ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN MALTREATMENT AND EMOTION DYSREGULATION IN YOUNG CHILDREN A Thesis Presented to the faculty of the Department of Psychology California State University, Sacramento Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in Psychology by Deanna Kathryn Boys FALL 2013 ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN MALTREATMENT AND EMOTION DYSREGULATION IN YOUNG CHILDREN A Thesis by Deanna Kathryn Boys Approved by: __________________________________, Committee Chair Marya C. Endriga, Ph.D __________________________________, Second Reader Susan G. Timmer, Ph.D __________________________________, Third Reader Kelly A Cotter, Ph.D ____________________________ Date ii Student: Deanna Kathryn Boys I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. __________________________, Graduate Coordinator Jianjian. Qin, Ph.D Department of Psychology iii ___________________ Date Abstract of ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN MALTREATMENT AND EMOTION DYSREGULATION IN YOUNG CHILDREN by Deanna Kathryn Boys Children exposed to violence and abuse have been found to exhibit emotional problems such as high emotional reactivity, which is indicative of maladaptive emotional regulation processes. Young children with dysregulated emotions have exhibited emotional and behavioral problems that can lead to more severe problems and psychopathology in later childhood. The present study evaluated emotional reactivity and emotional regulation among 186 maltreated children as compared to 33 non-maltreated children. Consistent with study hypotheses, child emotional reactivity as rated by caregivers was significantly negatively related to observed emotion regulation during caregiver-child interaction. Contrary to expectations, however, non-maltreated children showed a sharper decline in emotion regulation than maltreated children in response to increasingly aversive play situations with their caregivers, which may be due to lower baseline levels of regulation in the maltreated group. iv Study results suggest that examining emotion regulatory processes during dyadic interaction in a maltreated population is useful for developing a fuller understanding of the construct in a relationship context. _______________________, Committee Chair Marya C. Endriga, Ph.D _______________________ Date v ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the support of many people. I would first like to express my deepest gratitude to my advisor, Dr. Endriga, for her constant encouragement, patience, gentle guidance and her ability to always be “Team Deanna” throughout this entire process. Her dedication to helping her students succeed has been inspirational. I would like to thank Dr. Timmer for first sparking the fire in my research flame and also for creating an atmosphere where I could actively pursue and research so many of my burning questions. She has a wonderful ability to always challenge me to look closer and think harder; I am a better person because of it. I would also like to thank Dr. Cotter, for always being so positive and encouraging, and for her insightful and constructive comments on multiple drafts; these were very helpful. To my entire committee: I could not have asked for a more supportive “Team Deanna,” or better role models. I am truly grateful for everything you have done for me. I would also like to thank everyone at the CAARE Center for offering me this opportunity to be a part of something important for the community. Also, without all of the hard work of the research assistants, this project would not have been possible. Lastly, (but not least), I would like to thank my family and friends for enduring this process with me. To my parents who are always so proud of me every step of the way; my sisters for being my partners in crime and to my brother for being my big brother. And to Lee Klemens for always being there for me no matter what. And to Rosie, who with her puppy dog eyes always let me know when it was time for bed. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Acknowledegments ............................................................................................................ vi List of Tables .................................................................................................................... ix List of Figures ..................................................................................................................... x Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................ 1 2. LITERATURE REVIEW ............................................................................................. 5 Emotion Regulation and Dysregulation .................................................................. 5 Theoretical Framework: Developmental Psychopathology .................................. 12 Effects of Maltreatment and Violence Exposure on Children .............................. 13 Effects of Maltreatment and Violence Exposure on Children’s Emotional Dysregulation ........................................................................................................ 14 3. METHOD ................................................................................................................... 20 4. RESULTS ................................................................................................................... 27 Preliminary Analyses: Demographic Differences in Emotional Reactivity. ........ 27 Hypothesis 1.1: Child Emotional Reactivity and its Relation to Emotion Dysregulation ........................................................................................................ 29 Hypothesis 1.2: Child Emotional Reactivity and Change in Child Emotion Dysregulation Across Play Situations ................................................................... 31 Hypothesis 2.1: Child Emotional Reactivity and Abuse and Violence Exposure 32 vii Hypothesis 2.2: Maltreatment History and Change in Responsiveness ............... 33 5. DISCUSSION ............................................................................................................. 37 Summary ............................................................................................................... 37 Emotional Reactivity and Emotion Regulation .................................................... 38 Emotion Regulation and Abuse and Violence History ......................................... 39 Limitations ............................................................................................................ 44 Conclusion ........................................................................................................... 44 Appendix A. Emotional Availability Coding Sheet ......................................................... 46 Appendix B. Child Behavior Checklist 1 ½ to 5 .............................................................. 49 References ......................................................................................................................... 50 viii LIST OF TABLES Tables Page 1. Demographics for Sample …………………….……………………………….28 2. Mean and Standard Deviations of Child Responsiveness in its relation to Emotional Reactivity…………………………………………………………..30 3. Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Child Responsiveness as a Function of Emotional Reactivity…………………….…………………………………….32 4. Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Child Responsiveness as a Function of Abuse History and Violence Exposure ……………………………………. …34 ix LIST OF FIGURES Figures 1. Page Child responsiveness across three behavioral analogs as a function of their abuse and violence exposure history. . .……………………………….36 x 1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION The ability to regulate one’s emotions is an essential part of the socio-emotional developmental process (Cicchetti & Toth, 1995). During infancy and early childhood, infants and young children are still developing the ability to modulate and control how they react emotionally and behaviorally to stressful situations. Young children depend on caregivers to help them learn how to manage the intensity and expression of their emotions (Cole, Michel, & Teti , 2004; Eisenberg & Morris, 2002; Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers, & Robinson 2007; Thompson, 1994). In order to provide comfort and security, caregivers respond to crying infants by soothing and holding (Thompson, 1994) or by diverting children’s attention away from the distressing situation (Gross & Thompson, 2001). Parents also reinforce their children’s positive emotional strivings by responding with praise and sensitivity to pro-social behaviors (Eisenberg & Morris, 2002; Morris et al., 2007). Caregivers serve as models by demonstrating appropriate expression of emotions, especially in taxing and ambiguous situations, by reinforcing appropriate emotional responses and instructing them about the meaning of the responses. (Carson & Parke, 1996; Eisenberg, Valiente, Morris et al., 2003). As children age, extrinsic regulations by others eventually internalize and children are increasingly able to use these learned strategies on their own (Eisenberg & Morris, 2002; Gross & Thompson, 2004). Thus, young children’s ability to regulate their own emotions is contingent upon parents’ 2 ability to recognize potentially difficult situations, children’s emotional states, and to help soothe them when they become dysregulated (Eisenberg & Morris, 2002; Eisenberg et al., 2003). For caregivers to be able to address and facilitate this social learning of appropriate expression and managing of emotions, they must be able to understand their children’s emotional cues (Eisenberg & Morris, 2002) and make themselves emotionally available (Robinson, Emde, & Korfmacher, 1999). When parents are unable to make themselves emotionally available or are possibly emotionally negative in their interactions with their children, this can hinder their children’s emotional self-regulation development (Martins, Soares, Martins, Tereno, & Osorio, 2012) . Children who have been abused or have witnessed inter-parental violence are more likely to engage in negative emotional interactions, making the chances of developing successful emotion regulation strategies less likely when their emotional experiences with their caregivers are filled with anxiety and emotional unpredictability (Carson & Parke, 1996; Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002; Sroufe, 1996). Additionally, compared to non-maltreated children, maltreated children have been found to exhibit higher negative emotional reactivity (Sturge-Apple, Davies, Cicchetti, & Manning, 2012). We know from other research that the more severe the emotional reaction, the harder it is for children to regulate their emotions (e.g., Little & Carter, 2005), and the greater the likelihood that they will use maladaptive emotion regulation strategies (Morris et al., 2007), putting them at greater risk for later psychopathology (Cicchetti & Toth, 2000; Keenan, 2000). 