Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 1 Running Head: HIJACKED CONVERSATIONS? Hijacked conversations in counseling? Tom Strong PhD Division of Applied Psychology University of Calgary Presentation to the Social Construction and Caring Relationships conference, Lugano, Switzerland, May 18, 2007 All correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Tom Strong, Division of Applied Psychology, University of Calgary, 2500 University Drive NW, Calgary, Alberta Canada T2N 1N4 or strongt@ucalgary.ca Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 2 Abstract From a view that collaborative dialogue requires negotiating meanings and ways of talking agreeable to both client and counselor, this paper takes sideways glances at a stark alternative: conversational hijacking. Conversational hijacking is an invented term to denote how counseling dialogues can be overtaken by some monologic tendencies, tendencies often reinforced in counselors’ training and institutional expectations. Examples of conversational hijacking can include shoehorning counseling into the conversational scripts and protocols of particular theoretical models or institutional expectations, overtaking clients with counselor passions and certainties, and permitting oneself to be conversationally hijacked by clients. The general thrust in this paper is on how to keep and optimize counseling a dialogic encounter that engages the resourcefulness of both client and counselor. Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 3 Genuine dialogue requires at least two differently situated persons who recognize their mutual embeddedness in one another’s lives, who meet and work together. Conditions that thwart this possibility, whether based on a refusal to recognize the embedded nature of persons or on power arrangements that transform dialogues into monologues held by the powerful in order to rule the rest, undermine any serious hope that justice can prevail. E. Sampson, 1993, p. 174 I’d like to talk today about collaboration in counseling, but to do this I will refer to hijacking conversations. I recognize that talking about “hijacking conversations” in this post 911 world is a dramatic, if not desperate, way to try to draw attention to my preferred topic – resourceful and collaborative dialogue between clients and counselors. Counselors are clearly not similar to terrorists and hijackers. They mean well and generally help clients pursue the goals and concerns that brought them to counseling. They can be immensely influential as well. Their influences on clients and counseling – intended or unintended - are not to be under-estimated. I will use the word conversation a number of ways as we proceed. Sometimes I will borrow from Gadamer (1988), Foucault (1972), or Wittgenstein (1953) in suggesting that there are large cultural conversations in which we participate and that significantly shape our lives. Other times I take up a view closer to Erving Goffman (1967) or Derek Edwards and Jonathan Potter (1992) for whom important stuff happens in face to face dialogue. I will shift between macro and micro views of conversation and how counselors participate in both. Each kind of conversation can be a dialogue when it offers participants ways to influence or negotiate how it occurs. Conversational hijacking occurs when such negotiations or influence are not possible. Counselors generally seek open, responsive and generative dialogues with clients and I believe would be appropriately dismayed with any effort to hijack conversation, how ever well intended. Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 4 Hijacking? Conversational hijacking, in my view, occurs when dialogues and their related negotiations are overtaken by monologues or monologic tendencies (more on those later). To me, the dialogues of counseling offer clients opportunities - rare elsewhere - to reflect upon their use of language, and to find or negotiate alternative language to move forward. Clients can become ‘linguistically impoverished’ (Vico, 1984), so counseling can be where they join counselors in putting personally apt words to understandings worth enacting. But, such dialogue is an elusive thing and seldom occurs as well as it might. Conversational hijacking often occurs without counselors and clients being aware of it. People normally do not try to take conversational ‘hostages’ but they aren’t fans of conversations that don’t go their way, or as they professionally ‘should’, either. Avoiding this entails collaborative conversational work that doesn’t fit for counselors who see themselves as experts on clients’ concerns or the counseling process. It is further compounded when clients show up acculturated to defer to counselor expertise, but do what they want to beyond counseling regardless of what happens there. Needed, in my view, are dialogues that clients and counselors can mutually claim as their own for how each participates in them. Conversational hijacking as I am describing it stands in stark contrast with my dreamer’s view of resourceful dialogue (I am not alone, see also Anderson, 1997; Bakhtin, 1981; Buber, 1958; McNamee & Gergen, 1999; Sampson, 1993), as a relational process that makes possible more than what each speaker could come up with on their own. I want to be clear that I am also not suggesting that