THE MEDIAL PREFRONTAL CORTICAL CONTRIBUTION TO PATH INTEGRATION: AN ANIMAL MODEL OF MEMORY AND BEHAVIORAL DEFICITS FOUND IN NEURODEGENERATIVE DISEASES A Thesis Presented to the faculty of the Department of Psychology California State University, Sacramento Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in Psychology by Amanda L. Simmons FALL 2012 © 2012 Amanda L. Simmons ALL RIGHTS RESERVED ii THE MEDIAL PREFRONTAL CORTICAL CONTRIBUTION TO PATH INTEGRATION: AN ANIMAL MODEL OF MEMORY AND BEHAVIORAL DEFICITS FOUND IN NEURODEGENERATIVE DISEASES A Thesis by Amanda L. Simmons Approved by: __________________________________, Committee Chair Jeffrey Calton, Ph.D. __________________________________, Second Reader Kimberly Roberts, Ph.D. __________________________________, Third Reader Emily Wickelgren, Ph.D. ____________________________ Date iii Student: Amanda L. Simmons I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. __________________________, Graduate Coordinator ___________________ Lisa Harrison, Ph.D Date Department of Psychology iv Abstract of THE MEDIAL PREFRONTAL CORTICAL CONTRIBUTION TO PATH INTEGRATION: AN ANIMAL MODEL OF MEMORY AND BEHAVIORAL DEFICITS FOUND IN NEURODEGENERATIVE DISEASES by Amanda L. Simmons Theories of prefrontal cortical function in human and primate models include regulation of cognitive processes such as working memory and executive functions, both of which may be implicated in spatial navigation behavior. The role of working memory in path integration navigation is not well understood. Lesions of the medial prefrontal cortex administered to twenty rats assessed whether impairment in working memory associated with the lesions produced navigational deficits similar to those found in humans with neurodegenerative disorders. We hypothesized that medial prefrontal lesions would produce impairment in navigation performance during a Whishaw table top path integration task when compared with sham controls. We found no significant differences between lesioned and sham animals on measures of path integration performance. These v results are inconclusive in determining the possibility of functional similarities between the rodent prefrontal cortex and the human manifestation of symptoms found in abnormal aging affecting comparable brain regions. _______________________, Committee Chair Jeffrey Calton, Ph.D. _______________________ Date vi TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION ……………. ………………………………………………….…….. 1 Navigation.................................................................................................................... 3 Working Memory……… ............................................................................................ 4 Prefrontal Cortical Anatomy ........................................................................................ 8 Anatomical Similarities Between Animals and Humans ................................ 9 The Prefrontal Cortex in Humans and Animals ......................................................... 10 WM and the PFC in Humans and Non-Human Primates: Neuroimaging Studies................................................................................... 10 Single Cell Recordings in Non-Human Primates ......................................... 11 Human and Non-Human Primate Lesion Experiments................................. 12 Rodent Lesion Experiments .......................................................................... 12 Single Cell Recordings in Rodents ............................................................... 13 WM Debate .................................................................................................. 14 Navigation Requires WM .......................................................................................... 15 Path Integration and WM .............................................................................. 15 Lesions of the mPFC Impair Navigation ...................................................... 16 WM and Navigation Functions are Impaired by Lesions of the PFC ........... 16 The Connection Between WM, Executive Functions, and Navigation ..................... 17 Executive Functions Located in the PFC are Synonymous with Navigational Deficits: Single Cell Recordings and fMRI Studies........ 19 PFC Involvement in Neurodegenerative Disease Found in Animals and Humans ... 20 The Presentation of Symptoms in Animals After the PFC Sustains….….....22 Damage is Similar to the Symptoms Found in Humans with Neurodegenerative Disorders Complex Diseases such as FTD and Dementia of the Alzheimer’s Type Can Benefit From Research in Animal Models ............................................ 24 vii 2. METHOD ......................................................................................................................... 26 Animals ...................................................................................................................... 26 Apparatus .................................................................................................................. 26 Pre-Surgical Training ................................................................................................. 28 Surgical Procedure ..................................................................................................... 29 Post-Surgical Testing ................................................................................................. 30 Analysis ..................................................................................................................... 30 3. RESULTS ......................................................................................................................... 32 Histology.................................................................................................................... 34 4. DISCUSSION .................................................................................................................... 34 Conclusions and Future Study ................................................................................... 37 Appendix A. Subcortical Structures of the Rodent Brain .................................................... 39 Appendix B. An Infrared Picture of a Post-Surgical Rat ...................................................... 40 Appendix C. Pre-Operative Experimental Measures of Latency, Frequency of Emergence, and Error in Lesioned and Sham Controls for Five Trials, N = 16 .......................................................................................... 41 Appendix D. Post-Operative Experimental Measures Experimental Measures of Latency, Frequency of Emergence, and Error in Lesioned and Sham Controls for Five Trials, N = 16 .......................................................................................... 42 Appendix E. Post-Operative Latency Measures by Trials With Lesion and Sham Groups.. 43 Appendix F. Post-Operative Frequency Measures by Trials With Lesion and Sham Groups .............................................................................................................. 44 Appendix G. Neurotoxic Lesion Extent................................................................................. 45 References ............................................................................................................................... 46 viii 1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION The frontal cortex in humans is a complex region that regulates cognitive functioning through various psychological processes including perception (Baddeley, 2000), which involves the scanning, selection and integration of information taken from stimuli in the environment necessary for spatial and working memory (WM) processes (Stuss & Knight, 2002; Chudasama & Robbins, 2006). Research also indicates that frontal cortical functions contribute to WM and navigation in humans as well as rodents (Chudasama & Robbins, 2006). While the extent of differences in human models of disease include variability such as risk factors and demographics, animal models serve to better differentiate the extent of impairment as a result of the degree of cortical tissue damage in the absence of many of these confounding variables (Jones, 2002; Müller & Knight, 2006). Path integration navigation is the process by which an individual uses the distance and direction of travel from a previous known position to locate ones current position. We hypothesize that path integration navigation is dependent on WM, and because the medial prefrontal cortical region (mPFC) is thought to be necessary for WM processes, we further hypothesize that lesions in this area will impair this form of navigation in rodents. While navigation tasks such as the radial arm maze are commonly utilized to assess working memory performance in lesioned animals, a pure path integration task has never been used to examine the effects of lesions expected to interfere with WM 2 performance. Lesions of the PFC in rodents have been found to result in profound cognitive (Hoge & Kesner, 2007) and motor disturbances (Compton, Griffith, McDaniel, Foster, & Davis, 1997; Heidbreder & Groenewegen, 2003; Granon & Poucet, 1995) similar to neurodegenerative disorders in humans, such as Alzheimer’s disease, (Chudasama & Robbins, 2006; Huntley & Howard, 2010), and Frontotemporal Dementia (Boccardi, 2006; Hodges, 2001). Navigation such as path integration requires the use of a “cognitive map” (Newman, Caplan, Kirschen, Korolev, Sekuler, & Kahana, 2006) which is consistently updated in order to provide an “on-line” estimation of current location in the environment, based on movement direction and distance from previously known locations (Calton & Taube, 2009; Tolman, 1949). This use of information is similar to models of WM where information is manipulated for temporary use (Baddeley, 2000). Anatomical references of the PFC in human and animal models have also suggested several similarities between brain regions, indicating that rodents are capable of complex processes necessary to support higher brain functioning (Jones, 2002; Stuss & Knight, 2002). While the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) in humans appears largely responsible for WM, previous research has indicated that the rodent mPFC is analogous to this area by sharing structural similarities and many of the same functions (Jones, 2002; Uylings, Groenewegen, & Kolb, 2003). Brain areas responsible for functional similarities in humans and animal models could indicate that mPFC lesions in rodents are similar to brain insults found in humans with comparable symptoms, common to neurodegeneration of the frontal lobe which manifest as memory loss and spatial impairment (Stuss & Knight, 2002). 