3 The present study evaluated how emotional reactivity and emotional regulation in the context of parent-child interactions are related to one another in a maltreated population. First, the relationship between caregiver report of their child’s emotional reactivity and the child’s observed responsiveness to the parent was examined. Children’s responsiveness to their parents was observed in three increasingly aversive behavioral observations as a dyadic measure of the child’s emotional regulation and the parent’s effectiveness in helping the child to cope with these distressing situations. Second, the relationships between children’s maltreatment history, their emotional reactivity, and their emotional responsiveness to their parents were examined. Statement of the Problem Adaptive and appropriate emotion regulation is a core component of healthy development (Cicchetti, 2006; Gross & Thompson, 2004; John & Gross, 2004), and emotional reactivity is a key indicator of children’s ability to self-regulate (Morris et al., 2007). When children are highly emotionally reactive, they show high vigilance to threat and have difficulty containing their anxiety, anger, and frustration (Achenbach, 2000). They show by their emotional reactivity to environmental triggers that they have difficulty regulating their emotions. Problems with emotion regulation are closely associated with psychopathology (Mennin, Holoway, Fresco, Moore, & Heimburg, 2007; Nolen-Hokesema, Stice, Wade, Bohon, 2007) such as anxiety-based disorders (Kasdan & Breen, 2008; Mennin et al., 2007), major depressive disorder (Nolen-Hokesema et al., 2008), borderline personality disorder (Linehan, 1993; Putnam & Silk, 1995), alcohol and 4 substance abuse issues (Gratz & Tull, 2009) and eating disorders (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2007). During early childhood, the inability to regulate emotions appropriately or the use of maladaptive ways to control emotional expression can occur as an adaptation to poor, abusive or neglectful parenting (Cicchetti, 2006). Children who have experienced trauma or have been maltreated may have developed emotion regulation strategies that were adaptive in a dysfunctional family setting, but maladaptive when used in other contexts, contributing to difficulties in their socio-emotional development (Cicchetti & Lynch, 1995). With emotion regulatory problems often a function of high emotional reactivity (Little & Carter, 2005) and being at the forefront of problems that can persist in to adulthood, it is important to gain understanding into the ways maltreatment and violence exposure affect children’s emotional self-regulation. 5 Chapter 2 LITERATURE REVIEW Emotion Regulation and Dysregulation Emotion regulation has been described as the ability to respond to the ongoing requirements of experience with a pattern of emotions that is socially acceptable and reasonably flexible enough to allow variation in reactions as well as the ability to delay these reactions when necessary (Cole et al., 1994). Individuals vary significantly in how they manage and express their emotions. It is the norm for individuals to vary in their expression of their emotional patterns, either by type or intensity; however, in some settings emotional patterns can affect functioning and become associated with psychopathology (e.g., Cole et al., 1994). Emotion dysregulation can occur when management of emotional patterns operates in a dysfunctional way so that functioning is compromised (Cole et al., 1994). Dysregulated emotions in children might present as over-controlled or under-controlled depending on the situation in which they are viewed (Cole, Martin & Dennis, 2004; Cole et al., 1994). With under-controlled emotions, children exhibit an amplified negative reaction (Martin, et al., 2012). For example, undercontrolled emotions might be evident in children who present with an initial exaggerated negative emotional response to a disappointment (Liew, Eisenberg, & Riser, 2003) or an unpredictable fluctuation between calm and negativity with little provocation (e.g., Cole et al., 2003). 6 The construct of emotion regulation and dysregulation is complex and difficult to measure. As such, different paradigms exist to evaluate emotion dysregulation that depend on the age of the child and the exact variables under study. Most paradigms are observational, involving a series of shifts in conditions to elicit changes in emotions. For example, the “still-face” paradigm (Tronick, Als, Adamson, Wise, & Brazelton, 1978) uses a dyadic interaction between mothers and their very young infants in three separate situations to measure changes in the infant’s emotions in relation to changes in the mother’s emotional availability. In the first phase of the task, mothers engage with their infants in a free play interaction. Next, the caregiver assumes a neutral expression and remains unresponsive, keeping her face completely still, although oriented toward the infant. The infant usually responds to this change by trying to reengage by vocalizing and gesturing to the caregiver. Typically, the infant becomes distressed when the mother fails to respond, which provides an opportunity to observe emotion regulation and the infant’s attempts to regulate negative affect. In the last phase, mothers are instructed to provide comfort if necessary and resume face-to-face play. Infants’ emotional responses to the mother are used to evaluate their regulation (Weinbug & Tronick, 1996). Little and Carter (2005) used play situations to stimulate emotional reactivity in infants while measuring the infants’ emotion regulation. Caregivers were told to engage in a free play situation with their infants. After completion of this situation infants were then evaluated during a challenge condition that should elicit infant emotional reactivity by restricting their ability to move their arms and legs. Next, infants were placed in a 7 condition that would prompt the infant to engage in emotional self-regulatory behaviors (i.e., self-soothing), by removing the restraints, but not allowing the child to have any social contact. Lastly, infants were then allowed to be soothed by an experimenter and then a caregiver. This paradigm was designed to provoke emotionally reactive responses from the infants, but then provide them with opportunities to self-regulate, comfort themselves and also receive comfort their caregiver. The researchers measured how infants reacted emotionally (negatively), whether they attempted to regulate their reactions (fuss or immediate intense cry), and used the Emotional Availability Scales (Biringen, 2000) to evaluate infants’ emotional responsiveness. A significant relationship was found between infant emotional reactivity responsiveness and regulation attempts. Infants who presented with more negative initial reactions to a challenge and less emotional responsiveness towards their caregiver took longer than other infants to calm themselves during those distressing situations. This research demonstrated that more severe emotional reactions predicted more trouble regulating emotions. Furthermore these preliminary findings outline the importance of assessing emotional regulation within the caregiver-child relationship (Little & Carter, 2005). Emotional expression after infancy has been measured using a task to elicit disappointment (Cole, Zahn-Waxler, & Smith, 1994; Saarni, 1984). In the “disappointment task” paradigm, children are told that they will receive a desired prize after completing some tasks. Instead, children receive a broken item (e.g., a broken pencil). Researchers code children’s responses to this disappointment for discreet 8 displays of affect, other behavioral responses and active engagement in regulation strategies. Children who were able to effectively regulate their emotional reactions during this task were able to “mask” their disappointment when in the presence of an experimenter, i.e., socially acceptable emotion regulation. However, some children displayed dysregulated emotions, such as disappointment, anger and frustration, i.e., socially unacceptable emotion regulation (Cole, Zahn-Waxler, & Smith, 1994). Parent report measures of children’s emotion regulation such as the Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC: Shields & Cicchetti, 1997) have also been previously used and validated. On the ERC, parents rate how often children are able to regulate emotions appropriately and how often they show lability and dysregulated negativity. The ERC has demonstrated validity with observed emotion regulation. However, reliance on caregiver report measures of behavioral and emotional problems can be problematic. When using caregivers’ observations and ratings of their children’s behavior, it is important to remember that caregivers can often have their own motivations and biases for providing a rating of their children’s behavior especially in regards to what represents abnormal behavior (De Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2005). Even when including additional reporters such as the children themselves or teachers, ratings of children’s behavioral problems can still be discrepant (De Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2005). In an extensive literature review of 119 studies, Achenbach, McConaughy and Howell (1987) found that when rating the same child’s behavioral problems, different informants (e.g., parent, teacher, etc.) had significantly discrepant ratings. These findings have been replicated in clinical samples 9 (Grills & Ollendick, 2002) and have also been found when measuring emotion regulation (Hourigan, Goodman, & Southam-Gerow, 2011). Hourigan and colleagues (2011) found inconsistencies between both the parent’s view and the child’s view of the child’s emotional regulation in relation to the children’s internalizing and externalizing behavior problems. School-age children were asked to evaluate their own emotion regulation and then their reports were compared to parent’s reports of the child’s emotion regulation using the Children’s Emotional Management Scales (CEMS) and the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL). Children who presented with more internalizing behavior problems were more likely to report more discordant expression of sadness and anger when compared to their parents’ reports of their emotional regulation. However, when children had externalizing behavior problems and high emotional reactivity, parents reported them as exhibiting more dysregulated anger than what their child reported. The authors concluded that children act differently and express different emotional patterns within differing contexts and with different people. They posited that their findings demonstrated the complex nature of emotion regulation, and suggest using parent report in addition to observational assessments when examining emotion regulation (Hourigan et al., 2011). Infants and young children are highly reliant on their caregivers to provide appropriate emotional socialization and regulatory support (Eisenberg, Cumberland, & Spinrad, 1998). Infants are quite limited in their behavioral repertoire (Gross & Thompson, 2004; Little & Carter, 2005), especially in response to their need to maintain 10 optimal levels of arousal, calming, soothing and reassurance (van der Kolk & Fisler, 1994). In a typically developing child, a small disappointment or distressing situation might elicit a small display of negative affect, but the child is able to either be redirected by a caregiver or may redirect her or his own attention to a new stimulus. When the emotion regulatory system fails to develop or becomes overwhelmed, emotional dysregulation can be an inevitable consequence. During negative emotional experiences, children feel emotions like fear, anger, or frustration that push them away from a neutral state of emotions. Once the child becomes overwhelmed with emotion, he or she may not be able to self-soothe, redirect themselves or otherwise respond to the situation effectively (Cole et al., 1994; Little & Carter, 2005). Children who demonstrate emotional dysregulation might also exhibit a high amount of under-controlled, negative emotional reactivity, e.g., displaying an initial negative response to a stimulus (Little & Carter, 2005), or show an inappropriate or incongruous emotional responses relative to the stimuli (Kim & Cicchetti, 2010). In emotionally charged and stressful situations, children who exhibit high amounts of emotional reactivity have a difficult time being able to return to a calm state (Eisenberg et al., 1998). Furthermore, this difficulty returning to a calm state may be exacerbated by insensitive and detrimental parenting practices caused by the caregivers’ own inability to manage their emotions (Carrere & Bowie, 2012). 