counselors are only listeners or that they should be conversational eunuchs either. Clients generally seek more than dialogue within their monologues. Complicating things further, conversational hijacking isn’t something one could point out by stopping dialogue at any given time. People take turns in talking; some getting Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 5 more into talking at times than others. Nor am I suggesting deadening the counselor to enliven the client either. My preference is with how counselors can host conversations that engage the resourcefulness, passions, curiosities, discouragements, and hard-to-discuss features of clients’ and their lives – and how this all gets done with negotiable mutuality. Conversational hijacking comes with being held to any conversation that is non-negotiable, intended or otherwise. Hijacked by the Already Spoken Berger and Luckmann (1967) wrote years ago that conversation is our primary way of conserving understanding, problematic or otherwise. This extends as much to counselors as it does to clients. Either can get ensnared in closed or fixed circles of meaning that don’t serve them well as new developments arise. Counseling provides an opportunity to talk beyond the bounds of the already spoken, beyond meanings which have become fossilized or fetishized (Newman & Holzman, 1996). Life calls for people to consistently adapt but somehow this axiom seems to have not been extended to the language and ways of talking they use for such adapting. The already spoken might seem like an unusual hijacker. No overt hijacking is involved, and I contend that how this occurs owes more to dominant understandings of language than to one’s conversational partners. As Tom Andersen (1996) pointed out language is not innocent; it comes ‘peopled’ with others’ intentions (Bakhtin, 1981). Wittgenstein (1953; 1961/1921) said a lot relevant to conversational hijacking. For modern realists, he wrote one of the classic books on (Tractatus logico-philosophicus, 1961/1921) how rational science could benefit from a metalanguage. Most sciences, psychology included, try to develop such a metalanguage. It helps scientists talk to each other, conduct research, and convey scientifically derived and approved findings to the lay public, and so on. But, this linguistic uniformity (and it is anything but uniform across scientific disciplines) comes with fairly significant costs. Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 6 Wittgenstein came to think quite differently (1953) about language. For him, metalanguage was an unattainable ideal so he shifted focus to how people use their existing languages to go on in life (my emphasis on languages plural is intended). Everyday languages, while perhaps lacking in scientific precision, bring out aspects of life typically passed over in scientific discourse. Science seemed to promise certainties; but taken-for-granted certainties put to words have a habit of binding people, including counselors, to particular descriptions (Amundson, Valentine, & Stewart, 1993). One person’s ‘depression’ might, as different aspects of their everyday experience are discussed, more closely resemble oppression. That language was somehow connected to how one understands and relates to experience was not something people in Western societies reflected on much until recently. As John Heron once wrote, “The perceiving process becomes routinized by language. Its active participation sleep-walks inside the clothing of linguistic categories” (1996, p.16). Wittgenstein wanted us to wake up to how language shapes our experience in taken for granted ways, right down to how we understand and talk about our most subjective experiences. His shift on language will feel familiar to many counselors who have taken up a social constructionist approach to counseling. Wittgenstein’s wake-up call, so to speak, relates to his view that people could optimize their use of language in perspicuous representations and apt language games. He felt we too often are bewitched by language – in our descriptions and ways of conversing, something akin to the already spoken as a conversational hijacker. Perspicuous language suggests a linguistic ideal, but Wittgenstein’s point was that needed was language that helped people “go on” where they hadn’t been able to before. Some language serves us better than others, while less than adequate language often overtakes people who don’t get to reflect on alternatives. It is important to note Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 7 an absence of the word “correct” here. Effective, apt, fitting, adequate, satisfactory language can help us “go on”. Correct language – however this gets determined – can be a recipe for paralysis. Perspicuity? Finding perspicuous language – to aptly describe experience, to articulate personally meaningful and do-able solutions, to repair connections with others, and so on – can be an exacting process. Needed is a critical stance to reflect on the shortcomings of existing language, and a poetic and improvised knack at articulating alternatives. On the critical side, the already spoken can be like ‘hand-me-down’ clothing - ways of representing and communicating experience that don’t fit the user. On the poetic side, it can help to remember that different language can bring out