3 Navigation Navigation of the spatial environment can be observed in rodents as well has humans, and the basic processes of navigation seem to be somewhat similar among the two species. Spatial navigation occurs when the organism is motivated and plans to travel toward a goal in the environment, whether it is a building, car, or a reward in an animal learning experiment. Normally navigation in an unfamiliar environment requires the consistent update of spatial stimuli to process one’s orientation in the environment. Two basic types of navigation, landmark navigation and path integration, have been defined. Landmark navigation (also known as place recognition or piloting) involves locating ones position based on the location of familiar cues (landmarks) found in the environment (Gallistel, 1990). As humans, we commonly use landmarks such as road signs, buildings and geological features to make our way from one place to another. A demonstration of landmark navigation in the laboratory can be observed in the Morris water maze task when the rat uses landmarks in the surrounding room to return to a hidden platform in a pool of water regardless of where it entered the pool. A second form of navigation, path integration, relies not on the location of familiar landmarks in the spatial environment, but on the knowledge of the distance and direction moved since the start of the journey (Gallistel, 1990; Mittelstaedt & Mittelstaedt, 1980; Touretzky & Redish, 1996). Path integration assumes a continual process of integrating vestibular cues, efferent copy, and optic flow to locate one’s position in space, and thereby allows accurate navigation even when familiar landmarks 4 are absent (Calton & Taube, 2008). An example of path integration would be an animal taking a meandering route to find food in an unfamiliar environment and then using the knowledge of the distance and direction traveled since beginning the journey in order to return to its point of origin using a novel direct route. These two forms of navigation are most likely complementary, in that landmarks may be utilized when available, but when these external cues are absent, path integration can be used to accurately navigate in an unfamiliar environment, (Calton & Taube, 2009; Gallistel, 1990). Working Memory Traditionally, the term “working memory” (WM) is most often used in human and nonhuman primate studies because researchers are reluctant to attribute higher cognitive functioning to rodents. However, due to many anatomical and functional similarities between primates and rodents, it has now become more acceptable to design experiments studying WM in rodents in order to make comparisons with the human WM model (Jones, 2002). In the human literature, WM is defined as the system that uses temporary storage and manipulation of information required for complex processes such as learning, reasoning, language, and comprehension (Baddeley & Hitch, 1994; Baddeley, 2000). It is the process whereby sensory material is actively held, making it easy to retrieve or recall. This sensory material is temporarily held in an active state and becomes central to processing, whereby additional sensory information cannot interfere with the active state (Stuss & Knight, 2002). As one of the most specialized functions of the PFC, WM 5 includes attentional components which facilitate the on-line capabilities enabling information processing (Stuss & Knight, 2002). While this information processing can be analyzed through written and verbal neuropsychological testing methods in humans, rodent models may incorporate navigational tasks to identify these processing errors in WM functioning. In these animal studies, errors in the temporary storage of various navigational trials, such as the erroneous return to a previously visited (and no longer baited) arm of a radial arm maze, may indicate the presence of WM dysfunction. The constituents of WM which facilitate information processing consist of the “central executive,” the “visuospatial sketchpad,” the “episodic buffer,” and the “phonological loop,” (Jones, 2002). While the phonological loop pertains to sensory speech information in humans and is therefore not likely to be relevant to rodent cognition, the central executive, visuospatial sketchpad, and episodic buffer terms can still be applied to animal cognition. The central executive refers to the regulation of decision-making, reasoning, and the coordination of subsystems. Commonly referred to as the “supervisory attentional system” (Stuss & Knight, 2002) the subsystems of the central executive are responsible for the retention of visual and spatial information. This area is also responsible for directing the attention systems which aid in the retention of stimuli from the visuospatial sketchpad and phonological loop (Baddeley, 2000). With respect to rodent navigation, a defect in the central executive would present as an inability to focus on stimuli sufficiently enough to retain it in WM, or the failure to differentiate between correct and incorrect navigational paths toward a task goal. These errors can be visualized using 6 common paradigms such as the spatial delayed-alternation task, or the delayed-response task (Stuss & Knight, 2002). The visuospatial sketchpad controls the processing of temporary visual and spatial storage. In the human, the sketchpad activates the occipital lobe to process visual patterns in the environment which has neural pathway connections to the frontal lobe. The frontal lobes are also necessary for spatial processing, which is responsible for control and coordination in the environment (Baddeley, 2002). Detection and processing of visuospatial information by humans can be evaluated by neuropsychological testing and imaging studies. The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) is used to measure perceptual reasoning, visual processing, and spatial reasoning found in the Block Design, Matrix Reasoning, and Coding subtests of the battery. Testing impairments would indicate subsequent deficiencies in visuospatial processes. Evidence for human impairment in these abilities is also observable in the activation of brain areas from fMRI and PET scan studies, where individuals are required to remember letters and spatial locations after a brief delay (Prabhakaran, Narayanan, Zhao, & Gabrieli, 2000). Similar to the human, the rodent WM processes could be expected to hold visual and spatial information temporarily for task completion. As an example, in the Delayed Matching to Place T-maze task, the rat is first given a sample trial where it leaves the start box and only one of the two goal boxes is available. In the following “match” trial, both goal boxes are available but the rat is only rewarded if it chooses the goal box from the sample trial. In subsequent pairs of trials, the reinforced goal box will change. In 7 essence, this task requires the animal to temporarily hold on-line the correct spatial choice (Wenk, 2001). The newly incorporated idea of the episodic buffer, controlled by the central executive, has also been grouped in with the visuospatial sketchpad as a key component of WM. The episodic buffer refers to the storage of information not organized within WM, as well as the “rehearsal and coordination of temporally distinct but inter-dependent information” (Jones, 2002), retrieved consciously. Capable of organizing, coding, and storing information, this episodic buffer component is the intermediary between the phonological loop and the visuospatial sketchpad as well as the long-term memory (Baddeley, 2000). While the theory of the episodic buffer is referenced in humans, the idea of an animal episodic buffer (let alone a rodent central executive), has not been thoroughly investigated. However, research in single cell recordings in non-human primates have suggested that PFC cells can have a combination of properties which contribute to the WM process, and that these properties require the central executive episodic buffer (Stuss & Knight, 2002). These properties may also include the initial cellular response in conjunction with motor movement, the processing of stimuli, and the properties which hold information on-line. One or more of these properties may be apparent in any given cell at the time of stimuli exposure. During dlPFC single cell recordings of delayedresponse tasks, non-human primates were observed encoding information after briefly observing visual stimuli on the computer screen. Specifically, WM encoding was thought to have occurred when the cell showed peak activation or activity during the 8 delay periods when the animal was required to retain stimulus information in memory (Stuss & Knight, 2002). Episodic buffer impairment is presented in densely amnestic human patients that fail to learn new material, or fail to recall a list of words after a delay (Stuss & Knight, 2002). This is a result of a failure in the central executive, (Baddeley, 1996) a structure that when compromised, effects the integrated visuospatial sketchpad as well as the episodic buffer subsystems, (Baddeley, 