatedly together and separately to evaluate levels of educational inequality. The conceptualization of parental involvement via rule setting could use more ways to expand the view of parent’s attempting to customize their child’s everyday life. For example, the types of friends which are acceptable, the content of the television being watched, and the order in which activities may be done in relation to homework and playing outside. In terms of communication between parent and child, simply speaking during dinner or about the school day does not accurately assess open lines of communication. The type of relationship would help explain the level of successful communication and if any valuable information is being transmitted. An underlying crucial aspect of the family relationships is the positive/negative state in which it exists. Good communication cannot always be given when the emotional relationship is hurting. Research in this area specifically would improve our understanding of the family. Developing a deeper understanding of the content of the conversations during meals would articulate the importance and effect such verbal interactions could have on academic achievement. 39 Although the research was limited in which questions were asked and which groups were interviewed, the ECLS-K 1998-99 is a long and extensive data set accounting for over 20,000 respondents. The sampling for race was low enough to hinder the significance of certain variables such as AsianOther. The data was limited in terms of peer groups and their responses to questions on the survey. The variables accounting for grades over the year were vague and based upon a parental response. A more objective measure of the grades earned throughout the year would improve the operationalization of the dependent variable. 40 Chapter 4 FINDINGS The results presented some interesting data about the relationship between family and academic achievement. As would be expected there were variations between family types, as well as similarities such as family rules for homework and taking away privileges. My findings reinforce the idea that although concerted cultivation does produce positive results at times, as the hypotheses stated, it also produces results that vary in level of achievement. Instead of all the children experiencing concerted cultivation performing equally well, other variables created obstacles to academic achievement. The data supports the hypothesis that children of two parent families perform better academically but rule setting did not improve success in school for everyone. The differences which appeared in the analyses support the argument that variations exist within concerted cultivation. The findings suggest the hypotheses are valid under circumstances where concerted cultivation is not impeded by a lack of resources or opportunity and conditions. Although more variables would improve this analysis, the ones which were used in the following tables produced important conclusions about the family and academic achievement. The first table in the appendix describes the descriptive statistics used in this study and the responses of each family type. The following tables are regression outputs labeled 2a-2g and present the results for biological mother and biological father; biological mother-other-father; biological father-other-mother; biological mother; biological father; adoptive parent(s) and guardian(s). The sample as a whole is 14 percent 41 Black, 17 percent Hispanic, 11 percent AsianOther which include American Indian, Native Hawaiian, and more than one race as well as 58 percent white; 48 percent female and 52 percent male. The average grades were mostly b’s and the enforcement of curfew averaged between sometimes and always. Household rules for homework averaged 93 percent. Descriptive Analyses Table 1 shows descriptive statistics of the variables separated by the seven family structures. All family types reported higher percentages of mostly a’s and b’s, ranging between 30 and 40 percent. There were fewer responses for mostly c’s across all family types with the highest among biological father and guardians (20-25 percent). Family rules for homework received a larger amount of yes responses than no, with biological father-other-mother family type reporting 97.7 percent. Very few reported not setting rules for homework although biological father reported 7.5 percent against setting rules. There was a minor variance between whether family types felt it was necessary to set a curfew. All family types responded always more (75-85 percent) than never for setting curfew. Biological mother and biological father, biological mother-other-father and adoptive parents reported talking about the child’s day at school every day (80-85 percent). Biological father-other-mother, biological mother and biological father reported talking about school a few times a week (20-30 percent). For talking about the future biological father-other-mother and adoptive parents reported a few times a month (60-65 percent). A few times a week received the second highest level of responses from 42 biological mother-other-father, biological mother and guardians (25-35 percent). All seven models reported high levels of taking away privileges with biological motherother-father, biological father-other-mother and adoptive parents all responding yes almost 90 percent of the time. Rules for how late watch TV received more responses for yes than no from biological mother-other-father, biological father-other-mother and adoptive parents (90-95 percent). There are five categories for race, Black, Hispanic, Asian, Other and White. Guardians received the highest percent of responses for Black child (36.6 percent). Hispanic child received its highest responses in biological mother (19 