relevant, unvoiced aspects of experience while pointing to connections that promote a sense of being able to “go on”. Perspicuous language need not be a solo project either; it has to be usable back in the relationships and contexts where it is required. Counseling affords resourceful possibilities for perspicuous conversations. There counselors and clients can get curious about the already spoken, particularly as this relates to clients’ concerns or aspirations. As Bakhtin (1981) might say, in such conversations speakers make words come alive in the back and forth of their talk. More important, perspicuous conversations can help clients reflect upon and confront being hijacked by the already spoken while joining counselors in finding apt language for describing their concerns and solutions. The already spoken hijacking how dialogue occurs There is another aspect to Wittgenstein’s wake-up call that relates to how conversations occur; perspicuous representations or descriptions focus more on what gets said. How people talk shapes what can be said. Counselors know this from differences associated with how certain approaches to counseling sanction certain kinds of questions, curiosities and manners of Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 8 engaging with clients. In everyday life, religious conversations produce different understandings than conversations about sports. These can be seen as examples of Wittgenstein’s notion of “language games”, conversations involving different kinds of participation with their associated meanings. These “language games” bear a distant family resemblance to Foucault’s (1972) discourses. Within either certain ways of being, understanding and interacting are required. Where Wittgenstein (1953) saw “language games” as “forms of life”, Foucault’s concern was with the often taken-for-granted dominance any discourse had on both meanings and ways of interacting. Conversation, for Foucault, often occurs in dominated ways I have been referring to as hijacking. Gregory Bateson’s (1980) concerns about patterning also speak to this ‘how dimension’ of conversation. Systemically trained therapists are attuned to problems in stuck patterns of conversation between family members and partners (e.g., Imber-Black, 1989; Tomm, 1991). These patterns have a hypnotic conversational inertia about them that sustain particular (often unhelpful) understandings and ways of relating (Ritterman, 1983). Such conversational patterns can also be seen as hijackers. Stale or restrictive language games, dominant discourses, and maladaptive patterns are forms of the already spoken that, through taken-for-granted perpetuation, work against adaptive, or perspicuous, ways of understanding and relating. Scientifically correct conversations in counseling? Behind these views is a sense of how the already spoken keeps hijacking conversations; those of clients as well as counselors. The barn door isn’t closed on a lot of things in counseling, especially on what happens or is possible in dialogue. But, a growing movement aims to hold counseling to correct conversations – evidence-based ones. An extreme version sees the first phase of counseling as a correctly conducted diagnostic interview of pre-scripted questions tied to decision trees with answers culminating in a diagnosis. Efforts to standardize this part of the Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 9 counseling interview (e.g., Fisher & O’Donohue, 2006; Jensen Doss, 2005) dovetail neatly with the protocols of correctly used evidence based interventions. Thus, it would seem, counseling’s conversations can be scientifically mapped then legislated as ethically correct practice (Bryceland & Stam, 2005). To administrators and a technologically oriented public this brings order and legitimacy to the unruly house of counseling where over 350 approaches now compete for clients. But, it is a trend that has concerned critical psychologists for some time (e.g., Henriques, Holloway, Urwin, Venn & Walkerdine, 1984; Newman & Holzman, 1996; Parker, 1999; Prilleltensky, 1994; Rose, 1990). Where some see dependable algorithms for practice (e.g., Rush, 2001), I see a form of conversational hijacking. The word warranting all this is “science”. Science itself can be seen as rigorous and contested conversations where understandings and ways of practice are continuously worked out (e.g., Hacking, 1983; Latour, 1988; Toulmin, 1990). Newton’s sense of life as a knowable, tinkerable machine still holds sway as a root metaphor in psychology while the hardest of sciences, like physics, have shifted to other ways of conceptualizing and conducting their business. My point is not to denigrate science – its accomplishments arise out of very carefully scrutinized conversations. But, to paraphrase Victor Turner (1975), modern science has a habit of translating metaphors into calculus. And so it goes with mapping out the statistically-supported diagnostic and intervention protocols of counseling. Further entrenching this very persistent metaphor for counseling’s conversations are health care administrators, the legal profession ever-wary about ‘malpractice’, and mental health professions who seek scientific and thus public accountability and legitimacy. Journalist, Gary Greenberg (2007), recently described his being a client as a “journey into the economy of melancholy” (p. 47) where clients and therapists took up parts in such pre-scripted conversations. Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 10 Things need not be that bleak for counseling, of course. While the evidence-based conversation seems on the rise, debates at the highest levels of the profession (e.g., Norcross, Beutler & Levant, 2006) continue to shape where this discussion may go. A flipside discussion that also finds a lot of resonance for counselors relates to Milton Erickson’s (see Zeig, 1980) notion that every client needs their own customized therapy. Counselors have long had to sort out the extent to which their conversations with clients might be hijacked by a form of methodolatry, something particularly relevant to when they are in face-to-face dialogue. Hijacking face-to-face dialogue While social constructionist counselors recognize meaning-making occurring at a cultural level, many feel uneasy about their meaning-making contributions when conversing with clients (Strong, 2003). Wary of Foucault’s cautions (2006) about the professional gaze and about counseling as another “truth regime”, some retreat into the seemingly benign professional stance of Carl Rogers (Bavelas, McGee, Phillips, & Routledge, 2000). Counseling, from this stance, is portrayed as a manifest activity where a counselor’s good listening skills are sufficient in helping clients arrive at meanings and actions they need. And, there are ways to ‘postmodernize’ Rogers’ (Anderson, 2001) client-centeredness which has deservedly become a core value of counselors. What I most borrow from Rogers is something that can get lost sight of at times: who the conversation is for, the client. When there isn’t a pre-scripted conversation to have with clients ethical and moral concerns arise as to how counseling should take place, and whose criteria should be used in evaluating its progress. Most commonly, clients’ goals are focal to the counseling, but in some counseling models (e.g., Egan, 1998) the counselor seems like a taxi driver. Once he or she has the client’s goal (or desired destination), it is their job to get the client there. “Good intentions” and strongly held concepts of what clients need come up here. Such Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 11 intentions have contributed to the tragedy playing out for many indigenous people, globally. As Samoan Just Therapist, Kiwi Tamasese said at a conference years ago, ‘psychology is the last colonizer of my people’. My colleague David Pare would add that counselors can ‘colonize’ with what they feel passionately about, most coercively so when there is something they feel their clients MUST understand or do. Clients always have the last say on counseling (they’ll think and do what they want to after, regardless), so holding them to the good intentions of our counseling models and passions doesn’t make sense and can be another way of hijacking the conversation. Negotiated Dialogues? When it became clear to me that collaborative dialogue translated to negotiations then my clinical and research interest turned toward how these negotiations take place (Strong, 2003). One of Bakhtin’s (1981) core ideas has become a personal mantra: the word in any dialogue is only half mine. My take on this is that any word I use in counseling needs to accommodate how clients will respond to its use – if I ‘let’ them. Maturana and Varela (1988) helped rid me of the notion that I could install or transmit my thoughts to clients’ (or your) heads and hearts. How we talk from our interpretive histories and preferences won’t permit this but missing seems a step in how speakers negotiate then share words and ways of talking. A close look at conversation shows people doing things with each other’s utterances, working out how they will understand and relate as they talk. Presumably, collaboration owes something to how counselor and client talk so clients can take useful things from their talking. How this occurs without conversational hijacking is my concern, so, let’s now turn, micro-dynamically, to these negotiations with clients. Conversation, where personal stakes and moral inferences matter to speakers - as they talk (Potter, 1996) - can be hard work. Speakers face a challenge of pursuing personal outcomes while sustaining their relationships. The libertarians and Hobbesians would have us view each Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 12 conversation as dog-eat-dog competition of the fittest. Besting our conversational partners over points of logic or talking down to them from some moral or informational high ground is a recipe for conflict or isolation. Some counselors are ambivalent on this point, particularly if they see their role as correcting or directing clients from their expertise. Professionally rigged dialogues? Hijacked conversations in counseling hold clients to particular meanings and actions - the counselor’s. Counselors might be offended when some critical psychologists and sociologists see counseling as a power play determined by how well clients learn to speak the counselor’s discourse or meet their criteria for ‘appropriate dialogue’ (e.g., Davis, 1984; Rose, 1990). Negotiating what constitutes therapeutic success and then being guided by this “success” is one way constructionist counselors level