2002) structures known to effect WM. Prefrontal Cortical Anatomy The prefrontal cortex is important in maintaining a wide variety of processes pertaining to executive functioning, memory, and navigation in the human (Stuss & Knight, 2002), the non-human primate (Stuss & Knight, 2002), and the rat (Heidbreder & Groenewegen, 2003; Hoover & Vertes, 2007). This section will be used to briefly describe the anatomical regions which are suspected to be responsible for the processes stated above (see Figure 1 in Appendix A), in order to provide justification for the location of mPFC lesions in later sections. The rodent PFC contains the orbital prefrontal cortex located dorsal to the olfactory bulb, the rhinal sulcus, the agranular insular cortex located anterior to the rhinal sulcus, and the mPFC which is located anterior and dorsal to the corpus callosum. The mPFC can then be subdivided into the medial precentral, the anterior cingulate, the prelimbic, and the infralimbic areas, which include the dorsal lateral (dl) and ventral lateral (vl) PFC, (Heidbreder & Groenewegen, 2003). In primates, PFC structures have been implicated by neuroimaging and single cell recordings to have connections with 9 brain regions such as the hippocampus (Müller & Knight, 2006). Afferent projections using retrograde tracing techniques in rodents also discovered common neural pathways from substructures of the mPFC to other brain areas. Connected areas include the midline thalamus, amygdala, basal nuclei, subiculum of hippocampus, and the insular and entorhinal cortices (Hoover & Vertes, 2007). Anatomical Similarities Between Animals and Humans Anatomical differences between the rat, non-human primate, and human models are distinguished by cytoarchitecture, neurochemical, and anatomical regions in various species. Identifying similarities in brain structures between species has been necessary in order to determine the functional implications of damage to those structures. This in turn can be used between species to apply and justify working theories of etiology for neurodegenerative diseases in human models (Farovik, Dupont, Arce, and Eichenbaum, 2008; Jones, 2002). Review of the animal model literature indicated that the PFC structure in rodents was found to be homologous to several brain regions in non-human primates, (Kolb, 1990; Hoover & Vertes, 2007; Preuss, 1995; Uylings et al., 2003; Vertes, 2006). The infralimbic cortex in the rodent was found to be functionally equivalent to the orbitomedial PFC in non-human primates (Hoover & Vertes, 2007), as was the prelimbic cortex of rats, and Brodmann’s area 32 in primates (Heidbreder & Groenewegen, 2003; Ongur & Price, 2000), and the lateral and dlPFC in non-human primates and prelimbic cortex of the rat (Hoover & Vertes, 2007). The rodent mPFC has also been compared functionally to the human 10 ventromedial PFC (Hoover & Vertes, 2007) as well as the dorsomedial and dlPFC regions in non-human primates (Heidbreder & Groenewegen, 2003). Additionally, rodent PFC lesion studies have found comparable functional deficits to primates and humans with dlPFC damage (Uylings, Groenewegen, & Kolb, 2003), which further describes the prefrontal cortical reliance on both cortical and subcortical structures as described in the neural pathway description above, (Chudasama and Robbins, 2006). The Prefrontal Cortex and Working Memory in Humans and Animals WM and the PFC in Humans and Non-Human Primates: Neuroimaging Studies “The ability to hold information in the mind and work with it (manipulating, monitoring, and transforming it),” (Stuss & Knight, 2002, pp 483) has been continuously observed in neuroimaging studies. With the emergence of neuroimaging, the human PFC has since been labeled as essential to WM, commonly referenced as the “neural substrate of WM,” (Müller & Knight, 2006). Studies including functional imaging can reveal distributed activity in involved cortical regions while a patient is completing a WM task (Müller & Knight, 2006). Neuroimaging studies, such as those referenced by Stuss and Knight (2002), describe activation of the dlPFC in patients completing the backward digit span task, where the participants must repeat a series of numbers in the reverse order in which they were given. This process of manipulating the series of numbers, monitoring it in WM, and transforming it into a new backward number series, leads to activation of the PFC region (Stuss and Knight, 2002). Mathematical applications such as multiplication and addition, as well as randomization of numbers without repetitions also require the essential faculties of the PFC (Stuss & Knight, 2002). 11 Single-Cell Recordings In Non-Human Primates Lesion studies in humans that examine the role of the PFC in WM may be subject to confounds that can make interpretation of function difficult (Müller & Knight, 2006). For this reason, single cell recordings and lesion studies of this area in animals, have been utilized to elucidate the cognitive and behavioral functioning deficits found in humans with lesions of the PFC (Müller & Knight, 2006; Chudasama & Robbins, 2006). In particular, single unit studies in non-human primates allow for functional assessment of anatomical areas that at times cannot be identified with sufficient accuracy in human imaging studies (Stuss & Knight, 2002). During single cell recordings with non-human primates, the monkey dlPFC has been used to examine visual WM properties while the animals complete spatial behavioral tasks. In these tasks, the animal is required to focus on a visual stimulus for a brief moment, and then hold that image in WM during the subsequent trials. WM performance is then tested by observing the eye movements produced during each trial, where the monkey makes a saccade to the remembered location of the previously viewed stimulus. Cellular recordings during this type of delayed matching to sample task were able to explore the conglomeration of cells which seem to indicate the WM process (Stuss & Knight, 2002). Neural PFC activity was also reported in the research by Desimone (1996) suggesting the presence of WM processes in this cortical area. During the task, the animal was shown one of two shapes. Then, following a brief delay, the animal was rewarded upon correctly choosing the shape that corresponds to the first shape. Neural activity in the PFC during the delay period provided evidence for active WM processes. 12 Human & Non-Human Primate Lesion Experiments Lesion studies located in the mPFC also suggest that the ventral and dorsal stream neural pathways could help mediate WM functioning. Humans and non-human primate studies involving the holding of information in memory have helped to identify neural activity from the posterior cortex extending to the PFC region and neural connections between ventral PFC and temporal cortex, and dorsal PFC to parietal regions (Müller & Knight, 2006). Patients with lesions to both VM PFC and DL PFC areas displayed impairments on a two-back test of WM. These impairments were not solely due to the size of the lesions, but rather the area affected, suggesting that the PFC helps to sustain WM, but is not the only region contributing to WM, (Müller & Knight, 2006). Rodent Lesion Experiments Rodent studies using quinolinic lesions and lidocaine inactivations to the mPFC resulted in a reduction in WM during various egocentric and navigational tasks (Heidbreder & Groenewegen, 2003). WM deficits were also found in a study with rodent PFC lesions using a conditional associative learning (CAL) procedure. For each CAL trial, the rat was trained to select the retractable lever that was underneath one of four lights on the apparatus wall that had previously been illuminated. Correct responses were rewarded with a food pellet. Results indicated that rats with PFC lesions showed response-selection deficits that could be attributed to impairments in WM (Winocur & Eskes, 1998). Rodent studies have also determined that the PFC shares connections with other areas thought to be important for WM. In a study by Jo et al. (2007), pattern completion memory retrieval was tested on rodents with mPFC lesions. During a water 13 maze navigational task, rodents were trained to search for a submerged platform which varied in location dependent on the trial day. Between one and three cues were present to represent the full or partial-cue in each trial. Results of this delayed matching-to-place experiment indicated that spatial memory retrieval was inhibited regardless of full or partial-cue conditions, suggesting a WM impairment and interaction between the hippocampus and mPFC. Similarly, in a study by Izaki, Takita, and Akema (2008), based on results from delayed radial arm maze tasks, rodent WM performance was found to be affected by lesions to both the bilateral posterior dorsal hippocampus and the bilateral PFC. This study concluded that the hippocampal-PFC anatomical pathways are essential for efficient WM processes. Single-Cell Recordings In Rodents The presence of WM pathways in non-human primate data might also be used to suggest similar PFC pathways in rodent physiology. In procedures designed to examine WM during the delay period of spatial navigation tasks, Jones (2002) concludes that due to known relevance of the hippocampus with WM, PFC neurons may also encode spatial information. While single cell recording evidence for rodent WM processes in the PFC are currently lacking, electrophysiological recordings suggest changes in the the level of PFC activation during tasks designed to measure WM (Jones, 2002). Neurons located in the deep PFC layer also exhibit short-term plasticity necessary in the encoding process of WM; a complex process by which one could postulate that rodent PFC physiology regulates more than simple spatial navigation processes (Jones, 2002). 