percent). The adoptive parents’ family type had the most Asian children with 12.1 percent. Other child received the highest responses from guardians (19.5). White child responses were highest in biological father-other-mother (72.6 percent). The gender category varied evenly with children of biological mother-other-father, biological mother and guardians reporting 5060 percent yes for having a female child. There were more responses for having a male child with biological father-other-mother, biological father and adoptive parents reporting 50-55 percent. Out of the five SES quintiles the first and lowest quintile received the largest proportion from guardians (43.2 percent). The fifth and highest quintile received the largest proportion from biological mother and biological father and adoptive parents (30-35 percent). Biological Mother and Father For biological mother and biological father, the regression model significantly predicts grades at school. The R2 for this regression equals .147 (p<.001), indicating that 43 the model explains 14.7 percent of the variation in grades at school. Family rule for homework was significant and it was negatively correlated showing a reduction in grades by .166 levels (B= -.166, p<.001) compared to families without rules for homework, possibly as a reactionary measure to a drop in grades. Other possibilities include tutoring sessions the child receives at school which off-set the need for time spent doing homework at home. This finding conflicts with the data and calls into question other mediating variables. Often talking about the day at school was significant and is positively correlated with the dependent variable displaying an increase in grades at school by .078 levels (B=.078, p<.001) compared to families who spoke less often about the day at school. This variable supports the hypothesis proving to be a positive influence on academic achievement. How often parent(s) set curfews was significantly correlated showing a reduction in grades by .026 levels (B= -.026, p<.050) compared to families that did not frequently set curfews. This variable calls into question the use of curfews as a reactionary tool which would explain the negative correlation or the potential for overly strict rules to decrease grades. This variable could be improved with the use of ethnographic methodologies which are more qualitative and content orientated with a deeper focus on the quality or purpose of certain rules, to call into question the positive or negative effects customizing a child’s life in terms of curfews has on their academic achievement. How late children could watch television was also significantly correlated showing an increase in grades by .077 levels (B=.077, p<.050) compared to families that did not restrict viewing time. This variable expresses the positive effects of customizing a child’s daily life to promote educational activities and or opportunities for 44 communication. Another significant variable is taking away a privilege which expressed a reduction in grades by .074 levels (B= -.074, p<.050) compared to families that did not take away any privileges. Of the control variables Girl is significant at showing an increase in grades at school by .245 levels (B=.245, p<.001) compared to males. Black/Hispanic child is significant showing a reduction in grades at school by .154 levels (B= -.154, p<.001). AsianOther child is significant showing an increase in grades at school by .75 levels (B=.75, p<.050). Finally, SES measure is significant showing an increase in grades at school by .152 levels (B=.152, p<.001). Often talk about the future was not significant. SES also plays its part supporting the hypotheses as long as both parents are involved and providing resources allowing the concerted cultivation process the ability to produce positive results. Biological Mother-other-father For biological mother-other-father, the regression model significantly predicts grades at school. The R2 for this regression equals .101(p<.001), indicating that the model explains 10.1 percent of the variance in grades at school. Only two variables were significant. Girl is significant showing an increase in grades at school by .318 levels (B=.318, p<.001) compared to males. SES measure is significant showing an increase in grades at school by .165 levels (B=.165, p<.001). Transition in the family can disrupt school and would be a potential mediating variable showing the significance of rule setting and communication between children and parents. SES being significant potentially supports the first hypothesis based upon the underlying notion that more 45 resources provide, when activated, the opportunity for success also considering the help of kin. Family rule for homework, often set curfews, talk about day at school, talk about the future, how late watch TV, and take away a privilege were not significant. Of the control variables race was not a factor. Biological Father-other-mother For biological father-other-mother, the regression model significantly predicts grades at school. The R2 for this regression equals .278 (p<.001), indicating that the model explains 27.8 percent of the variation in grades at school. How late watch television was significantly correlated showing a decrease in grades by .590 levels (B= .590, p<.050) compared to families not setting rules on how late watch television. This variable would benefit from deeper analysis to understand how limiting television could hurt grades at school as compared to helping. Girl is significant increasing grades at school by .501 levels (B=.501, p<.001) compared to males. SES measure is significant showing an increase in grades at school by .234 levels (B=.234, p<.001). A similar pattern arose here as it did with biological mother-other-father with rule setting and