conversational hierarchies, customizing and revising counseling’s understandings and direction with clients as they talk. Many taken-for-granted junctures in counseling arise where decisions and evaluations regarding the counseling process can be collaboratively made. How important aspects of experience are named, how client understandings are selected for reflection, how conversational directions are decided, how particular subjects get discussed, how success is defined (and so on) can be sites of conversational hijacking or places for collaborative decisions on how to go forward together. For John Shotter (1993), conversations can develop seemingly inconstestable realities. They can develop a speakers’ logic, as to what gets deemed “appropriate” participation over time (e.g., Arminen, 1999). Add to this cultural power differentials in professional helping favoring the counselor (Miller & Silverman, 1995) – and a counselor’s potential to label clients “resistant” – and unsurprisingly, clients can become cooperative where we would prefer them collaborative. Cooperating is not collaborating; it is like going along with a hijacker. As with the Stockholm or Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 13 Patti Hearst syndrome, in time clients can learn to cooperate and start sounding like their hijacker. Long ago, family therapy pioneer, Jay Haley (1963), said that crazy-making situations were ones people couldn’t comment on or influence. There is an ample supply of such nonnegotiable conversations in everyone’s lives, including counselors’ professional lives. Hijacking or negotiating counseling’s aporectic moments I want to speak to a few nuances on this theme. One such nuance relates to when speakers feel held to being clear or certain about experience (Byrne & McCarthy, 1988). Ambiguity and ambivalence is generally unwelcome in Western cultural discourse (Levine, 1985). Moral and conceptual clarity are much trumpeted virtues these days, in ways that can hijack or prematurely foreclose on important reasoning and preference-making processes. Such careful reasoning and preference making is often needed on matters of great delicacy and importance for clients. Why can’t there be room for ambiguity and ambivalence about things for which language remains inadequate!? For Paolo Freire (1985) it is critical that the disempowered acquire their language for such matters and not some professional or otherwise ‘correct’ language. But, funders of counseling need goals, diagnoses, or empirically supported interventions to fill such unprofitable conversational “voids”. Counseling models offer counselors rich vocabularies for clients’ experiences while counselors can feel uncomfortable in joining clients in such voids of meaning. I am interested in how clients and counselors relate to emotionally charged and ambiguous moments (e.g., Fuller & Strong, 2001; Strong, 2007). Many of these slip by in counseling. Derrida (1976) questioned how humans give experiences meaning, when words can never be the same as what they represent, and experiences can not name themselves. Aporias are experiences for which there is no finally determinable meaning and counseling has lots of them. Suppose clients review tapes of their counseling, pick emotionally charged moments that, upon Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 14 further reflection still matter to them, and then come back to discuss such moments (see Elliott, 1987; Gass & Mackey, 2000)? What new might get talked into being when, for Bakhtin (1981), any meaningful moment promises multiple conversational pathways or possibilities for meaning? Margaret Fuller, a graduate student I supervised, asked clients five sessions into their counseling to pick a passage they considered most “alive” from reviewing videotapes of their counseling. From their selected “alive” moment, Margaret interviewed the clients in the presence of their counselor. Clients had varied reasons for choosing their “alive” moments and Margaret had some curiosities as well. If ever there was a place where she could hijack the research conversation it came in asking: did clients associate anything spiritual with these moments? Each client suspiciously responded by asking Margaret “what do you mean by spiritual” to which she offered a broad, pantheistic definition. A shared period of ambiguity and ambivalence followed until the clients tentatively came to talk about their moment in ways consistent with this broad definition. As these tentative efforts were put to words, Margaret joined in their elaboration, and the utterances grew to sound less tentative, more confident. Three months later the clients reported still thinking and acting on this elaborative discussion. This research evoked an understandable wariness from some readers about how Margaret ‘held’ clients to her idea of the conversation. After all, it was her question that invited a spiritual discussion about the “alive” moments. The “co” prefix in co-constructing understandings or actions, a prefix dear to many constructionist counselors, doesn’t wash for those concerned here. When conversation is viewed as a contest, or its course can’t be negotiated or co-developed, one speaker has to dominate. Margaret had to be hijacking the conversation or clients were simply indulging her. Note how such lines of thought