11 Emotional Reactivity and Emotional Regulation While emotional reactivity and emotional regulation are separate, they are inherently intertwined. “Emotional reactivity” is the term that describes the sensitivity and intensity of emotional reactions to situations or events, while “emotional regulation” describes the process of attempting to manage or reduce the intensity of initial and subsequent emotional reactions. Stifter and Jain (1996) point out that emotional reactivity and emotion regulation are interdependent. In a longitudinal study of infant emotion reactivity and regulation, they examined infants in a frustration task (i.e., toy removal). At 5 months, emotional reactivity during the frustration task predicted the presence of regulatory behaviors such as self-soothing at 10 months and at 18 months during the same task. The authors posited that in infants, alertness and reactive emotional awareness is a trait during infancy that is adaptive and helps infants try to engage in behaviors to remove aversive stimuli from their environments. However as children age, emotional reactivity, especially negative emotional reactivity, becomes less appropriate and maladaptive especially within social interactions (Cole et al., 2004). Additionally, children’s emotion regulation is often occupied with the modulation of negative emotional expressivity and helping children return to a calm state after an initial reaction (Little & Carter, 2005). As a result, the magnitude of the reaction, whether it be appropriate or inappropriate, is indicative of the effectiveness of the emotional regulatory system. When there is a highly negative emotionally reactive response, there are still emotion regulation processes at work (Cole et al., 1994). Learning and developing an 12 appropriate strategy for how to react to emotion-eliciting experiences and successfully maneuvering toward an appropriate and adaptive response is especially important for children’s development (Eisenberg et al., 2002) as it affects later social and behavioral functioning (Calkins & Fox, 2002). Theoretical Framework: Developmental Psychopathology In order to understand the diverse and complex pathways of children’s emotional development, it is advantageous to take a developmental psychopathology perspective. Developmental psychopathology draws from multiple disciplines including neurology, embryology, psychiatry and psychology (Cicchetti, 1990). The integration of these fields provides a more thorough understanding of multiple developmental processes and domains (Chaffin, 2006). Furthermore, developmental psychopathology focuses on how behaviors can become maladaptive when they diverge from their optimal developmental trajectories or pathways (Cicchetti & Toth, 1994; Sroufe & Rutter, 1984), which is important for understanding emotional dysregulation (Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002). The concepts of equifinality and multifinality are useful for understanding trajectories in child development and explaining how emotional regulation develops differently (Shields & Cicchetti, 1998). Equifinality assumes the same developmental problems or disorders can stem from different factors (Cicchetti & Toth, 2008; Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1996; Sroufe, 1989). For example, both physical and sexual abuse have been linked to depression in children (Cicchetti & Valentino, 2006). Conversely, multifinality proposes that risks and protective factors will combine to create different outcomes for different children 13 (Cicchetti & Toth, 2008). To illustrate, children who have been sexually abused may go on to develop social and emotional disorders of different types or may not develop a disorder at all (Kendall-Tackett, Williams & Finkelhor, 1993). This may also be true for psychological phenomena outside of specific disorders, such as emotional dysregulation. Equifinality would propose that many different circumstances, including violence exposure and abuse, could result in a child who has difficulty understanding and managing their own emotions when stressed (Shields & Cicchetti, 1998). Children’s prior negative experiences would make it difficult to return back to an emotional state of equilibrium. As a result, children might drift away from a healthy developmental trajectory. Their early difficulty managing their emotional reactions could affect how they deal with later stressful social interactions (Shields & Cicchetti, 1998). However, multifinality would suggest that at least some children with histories of violence exposure or abuse will not develop difficulties with emotion regulation. Effects of Maltreatment and Violence Exposure on Children Research has consistently documented the wide ranging effects that exposure to abuse and violence has on children (for review see Kitzman et al., 2003). After the child has left the stressful or traumatic environment, the effects of the trauma manifest themselves in many different ways and do not always develop in the same ways for each child (Cicchetti & Valentino, 2006). Links have been found between maltreatment and problems with children’s neurological functioning (Heim et al., 2008), biological and physiological developmental processes (Alink, Cicchetti, Kim, & Rogosch, 2012; 14 Cicchetti, Rogosch, Gunnar, & Toth, 2010; Zalewski et al. 2012), cognitive functioning (Shonk & Cicchetti, 2001) emotional issues (Alink et al. 2009; Toth et al. 1992) and internalizing and externalizing behavior problems (Manly et al., 2001; Rogosch & Cicchetti, 1994). Additionally, research has documented that children exposed to violence exhibit similar social and emotional problems to children who were directly abused (Timmer, Thompson, Culver, Urquiza, & Altenhoffen, 2012 & Martin, 2012). For example, abused and violence-exposed children have exhibited physical aggression, negative affect and noncompliant behaviors (Chaffin, 2006; Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1994; Shields & Cicchetti, 1998; Urquiza & McNeil, 1996). Furthermore, as children mature, research has shown that both groups of children also tend to exhibit more withdrawn behaviors (Haskett & Kistner, 1991) and more disorganized and labile emotions than children who had not been maltreated (Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002; Davies et al., 2012). Effects of Maltreatment and Violence Exposure on Children’s Emotional Dysregulation Families from violence-ridden or abusive environments have been shown to experience more stress than typical non-maltreating families (Fantuzzo, Boruch, Beriama, Atkins, & Marcus, 1997). For example, exchanges between parents and children who have experienced abuse or violence tend to be less positive and more negative than their non-abusing counterparts (Bousha & Twentyman, 1984). Small stressors, such as disappointments or disagreements serve as triggers to larger conflicts which, in turn, provide children poor models of emotional and behavioral responses (Carrere & Bowie, 15 2012). In a comprehensive review of the development of emotion regulation, Morris and colleagues (2007) argued that children’s development of emotion regulation occurs as a result of children’s observations of their close family. These include observations of parenting practices, emotional climates, and the emotional reactions of the caregivers. When the caregiving environment is impoverished or harmful, such as with physical abuse or in households with domestic violence, children’s emotional regulation capacities are more likely to be non-optimal (Davies et al., 2012; Manly et al, 2001). In these cases, children are unlikely to observe appropriate emotional responses (Carson & Parke, 1996) or experience healthy and positive parenting practices (Cerezo, D’Ocon, & Dolz, 1996; Shipman & Zeman, 1999). Previous research has documented how some young maltreated children demonstrate more anger and reactivity (Alessandri, 1991; Shields & Cicchetti, 1998), blunted emotional expression (Gaensbauer, 1980) and the inability to properly recognize emotions (Pollak, Cicchetti, Hornung, & Reed, 2000) as compared with non-maltreated children. Furthermore, studies have examined how in response to conflict, abused children and children exposed to violence in the home exhibit dysregulated emotion patterns (Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002) and more emotional reactivity (Davies et al., 2012). Taken together, these findings display a pattern that demonstrates how children are emotionally and behaviorally impaired by abuse and violence exposure. These findings also reveal the importance of the caregiver-child relationship in relation to young children’s emotion regulation within the context of a maltreating environment. 16 Maltreated children are often exposed to these harsh and detrimental experiences by their caregivers (Chaffin 2006). If children are exposed to harmful events and experience negative unpredictable caregiving (Morris et al., 2007), one might expect that children’s emotional difficulties would be most apparent in observations with the caregiver. Just as caregivers who are maltreating have difficulty being emotionally available (Martins et al., 2012) and emotionally responsive (Eisenberg, Fabes, Shepard, Guthrie, Murphy, & Reiser, 1999), children might also demonstrate less emotional responsiveness towards their caregivers. A previous study (Little & Carter, 2005), found that child responsiveness was related to infants’ reactive emotions and their emotional self-regulation attempts in distressing situations, such that infants who were less emotionally responsive to their caregivers and more emotionally reactive took longer to regulate their emotions and return to a calm state after a distressing situation. Based on this evidence, young children’s responsiveness towards their caregivers in an aversive situation might be related to young children’s emotional reactivity. Additionally, even when children respond in a highly emotionally reactive manner, there are still emotional regulatory processes that occur, even if they appear dysregulated (Cole et al., 2003). If there is a relationship between children’s emotional reactivity and their responsiveness to their parents, then based on previous research (e.g., Little & Carter, 2005), one might expect that when children experience changing situations that increase in aversion, the change in how they emotionally react and respond to their caregivers might reflect children’s emotion regulation attempts. Given that maltreated children are products of such an 17 impoverished caregiving environment (Carson & Parke, 1996; Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002; Morris et al., 2007; Sroufe, 1996), it would be important to gain understanding of how these children change the way they interact emotionally with their caregivers and how children are able to emotionally navigate aversive or distressing situations. Purpose of the Current Study Previous research has pointed out the deleterious effect of child maltreatment on their social and emotional development; in particular,. abusive and violent environments may lead to emotional regulation difficulties in the young child. To examine young children’s emotional regulation, past research methodologies have used both caregiver report and