communication failing to sustain significance. Family rule for homework, talk about day at school and race were not significant. Biological Mother For biological mother, the regression model significantly predicts grades at school. The R2 for this regression equals .100 (p<.001), indicating that the model explains 10 percent of the variation in grades at school. How often set curfews was significantly correlated showing a reduction in grades by .059 levels (B= -.059, p<.050) 46 compared to families that did not frequently set curfews. As stated before ethnographic research would further develop this variable’s effect on grades and whether or not it is reactionary to already depreciated grades. Often talk about the day at school is significant showing an increase in grades at school by .106 levels (B-=.106, p<.050) compared to families which talk less about the child’s day at school. The significant control variables are Girl, showing an increase in grades at school by .248 levels (B=.248, p<.001) compared to males, and Black/Hispanic child, showing a decrease in grades at school by .215 levels (B= -.215, p<.001). SES measure is significant showing an increase in grades at school by .124 levels (B=.124, p<.001). This category conflicts with the first hypothesis that two parent families are the desired structure for academic success although the second hypothesis is validated due to the significance communication had. Access to resources, a potential aspect of hypothesis one is depicted here where SES is significant. This category represents the ways concerted cultivation varies in level exercised and types of practices reproduced. Family rule for homework, often talk about the future, how late watch TV, take away a privilege and AsianOther child were not significant. Biological Father For biological father, the model significantly predicts grades at school. The R2 for this regression equals .233 (p<.001), indicating that the model explains 23.3 percent of the variation in grades at school. Girl is significant showing an increase in grades at school by .595 levels (B=.595, p<.001) compared to males. SES measure is significant showing an increase in grades at school by .281 levels (B=.281, p<.001). Biological 47 father shows similar results as biological parent and step parent. Family rule for homework, talk about day at school, how late watch TV and race were not significant. Adoptive Parents For adoptive parents, the regression model does not significantly predict grades at school indicating that the model is not significant (p>.105) and cannot explain the variation in grades at school. Within adoptive parent(s) the significant variable is SES measure which if significant would potentially show an increase in grades at school by .153 levels (B=.153, p<.010). Guardians For Guardians, the regression model significantly predicts grades at school. The R2 for this regression equals .070 (p<.010), indicating that the model explains 7 percent of the variation in grades at school. Girl is significant showing an increase in grades at school by .450 levels (B=.450, p<.001) compared to males. This category, aside from adoptive parents was the strongest case against the hypotheses showing no significance for communication or rule setting. The unseen or unstudied variations require further research. Family rule for homework, talk about day at school, how late watch TV, race, and SES measure were not significant. Summary Throughout the family types there is a high level of variability. Although a large portion of the models confirmed few significant variables, some main points stand out. The child’s gender was significant in every model excluding adoptive parents with girls performing better than males. Although the research does not directly deal with the issue 48 of gender it has important implications for academic achievement and other studies will help explain further this finding (American Association of University Women 1992). Race was also significant in many models only failing to be significant in three of the seven. The variation between grades going up for AsianOther and going down for Black/Hispanic highlight structural discrimination and disadvantage that exist for minorities in today’s society. A possible influence on AsianOther may be the low level of samples for this category. SES measure was significant for all but guardians and adoptive parents. Many variables such as family rules about homework, talking about the day at school, how late watch TV (which pertains to the hypotheses) and race were statistically not significant in four or more family types. This creates problems for the hypotheses and reflects the existence of other mediating variables. Although the hypotheses may be correct in some circumstances it certainly cannot be said that they hold true over different family types controlling for resources and lived experiences. These trends in the data will highlight areas that require further research in the field of children’s education. 