can sanction conversational hijacking. Margaret’s research points to important considerations about negotiating counseling conversations without Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 15 hijacking them. How can one talk of co-constructing understandings and actions, if both clients and counselors are not negotiating, and customizing their content and process? Many pause here and ask: but don’t clients come to counselors for their expertise, for the conversational process they host? I agree. They come for a conversation to help overcome distress or achieve a goal they could not attain on their own. But this can sound like a blood transfusion - clients coming for a transfer of counselor expertise knowledge. It also implies a client who, like a blood recipient, is passive until the knowledge transfer is complete. Foucault (2006) harshly saw professionals, like counselors, needing docility from their clients in order to do their work. This contrasts dramatically with responsive conversations that mobilize the resourcefulness of both clients and counselors. Consistent with this view, some counselors see their expertise as “conversation or process managers”. Personally, I prefer notions like discursive flexibility (Strong, 2002) and balk at conversation “management”, or dialogues ‘administered’ by counselors. To turn to a narrative metaphor, with the counseling dialogue a developing client story, I am mindful of who is editor in chief. And, in many editorial boardrooms, the discussions that lead to editorial decisions can be quite imaginative and sometime even heated, on the way to what finally makes it into print. Clients take forward what they find useful from counseling. Missing from most accounts of the counseling process is a sense of clients and counselors simultaneously engaged in meaning-making, as they talk with each other. Close examinations of talk show listeners’ subtly and often not so subtly indicating to each other – as they talk – that something in what they are being told is or is not on for them. For example, speakers often start saying something and, depending on how they are being received, make mid-course corrections to what they say so they can be understood more as they wish. There is a “co” thing going on here. Such speakers point out an axiom of dialogue: that being in dialogue means opening Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 16 oneself to the reciprocal influence of one’s conversational partners. It isn’t a matter of either/or; even the most overbearing conversational partner usually cedes some ground to their conversational other to sustain a conversation. What matters most is how the stakes of the conversation are worked out as people talk to each other. In counseling these stakes are varied. In part, I am talking about the yin and yang of dialogue; it’s instrumental and goaloriented dimension and its relationally responsive dimension. Counseling needs both dimensions though the instrumental dimension has been particularly emphasized in North American counseling this last while. Some social constructionist counselors have also become particularly enamored with conversational techniques. John Shotter speaks of a Rambo-esque (i.e., a la Sylvester Stallone) kind of agency shown by some counselors – for themselves and for their clients. What counselor doesn’t like to see Michael White ask a brilliant question, or read spectacular accounts of Milton Erickson’s therapeutic wizardry? Carl Rogers’ empathic ahhmms and reflective paraphrases seem pretty mundane and useless by comparison. Counselors are sometimes likened to carpenters who have intervention toolkits. But, clients have their own evolving ideas on what needs to get constructed in their work with counselors. In this regard, counseling can seem like the challenge of choosing then hanging wallpaper in a friend’s home. Talk in this sense helps speakers coordinate their preferences, understandings and actions around projects aimed to better things for one of them. Like hanging wallpaper, it is easy for counseling to become competing monologues (note how clients can simply go silently underground here) on how things should develop in ways that serve no one. In considering the yin (“feminine”, Carl Rogers) side of dialogue, lately my interest in nuance has grown around conversations about topics difficult to initiate and elaborate. Some of the difficulty seems to come with the moral aspects of such topics, in what James and Melissa Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 17 Griffiths (1994) once described as “unspeakable dilemmas”. Professional or institutionally driven dialogue sanctions some kinds of conversation over others. For example, emergent “alive” moments in dialogue are passed over if counseling is too goal-directed or if the counselor fails to engage with evocative client utterances (Katz & Shotter, 1999). Difficult conversations are pregnant with aporias of meaning-making potential, beckoning counselors to respond as “conversational midwives” (Weingarten, 1995). David Silverman (2001) described such conversations as constructing “delicate” objects. These are conversations that often start in incoherence, ambiguity, confusion or even moral or politically sensitive circumstances. They typically became difficult because they failed to find a response in