observations of children, but few have looked at children that have been exposed to abusive and violent environments (Shields & Cicchetti, 1998; Little & Carter, 2005), with even fewer that have evaluated how maltreated children are able to regulate their emotions within a caregiver-child observation (e.g., Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002). By observing the caregiver-child relationship, we can gain further understanding of the mechanisms by which abuse and a violent home environment can affect young children’s emotion regulation. Based on previous methodology and construct conceptualization, if children’s emotion regulation attempts are evident in a caregiver-child observation, it could be observable two possible ways: A single aversive situation or the change from a nonaversive to an aversive situation. In a single aversive situation with their caregivers, young children might present with an emotional response to the caregiver that will be 18 representative of underlying emotion regulatory processes. Alternately, it might require a change in aversive situations to be able to observe emotion regulation in a caregiver-child observation. Evaluating the quality of children’s responses to their caregivers in situations varying in stressfulness to the child, and measuring the degree of the shift in child’s responsiveness might be an effective way of measuring children’s emotional regulation. The present study seeks to evaluate these claims using observational and caregiver report assessments. The current study examined the relationship between emotional reactivity and emotional regulation in young children. Additionally, the study evaluated the effect of violence and abuse on a child’s observed emotional regulation. Children’s emotional reactivity, as reported by their caregiver was compared to observed responsiveness in specific aversive play scenarios with their caregivers. Children’s observed responsiveness to their parents in increasingly aversive play situations, as well as the change in responsiveness to the parent from situation to situation was used as a measure of children’s emotion regulation. Secondly the study compared how abused and/or exposed to violence children differed from non-abused and/or exposed to violence children in emotional reactivity and regulation. Hypotheses Aim 1: To investigate the connection between emotional reactivity and emotion regulation and assess how parent ratings of their children’s emotional reactivity are 19 related to observations of children’s responsiveness in an increasingly aversive semistructured play situation. Hypothesis 1.1: Children who are rated by their caregivers as more emotionally reactive will demonstrate more emotional dysregulation as measured by low children’s responsiveness to their caregivers. Hypothesis 1.2: Compared with children with low to moderate emotional reactivity,, children who are rated as having high emotional reactivity will demonstrate more emotional dysregulation as measured by a significant decrease in children’s responsiveness to their caregivers across increasingly aversive play situations. Aim 2: To investigate the connection between emotional dysregulation, emotional reactivity and exposure to violence and abuse history of children in observations of increasingly aversive play situations with their caregivers. Hypothesis 2.1: Children who have experienced abuse or have been exposed to violence will be rated as more emotionally reactive by their caregivers. Hypothesis 2.2: Compared to children with no violence exposure or abuse history, children exposed to violence or with an abuse history will demonstrate more emotional dysregulation as measured by a significant decrease in children’s responsiveness to their caregivers in increasingly aversive play situations. 20 Chapter 3 METHOD Participants Participants were 219 children and their biological mothers who are part of an research database from a larger, ongoing study of the effectiveness of Parent Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT) for maltreated children: Thirty two had no abuse or violence exposure history and 186 had a suspected or documented history or violence exposure through inter-parental violence or physical abuse. Participants were referrals to a Northern California university-based medical center and child abuse treatment clinic from February 2003 to October 2012. In particular, most of the children were referred because of their disruptive behaviors and the caregiver’s inability to effectively parent and to reduce the risk of child abuse and neglect. Only children eligible to receive MediCal insurance and were treated at this clinic. Participants were included in this study if they were between 3 to 5 years of age (mean age = 4.00, SD = 1.14), participating with their biological mothers in PCIT, and consented to participate in research. Almost 60% of the sample children were male (59.8%). Approximately 48.9% of the mothers 42.4% of the children were White/Non-Hispanic, 20.1% of mothers and 28.6% of children were African American, 19.6% of mothers and 24.1% of children were Latino and 11.4% of mothers and 4.9% of children categorize themselves as “Other.” The mother’s average age was 28.17 (SD = 5.86). Mother’s had an average of 11.51 years of education (SD = 1.95). Additionally, 73.9% of the mothers considered themselves single mothers. Risk 21 history for the children was also evaluated. Around 71.2% of children had been placed in foster care and 85.4% having experienced at least one type of maltreatment (e.g., physical abuse, sexual abuse, inter-parental violence). Procedure If a participant was eligible for PCIT, therapists obtained a signed consent form (as approved from the UC Davis’ Internal Review Board) from the clients’ legal guardian. In a pre-treatment assessment, parents received a battery of standardized measures and a demographic questionnaire; these standardized measures address issues with the child as well as measures that are focused on stress and issues faced by the parent. These measures were given to the mother at the community clinic, who filled them after the first appointment and returned them before beginning treatment. Observational Assessment. Before PCIT began, a pre-treatment observational assessment was conducted. The assessment consisted of three 5 minute semi-structured interaction, which were analogs of typical parent-child interactions and designed to elicit differing amounts of parental control and child compliance. The first 5 minutes (Child Directed Interaction; CDI) was devoted to allowing the child to pick a toy to play with their caregiver and for the caregiver to follow the child’s lead in play. The second 5 (Parent Directed Interaction; PDI) minutes was structured so that the caregiver has to change the current game or activity and have the child follow their lead in play. The transition between CDI and PDI required the parent to move from no control over the play to exercising control and try to get the child to play along with their game and their 22 rules. The third 5 minutes (Clean Up; CU) was arranged so that the caregiver had to have the child clean up without their help. The caregiver had to get the child to comply with their commands as well as end the play. As each observation section progressed, the caregiver had to assert more control over the child. Additionally as each observation section progressed, this offered an opportunity to observe how the child reacted to escalating control from the parent. These recorded observations were then coded by the undergraduate and graduate level reliable coders. Measures Observational Coding: Emotional Availability Scales (EAS; 3rd Ed.). The Emotional Availability Scales (EAS; Biringen, 2000) (See Appendix A) were used to assess children’s emotional regulation capabilities and thus their emotional dysregulation during a semi-structured observational assessment of mother-child interaction . Emotional dysregulation has been measured by assessing the intensity of children’s positive and negative affect in response to a changing situation with an aversive stimulus and also noting how long it takes them to recover to a neutral emotional state (e.g., Ellis, Beavers, Wells, 2009; Gratz, Rosenthal, Tull, Lejuez & Gunderson 2010). The changes in the intensity of children’s affective responses to their parents in play situations is one way to measure their emotional dysregulation. Additionally, due to the fact that this study used archival data, many of the assessments of emotion regulation and dysregulation were not available or feasible to use retroactively, so the Emotional Availability Scales (EAS) will serve as a way to measure of emotional dysregulation. 23 The EAS is an observational coding system that evaluates the quality of the parent child relationship. There are four parent scales: sensitivity, non-hostility, nonintrusiveness and structuring. For the child, there are only two: responsiveness and involvement. For this particular study only on the child responsiveness scale was used as an indicator of the child’s emotional dysregulation in an observational setting. Child responsiveness reflects the child’s ability to be responsive to their parent in an “affectively available way.” (Biringen, 1999, pg.107) including their emotional responsiveness to the caregiver’s bids and their emotional connection to the caregiver (Biringen, 1999) ( Scores on the child-responsiveness of the EAS range in 1-unit increments from 7 to 1.) For this study, scores will be examined as individual discrete scores in each analog. Higher scores will indicate that the child is more positive and emotionally available to the parent; lower scores will indicate a child who is observed to be more negative and less emotionally available. Coders were doctoral level researchers, undergraduate and graduate level students in psychology or human development who have received extensive training in Emotional Availability coding and procedures. Two coders coded each tape and agreed on 85% of their codes; any disagreements in codes were resolved through a discussion, and if needed, a third coder was brought in to resolve any discrepancy. Additionally, 13 of the tapes were recoded and evaluated using intra-class coefficients to determine sufficient reliability and address any coder-drift. Reliability coefficients for each category are as follows: Parental Sensitivity, r = .97, Parental Sensitivity, r = .92, Parental Intrusiveness, 24 r = .88, Parental Structuring, r = .88, Child Responsiveness, r = .93 and Child Involvement, r = .91. Each analog (i.e., CDI, PDI & CU) of the pre-observation assessment was coded separately, allowing the examination of change in EA from analog to analog. Emotional Reactivity. The child’s emotional reactivity as rated by the caregiver was measured using Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL 11/2 – 5; Achenbach, 2000). Emotional Reactivity is a between subjects variable that represents parent reports of children’s emotional reactive behaviors on the Emotionally Reactive Syndrome Scale on the CBCL 1 ½ - 5 (Achenbach, 2000). The CBCL (Achenbach, 2000) (See Appendix B) is a standardized parent report measure that lists 100 child behavior problems. Caregivers as asked to rate the frequency of their child’s behavior on a 3 point scale (0= never, 2=often). On the CBCL, there are multiple smaller subscales addressing specific child behavior problem groups; these are called syndrome scales. For this particular study, the Emotionally Reactive Syndrome Scale will be used as a measure of the child’s emotion dysregulation. The scale examines parent’s report of their child’s emotionally reactive behaviors and questions about whether