49 Chapter 5 CONCLUSION Inequality in school has many causal factors. This study investigated the effects the family has on academic achievement with a focus on family structure and parental involvement. Instead of focusing on the school’s structural influences I focus on the role family plays in mediating a child’s experience in school through various forms of concerted cultivation. A child’s time spent within the family is significantly related to their overall cognitive abilities and speech code which influences their experiences in school (Hess et al. 1965; Bernstein 1977). Analyzing the different cultural practices as they exist in various family structures deepens our understanding of why inequality persists. One aspect of the family that I felt was vital to understand is the fluctuations in family structure and how that can impede a functioning educational environment and the ability of the family to fully and effectively apply concerted cultivation. My analysis of academic achievement focuses on the impact family structure and parenting practices, such as customization of a child’s daily life through rules setting, have on creating an educational environment. The success children experience in school is influenced by the fluctuations in family structure and level of involvement in the family. Cultural practices which value activities such as eating dinner together, talking about the future or day at school increase opportunities for communication and present the opportunity for parents to get involved. When both parents are not present in the home involvement becomes more difficult and practices such as setting curfews and taking away privileges become harder 50 to institute, thus disrupting aspects of concerted cultivation. Difficulties arise from the absence of adults in the home which puts pressure on one person to complete all the tasks necessary to provide an educational atmosphere conducive to academic achievement. This can be difficult when work hours are irregular or multiple jobs are held to support the family. The findings from the regressions shed light on my hypotheses and the larger field of research on inequality in education. My analysis both supports and refutes my hypotheses and my discussion will address the ways even negative correlations say a significant amount about causal factors influencing disparate levels of academic achievement. Two Biological Parents Findings The strongest family type in terms of significant variables is biological mother and father in which only one variable, talking about the future, is not significant. The significance of setting rules for homework and talking about the school day lie in the ability of parents to gather knowledge of their child’s progress. Part of the process of being involved requires parents to stay up to date on the progress their children make in each class they are in and their overall experience in school. Aside from the arena of comfort that can be developed through routine practices like homework or talking about school during dinner time, significant effects of these practices are that they provide opportunities to practice the types of skills important to academic success (Roche and Ghazarian 2011). The family is the foundational unit on which a child builds their life 51 and when the support they receive is forthcoming and consistent they feel as though they can achieve more (Lareau 2003). The application of rules is also significantly related to academic success because it provides structure to support an academic lifestyle. Structure can be a soothing and comforting feeling as well as create standards and expectations that promote academic achievement. If the family allows a child to come home when they please, watch television late into the night, and not restrict privileges when behavior is unsatisfactory the child is likely to develop habits that disrupt academic achievement. Expectations promote success when applied sensibly and not irrationally driving a child to work harder and attempt to achieve the standards expected of them (Musick and Meier 2009). My study does not explore the impact of race and gender as deeply as cultural and parental practices, but research has shown the steady progress females have made in relation to males and my data was supportive of this (American Association of University Women 1992). It is possible the types of parental involvement differ by gender in the family, but this is not something I can substantiate with my research. The impact of race was also supportive of past research showing African American and Latin American students suffering while Asian American students succeeded. As could be expected, the significance of socioeconomic status was present which supports and reinforces the argument that a lack of access to resources impedes parents from providing educational supplies and activities which support a child’s academic progress. When children attend poor schools or during the summer time when they have no instruction, those parents who possess the means to acquire supplies to create an 52 educational environment outside of school benefit their children and give them a greater opportunity for success. When the family is unable to provide these “extra” resources the child suffers and differences appear in long-term academic achievement. Gaps develop between children while they are in school and once they are formed it is difficult if not impossible to close. This is why family plays a major role in the development of a child along with school. It is important to note some variables which were significant but had a negative correlation with grades which suggests that rules for homework, curfew, and taking away privileges may have hurt grades (though it may be more likely a reaction to already falling grades). This aspect of my study is significantly hindered by the unknown type of relationship various variables used in the regressions had with a child’s overall grades. One Biological-Other Parent Findings Significant differences which appeared in cases for biological mother-other-father and biological father-other-mother call into question the importance of two-parent families. The importance two parents play in the family reside most significantly in the increased amount of time and resources made available to the child. Although the other parent is not biological the assumption would be that their presence would have a similar effect in providing support and comfort to the child. Without having the ability to discern the state of the relationship between the child and other parent it