others. Sensitive to still not being heard, a raised eyebrow, an unresponded to tentative utterance, a change of topic via a counselor’s question at a mounting emotional juncture in the conversation – all point to junctures where counselors may unintentionally hijack or bypass delicate conversations with clients. Blunted responsivity? I want to re-emphasize that behind most hijacked conversations in counseling are good intentions or blunted awareness. Counselors channel their good intentions through training and supervision, counseling models, professional codes of practice, theories of development and communication, personal theories, and so on. Often what comes out the end of this professional sausage-maker are finely-honed intentions, improvements on those prior to training but sometimes blunting what Shotter (2006) refers to as relational responsivity. Relational responsivity is something we most humans are born into this world with, being well-attuned to their primary caregivers. This gets blunted as one learns ‘how things are or should be’. I don’t want to romanticize the untrained helper but research shows that paraprofessionals can be as helpful to clients as professionals (Durlak, 1979). My hunch is that, despite greater self- Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 18 awareness, refined sensitivities to the helping encounter, and diverse knowledge about human development and change, the counselors had more of a professional agenda with which to hijack the conversation from clients. But, I would not idealize everyday conversation either. In everyday conversation, the norm is that one encounters many agendas in asking for help, so counseling can be a definite improvement over those conversations. Similarly, in the hurly-burly of every day discourse, people are faced with multiple claims on their meanings and actions. We don’t think, talk or act without reference to how others might receive us (Billig, 1996). As relational beings we can’t just say and do whatever pleases us individually, without some responses or other ripple effects in our relationships. Ask someone who has come out as gay or lesbian about this. Observe what happens when a politician “flip-flops” on an important policy issue. Such circumstances point out an important feature, not of changing one’s mind or one’s identity, but of changing one’s conversations and ways of interacting with others (Edwards, 1997). And such changes might seem tantamount to conversational hijacking to these others, though that need not be the case. We are talking about the politics of everyday conversation (Laing, 1967; Ochs & Taylor, 1992) where conversations sustain some relational orders and shared versions of experience over others (Garfinkel, 1967). Counseling can offer a place to reflect on such conversational politics and how to negotiate one’s involvement in them anew. Where that is not possible, broader social action and support may be required. As I indicated earlier, some see counseling involving negotiations with clients, with these tending to favor counselors and the institutions in which they work (Parker, 1999; Rose, 1990). In my research I look for how such negotiations transpire using conversation analysis (CA). I am interested in how clients and counselors put words to difficult conversations, negotiate critical junctures in counseling, contest overdetermined meanings from prior conversations, construct Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 19 generative relationships, and so on. For Harold Garfinkel (1967), conversations engage speakers in practically interpreting, or making sense of, each other as they talk. In this sense-making they each propose where the conversation, and thus their relationship, might next to go. This generally occurs in taken for granted (seen but unnoticed is how they say things in CA) ways – much slips under our conversational radar as we learn to attend to some things more than others. CA regards speakers as talking across a gap in which they propose things to each other – to which the conversational partner responds based on her or his interpretation of what is proposed. Interpretations, judgments and conversational actions are all worked out on the fly, between speakers, though prior conversations cover ground, that unless seen as controversial, generally escapes notice. While speakers can become good or familiarized at packaging their talk for each other, as they talk, there will always be some element of dialogic mystery as others respond to them in taking their turns in talking. What matters in CA is what speakers do with their parts in these interactions, how they use their turn for proposing initiatives, declining proposals, taking up others’ talk, extending or departing from what has been said, and so on. In counseling, these negotiations are interesting to see worked out (or not). In the absence of “telementation” (literally, knowing what goes on in another’s thinking) speakers have only what they show each other to go on for understanding how they are being received and understood (Wittgenstein, 1953). So, understanding itself is seen as an observable accomplishment, though speakers having histories with each other, typically have worked out many prior understandings that don’t require further effort. In conversations with new people, however, such