the child is disturbed by change, has sudden mood changes and other internalizing issues. For the purposes of this study, this variable will be examined both as a continuous indictor of children’s overall emotional reactivity and also categorically. To increase the clinical relevance of these analyses, the participants were divided into three levels of emotional reactivity: The Low category includes those with T-scores 54 and below, the Medium category includes those with T-scores 55 to 64 and 25 the High category includes those with T-scores of 65 or higher representing borderline and clinical levels of emotional reactivity symptoms. This three-level categorization allows easy comparison of children scoring in the Borderline and Clinical range (High scores) and those well within the normal range (Low scores). Abuse and Violence Exposure History. Information about each child’s history of abuse and violence exposure was obtained from therapist, social worker and court reports that were present in the client files. For purposes of this study, abuse and violence exposure signifies a child experiencing physical abuse or exposure to violence. When there was information suggesting that a child may have been abused or exposed to violence, the research staff coded “suspected abuse or violence.” When there is a substantiated report of abuse or exposure to violence, the research staff coded “documented abuse or violence.” If there was no evidence in any of the client file of abuse or violence exposure, we coded “no abuse or violence history.” For the purposes of this study, a dichotomous indicator of child abuse and/or violence history was created with categories of No Abuse and/or Violence History (zero reports of suspected or documented abuse and/or violence) and Abuse and/or Violence History (one or more reports of suspected or documented abuse and/or violence history). The two groups of suspected and documented were combined to guard against any possibility that a child in the No Abuse and/or Violence History group would have had some exposure, which could confound the variable. Previous research has documented the effectiveness of combining both suspected and documented into one group (Timmer et al., 2012). 26 Demographics Information. All participants filled out forms addressing demographic information assessing parent and child gender, age, racial/ethnic background, as well as parent marital status and education. 27 Chapter 4 RESULTS Preliminary Analyses: Demographic Differences in Emotional Reactivity To determine if significant demographic differences existed among the three emotional reactivity groups (low, medium, high), analysis of variance tests were conducted with continuous variables and chi-square tests of independence were conducted with categorical variables. Results are presented in Table 1 and show that no significant differences in children’s age, sex, and ethnicity existed between the three groups. Table 1 also shows that mothers’ age, ethnicity, educational attainment, and marital status does not differ significantly by emotional reactivity group. Chi-square tests of independence were performed to determine if any demographic differences existed among the three emotional reactivity groups with regard to their maltreatment and violence exposure history. There were no significant differences between the presence/absence of maltreatment and violence exposure across the three reactivity groups, χ² (6, N= 219) =7.82, p = .25. All emotional reactivity groups had similarly high percentages of children who had experienced abuse or violence: 81.8%, 86.5% and 89.5%, for the low, medium, and high emotional reactivity groups, respectively. 28 Table 1 Demographics for Sample Characteristic Child Mean Age (SD) Percent Male Low Emotional Reactivity (n=88) Medium Emotional Reactivity (n=74) High Emotional Reactivity (n=57) 4.04 (1.26) 3.92 (1.07) 4.07 (1.00) F (2, 218) .35, p = .70 60.2% 58.1% 61.4% χ² (2, N= 219) =.16, p = .92 Ethnicity Effects χ² (6, N= 203) = 5.78, p = .45 Caucasian Latino/a African American Other Mother Mean Age (SD) 41.7% 37.9% 49.1% 25.0% 24.2% 22.6% 25.0% 34.8% 26.4% 8.3% 3.0% 1.9% 28.84 (6.68) 28.16 (5.09) 27.13 (5.36) F (2, 217) 1.46, p = .23 χ² (6, N= 219) = 2.75, p = .83 Ethnicity Caucasian Latino/a African American Other Education Mean Years (SD) Marital Status (% single) 50.0% 43.2% 54.4% 18.2% 21.6% 21.1% 19.3% 21.6% 17.5% 12.5% 13.5% 7.0% 11.47 (1.87) 11.56 (1.94) 11.52 (2.14) 72.7% 71.6% 78.6% F (2, 213) .04, p = .96 χ² (2, N= 218) = .89, p = .64 Note. Emotional Reactivity= Emotional Reactivity Syndrome Scale scores. Low Emotional Reactivity = TScore 54 and below, Some Emotional Reactivity = T-Score 55 to 64, High Emotional Reactivity = T-Score 65 and above. 29 Hypothesis 1.1: Child Emotional Reactivity and its Relation to Emotion Dysregulation To see if children’s emotional reactivity was related to children’s emotion dysregulation as measured by their observed responsiveness to their mothers in the three separate analogs (i.e., CDI, PDI, CU), a multivariate analysis of variance was conducted using overall emotional reactivity (as measured by the CBCL) as a covariate on child responsiveness (as measured by the EAS) in the three separate play situations of CDI, PDI and CU. All 219 participants were included with no missing data on the outcome measures. Results of evaluations of assumptions of normality, homogeneity of variancecovariance matrices, linearity and multicollinearity were satisfactory. With the use of Wilks’s Lambda, results showed that there was a statistically significant overall multivariate main effect, F (2, 214) = 3.44, p = .018, partial η2 = .046 of emotional reactivity on the observed child responsiveness. When results for the dependent variables were considered separately, two statistically significant differences were observed. Univariate ANOVAs demonstrated that there was a significant effect of emotional reactivity on observed child responsiveness in Parent Directed Interaction, F (2, 214) = 5.99, p = .02, and in Clean up, F (2, 214) = 7.50, p = .00. No statistically significant effect was found for observed child responsiveness in Child Directed Interaction, F (2, 214) = .13, p = .72. Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 2. 30 Table 2 Mean and Standard Deviations of Child Responsiveness in its relation to Emotional Reactivity Emotional Reactivity Observational Analog CDI PDI CU M SD 4.50 3.83* 3.25* 1.04 1.04 1.09 Note. CDI= Child Directed Interaction, PDI = Parent Directed Interaction, CU= Clean Up, Child Responsiveness = Child Responsiveness scales of Emotional Availability Scales (Biringen, 2000). *p< .05 In order to detect the direction of the relationship between reactivity and responsiveness in each of the analogs separately, Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients were computed for overall caregiver-reported emotional reactivity and observed child responsiveness in CDI, PDI and CU. Results showed a significant negative relationship between emotional reactivity and child responsiveness in Parent Directed Interaction and, (r = -.16, n = 22, p = .02) and in Clean up (r = -.18, n = 22, p = .00). However, there was no correlation between emotional reactivity and responsiveness during Child Directed Interaction (r = -.02, n = 22, p = .76). Taken together, the results suggest that as children who were rated as more reactive were likely to less responsive to their parents in more aversive situations. These findings lend some validity to parents’ ratings of children’s emotional reactivity. This also lends some support to using Child Responsiveness as a proxy for emotional reactivity, and variation across the three play situations as an indicator of emotional regulation. 31 Hypothesis 1.2: Child Emotional Reactivity and Change in Child Emotion Dysregulation Across Play Situations In order to assess whether change in children’s responsiveness from non-aversive to more aversive play situations is an indicator of emotional regulation, the differences across analogs in children’s responsiveness to their parents was examined by levels of emotional reactivity. A 3 (Emotional Reactivity Level; Low, Medium, High) X 3 (Observational Analog; CDI, PDI, CU) repeated measures multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted with the dependent variable of emotional dysregulation (as measured by Child Responsiveness). There was a total N of 219, with no cases missing. Additionally, results of evaluations of assumptions of normality, homogeneity of variance-covariance matrices, linearity and multicollinearity were satisfactory. With the use of Wilks’s Lambda, results showed that there was a significant overall multivariate main effect, F (2, 213) = 35.78, p = .00, partial η2 = .25, of observational analog on child responsiveness and a non-significant multivariate interaction of observational analog and emotional reactivity on child responsiveness, F (4, 426) = 1.50, p = .20. Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 3. ) Results showed that children’s responsiveness to their parents decreased as the aversiveness of the task increased, and that the more emotionally reactive parents judged their children to be, the less responsiveness they were to their parents, but that all groups showed similar decreases across analogs. 32 Table 3 Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Child Responsiveness as a Function of Emotional Reactivity Low Emotional Medium Emotional High Emotional Reactivity Reactivity Reactivity (n=88) (n=74) (n=57) M SD M 4.56 .89 4.46 1.15 4.48 1.09 PDI 4.05 .95 3.77 1.13 3.59 .93 CU 3.56 1.06 3.05 1.03 3.04 1.11 Observational Analog CDI SD M SD Note. Emotional Reactivity = Scores Emotional Reactive Syndrome Scale on Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach, 2000), CDI= Child Directed Interaction, PDI = Parent Directed Interaction, CU= Clean Up, Child Responsiveness = Child Responsiveness scales of Emotional Availability Scales (Biringen, 2000). Hypothesis 2.1: Child Emotional Reactivity and Abuse and Violence Exposure In order to test the hypothesis that children exposed to more abuse and violence would demonstrate more emotional reactivity when rated by their caregivers, an Analysis of Variance was computed, with emotional reactivity as the dependent variable and exposure to violence (Violence/Abuse History; 0 counts or 1 or more counts). as the between-subjects independent variable. The results of this analysis showed a nonsignificant trend for non-abused or violence exposed children to be rated as less reactive (Mean = 56.03, SD = 6.90) than abused and violence exposed children (Mean = 58.77, SD = 8.80; F(1, 217) = 2.78, p = .10). The results of this analysis demonstrates that children who have experienced abuse and had been exposed to violence were not rated as significantly more emotionally reactive by their caregivers. 