is not enough that they are available and providing resources to suggest they are as successful as biological parents in establishing a working bond. The only significant variables in these two regressions were gender and socioeconomic status. The previous significance rule setting 53 had on academic achievement faded when the second biological parent was replaced in the home. The significance of gender and socioeconomic status was previously established, and for these two cases similar effects were found. Different dynamics and experiences such as fluctuations in family structure (when the biological parent is replaced by the other parent) could be a cause for disruption of grades and effectiveness of rules and certain types of involvement. The ability of the family to provide comfort and support (excluding economic) may be inhibited by the restricting of the family type from two biological parents to one. An ethnographic approach would allow this argument to be pursued and locate the mediating variables that may nullify the significance of rule setting and parental involvement. There are obvious difficulties faced by the other parent in assuming the role established by a biological parent. Children are not necessarily as open to input from other parents and that creates obstacles to involvement. Changes experienced in the family have an effect on children’s academic achievement and without a deeper understanding of the variables mediating the significance of rule setting and parental involvement questions will continue to persist. For this reason I highlight the significance that qualitative studies could have to increase conceptualization of certain variables in my study. It is important to note between the two types of family structures with biological and other parent between 10 and 27 percent of the variation can be explained meaning there are more variables that require consideration for a full understanding of what mediates a child’s academic achievement. 54 Single Biological Parent Findings The findings for single parent homes were largely similar with the exception of curfews and talking about the school day being significant for biological mothers but not fathers. Although curfews held a negative correlation suggesting a decrease in grades when enforced, the relationship is somewhat disguised by the limited amount of information available on the application and consistency of setting a curfew. Gender and socioeconomic status were significant and positively correlated suggesting they helped improve grades. These findings support past research, as shown above and help establish the over-reaching significance gender and access to resources had on academic achievement. Due to the limited number of parents in the home access to resources is more closely tied to a child’s grades. There is the possibility of extended kin being involved in the family and providing support in ways other than monetarily. This would flesh out the ways in which providing an educational environment is supported or prohibited by adults being present in the home. Before considering the significance of rule setting and parental involvement for one biological other parents, it could be possible to assign the difficulties children experience in school to the absence of another parent in the home. Due to the results of the one biological other parents more questions arise as to what creates difficulties for children in school. Types of capital whether social, human, or cultural play their part in mediating the relationship between rule setting and involvement as do an individual’s experiences, but these are not enough to completely develop the reasons for fluctuating success academically amongst children. 55 From this discussion of the findings it becomes apparent that certain parental practices, such as rule setting and communication about the school day are useful to some but not all family types. The presence of two parents in the home is not the same when accounting for non-biological parents, adoptive parents, or guardians. For cases where fluctuations in family structure do not exist there may be an increased opportunity for applying concerted cultivation. For families experiencing fluctuations such as divorce or adoption, obstacles impeding involvement or rule setting arise. The consistency that is present within a family that is involved and supportive gives a child comfort and security enabling them to be successful in school (Roche and Ghazarian 2011). Questions about who will be in the home to seek help from or talk with may impede a child’s ability to communicate their needs, specifically in relation to academic achievement. Due to the needs of each child varying according to how they learn and adapt to situations, concerted cultivation takes on various forms to best suit what is needed. Rules for homework or setting curfews may not be necessary for all children to be successful in school and because of this a family’s implementation of rules and involvement will vary. The significance of concerted cultivation is not necessarily to be involved or create rules according to established standards, but to communicate and understand the needs of a child to customize their lives to best support their academic achievement. This is why various forms of concerted cultivation exist and there are multiple ways to cultivate in a child the standards and skills important to academic achievement. This suggests that although changing family structures affect academic achievement so does the application of rules and communication differ across and within 56 family structures. My research supports the notion that two-parent biological families that enact rule setting and practice communication (talking about school day) provide a better academic opportunity to