14 WM Debate Despite the recent evidence to the contrary, debate still exists regarding PFC involvement in WM. Non-human primate lesion studies by Rushworth, Nixon, Eacott, Passingham, (1997) have concluded that individuals with ventromedial lesions of PFC preformed identical to normal controls in WM tasks. Similar observations were noted for the dlPFC lesions (Müller & Knight, 2006), although many others have found spatial WM (Ptito, Crane, Leonard, Amsel, & Caramanos, 1995; Bechara, Damasio, Tranel, & Anderson, 1998; Shimamura, Janowsky, & Squire, 1990; Chudasama & Robbins, 2006; Kesner, Hunt, Williams, & Long, 1996), as well as decision-making and WM to be significantly impaired after lesions of this area (Chudasama & Robbins, 2006). WM in particular, was found to have the greatest degree of impairment when both the ventromedial and dorsolateral regions were lesioned (Müller & Knight, 2006), similar to findings in human studies. Furthermore, in a task involving visual association with delay periods, monkeys with lateral lesions of the inferior convexity maintained WM processes, leading the authors to conclude that affected cortical regions were responsible for attentional processes rather than directly responsible for WM impairments (Müller & Knight, 2006, and Rushworth, Nixon, Eacott, Passingham, 1997). Similarly, neuroimaging research also has suggested a non-mnemonic role of the PFC, where processes that aid in memory are not mediated by the cortical region. Specifically, the model of attention, monitoring, and integration of information from neuronal networks was not theorized to come from the PFC (Jones, 2002; Müller & Knight, 2006). 15 Navigation Requires WM Path Integration and WM As discussed in the navigation section above, path integration relies on the continual on-line use of movement related information in order to orient oneself in space. One type of neuron that has been postulated to play a role in spatial orientation are place cells. These cells, typically recorded in the CA region of the hippocampus, become active when the animal is in a particular location of a known environment (O’Keefe & Dostrosky, 1971). These cells have been shown to maintain an accurate navigational signal for some time when landmarks are eliminated, suggesting that they are able to maintain orientation using path integration (Touretzky & Redish, 1996). The presumptive use of a “cognitive map” while navigating suggests some neural scheme that organizes the locations of places in the world relative to each other, which would allow for choosing optimal paths during navigation (Tolman, 1949). Since this map is used to calculate the path between two points in the environment (Newman, Caplan, Kirschen, Korolev, Sekuler, & Kahana, 2006), the “online” cognitive map could suggest the presence of a WM component during path integration navigation when landmarks are no longer present. Furthermore, since path integration employs the ability to remember the environment when visual cues are absent, it can be deduced that this is a function provided by the WM system. Consequently, if this system were to be impaired, the continual integration of motor, vestibular, and visual cues during navigation would be also be significantly compromised. 16 Lesions of the mPFC Impair Navigation In support of the assumption that path integration navigation requires working memory processes and the mPFC is necessary for working memory, a number of studies have found navigational deficits following lesions of this area. Research by Grannon and Poucet (1995) describe this impairment in trained animals with mPFC or sham lesions on a water maze task. The task changed with increasing levels of difficulty, incorporating up to four potential start positions and varying the occluded platform positions from trial to trial. The authors found no apparent effects of mPFC lesions until all four start positions were utilized. This indicated that mPFC lesioned animals were able to learn the spatial position of the platform, but may have been impaired in WM functions involved in forming a representation of the course of movements required to reach the platform. WM and Navigation Functions are Impaired by Lesions of the PFC Wolbers, Wiener, Mallot, and Büchel, (2007) utilized a virtual paradigm in humans to examine the role of the mPFC in navigational behavior. To examine path integration and relate its function to non-human primate and rodent models, a magnetic resonance imaging virtual paradigm was presented to allow a first-person perspective devoid of available landmarks. By navigating using a joystick, self-motion could be inferred. For each task, the participant was required to path integrate to varying locations within the virtual plane, then point to the start location. Activation of bilateral hippocampal and mPFC regions were present during the path integration exercise and correlated with higher response consistency in the hippocampus and mPFC. The mPFC also corresponded to observed random error in navigation; results which effectively 17 associates the deficits from a complex spatial path integration task with higher order WM processes in the PFC (Wolbers, Wiener, Mallot, and Büchel, 2007). The Connection Between WM, Executive Functions, and Navigation The process of navigation, in humans as well as animals, requires coordinated movements based on independent task cues integrated together to form a goal or purpose (Chudsama & Robbins, 2006; Müller & Knight, 2006; Heidbreder & Groenewegen, 2003). As described in previous sections, the functions of the PFC includes elements of perception, sensory and motor processes in navigation, executive functioning, learning, and memory components (Miller & Cohen, 2001; Jones, 2002). The organization of spatial information is an executive process, whereby perceptual, sensory and motor stimuli from the navigating individual are integrated and manipulated, then passed through various neural networks in the PFC, thereby producing “executive behavior,” (Jones, 2002). An example of executive behavior would be the “cognitive map” used in a path integration experiment, where the rat would produce a visual image of the immediate environment using WM in order to effectively navigate the surroundings from various locations within that environment. As described in the WM section, the central executive is responsible for executive processes and is essential to functions such as WM and navigation (de Saint Blanquat, Hok, Alvernhe, Save, & Poucet, 2010). The central executive as well as WM processes could be thought of as components of spatial learning, where it is theorized that these components require various demands on different structures and processes. If one of these structures such as the PFC is compromised by a lesion or other brain insult, the result is the loss of one or more 18 components necessary for navigation (Compton, Griffith, McDaniel, Foster, & Davis, 1997). Jacobsen (1935) was among the first to reference a connection with bilateral PFC lesions and memory dysfunction through observed impairment in delayed response tasks in non-human primates. After presenting two objects, the task required the ability to locate and choose the object that had been rewarded on the previous trial. Subsequent studies noted in a review by Chudasama & Robbins (2006), found that PFC lesioned animals had significant spatial navigation and WM deficits when completing a test of spatial short-term memory. The resulting impairments from those studies were related to the inability to hold information on-line, and the way in which the PFC neural networks manipulate information on new stimuli to produce a behavior (Chudasama & Robbins, 2006) in, for example, a path integration task. Mùˆller & Knight, (2006) also noted that the human ventrolateral PFC supports object information and the dlPFC the maintenance of spatial information, with executive functions distributed along the aforementioned brain regions. When both regions are lesioned in humans, simple spatial tasks were impaired. Results from the one-back test required the participant to inhibit previous responses to stimuli in order to prevent false alarms when selecting the stimulus in the environment during future trials (Müller & Knight, 2006). Several other studies also report similar findings of patients with frontal lobe lesions (see review by Baldo and Shimamura, 2002). WM and spatial navigation have also been tested with rodent PFC lesion studies using the T-maze, radial arm, and water mazes which were effective in identify the 19 resulting level of impairment in WM and associated navigation processes (Jones, 2002; Grannon & Poucet, 1995; Heidbreder & Groenewegen, 2003; Hoge & Kesner, 2007). These mazes employ the use of available cues in testing spatial navigation through delayed response tasks in which the frequency of error and inability to navigate in a maze environment suggest both WM and navigation dysfunction. Since these functions employ the use of PFC networks to operate successfully, PFC impairment would produce a negative effect on WM and spatial navigation processes (Kolb, Buhrmann, McDonald, & Sutherland, 1994; Jones, 2002). Research by Kolb, Buhrmann, McDonald, & Sutherland (1994), also concluded that spatial navigation was impaired in rodents with mPFC lesions when completing a water maze WM paradigm. During this task, prior to the lesion, the rat was trained to navigate to search for a visual platform. After surgery, rodents with mPFC lesions presented with a higher frequency of directional “heading error,” increased latency in learning and task completion, as well as perseveration or the repetition of a fixed navigation pattern in the water maze. These findings support the link between spatial orientation and navigation in the PFC, but also suggest that impairment could result from failure to organize spatial information required to produce cognitive maps of the immediate environment necessary for effective navigation and WM