33 Hypothesis 2.2: Abuse History and Violence Exposure and Change in Responsiveness To test the hypothesis that children who have experienced abuse and been exposed to violence would demonstrate a greater magnitude of change in child responsiveness from CDI to PDI to Clean UP, possibly indicating more emotional dysregulation, a repeated measures multivariate analysis of variance was conducted. Child Responsiveness was the repeated measure across the three behavioral analogs (Observational Analog; CDI, PDI, CU), and Exposure to violence was the Between Subjects factor (Violence/Abuse History; 0 counts or 1 or more counts) repeated measures multivariate analysis of variance was conducted on child responsiveness across the three play analogs (CDI to PDI to CU). There was a total N of 218, with one case missing. Additionally, results of evaluations of assumptions of normality, homogeneity of variance-covariance matrices, linearity and multicollinearity were satisfactory. With the use of Wilks’s Lambda, results indicated there was a significant main effect of behavioral analog on children’s responsiveness to their parents F (2, 212) = 41.13, p = .00, partial η2 = .28, and a statistically significant interaction between analog and exposure to violence on children’s responsiveness F (2, 212) = 7.12, p = .00, partial η2 = .06, suggesting that the magnitude of the slopes indicating change in Child Responsiveness across the three analogs differed by violence exposure (See Table 4). To isolate specific abuse/violence group differences in the change in responsiveness across the three observational analogs, I performed two separate repeated 34 Table 4 Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Child Responsiveness as a Function of Abuse History and Violence Exposure No Abuse or Violence Exposure (n=32) Abuse and Violence Exposure History (n=186) M SD M SD CDI 4.91* .92 4.44 1.03 PDI 3.66 .93 4.05 .95 CU 3.09 1.11 3.28 1.08 Observation Analog Note. CDI= Child Directed Interaction, PDI = Parent Directed Interaction, CU= Clean Up, Child Responsiveness = Child Responsiveness scales of Emotional Availability Scales (Biringen, 2000). *p < .01 measures multivariate analysis of variance: first from CDI to PDI and then from PDI to CU. For the first analysis I performed a 2 (Observational Analog; CDI, PDI) X 2 (Violence/Abuse History; 0 counts or 1 or more counts) repeated measures multivariate analysis of variance. With the use of Wilks’s Lambda, results indicated there was a significant main multivariate effect of violence exposure and abuse history on the overall change in child responsiveness from CDI to PDI, F (2, 215) = 5.50, p = .00, partial η2 = .049. Univariate F tests indicated that there was a significant univariate effect of violence exposure and abuse history on child responsiveness in CDI, F (2, 215) = 5.77, p = .02, and a non-significant univariate effect of violence exposure and abuse history on child responsiveness in PDI, F (2, 215) = 1.14, p = .29. Children who had been exposed to 35 violence had significantly lower responsiveness in CDI compared to children who had not been exposed to violence. For the second analysis, I performed a 2 (Observational Analog; PDI, CU) X 2 (Violence/Abuse History; 0 counts or 1 or more counts) repeated measures multivariate analysis of variance. With the use of Wilks’s Lambda, results indicated there was a nonsignificant main multivariate effect of violence exposure and abuse history, F (2, 215) = .68, p = .50, partial η2 = .006 on the overall change in child responsiveness from PDI to CU. Univariate F tests indicated that there was a non-significant univariate effect of violence exposure and abuse history on child responsiveness in PDI, F (2, 215) = 5.77, p = .01, and a non-significant univariate effect of violence exposure and abuse history on child responsiveness in CU, F (2, 215) = 1.14, p = .29. These findings demonstrate an overall significant change in child responsiveness from CDI to CU based on abuse history and violence exposure. I examined group means to determine where the group differences in child responsiveness lay in relation to abuse and violence history. This result is presented graphically in Figure 1. Figure 1 shows the decline in child responsiveness over the three behavioral situations of CDI, PDI and CU as a function of abuse or violence exposure history. The results revealed a pattern contrary to my proposed hypothesis that children with abuse histories and previous violence exposure would demonstrate a significantly greater decline in emotion regulation (i.e., dys-regulation) over the three observational analogs as compared with children with no abuse history or violence exposure. Children who had never experienced 36 abuse or witnessed violence actually demonstrated a significant change in responsiveness (i.e., more dysregulation) from CDI to PDI but children with abuse histories and violence exposure did not. Emotional Responsivness Scale Scores 5 No Abuse or Violence Exposure Abuse and/or Violence Exposure 4.5 4 3.5 3 2.5 CDI PDI CU Behavioral Analog Figure 1. Child Responsiveness across three behavioral analogs as a function of their abuse and violence exposure history. 37 Chapter 5 DISCUSSION Summary The current study examined the relationship between emotional reactivity and emotional regulation in young children. Additionally, the study evaluated group differences in the presence or absence of child abuse and exposure to violence on a child’s observed emotional regulation. Children’s emotional reactivity, as reported by their caregivers was compared to observed child responsiveness in three increasingly aversive play scenarios with their caregivers. Children’s observed responsiveness as a result of the change in play situations was used as a measure of their emotion regulation in the face of a distressing situation. Based on the present findings, emotional reactivity did appear to be related to children’s responsiveness to their parents in the more aversive play situations, but was not related to the change in responsiveness from situation to situation. Developmental psychopathology posits that children may be adversely affected by an event such as witnessing violence and experiencing abuse, which may present in various ways such as high emotional reactivity and difficulties regulating their emotions. Contrary to hypothesis 2, which stated that children exposed to violence would exhibit a significant decrease in child responsiveness across the play situations, children exposed to violence did exhibit a decrease in responsiveness across the three play situations; however, since these children were significantly less responsive to their caregiver in the 38 first situation, these children did not exhibit a significant overall decrease in child responsiveness across the three play situations. However, children who had not experienced abuse or witnessed violence did actually demonstrate a significant change across the three behavioral analogs. Both larger results taken together provide a complex and unique picture of how emotional reactivity and emotion regulation are observable in a caregiver-child relationship. Emotional Reactivity and Emotion Regulation The first goal of this study was to evaluate how emotional reactivity and emotion regulation are related to each other in young children. Specifically, Hypothesis 1.1 stated that children reported as more emotionally reactive by their caregivers would demonstrate more situational emotional dysregulation as represented by lower responsiveness to their caregivers in more aversive play situations than children who were rated less emotionally reactive. This hypothesis was supported in that children reported as more emotionally reactive by their caregivers demonstrated lower responsiveness in the more aversive play situations of PDI and CU. These findings also support the validity of parents’ ratings of children’s emotional reactivity. They also support the use of the Child Responsiveness scale from the EAS as a measure of emotional regulation in the context of parent-child interaction in increasingly aversive situations. The next step was to address whether children’s emotional reactivity was related to the change in child responsiveness across the three play situations in a sequence from 39 CDI to PDI to CU and address whether this would be representative of children’s emotional regulatory processes within the caregiver-child observation. Hypothesis 1.2 posited that across the three increasingly aversive play situations, children rated as more emotionally reactive by their caregivers would demonstrate significantly lower responsiveness toward their caregivers, thus indicating the presence of emotional dysregulation. This hypothesis was not supported. Results indicated that in the caregiverchild observation, children did show a significant decrease in their responsiveness across the three play situations, but it did not differ significantly based how emotionally reactive they were when rated by their caregivers. Overall, the findings indicate that emotional reactivity was related to children’s responsiveness to their parents in the individual observations of PDI and CU, but not related to the overall change across the three observations. These findings suggest that responsiveness in aversive situations might actually reflect emotional regulation. It is during the more stressful situations of PDI and CU where it would be critical for caregivers to help their child manage the distress. The current study adds to previous research in that few studies have used observations of parent-child interactions to assess emotion dysregulation and also as a supplement to caregivers’ ratings of their children’s negative emotional reactivity. Emotion Regulation and Abuse and Violence History To examine the second goal that compared how abused and/or exposed to violence children differed from non-abused and/or violence exposed children in 40 emotional regulation in observations with their caregivers, I hypothesized that children who have experienced abuse and have been exposed to violence would be rated as more emotionally reactive by their caregivers. Results demonstrated that abused and violence exposed children showed a non-significant trend to be rated as more emotionally reactive by their caregivers when compared to children who did not have an abuse history or violence exposure past. Previous research has found stronger differences between stressexposed groups. A recent study found that in response to a stressful and negative home life, children exhibited high levels of emotional reactivity which then predicted higher levels of emotional problems in adolescence (Shapero & Steinburg, 2013). Additionally, since children are very much influenced emotionally by the environmental stress of the surrounding home life, one might expect that children exposed to an unpredictable and negative family environment (Morris et al., 2007) would demonstrate more emotional reactivity. Hypothesis 2.2 addressed the supposition that compared to children with no violence exposure or abuse history, children exposed to violence and with an abuse history will demonstrate more observed emotional dysregulation as measured by a significant decrease in responsiveness in the caregiver-child relationship across increasingly aversive play situations. This particular hypothesis was concerned with looking at the change in child responsiveness in response to the increasingly aversive situation with their caregivers and seeing if children with histories of abuse and violence exposure would exhibit emotional dysregulation. Developmental psychopathology 41 provided a supportive frame to address this hypothesis: in response to harsh events such as abuse, children might be driven off their optimal behavioral trajectory, thus they develop maladaptive behaviors that are evident in emotional dysregulation (Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002). However Hypothesis 2.1 was not supported: Both groups demonstrated a decrease in responsiveness as interactions became more aversive and distressing for the child. However, when faced with increasingly aversive situations, children with no violence exposure or abuse history demonstrated a significant decrease in responsiveness, and thus, contrary to what was expected, presented with an overall dysregulated emotional response. To understand what was driving his result, two additional analyses were performed examining the change in responsiveness from CDI to PDI and from PDI to CU. Results indicated