those with a step or other parent, single parent, adoptive parents or guardians. The notion that family structure affects academic achievement outside of rule setting and communication is also supported with a greater difficulty for students lacking adults in the home as well as resources. It is not clearly explained how these relationships have an effect on grades with variables, setting a curfew and limiting how late television can be watched showing a decrease in grades. I have not presented nor was I able to find data explaining these types of results which leaves some questions still to be answered. The possibility that certain rule setting expresses a decrease in grades could be a response to already struggling achievement rather than actually being detrimental to academic achievement. Based on a review of the literature and the regressions run for this study it becomes evident that what enables a child to be successful in school is not necessarily access to resources or parents in the home, but an active implementation of resources to promote an educational environment and parental involvement based on understanding when and how to provide support to a child. The forms of concerted cultivation Lareau (2003) established have been supported here and developed further to show the ways various forms exist. The findings highlight the ways similar family structures differently apply practices for academic success and experience varied results. Similar to Lareau’s earlier analysis of cultural capital in Home Advantage (1989), educational success 57 depends not simply on holding valued cultural capital as it does on how such capital is invested and used to aid the child’s academic achievement. The limitations of this study based on available data and literature could be significantly eliminated if a qualitative approach was added to the quantitative focus. Understanding the deep personal relationships in the family is crucial to comprehending the intersections and compatibility of certain parental practices, and would generate new ways to help children with their academic achievement. 58 Variables APPENDIX Table 1: Coding of Variables and Descriptive Statistics (N=8,809) Family Structure Biological Biological Biological Biological Biological Adoptive Mother MotherFatherMother Father Parents Biological Otherother(7.2%) (1%) (Two and Father Father Mother Single) (26.5%) (4.2%) (.6%) (.7%) Grades Mostly F’s Mostly D’s Mostly C’s Mostly B’s Mostly A’s Homework Rules Yes No Set Curfews Never Rarely Sometimes Always Talk About Day at School Not at all A few times a month A few times a week Everyday Guardian (Related and Unrelated) (1%) .3% 1.3% 8.6% 34.5% 55.3% .7% 2.7% 15.9% 43.5% 37.3% N/A 4% 21.6% 42.4% 32% 1.2% 2.9% 17.9% 41.5% 36.5% 1.5% 2.6% 25.6% 29.7% 40.5% 2.2% 2.9% 18.1% 45.7% 31.2% 2.4% 1% 24.8% 49% 22.8% 93.1% 6.9% 95.5% 4.5% 97.7% 2.3% 94% 6% 92.5% 7.5% 93.8% 6.2% 96.3% 3.7% 11.4% 3.2% 6.4% 79% 7.8% 1.6% 4% 86.6% 5.4% 2.3% 6.2% 86% 7.4% 2.6% 8.5% 81.5% 10.1% 2.5% 10.6% 76.9% 10.3% 4.8% 2.8% 82.1% 6.9% 1.9% 7.9% 83.3% .3% 2.2% 17.3% 80.2% .5% 2% 16.8% 80.7% .8% 2.3% 30.5% 66.4% .5% 2.5% 20.8% 76.2% .5% 5.5% 27.6% 66.3% N/A 2.1% 13.7% 84.2% 1.4% 6.1% 20.2% 72.3% 59 Talk About Future Not at all A few times a month A few times a week Everyday Taking Away Privilege Yes No Late Watch TV Yes No Race Black/Hispanic Asian/Other White Gender Male Female SES Measure First Quintile Second Quintile Third Quintile Fourth Quintile Fifth Quintile 4.8% 63.6% 22.2% 9.4% 3.2% 58.2% 25.8% 12.7% 4.7% 61.7% 20.3% 13.3% 4.3% 53.7% 26.5% 15.6% 5.5% 53.8% 24.6% 16.1% 2.1% 65.8% 21.9% 10.3% 5.6% 45.5% 32.4% 16.4% 86% 14% 91.9% 8.1% 89.1% 10.9% 86.7% 13.3% 84% 16% 89% 11% 84.9% 15.1% 89.3% 10.7% 91.3% 8.7% 93% 7% 87.5% 12.5% 86.4% 13.6% 94.5% 5.5% 88.9% 11.1% 20.7% 10.6% 68.6% 26.3% 7% 66.7% 23.4% 4% 72.6% 42.8% 7.9% 49.3% 29.7% 9.9% 60.4% 36.9% 24.8% 68% 49.8% 20% 62% 51% 49% 50.3% 49.7% 54.2% 45.8% 50.1% 49.9% 53.9% 46.1% 51.7% 48.3% 41.5% 58.5% 10.5% 14.3% 17.8% 25% 32% 16.8% 22.4% 26.2% 20.9% 13.7% 15.1% 25.4% 28.6% 16.7% 14.3% 27.2% 22.9% 19.7% 15.3% 14.9% 20.1% 25% 20.7% 22.8% 11.4% 16.3% 15.6% 10.9% 22.4% 34.7% 43.2% 28.2% 15% 9.7% 3.9% 60 Table 2a. Regression Predicting Grades at School for Biological Mother and Biological Father Predictor Coefficient SE T P Value Constant 3.801 .094 40.517 .000 Homework Rules -.166 .039 -4.275 .000 Set Curfews -.026 .010 -2.651 .008 Talk About Day at School .078 .020 3.901 .000 Talk About Future -.018 .014 -1.296 .195 .077 .031 2.478 .013 -.074 .028 -2.652 .008 .245 .019 13.211 .000 -.154 .025 -6.080 .000 AsianOtherChild .075 .032 2.353 .019 SES Measure .152 .008 20.087 .000 Adjusted R-squared .147 Late Watch TV Take Away Privilege Girl BlackHispanicChild 61 Table 2b. Regression Predicting Grades at School for Biological Mother and Other Father (Step, Adoptive, Foster) Predictor Coefficient SE T P Value Constant 3.632 .294 12.374 .000 Homework Rules -.138 .140 -.983 .326 Set Curfews -.022 .033 -.676 .499 Talk About Day at School .013 .058 .233 .816 Talk About Future -.020 .038 -.513 .608 .093 .101 .921 .357 -.020 .104 -.192 .848 .318 .056 5.727 .000 -.023 .066 -.355 .723 AsianOtherChild .145 .110 1.313 .189 SES Measure .165 .023 7.167 .000 Adjusted R-squared .101 Late Watch TV Take away privilege Girl BlackHispanicChild 62 Table 2c. Regression Predicting Grades at School for Biological Father and Other Mother (Step, Adoptive, Foster) `Predictor Constant Homework Rules Coefficient SE T P Value 3.321 .778 4.269 .000 .041 .426 .096 .924 Talk About Day at School .088 .134 .658 .512 Late Watch TV -.590 .272 -2.167 .032 .501 .137 3.660 .000 -.271 .159 -1.707 .091 AsianOtherChild .208 .338 .615 .540 SES Measure .234 .053 4.435 .000 Adjusted R-squared .278 Girl BlackHispanicChild 63 Table 2d. Regression Predicting Grades