function (Chudasama & Robbins, 2006; Kolb et al., 1994; Newman et al., 2006; Tolman, 1949). Executive Functions Located in the PFC are Synonymous with Navigational Deficits: Single Cell Recordings and fMRI Studies As referenced by Curtis and Lee (2010), persistent neuronal activity in the PFC from a transient stimulus and resulting behavioral response also provides evidence for 20 WM processes by way of the central executive. Rodent “executive control” can be exhibited during the training of navigational tasks which require previous trial information to be integrated within the context of current task demands. To summarize, previous task experiences are used to make predictions about future tasks, in which behaviors are adjusted to produce the most desired outcome in the spatial navigation task (Curtis and Lee, 2010; de Saint Blanquat et al., 2010). Additionally, the rodent mPFC region has been implicated in the management of executive processes when completing a navigation task (de Saint Blanquat et al., 2010). In this experiment the ability to learn the location of the four rewarded radial arms in an eight arm radial maze was measured during single cell recordings of the PFC. Neural correlates of the PFC were shown to reflect navigation to various baited and non-baited radial arms (de Saint Blanquat et al., 2010). These results represent the neural correlates associated with the identification of previously baited arms and the executive control of decision making during navigation of the radial arm maze. PFC involvement in neurodegenerative disease found in animals and humans Pathological aging in humans is represented by neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease and fronto-temporal lobe dementia. Alzheimer’s disease is the most common neurodegenerative disease of the elderly, categorized by the presence of neurofibullary plaques and tangles found in the hippocampus and temporal cortical tissues (Kendel, Schwartz, & Kendle, 2000). Amyloid plaques build up between the neurons in the brain tissues in conjunction with proteins called Beta Amyloid, accumulating to produce insoluble, hard bundles around neurons. Tangles or stringy 21 fibers clumped together inside the neurons of the brain are formed out of Tau proteins, which help carry nutrients to the neurons. The accumulation of these tangles inhibit neural connections causing cell death and resulting atrophy (Kendel, Schwartz, & Kendle, 2000. Older adult populations with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type often sustain severe hippocampal, PFC, and temporal lobe atrophy (Sweeny & Ranstack, 2009) concurrent with cognitive impairment. Cognitive impairment in the disease presentation is most often characterized by an amnestic type, affecting memory processes. However, several cognitive domains such as executive functioning, language, and visuospatial processes have also been implicated in contributing to chronic disease progression (Tomaszewski Farias et al., 2011). Several fMRI studies, (Olichney et al., 2010; Nordahl et al. 2006; Nordahl et al., 2005), have also validated the presence of multiple cognitive domains regulated by frontal cortical systems (Marshuetz, Reuter-Lorenz, Smith, Jonides, & Noll, 2006), specifically memory and executive functions which regulate verbal encoding and the episodic memory of autobiographical events. Frontotemporal Dementia (FTD) is also affected by similar structural neurodegeneration as Alzheimer’s disease with acute frontal and temporal lobe atrophy. The manifestations of disease present as behavioral abnormalities, often mistaken for psychiatric disorders, in which dramatic changes to personality, social or emotional inhibitions, and the ability to use and understand language (Stuss & Knight, 2002). Previously defined as Pick’s Disease, this form of dementia is characterized by pick-like bodies which occur in the affected frontal lobe and surrounding regions. Pick bodies 22 contain abnormal spherical Tau proteins which accumulate within neurons, damaging neural connections. In addition to perfuse gray and white matter atrophy of the frontotemporal region (Hodges, 2001), the amygdala, striatum, and hippocampal regions can also sustain considerable brain insult (Boccardi, 2006). Damage to the PFC can cause difficulty encoding and recalling contextual information, in which the ability to learn is impaired (Müller & Knight, 2006). In addition to these deficits, individuals can be prone to distraction, have long-term memory retrieval problems, and display deficient spatial WM during navigation (Hoge & Kesner, 2007; Kesner, 1998). The primary motor cortex located in the precentral gyrus, and the motor association cortex found rostral to the primary motor region in humans, is responsible for the movement of joints and motor preparation necessary for navigation (Kendel, Schwartz, & Kendle, 2000). Damage to these regions found in the frontal lobe can negatively affect spatial navigation functions often found in virtual reality paradigms where an inability to navigate using efficient routes from cues in the environment is important for orientation (Newman et al., 2007), and arguably the cognitive map. The presentation of symptoms in animals after the PFC sustains damage is similar to the symptoms found in humans with neurodegenerative disorders In rodent studies, perseveration has been observed after the PFC was lesioned, suggesting a deficit in the ability to hold information in WM sufficiently in order to develop a behavioral navigation strategy to reach a new stimulus (Gemmell & O’Mara, 1999). Perseveration occurs when attention is fixed to one task, and that task is consistently repeated over a period of time. When perseveration is present during 23 navigational tasks, it may represent an impairment in “response shifting” to a new stimulus allowing methodical navigation toward it in the relative environment (Gemmell & O’Mara, 1999). As discussed in earlier sections, this behavior is also a function of executive control and requires attention necessary for planning and organization of actions (Chudasama & Robbins, 2006), a function which is likely controlled by the PFC. This perseveration is also found in humans with dementia disorders, such as Alzheimer’s disease. Damage to the frontal lobe can also inhibit the ability to organize and plan behaviors in humans with FTD. Dopaminergic neural connections in both humans and animals are comprised in the PFC region (Chudasama & Robbins, 2006; Stuss & Knight, 2002; Kendel, Schwartz, & Kendle, 2000). In a study designed to test the effects of dopamine on behavior, dopamine was depleted from that area in monkeys using an injection of 6-hydroxydopamine, destroying post-synaptic dopamine terminals in neurons. Following this, monkeys were unable to remember the stimuli after a short delay, exhibiting WM dysfunction similar to humans with lesions of the same area (Kendel, Schwartz, & Kendle, 2000). Human neuropsychological testing involving the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task is also designed to test frontal lobe functioning using set shifting rules with cards designed with colored shapes and symbols. Impairment on this test would also present with impairment in response-shifting commonly exhibited by perseveration (Stuss & Knight, 2002). Tests of visual discrimination in monkeys apply these same response-shift concepts to test the ability of “reversal learning” involved in remembering the changing rules of the task in order to accomplish a goal, also known as 24 set shifting (Stuss & Knight, 2002). The serial reaction time task (5CSRTT) for animals is modeled after the human continuous performance test (CPT), used to measure attentional processes to monitor infrequent and random pictures of letters; a process dependent on neural networks of the PFC (Chudasama & Robbins, 2006). The animal paradigm involves food reinforcement and an operant chamber with apertures required to monitor rodent attention and response accuracy to attend to the correct stimulus. These procedures are used to examine the impulsivity of responses or inhibition, as well as compulsions to perseverate toward one stimulus verses others and the level of motivation to complete the task (Chudasama & Robbins, 2006). These are common behavioral deficits exhibited by individuals with Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal lobe dementias. Complex Diseases such as FTD and Dementia of the Alzheimer’s Type can Benefit from Research in Animal Models Animal research has been critical in understanding new models of disease, in particular the understanding of the PFC structures and function (Stuss & Knight, 2002). Animal models of cognition provide advantages with minimal cohort differences by eliminating variation in age, education, and intelligence; thereby determining a more precise contribution between various brain structures and corresponding cognitive behavior (Chudasama & Robbins, 2006). Studies involving lesions also have similar benefits with the ability to resect a region and duplicate the procedure to determine neural pathways responsible for behavior and cognition during particular cognitive tasks. Even imaging and single cell recordings in animals have advanced our understanding of the 25 functions dependent on the PFC. Research on the learning abilities and plasticity of PFC neurons in particular have challenged the idea that brain areas are only responsible for certain processes (Stuss & Knight, 2002). In this respect, the contribution of animal research regarding PFC systems and functions has proved to be indispensable in advancing our understanding of complex disease processes in humans. 