that the significant group difference occurred in the change from CDI to PDI but not from PDI to CU. As graphically depicted in Figure 1, this interaction effect is best explained by the fact that child responsiveness in the abuse and/or violence exposure group was lower in CDI compared to the no-abuse and/or violence group. In other words, the maltreatment group showed less of a decline from CDI to PDI because they were less responsive (i.e., more dysregulated) to begin with. To interpret these results further, it is advantageous to look within the separate play situations and address what might have led to unexpected results. Child Directed Interaction is designed to provide the child with an opportunity to play with new toys and the ability to decide how they would like to play. Within this study, CDI provided the child with the ability to be in control and not face any aversive problems and acted as an 42 emotional baseline. However, for children with abuse and violence exposure histories, the parent-child relationship can already be fairly negative. Bousha and Twentyman (1984) documented how maltreating parent child relationships are less positive and more negative, with children that exhibit more negative affect towards their caregivers (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1994). Additionally parents tend to engage in unhealthy parenting practices (Cerezo et al., 1996) and home environments are all around more negative and stressful (Fantuzzo et al., 1997). Essentially, these children’s non-optimal responsiveness to their parents might have been influenced by their previous negative experiences with their caregiver and home environment. This might have carried over into the semistructured observational assessment creating aversion within CDI, which was supposed to have been a non-aversive baseline analog. Thus, even in the less aversive situation these children were still having difficulty regulating their emotions during interaction with their caregivers. In contrast, children in the group who had not been abuse or exposed to violence may experience somewhat more optimal parent-child relationships (although they were still clinic-referred) such that CDI was a more positive experience for them. This difference in baseline responsiveness created a sharper decline during the change to PDI, which children not abused or violence exposed were more likely to experience as more aversive and more dysregulating. While the results are not consistent with the hypothesis, the change in children’s responsiveness to parents from CDI to PDI does seem to in some way to reflect children’s emotion regulation in caregiver-child observations in children who have not been exposed to violence or experienced abuse. 43 In this study, I used the Emotional Availability Scales, more specifically the Child Responsiveness Scale in an attempt to measure emotional dysregulation in the context of the parent-child relationship, which is an innovative contribution of the study. Studying emotion dysregulation in the context of parent-child interaction may be especially relevant for the child maltreatment population because excessive dysregulation may place a child at greater risk for future abuse, which makes it a potentially important target for clinical intervention. While a strength of the EAS is that it is a dyadic assessment, it is also a more global evaluation of responsiveness toward caregivers than more conventional measures of emotion regulation. Past researchers have measured emotion regulation and dysregulation focusing on more discreet displays of children’s emotions and behaviors independent of parent behavior. For example, Cole and colleagues (1994) used the “disappointment task” to elicit emotion regulation behaviors and coded specific emotional displays in response to a disappointment. This is a more widely accepted approach to assessment of emotion regulation because it is looking at small distinct emotional changes and how children are able to manage them. Based on my findings, the EAS Child Responsiveness scale appeared to reflect children’s emotional regulation, but may not have been sufficient to fully understand these processes in abused and violenceexposed children. 44 Limitations Using data from a larger clinical study presented several challenges. For example, I was limited to variables within the parameters of what had already been collected or approved as part of the research and clinical protocols. Thus, the variables used in this study may not be completely representative of the larger constructs of emotion regulation and dysregulation which presents a major limitation in measurement. Another limitation of the study stems from the coding of maltreatment through retroactive chart review. Information regarding abuse history and violence exposure from client charts can often be limited to what is pertinent to how the child is being treated.. There is typically very little information regarding the chronicity or timing of when the abuse or violence occurred, which, if present, could have created opportunities for finer grained and more accurate analysis of the associations between maltreatment and children’s emotion regulation capabilities (Shipman & Zehman, 1999). Conclusion The overarching purpose of this research was to evaluate how caregiver-reported emotional reactivity and observed emotional regulation in children were related to one another in an at-risk population. Emotional reactivity appeared to be related to children’s responsiveness, but not related to the change in responsiveness in the face of an aversive situation. Children exposed to violence and with abuse histories did present with low emotional responsiveness in aversive interactions with their caregivers. However, the findings here indicate that when presented with an aversive situation, abused and 45 violence-exposed children started with lower responsiveness, and maintained low responsiveness, possibly providing an attenuated picture of emotional regulation and dysregulation. A key concept of developmental psychopathology is that when exposed to an aversive experience, children may be affected emotionally and behaviorally such that their behaviors become maladaptive which may affect later functioning. Based on the findings, children who were not abused or violence-exposed actually demonstrated an emotional pattern that might be indicative of dysregulation. This difference appeared to work as a function of the observational paradigm in which both the caregiver and child were involved. Only a few studies have looked at how caregiver rated emotional reactivity are related with observed emotion regulation and very few studies have attempted to look at children with their caregivers in an observational assessment. Furthermore, this study also attempted to use a global assessment of emotional responsiveness to see if it were related to emotional reactivity and possibly emotion regulation, which had not been typically measured this way. Study findings, while not as expected, left the impression that the caregiver-child relationship is an important element in the developmental task for children to learn how to regulate their own emotions. Future studies should address how to measure and evaluate emotion regulation in the caregiver-child relationship. 46 Appendix A. Emotional Availability Coding Sheet 47 48 49 Appendix B. Child Behavior Checklist 1 ½ to 5 50 References Achenbach, T. M. (2000). Manual for the ASEBA school-age. Forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth & Families. Alessandri, S.M. (1992). Play and social behavior in maltreated preschoolers. Development and Psychopathology, 3 (2), 191-205 Alink, L.A., Cicchetti, D., Kim, J., & Rogosch, F. A. (2009). Mediating and moderating processes in the relation between maltreatment and psychopathology. Motherchild relationship quality and emotion regulation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 37 (6), 831-843 Alink, L.A., Cicchetti, D., Kim, J., & Rogosch, F. A. (2012). Longitudinal associations among child maltreatment, social functioning, and cortisol regulation. Developmental Psychology, 48 (1) 224-236 Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Oxford England: Prentice-Hall Biringen Z (2000) Emotional availability: conceptualization and research findings. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 70, 104–111 Burgess, R. L., & Conger, R. D. (1978). Family interaction in abusive, neglectful, and normal families. Child Development, 49 (4), 1163-1173 Bousha, D. M., & Twentyman, C. T. (1984). Mother–child interactional style in abuse, neglect, and control groups: Naturalistic observations in the home. Journal Of Abnormal Psychology, 93(1), 106-114. 51 Cerezo, M. (1997). Abusive family interaction: A review. Aggression And Violent Behavior, 2(3), 215-240. Carson, J. L., & Parke, R. D. (1996). Reciprocal negative affect in parent-child interactions and children’s peer competency. Child Development, 67, 2217–2226. Chaffin, M. (2006). The changing focus of child maltreatment research and practice within psychology. Journal of Social Issues, 62 (4), 663-684. Chaffin, M., Silovsky, J., Funderburk, B., Valle, L., Brestan, E., Balachova, T., Jackson, S., Lensgraf, J., & Bonner, B. (2004). Parent-Child Interaction Therapy with physically abusive parents. Efficacy for reducing future abuse reports. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 500–510. Cicchetti, D. (1990). The organization and coherence of socioemotional, cognitive, and representational development: Illustrations through a developmental psychopathology perspective on Down syndrome and child maltreatment. In R. Thompson (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 36. Socioeconomical development (pp. 259-366). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Cicchetti, D. (2006). Development and psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti, D. J.Cohen (Eds). Developmental Psychopathology, Vol 1: Theory and method (2nd ed.) (pp.1-23). Hoboken, NJ US: Wiley & Sons Inc 52 Cicchetti, D., & Lynch, M. (1995). Failures in the expectable environment and their impact on individual development: The case of child maltreatment. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 2. Risk, disorder, and adaptation (2nd ed., pp. 32–71). New York, NY: Wiley. Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F.A. (1994). The toll of child maltreatment on the developing child: Insights from developmental psychopathology. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 3(4), 759-776 Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F.A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 597–600. Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F.A., Gunnar, M.R., & Toth, S. L., (2010). The differential impact of early physical and sexual abuse and internalizing problems on daytime cortisol rhythm in school-aged children. Child Development. 81 (1) 252-269) Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (1995). A developmental psychopathology perspective on child abuse and neglect. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent, 34(5), 541-565 Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. (2000). Developmental processes in maltreated children. In D. Hansen (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation. Vol. 46: Motivation and child maltreatment (pp. 85–160). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S.L. (2009). The past achievements and future promise of developmental psychopathology: The coming of age of a discipline. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50 (1-2), 16-25 53 Cicchetti, D., & Valentino, K. (2006). An ecological transactional perspective on child maltreatment: Failure of the average expectable environment and its influence upon child development. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 3. Risk, disorder, and adaptation (2nd ed., pp. 129–201). New York: Wiley. Cole, P. M., Michel, M. K., & Teti, L. O. (1994). The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation: A clinical perspective. In N. A. Fox (Ed.), Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (Serial No. 240 ed., Vol. 59, pp. 73– 100). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Cole, P.M., Martin, S.E., & Dennis, T.A. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Development, 75 (2) 317-333 Crume, T.L., Digueseppi,C., Byers, T., Sirotnak, A. P., & Garret, C.J. Underascertainment of child maltreatment fatalities by death certificates, 19901998. Pediatrics, 110 (2), 1-18 Davies, P. T., Cicchetti, D., & Martin, M. J. (2012). Toward greater specificity in identifying associations among interparental aggression, child emotional reactivity to conflict, and child problems. Child Development, 83(5), 1789-1804. 54 Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 241–273. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., & Reiser, M. (1999). Parental reactions to children’s negative emotions: Longitudinal relations to quality of children’s social functioning. Child Development, 70, 513–534. Eisenberg, N., & Morris, A. S. (2002). Children's emotion-related regulation. (pp. 189229). San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press, San Diego, CA. Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Morris, A. S., Fabes, R. A., Cumberland, A., Reiser, M., et al. (2003). Longitudinal relations among parental emotional expressivity, children’s regulation, and quality of socioemotional functioning. Developmental Psychology, 39, 3–19. Ellis, A.J., Beevers, C.G., & Wells, T.T. (2009). Emotional dysregulation in dysphoria: Support for emotion context insensitivity in response to performance-based feedback. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 40 (3), 443454 Eyberg, S., & Robinson, E. A. (1982). Parent-Child Interaction Training: Effects on family functioning. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 11 (2), 130–137. Fantuzzo, J., Boruch, R., Beriama, A., & Atkins, M. (1997). Domestic violence and children: Prevalence and risk in five major U.S. cities. Journal Of The American Academy Of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(1), 116-122. 55 Fernandez, M.A., & Eyberg, S.M. (2009). Predicting treatment and follow-up attrition in parent-child interaction therapy. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 37 (3), 431-441. Fox, N.A. (1989). Psychophysiological correlates of emotional reactivity during the first year of life. Developmental Psychology, 25, 364–372. Gaensbauer, T.J. (1980). Analytic depression in a three-and-one-half-month-old-child. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 137 (7) 841-842 Gratz, K. L., & Tull, M. T. (2010). The relationship between emotion dysregulation and deliberate self-harm among inpatients with substance use disorders. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 34 (6), 544-553. Gratz, K.L., Rosenthal, M., Tull, M.T., Leiuez, C.W.,& Gunderson, J. G. (2009). An experimental investigation of emotion dysregulation in borderline personality disorder. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 5 (1), 18-26 Grills, A. E., & Ollendick, T. H. (2002). Issues in parent– child agreement: The case of structured diagnostic interviews. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 5, 57–83. Gross, J.J., & Thompson, R.A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In J.J., Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp.3-24). New York, NY, Guliford Press 56 Han, Z., & Shaffer, A. (2013). The relation of parental emotion dysregulation to children’s psychopathology symptoms: The moderating role of child emotion dysregulation. Child Psychiatry And Human Development, 44(5), 591-601. Hamby, S., Finkelhor, D., Turner, H., & Ormrod, R. (2010). The overlap of witnessing partner violence with child maltreatment and other victimizations in a nationally representative survey of youth. Child Abuse & Neglect, 34(10), 734-741 Haskett, M. E., & Kistner, J. A. (1991). Social interactions and peer perceptions of young physically abused children. Child Development, 62, 979-990. Heim, C., Newport, D. J., Mletzko, T., Miller, A. H., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2008). The link between childhood trauma and depression: Insights from HPA axis studies in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33, 693–710. John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. Journal of Personality, 72, 1301–1333. Jonson-Reid, M., Drake, B., Chung, S., &Way, I. (2003). Cross-type recidivism among child maltreatment victims and perpetrators. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27(8), 899– 917. Kashdan, T.B., & Breen, W.E. (2008). Social anxiety and positive emotions: A proscpectice examination of a self regulatory model with tendencies to suppress or express emotions as a moderating variable. Behavior Therapy, 39 (1) 1-12. 57 Keenan, K. (2000). Emotion dysregulation as a risk factor for child psychopathology. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice,7(4), 418-434. Kendall-Tackett, K.A., Williams, L.M., & Finkelhor, D. (1993). Impact of sexual abuse on children: A review and synthesis of recent empirical studies. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 164–180. Kim, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Longitudinal pathways linking child maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer relations and psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(6), 706-716 Kitzmann, K. M., Gaylord, N. K., Holt, A. R., & Kenny, E. D. (2003). Child witnesses to domestic violence: A meta-analytic review.Journal Of Consulting And Clinical Psychology, 71(2), 339-352. Little, C., & Carter, A.S. (2005). Negative Emotional Reactivity and Regulation in 12month olds following emotional challenge: Contributions of maternal-infant emotional availability in a low income sample. Infant Mental Health Journal, 26 (4), 354-368 Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Manly, J. T., Kim, J. E., Rogosch, F. A., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Dimensions of child maltreatment and children’s adjustment: Contributions of developmental timing and subtype. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 759–782. 58 Maughan, A., & Cicchetti, D. (2002). Impact of child maltreatment and interadult violence on children’s emotion regulation abilities and socioemotional adjustment. Child Development, 73, 1525–1542. McMahon, R., Forehand, R., & Griest, D. (1981). Effects of knowledge of social learning principles on enhancing treatment outcome and generalization in a parent training program. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49,526-532. McNeil, C., Hembree-Kigin, T.L., Anhalt, K., Bjorseth, A., Borrego, J., Chen, Y., &…Wormdal, A. (2010). Parent-child interaction therapy (2nd ed.) New York, NY US: Springer Science +Business media. Mennin, D. S., Holaway, R., Fresco, D. M., Moore, M. T., & Heimberg, R. G. (2007). Delineating components of emotion and its dysregulation in anxiety and mood psychopathology. Behavior Therapy, 38, 284–302. Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L.R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16, 361–388. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Stice, E., Wade, E., & Bohon, C. (2007). Reciprocal relations between rumination and bulimic, substance abuse, and depres- sive symptoms in female adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 198–207. Perlman, S. B., Kalish, C. W., & Pollak, S. D. (2008). The role of maltreatment experience in children's understanding of the antecedents of emotion. Cognition And Emotion, 22(4), 651-670. 59 Pollak, S. D., Cicchetti, D., Hornung, K., & Reed, A. (2000). Recognizing emotion in faces: Developmental effects of child abuse and neglect. Developmental Psychology, 36 (5), 679-688 Robinson, J. L., Emde, R. N., & Korfmacher, J. (1997). Integrating an emotional regulation perspective in a program of prenatal and early childhood home visitation. Journal of Community Psychology, 25(1), 59-75 Rogosch, F. A., & Cicchetti, D. (1994). Illustrating the interface of family and peer relations through the study of child maltreatment. Social Development, 3 (3), 291308 Shackman, J. E., Shackman, A. J., & Pollak, S. D. (2007). Physical abuse amplifies attention to threat and increases anxiety in children. Emotion,7(4), 838-852. Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1998). Reactive aggression among maltreated children: The contributions of attention and emotion dysregulation. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27, 381–395. Shipman, K. L., & Zeman, J. (1999). Emotional understanding: A comparison of physically maltreating and nonmaltreating mother–child dyads. Journal Of Clinical Child Psychology, 28(3), 407-417. Shonk, S. M., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Maltreatment competency deficits, and risks for academic and behavioral maladjustment. Developmental Psychology, 37 (3-14) 60 Sroufe, L.A. (1989). Pathways to adaptation and maladaptation: Psychopathology as developmental deviation. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.), Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology: The emergence of a discipline (vol. 1, pp. 13– 40). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press, New York, NY. Sroufe, L.A., & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 17–29. Taylor, T. K., & Biglan, A. (1998). Behavioral family interventions for improving childrearing: A review of the literature for clinicians and policy makers. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 1(1), 41-60. Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. [Monograph]. The Society for Research in Child Development Timmer, S.G., Urquiza, A. J., Zebell, N., McGrath, J. (2005). Parent–child interaction therapy: application to physically abusive and high-risk dyads. Child Abuse and Neglect, 29, 825–842 Timmer, S. G., Thompson, D., Culver, M. A., Urquiza, A. J., & Altenhofen, S. (2012). Mothers’ physical abusiveness in a context of violence: Effects on the mother– child relationship. Development And Psychopathology, 24(1), 79-92. Toth, S. L., Manly, J. T., & Cicchetti, D. (1992). Child maltreatment and vulnerability to depression. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 97–112. 61 Tronick, E. D., Als, H., & Brazelton, T. B. (1977). Mutuality in mother–infant interaction. Journal Of Communication, 27(2), 74-79. Urquiza, A. J.,&McNeil, C. B. (1996). Parent-Child Interaction Therapy: An intensive dyadic intervention for physically abusive families. Child Maltreatment, 1(2), 134–144. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration on Children, Youth, & Families. (2010). Child maltreatment 2010. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. van, d. K., & Fisler, R. E. (1994). Childhood abuse and neglect and loss of selfregulation. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic,58(2), 145-168. Weinberg, K. M., & Tronick, E. Z. (1996). Infant affective reactions to the resumption of maternal interaction after the Still-Face. Child Development, 67(3), 905-914. Zalewski, M., Lengua, L. J., Kiff, C. J., & Fisher, P. A. (2012). Understanding the relation of low income to HPA-axis functioning in preschool children: Cumulative family risk and parenting as pathways to disruptions in cortisol. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 43(6), 924-942