at School for Biological Mother Predictor Coefficient SE T P Value Constant 3.610 .206 17.564 .000 Homework Rules -.067 .102 -.649 .516 Set Curfews -.059 .027 -2.172 .030 Talk About Day at School .106 .044 2.401 .016 Talk About Future .028 .029 .963 .336 Late Watch TV .076 .072 1.058 .290 -.113 .067 -1.689 .091 .248 .044 5.628 .000 BlackHispanicChild -.215 .050 -4.332 .000 AsianOtherChild -.076 .087 -.879 .380 SES Measure .124 .017 7.381 .000 Adjusted R-Squared .100 Take Away a Privilege Girl 64 Table 2e. Regression Predicting Grades at School for Biological Father Predictor Coefficient SE T P Value Constant 2.996 .483 6.198 .000 Homework Rules -.241 .233 -1.036 .302 .099 .689 .491 -.089 .187 -.474 .636 Girl .595 .130 4.557 .000 BlackHispanicChild .240 .153 1.573 .118 AsianOtherChild .075 .211 .358 .721 SES Measure .281 .052 5.385 .000 Adjusted R-squared .233 Talk About Day at School Late Watch TV .068 65 Table 2f. Regression Predicting Grades at School for Adoptive Parents (Two and Single) Predictor Coefficient SE T P Value 3.603 .806 4.472 .000 .119 .362 .329 .743 -.048 .189 -.256 .799 Late Watch TV .010 .355 .029 .977 Girl .125 .159 .789 .431 BlackHispanicChild -.232 .186 -1.251 .213 AsianOtherChild -.045 .195 -.232 .817 SES Measure .153 .053 2.889 .005 Adjusted R-squared .038 Constant Homework Rules Talk About Day at School 66 Table 2g. Regression Predicting Grades at School for Guardians (Related and Unrelated) Predictor Coefficient SE T P Value 2.855 .410 6.970 .000 Homework Rules .110 .316 .349 .727 Talk About Day at School .111 .096 1.151 .251 Late Watch TV .161 .213 .755 .451 Girl .450 .123 3.656 .000 -.056 .141 -.393 .695 AsianOtherChild .042 .173 .244 .807 SES Measure .079 .055 1.444 .150 Adjusted R-squared .070 Constant BlackHispanicChild 67 REFERENCES Alexander, Karl L, Doris R. Entwisle, and Maxine S. Thompson. 1987. “School Performance, Status Relations, and the Structure of Sentiment: Bringing the Teacher Back In.” American Sociological Review 52(5):665-682. American Association of University Women. 1992. How Schools Shortchange girls. The AAUW Report. New York, NY: Marlowe and Co. Anderson, C. Arnold. 1961. “A Skeptical Note on the Relation of Vertical Mobility to Education.” American Journal of Sociology 66(6):560-570. Ballarino, Gabriele, Fabrizio Bernardi, Miguel Requena, and Hans Schadee. 2009. “Persistent Inequalities? Expansion of Education and Class Inequalities in Italy and Spain.” European Sociological Review 25(1):123-138. Becker, Gary S. 1964. Human Capital: A Theoretical and Empirical Analysis, with Special Reference to Education. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Bernstein, Basil. 1977. "Social Class, Language and Socialization." Pp.473-486 in Power and Ideology in Education, edited by J. Karabel and A.H. Halsey. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Bourdieu, Pierre. 1977. Outline of A Theory of Practice. Cambridge CB2 1RP, Great Britain: University Press, Cambridge. Bowles, Samuel and Herbert Gintis. 1976. Schooling in Capitalist America. New York, NY: Basic Books. 68 Bowles, Samuel and Henry M. Levin. 1968. “The Determinants of Academic Achievement: An Appraisal of Some Recent Evidence.” Journal of Human Resources 3(1):3-24. _____. 2000. “Does Schooling Raise Earnings by Making People Smarter?” Pp. 118-136 in Meritocracy and Economic Inequality, edited by Kenneth Arrow, Samuel Bowles, and Steven Durlauf. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. _____. 2002. “Schooling in Capitalist America Revisited.” Sociology of Education 75(1):1-18. Bowles, Samuel, Herbert Gintis, and Melissa Osborne. 2001. “The Determinants of Earnings: A Behavioral Approach.” Journal of Economic Literature 39(4):11371176. Breen, R., Ruud Luikx, Walter Muller, and Reinhard Pollak. 2009. “Non-Persistent Inequality in Educational Attainment: Evidence from Eight European Countries.” American Journal of Sociology 114(5):1475-1521. Bronfenbrenner, Urie and Pamela Morris. 1998. “The Bioecological Model of Human Development” Pp. 793-828 in Handbook of Child Psychology, Theoretical Models of Human Development, edited by W. Damon and R. M. Lerner. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons Inc. Cheadle, Jacob E. 2008. "Educational Investment, Family Context, and Children's Math and Reading Growth from Kindergarten through the Third Grade." Sociology of Education 81(1):1-31. 69 Cheadle, Jacob E. and Paul R. Amato. 2010. “A Quantitative Assessment of Lareau’s Qualitative Conclusions About Class, Race, and Parenting.” Journal of Family Issues 32(5):679-706. Coleman, James S. 1988. “Social Capital in the Creation of Human Capital.” American Journal of Sociology 94:S95-S120. Coleman, James S., Ernest Q. Campbell, Carol F. Hobson, James M. McPartland, Alexander M, Mood, Frederic D. Weinfeld, and Robert L. York. 1966. Equality of Educational Opportunity. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Collins, Randall. 1971. “Functional and Conflict Theories of Educational Stratification.” American Sociological Review 36(6):1002-1019. Dauber, Susan L., Karl L. Alexander, and Doris R. Entwisle. 1996. “Tracking and Transitions Through the Middle Grades: Channeling Educational Trajectories.” Sociology of Education 69(4):290-307. Dore, R.P. 1976. “Human Capital Theory, the Diversity of Societies and the Problem of Quality in Education.” Higher Education 5(1):79-102. Entwisle, Doris R. and Karl L. Alexander. 1996. “Family Type and Children’s Growth in Reading and Math Over the Primary Grades.” Journal of Marriage and Family 58(2):341-355. Esping-Anderson, G. 2004.“Untying the Gordian Knot of Social Inheritance.” Research in Social Stratification and Mobility 21:115-138. Fordham, Signithia and John U. Ogbu. 1986. "Black Students' School Success: Coping With the "Burden of 'Acting White.'" Urban Review 18(1):275-279. 