26 Chapter 2 METHOD It is hypothesized that rodents with mPFC lesions will show indications of impaired navigation when compared with controls during a path integration task. This hypothesis has been constructed from the assumptions introduced above, namely that rodents with lesions of mPFC will possess deficits in WM and our hypothesis that navigation through the use of path integration relies on WM processes. Since evidence suggests an analogous relationship between the PFC of rats and humans, it can also be suggested that any observed WM or navigational deficit would be similar to those found in humans. Deficits in navigation and WM are also commonly found in many neurodegenerative diseases such as: Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal lobe dementia. The similarities between animal and human deficits found in navigation and WM would also bring further insight into the process of human neurodegeneration. Animals Twenty adult female Long–Evans rats, weighing 250–300 gm, were housed in wire metal cages located in the Department of Psychology animal vivarium on the campus of California State University, Sacramento. They were maintained on a 16/8 hour light/dark cycle in a room with a consistent temperature of 20–21°C. A food restricted diet was used to establish motivation during navigational tasks. Water was freely available. All cages were located next to one another to ensure social contact. 27 Apparatus The Whishaw Tabletop Homing Task (Whishaw & Maaswinkel, 1998) was utilized to assess the effects of mPFC lesions on path integration navigation and WM. Figure 2 presents a picture of the apparatus used in the task. A circular wooden table 204 cm in diameter and elevated 64 cm off the floor served as the primary apparatus. A bearing located in the middle of the table allowed rotation during the individual task trials. The table was painted white to eliminate landmark cues and contained eight holes, 11.5 cm each, spaced at equal distances from each other and located 13.5 cm from edge of the table. Below one of these holes was the refuge ( home cage), where the rat was able to enter and exit the table with the food pellet for each trial. The top of the table was marked with 17 pseudo-randomly placed black dots located relatively equal distance from one another. These black dots were made with black permanent marker rather than raised black markers or cups to decrease landmark cues during trials. During each trial, a sugar pellet (Bioserve; Frenchtown, NJ) was randomly placed on one of the 17 black dots. The apparatus was located in a classroom with many cues such as, tables, chairs, computers, cupboards, and a sink. However, these cues were available only during the initial phase of pretraining described below. During other phases the lights were extinguished and curtains were drawn around the table to eliminate the use of external visual cues from the testing room. An infrared camera was positioned over the table to document performance and navigation behavior. The signal from the camera was viewable live in an adjacent room away from the training and was also saved to videotape for later analysis. 28 Pre-Surgical Training Pre-surgical training was necessary to habituate the rats to the path integration task prior to commencing the experimental procedure. Pre-training involved placing the rat in the refuge cage, rewarding the rat for climbing out of the refuge onto the table, navigating across the surface of the table to the sugar pellet, and returning to the refuge to eat the sugar pellet. Pre-surgical trials began by placing one sugar pellet near the entrance/exit of the home cage with a small cardboard box surrounding the region occluding all visual cues. Over time, the rat began to search for the pellet on the table. The cardboard box was then removed, and the sugar pellet was placed randomly on one of the black dots marked on the table. Once the rat entered the table it was given five minutes to find the sugar pellet and return with it to the refuge. If this time limit was reached or the rat located the sugar pellet, the trial would end and the rat would have 30 seconds to reach the refuge, after which it would be removed from the table by the experimenter. Each rat was rewarded with one additional sugar pellet if the task was completed successfully. If the sugar pellet was consumed on the table or the task was not completed, a reward was not given. Each rat was rotated in order though the pre-training trails. For each trial the locations of the pellet and home cage would change through random assignment. After a rat completed 20 consecutive trials with at least a 95% success rate, the procedure was repeated (without the initial carboard box phase) with the lights turned off and the curtains drawn to eliminate visual cues. After the animal performed the task successfully 29 95% of the time over a 30 day period the animal was moved to the surgical phase of the experiment. Surgical procedure A surgical procedure approved by the CSUS Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee was used. After the pre-training phase ten of the rats received surgical neurotoxic lesions of the mPFC and the ten remaining rats underwent a sham operation. Prior to surgery, the rodents were anesthetized with an intra-muscular injection containing ketamine (30 mg/kg), xylazine (6 mg/kg), and acepromazine (1 mg/kg). The head was then shaved and disinfected using Betadine. After being placed in the stereotaxic device, an incision was made to expose the skull. In the case of the rats receiving neurotoxic ibotenic acid lesions (Jarrard, 2002), burr holes were drilled in the skull and injections were made at two sites by slow infusion of ibotenic acid (0.06 M in sterile phosphate buffer saline solution; 0.3 μl) over 3 minutes using a 0.5 μl Hamilton syringe with a 30-gauge beveled needle (Tait et al., 2009). Injections were administered with the bevel of the needle facing medially using the following coordinates relative to Bregma provided by Paxinos & Watson (1996): (1) AP = +2.5 mm; ML = +0.6 mm; DV = -5.0 mm, and (2) AP = +3.5 mm; ML = +0.6 mm; DV = -5.2 mm, (Tait et al., 2009). For each injection, the needle was left in place for two minutes to allow for diffusion. Once the lesion procedure had been completed, the incision was sutured closed and a topical Neosporin antibiotic was placed on the site. The rats receiving sham surgeries experienced the same procedures except that no burr holes were drilled and no neurotoxin was injected. Analgesics were given for one to two days following the surgical 30 procedure. Prior to post-surgical testing, the rodents were allowed to recover for an average of 25 days. Post-Surgical Testing Following the recovery period, each animal identity was recoded by non-research personnel to ensure that the testing was performed blindly relative to the identity of the animals. The rats were then giving testing for 5 trials each using the same testing method as the pre-surgical training phase. During the testing period, rats in both the control and lesioned group were subjected to up to four path integration trials per day. Infrared video recordings were made of these trials for later scoring purposes. Analysis After post-surgical testing, all video recordings for lesioned and control groups were individually analyzed to document navigational performance. Performance was analyzed by counting the number of incorrect refuge choices on each trial. A refuge choice was indicated if the snout of the rat was within 3 cm of the hole. Latency of emergence, the time the pellet was grabbed, and the time necessary to find the correct refuge entrance was also assessed, as well as the frequency of emergence onto the table. The total time was calculated in seconds after the body of the rat entered onto the table in search of the food pellet. Once the food pellet was retrieved, time was then calculated from the pellet to the correct refuge choice (Tait, Marston, Shahid, & Brown, 2009; Whishaw & Maaswinkel, 1998). Navigational routes were documented and “wandering” or perseverative behaviors were noted if applicable. 31 To determine if lesioned and control animals differed in these measures, an analysis of variance (ANOVA) tests were conducted. 32 Chapter 3 RESULTS The total sample included 191 trials, with pre-operative (111 trials) and postoperative trial observations (80 trials) for 16 rats. Out of the 20 original rats, three rats died of surgical complications and one developed health complications that required its removal from the study. Differences between lesion and sham groups in pre-surgical training performance were assessed using each measure of latency, the frequency of emergence onto the table, and number of errors. Results of the seven one-way between subjects ANOVAs determined that the emergence time [F(1, 14) = 0.0002, p = 0.99)], the time to take the pellet [F(1, 14) = 1.27, p = 0.28)], the time to return to the cage [F(1, 14) = 0.81, p = 0.38)], the difference between the emergence time and the time the pellet was grabbed [F(1, 14) = 2.44, p = 0.14)], the difference between the time the pellet was grabbed and the time to return to the cage [F(1, 14) = 3.32, p = 0.09)], the number of times on the table [F(1, 14) = 1.80, p = 0.20)], and the frequency of error [F(1, 14) = 0.62, p = 0.44)] were not statistically significant (see Table 1 in Appendix C). The fact that the groups did not show differences in the pre-surgical training phase is important, as it demonstrates that the groups were equivalent prior to the surgical procedure. The average performance for each group across the five post-training test trials are presented in Table 2 of Appendix D, and Figures 3 and 4 present these measures calculated for each individual test trial. To determine a possible main effect of the lesion 33 condition on post-surgical performance, seven 2 x 5 (Groups x Trials) ANOVAs were performed on the post-surgical test data on each of the dependent measures. For the dependent measure of emergence time, the analyses showed no significant main effect for group [F(1, 36) = 3.08, p = 0.09], trials [F(4, 36) = 0.64, p = 0.09] or the interaction between these factors [F(4, 36) = 0.42, p = 0.79]. For the measure of the time the pellet was grabbed, the main effect of group was not significant [F(1, 36) = 0.18, p = 0.67], nor was the main effect of trials [F(4, 36) = 0.02, p = 1.0], or the interaction between the two factors [F(4, 36) = 1.0, p = 0.42]. On the variable of the time the rodent returned to the home cage the main effect of group was not significant [F(1, 36) = 0.14, p = 0.72], nor was the main effect of trials [F(4, 36) = 0.01, p = 1.0], or the interaction [F(4, 36) = 1.0, p = 0.42]. Regarding the difference between the time of emergence and the time the pellet was grabbed, there was no main effect of group [F(4, 36) = 0.02, p = 0.96], trials [F(4, 36) = 0.10, p = 0.98], or interaction between these factors [F(4, 36) = 1.11, p = 0.37]. Similarly, regarding the difference between the time the pellet was grabbed and the time returned to the home cage, there was no main effect of group [F(1, 36) = 01.08, p = 0.31], trials [F(4, 36) = 1.51, p = 0.22], or interaction between the two [F(4, 36) = 0.23, p = 0.92] (see Figure 3 in Appendix E). Measures of frequency also failed to show significant differences, including the frequency of emergence main effect of group [F(1, 36) = 0.69, p = 0.41], trials [F(3, 36) = 1.47, p = 0.23], and interaction [F(1, 4) = 0.24, p = 0.91] and number of errors main effect of group [F(1, 36) = 0.06, p = 0.81], trials [F(4, 36) = 1.54, p = 0.21], and the interaction between these factors [F(4, 36) = 0.19, p = 0.94] were also 34 not significant (see Figure 4, in Appendix F). To summarize, there were no significant differences in post-operative performance between lesioned and sham animals. Histology Following the completion of the study, the histological study of the brains of lesioned rats was conducted to determine the extent of the neurotoxic lesions. During this process, the rats were anesthetized deeply and perfused with saline and then a 10% formalin solution. After two formalin rinses over a 48 hour period, the brains were cut into 40μm slices in the coronal plane, and stained with cresyl violet. Lesion extent was determined by microscopic examination of obvious cell death and identified by the largest and smallest affected area at selected coordinates from Paxinos and Watson (1996). All lesions were consistent with complete mPFC ablation (see Figure 3 in Appendix G). 35 Chapter 4 DISCUSSION The present study tested the hypothesis that rodents with neurotoxic lesions of the mPFC are impaired relative to sham controls in a path integration task designed to assess navigation and possibly WM. Results failed to find differences between rodents trained in the task prior to surgery verses those that received the sham or lesion surgery and completed post-surgical testing. Measures of mean latency suggested that post-surgical (both sham and control) animals took longer than their pre-surgical counterparts to emerge from the home cage and once the pellet was grabbed, path integrate back to the home cage. Whereas the lesion group took longer than the sham control group to emerge and locate the pellet, grab the pellet and return to the home cage. The difference between the time of emergence and the time the pellet was retrieved was also found to be longer than the sham control group. However, since the differences were slight, and most likely due to the effects of surgery, none of the interactions were statistically significant. Additional measures found that the post-surgical lesioned group committed more errors on average than sham controls and that the sham group had a higher frequency for the number of times on the table in order to find the food pellet, but again these differences were not found to be significant. Potential differences in the frequency of emergence from the refuge was first noted as a behavioral observation. Rats searching for the pellet appeared to be monitoring the location of the home cage by creating various 36 short trajectories in order to cover different areas on the apparatus. During the test procedure it was suspected that this navigation technique was used to compensate for possible WM impairment resulting from the inability to locate the home cage once the pellet was retrieved. Altogether, however, the hypothesis that mPFC lesions would impair path integration navigation was not supported. The failure to uncover deficits in the path integration task following mPFC lesions could be due to several possibilities. First, path integration navigation may not depend on WM processes as originally hypothesized. Our view that path integration relies on working memory is based on the fact that previous movement events are continually integrated in order to provide a “real time” estimate of position in space. While this process likely contains some demands on the WM system, the demand on the system may not be as strong as hypothesized. As an example, suppose the animal travels to position A, then B, then C, before finally arriving at point D. A path integration navigation strategy that would impose a high demand on the WM system might require the animal to retain all of the previous movements leading to the arrival at position D. On the other hand, a navigation strategy that would impose a lower demand on the WM system would involve the animal only retaining in memory the most recent position and the movements since leaving that position. Diffuse neural networks unaffected by the lesion could also explain the plasticity of WM and navigation processes. Structures such as the cerebellum, responsible for motor memory during navigation, could potentially regulate WM functional impairments to a greater degree 37 than anticipated. It is possible that our lesions only marginally impaired WM and hence did not produce a measurable effect on path integration. On a related note, it is possible that the mPFC lesions utilized were somehow ineffective at impairing WM processes as expected. This could have occurred if our lesions were not inclusive or if other areas of the brain besides mPFC are involved in working memory, such as the hippocampus, amygdala, or striatum. This possibility could have been assessed by inclusion of a task that is already recognized as a valid measurement of working memory performance, such as the working memory version of the radial arm maze where the rats must remember the previously visited arms to avoid entering an unbaited arm (Izaki, Takita, and Akema, 2008; Jones, 2002). Finally, it is possible that the present methods were insensitive at detecting impaired path integration performance in our animals. For instance, the small sample size (especially in the lesion group due to postsurgical mortality) likely resulted in a decreased ability to detect lesion effects. A sample of 20 or more in each group would have been optimal. In addition, an extended postsurgical testing period may have allowed group differences to emerge. Finally, no direct controls were utilized to ensure that the animals were truly performing the task via path integration rather than using an undetected landmark. Conclusions and Future Study This study failed to provide evidence that mPFC lesions in rats results in a deficit in path integration processes. Additional research is needed to assess the theory that mPFC lesions contribute to functional behavioral impairment similar to humans with 38 neurodegenerative diseases. However, results can be compared to the research in aforementioned sections which describe the plasticity of the PFC which produces complex behaviors (see review by Stuss & Knight, 2002) such as path integration. Although results were negative regarding impairment in this task following mPFC lesions, one could argue that successful completion of the path integration task, even prior to PFC ablation, is evidence of higher cortical functioning and the presence of intact WM processes. 39 APPENDIX A Subcortical Structures of the Rodent Brain Figure 1. Figure adapted from Paxinos, G. & Watson, C. (1996) 40 APPENDIX B An Infrared Picture of a Post-Surgical Rat Figure 2. An Infrared Picture of a Post-Surgical Rat Completing the Path Integration Task Using the Whishaw Table Top Apparatus 41 APPENDIX C Table 1 Pre-Operative Experimental Measures of Latency, Frequency of Emergence, and Error in Lesioned and Sham Controls for Five Trials, N =16. Measures Procedure Sham Lesion Emergence Time Seconds 18.48 (19.53) 10.53 (7.79) Time Grabbed 67.55 (67.86) 79.42 (68.58) Time Returned to Cage/Total Time 96.0 (78.08) 90.04 (68.14) 1.0 (0.0) 1.27 (0.64) 0.44 (0.71) 0.27 (0.52) Difference Emergence-Grabbed Pellet 49.07 (65.62) 68.88 (68.72) Difference Grabbed Pellet-Cage 28.45 (42.10) 10.62 (7.93) Times on Table Errors Note. Numbers in parentheses are standard deviations. 42 APPENDIX D Table 2 Post-Operative Experimental Measures of Latency, Frequency of Emergence, and Error in Lesioned and Sham Controls for Five Trials, N =16. Measures Procedure Sham Lesion Emergence Time Seconds 20.73 (22.16) 37.56 (44.27) Time Grabbed 123.14 (89.05) 187.86 (157.89) Time Returned to Cage/Total Time 135.41 (88.45) 196.97 (157.66) Times on Table 1.59 (1.01) 1.38 (0.82) Errors 0.59 (0.91) 0.5 (0.93) Difference Emergence-Grabbed Pellet 102.41 (79.75) 150.30 (159.66) Difference Grabbed Pellet-Cage 12.27 (10.37) 9.13 (4.89) Note. Numbers in parentheses are standard deviations. 43 APPENDIX E Figure 3. Error and Frequency of Emergence by Condition and Procedure 44 APPENDIX F Figure 4. Measures of Latency by Condition in Lesion & Sham Groups 45 APPENDIX G mPFC Neurotoxic Lesion Extent Figure 5. Black and gray regions indicate maximum and minimum ablation. Figure adapted from Paxinos, G. & Watson, C. (1996) 46 References Baddeley, A. & Hitch (1994). Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology, 8(4), 485-493. Baddeley, A. (2002). Is working memory still working? European Psychologist, 7(2), 85-97. Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Science, 4, 417-423. Baldo, J. & Shimamura, A. (2002). Letter and category fluency in patients with frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychology, 12(2), 259-267. Bechara A., Damasio H., Tranel D., & Anderson S. (1998). Dissociation of working Memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 428–437. Boccardi, M. (2006). MRI studies in frontotemporal dementia. Department of Neurology Series of Reports, 80, 1-109. Calton, J. & Taube, J. (2009). Where am I and how will I get there from here? A role for posterior parietal cortex in the integration of spatial information and route planning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 91, 186-196. Chudasama, Y. & Robbins, T. (2006). Functions of frontostriatal systems in cognition: Comparative neuropsychopharmacological studies in rats, monkeys, and humans. Biological Psychology, 73, 19-35. 47 Compton, D., Griffith, H., McDaniel, W., Foster, R., & Davis, B. (1997). The flexible use of multiple cue relationships in spatial navigation: A comparison of water maze performance following hippocampal, medial septal, prefrontal cortex, or posterior parietal cortex lesions. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 68, 117-132. Curtis, C. & Lee, D. (2010). Beyond working memory: the role of persistent activity in decision making. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(5), 216-222. De Saint Blanquat, P., Hok, V., Alvernhe, A., Save, E., & Poucet B. (2010). Behavioural Brain Research, 209(2), 267-273. Desimone, R. (1996). Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93(24), 13494-13499. Farovik, A., Dupont, L., Arce, M., & Eichenbaum, H. (2008). Medial prefrontal cortex supports recollection, but not familiarity, in the rat. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28(50), 13428-13434. Gallistel, C.R. (1990). The Organization of Learning, Cambridge, MA, MIT Press. Gemmell, C. & O’Mara, S. (1999). Medial prefrontal cortex lesions cause deficits in a variable-goal location task but not in object exploration. Behavioral Neuroscience, 113(3), 465-474. Granon, S. & Poucet, B. (1995). Medial prefrontal lesion in the rat and spatial navigation: Evidence for impaired planning. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109(3), 474-484. 48 Heidbreder, C. & Groenewegen, H. (2003). The medial prefrontal cortex in the rat: Evidence for a dorso-ventral distinction based upon functional and anatomical characteristics. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 27, 555-579. Hodges, J. (2001). Frontotemportal dementia (pick’s disease): Clinical features and assessment. Neurology, 56, 6-10. Hoge, J. & Kesner, R. (2007). Role of CA3 and CA1 subregions of the dorsal hippocampus on temporal processing of objects. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 88(2), 225-231. Huntly & Howard (2010). Working memory in early alzheimer’s disease: A neuropsychological review. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25(2), 121-132. Hoover, W. & Vertes, R. (2007). Anatomical analysis of afferent projections to the medial prefrontal cortex in the rat. Brain Structure and Function, 212(2), 149179. Izaki, Y., Takita, M., & Akema, T. (2008). Specific role of the posterior dorsal hippocampus-prefrontal cortex in short-term working memory. European Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 3029-3034. Jacobsen CF (1935) Functions of the frontal association area in primates. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 33, 558-569. Jarrard, L. (2002). Use of excitotoxins to lesion the hippocampus: update. Hippocampus, 12, 405-414. 49 Jo, Y., Hye Park, E., Kwon Park, S., Kim, H., Taek Kim, H., & June-Seek, C. (2007). The medial prefrontal cortex is involved in spatial memory retrieval under partialcue conditions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(49), 13567-13578. Jones, M. (2002). A comparative review of rodent prefrontal cortex and working memory. Current Molecular Medicine, 2, 639-647. Kandel, E., Schwartz, J., & Jessell, T. (2000). Principles of Neural Science, 4th ed., (pp. 349-380). New York: McGraw-Hill. Kesner, R., Hunt, M., Williams, J., & Long, J. (1996). Prefrontal cortex and working memory for spatial response, spatial location, and visual object information in the rat. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 311-318. Kesner R. (1998). Neural mediation of memory for time: role of hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex. Psychological Bulletin Reviews, 5, 585–596. Kolb B. (1990). Prefrontal Cortex. Kolb B. & Tees R., (Ed.), The Cerebral Cortex of the Rat. (pp. 437–458). Cambridge, MA: MIT. Kolb, B., Buhrmann, K., McDonald, R., & Sutherland, R. (1994). Dissociation of the medial prefrontal, posterior parietal, and posterior temporal cortex for spatial navigation and recognition memory in the rat. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 664-680. Marshuetz, C., Reuter-Lorenz, P., Smith, E., Jonides, J., Noll, D. (2006). Working memory for order and the parietal cortex: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroscience, 139, 311-316. Mittelstaedt, M. L., & Mittelstaedt, H. (1980). Homing by path integration in a mammal, Naturwissenschaften, 67, 566-567. 50 Miller, E. & Cohen, J. (2001). In integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review: Neuroscience., 24, 167-202. Müller, N. & Knight, R. (2006). The functional neuroanatomy of working memory: Contributions of human brain lesion studies. Neuroscience, 139, 51-58. Newman, E., Caplan, J., Kirschen, M., Korolev, I., Sekuler, R., & Kahana, M. (2006). Learning your way around town: How virtual taxicab drivers learn to use both layout and landmark information. Cognition, 104, 231-253. Nordahl, C., Ranganath, C., Yonelinas, A., DeCarli, C., Reed, B., & Jagust, W. (2005). Different mechanisms of episodic memory failure in mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1688-1697. Nordahl, C., Ranganath, C., Yonelinas, A., Decarli, C., Fletcher, E., Jagust, W. (2006). White matter changes compromise prefrontal cortex function in healthy elderly individuals. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 418-429. Olichney, J., Taylor, J.R., Chan, S., Yang J., Stringfellow, A., Hillert, D.G., Simmons, A.L., Salmon, D.P., Iragui-Madoz, V., Kutas, M. (2010). fMRI responses to words repeated in a congruous semantic context are abnormal in mild alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia, 48(9), 2476-2487. Ongur, D. & Price J. (2000). The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys, and humans. Cerebral Cortex, 10(3), 206-219. Paxinos, G. & Watson, C. (1996). The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates (3rd ed.). New York: Academic Press. 51 Prabhakaran, V., Narayanan, K., Zhao, Z., and Gabrieli, J.D. (2000). Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 85–90. Preuss T. (1995). Do rats have prefrontal cortex? The rose-woolsey-akert program reconsidered. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 1–24. Ptito, A., Crane, J., Leonard, G., Amsel, R., & Caramanos, Z. (1995). Visualspatial localization by patients with frontal-lobe lesions invading or sparing area 46. Neuroreport, 6, 1781–1784. Rushworth, M., Nixon, P., Eacott, M., Passingham, R. (1997). Ventral prefrontal cortex is not essential for working memory. Journal Neuroscience, 17, 4829–4838. Shimamura, A., Janowsky, J., & Squire, L. (1990). Memory for the temporal order of events in patients with frontal lobe lesions and amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 28, 803–813. Stuss, D. T. & Knight, R., (Ed.), (2002). Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. (pp. 1-640). New York, New York: Oxford University Press Inc. Sweeney, M. & Restack, R. (2009). Brain: The complete mind. Learning & Memory (pp. 235-247). National Geographic Society. Tait, D., Marston, H., Shahid, M., & Brown, V. (2009). Asenapine restores cognitive flexibility in rats with medial prefrontal cortex lesions. Psychopharmacology, 202(1-3), 295-306. Tolman, E.C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55, 189208. 52 Tomaszewski Farias, S., Mungas, D., Reed, B., Carmichael, O., Beckett, L., Harvey, D., Olichney, J., Simmons, A., & DeCarli, C. (2011). Maximal brain size remains an important predictor of cognition in old age, independent of current brain pathology. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(8), 1758-1768. Touretzky & Redish. (1996). Theory of rodent navigation based on interacting representations of space. Hippocampus, 6, 247-270. Uylings, H., Groenewegen, H., & Kolb, B. (2003). Do rats have a prefrontal cortex? Behavior Brain Research, 146, 3–17. Vertes, R. (2006). Interactions among the medial prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and midline thalamus in emotional and cognitive processing in the rat. Neuroscience, 142, 1–20. Wenk (2001). Assessment of spatial memory using the T maze. Current Protocols in Neuroscience, 8(8), 8.5B1-8.5B7. Whishaw, I. & Tomie (1998). Piloting and dead reckoning dissociated by fimbria-fornix lesions in a rat food carrying task. Behavioral Brain Research, 89(1-2), 87-97. Whishaw, I. & Maaswinkel, H. (1998). Rats with fimbria-fornix lesions are impaired in path integration: A role for the hippocampus in “sense of direction.” The Journal of Neuroscience, 18(8), 3050-3058. Wincour, G. & Eskes, G. (1998). Prefrontal cortex and caudate nucleus in conditioning associative learning: Dissociated effects of selective brain lesions in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 112(2), 89-101. 53 Wolbers, T., Wiener, J., Mallot, H., & Büchel, C. (2007). Differential recruitment of the hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex, and the human motion complex during path integration in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(35), 9408-9416.