70 Gamoran, Adam and Mark Berends. 1987. “The Effects of Stratification in Secondary Schools: Synthesis of Survey and Ethnographic Research.” Review of Educational Research 57(4):415-435. Goldthorpe, J.H. 1996. “Class Analysis and the Reorientation of Class Theory: The Case of Persisting Differentials in Educational Attainment.” British Journal of Sociology 47(3):481-505. Gould, Jay. 1996. The Mismeasure of Man. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc. Guo, Guang. 1998. “The Time of the Influences of Cumulative Poverty on Children’s Cognitive Ability and Achievement.” Social Forces 77(1):257-288. Han, Wen-Jui and Liana E. Fox. 2011. “Parental Work Schedules and Children’s Cognitive Trajectories.” Journal of Marriage and Family 73(10):962-980. Heath, Shirley Brice. 1983. Ways with Words: Language, Life, and Work in Communities and Classrooms. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Hedges, Larry V. and Amy Nowell. 1998. “Black-White Test Score Convergence Since 1965.” Pp. 149-181 in The Black-White Test Score Gap, edited by Christopher Jencks and Meredith Phillips. Washington, DC: Brookings Institute Press. Hernstein, Richard. 1973. IQ in the Meritocracy. Boston, MA: Little-Brown. Hess, Robert D., Virginia Shipman, and David Jackson. 1965. "Some New Dimensions in Providing Equal Educational Opportunity." The Journal of Negro Education 34(3):220-231. 71 Hurn, Christopher. 1993. The Limits and Possibilities of Schooling: An Introduction to the Sociology of Education. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon. Jensen, Arthur. 1973. Educability and Group Differences. New York, NY: Harper and Row. Kozol, Jonathan. 2005. The Shame of the Nation. New York, NY: Three Rivers Press. Lareau, Annette. 1989. Home Advantage: Social Class and Parental Intervention in Elementary Education. New York, NY: Falmer Press. _____. 2003. Unequal childhoods Class, race, and family life. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Lee, Valerie E. and David T. Burkam. 2002. Inequality at the Starting Gate. Washington, DC: Economic Policy Institute. Lee, Sang Min, Jason Kushner, and Seong Ho Cho. 2007. “Effects of Parent’s Gender, Childs’s Gender, and Parental Involvement on the Academic Achievement of Adolescents in Single Parent Families.” Sex Roles 56:149-157. Lee, Sang Min and Jason Kushner. 2008. “Single-Parent Families: he Role of Parent’s and Child’s Gender on Academic Achievement.” Gender and Education 20(6):607-621. MacLeod, Jay. 2009. Ain't Not Makin It. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Magnuson, Katherine A., Marcia K. Meyers, Christopher J. Ruhm, and Jane Waldfogel. 2004. “Inequality in Pre-school Education and School Readiness.” American Educational Research Journal 41(1):115-157. 72 McNeal Jr., Ralph B. 1999. “Parental Involvement as Social Capital: Differential Effectiveness on Science Achievement, Truancy, and Dropping Out.” Social Forces 78(1):117-144. Merida, Kevin. 1995, October 9. “Worry, Frustration Build for Many in the Black Middle-Class.” Washington Post, pp. A1, A22-A23. Musick, Kelly and Ann Meier. 2009. “Are Both Parents Always Better Than One? Parental Conflict and Young Adult Well-Being.” Social Science Research 39:814830. Oakes, Jeannie. 1985. Keeping Track: How Schools Structure Inequality. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Orr, Amy J. 2003. “Black-White Differences in Achievement: The Importance of Wealth.” Sociology of Education 76(4):281-304. Oseguera, Leticia, Gilberto Q. Conchas, and Eduardo Mosqueda. 2011. “Beyond Family and Ethnic Culture: Understanding Preconditions for the Potential Realization of Social Capital.” Youth Society 43(3):1136-1166. Phillips, Meredith, Mames Crouse, and James Ralph. 1998. “Does the Black-White Test Score Gap Widen After Children Enter School?” Pp. 229-272 in The Black-White Test Score Gap, edited by Christopher Jencks and Meredith Phillips. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press. Pong, Suet-Ling, Jaap Dronkers and Gillian Hampden. 2003 “Family Policies and Children’s School Achievement in Single and Two Parent Families.” Journal of Marriage and Family 6(8):681-699. 73 Ravitch, Diane. 2010. The Death and Life of the Great American School System: How Testing and Choice Are Undermining Education. New York, NY: Basic Books. Rist, Ray C. 1970. “Student Social Class and Teacher Expectations: The Self-Fulfilling Prophecy in Ghetto Education.” Harvard Educational Review 40(3):411-451. Roche, Kathleen M. and Sharon R. Ghazarian. 2011. “The Value of Family Routines for the Academic Success of Vulnerable Adolescents.” Journal of Family Issues 33(7):874-897. Roscigno, Vincent J. 1995. “The Social Embeddedness of Racial Educational Inequality: The Black-White Gap and the Impact of Racial and Local Political-Economic Contexts.” Research in Social Stratification and Mobility 14:137-168. _____. 1998. “Race and the Reproduction of Educational Disadvantage.” Social Forces 76(3):1033-1060. Rossi, Alice. 1984. “Gender and Parenthood.” American Sociological Review 49(1):1-19. Ricciuti, Henry N. 2004. “Single Parenthood, Achievement, and Problem Behavior in White, Black, and Hispanic Children.” The Journal of Educational Research 97(4):196-206. Smith, Judith R., Jeanne Brooks-Gunn, and Pamela Klebanov. 1997. “Consequences of Living in Poverty for Young Children’s Cognitive and Verbal Ability and Early School Achievement.” Pp. 132-189 in Consequences of Growing Up Poor, edited by Greg J. Duncan, and Jeanne Brooks-Gunn. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. 74 US Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. 2005. Early childhood longitudinal study, kindergarten class of 1998-1999. Psychometric report for the third grade. Washington, DC: Us Department of Education.