as in counseling, this may not be the case. Understanding may require considerable conversational effort before client and counselor adequately acknowledge to each other that they have worked out an understanding. The same goes for influence. In trying to Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 20 propose an action, or invite a particular kind of response, one has what is shown back to go on. So, not surprisingly considerable effort can be spent in ‘packaging’ what is sent. Here, however, I must turn to a point raised by Vygotsky (1987), that expressed thought is completed in words as they are spoken – not before they exit one’s head. Much packaging of talk occurs based on how speakers see themselves being received. What I am about to say may seem like an odd observation but I’ll back it up in a moment. I have actually come to appreciate the many umms and ahs, and pauses, - the stuff many associate with unclear communications – with what people are doing as they package what they say to each other. To me, they can be artful ways of showing responsivity to each other as they talk and not always some kind of conversational clumsiness. Delicate negotiations and those with unfamiliar others require more work and elements of tentativeness and responsiveness to one’s negotiating partner. These efforts are all observable, and guide speakers on what next to say how to say it to their conversational others. Counseling as Negotiation: Finding “Common Ground” on Words and Ways of Talking Good therapeutic dialogue involves an interweave of client and counselor words and ways of talking (Ferrara, 1993). By this I am referring to what Herbert Clark (1996) has called “common ground” in communications. Developing “common ground” comes as speakers work out and take up ways of understanding and relating that they then use in common. The common ground or interweave in these ways of understanding and relating becomes evident in an increasingly shared (and uncontrived) vocabulary and manner of speaking. For counselors, this discursive perspective suggests being mindful of how their use of language fares in terms of what clients do with it. Common ground, if achieved, is a discursive accomplishment of both client and counselor, but the back and forth negotiations to get there are quite observable. Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 21 Getting specific, it can help to see dialogue occurring across a gap, like neuroscience’s synaptic gap. This dialogic gap is where speakers show how they interpret and put forward their preferences for how they want the conversation to go next. Common ground occurs when the gap isn’t quite so large or doesn’t require so much conversational work. But, when it is about, say, answering a new style of question in counseling, activity across the gap is something to behold as I hope this next passage of counseling dialogue shows. John asks and Sid answers a solution-focused scaling question 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 John: Sid: John: Sid: John: Sid: John: Sid: John: Sid: John: Sid: John: Sid: John: Sid: John: Sid: John: Sid: John: Okay, on like (.) on a scale of one to ten↓ Okay ((nods))= =Okay, like TEN would be (1.5) ah >you’ve got a handle on this<↓ [Uhm huh] ((nodding)) [Right] and, and its, its (.) you know, maybe ten would be like the balance (.) if there’s a balance between (.3) you know, sometimes its (.) its (.) helpful to plan= =Okay ((nodding)) and o, you know maybe over plan (.6) and (1.2) uhm (1) sometimes its, you know, it it >stresses you out< [Uhm uhm] ((nodding)) [*Right?*] Ten would be:: (.4) like (.3) the balance is >this is nothing but helpful< Okay ((nodding)) Like it’s totally on [Yea ((nods)) (1.5) Yep] [on the continuum of] this is helpful. And one would be (.) like you’re (.6) you know fretting and worry about everything and you’re you know, it’s (2.5) making you’re stomach [Yea ((nods)) (1) Yea ((nods)) (1.5) Yea] [go in a knot or whatever happens for you when you’re stressed out] Uhmm (1.1) >Where’re you at right now<?= =Somewhere probably a 6 or a 7. Okay. Yep. So, that’s, that’s (.) that’s pretty good? Yea, yea it’s not hind (.7) its >hindering me in the sense that I have those stressful moments throughout the day< or throughout the week or whatever where I am like ge::ez, you know, but its not killing me↓ and I think it is beneficial in that (.) it’s gotten me to where I am and its helping me cope with my time and it’s, you know (.8) keeping me on track↑ Okay, okay. Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 22 Normally, one thinks of questions as simply posed and answered. Here, however, there is a lot of conversational work going on as John (the counselor) has to work out with Sid a question that can be answered. The pauses, the underlines (for emphases), the umms and ahs, and so forth show intricate conversational work as Sid and John propose things and respond to them. Most counseling textbooks leave out this level of detail in clients’ and counselors’ dialogues. My point is to bring out relevant details (i.e., to the speakers involved) involved in negotiating this question and answer sequence. What seemingly could have been asked in line 1 and answered in line 2 took 23 lines before an answer was started. The conversational journey there had lots of backs and forths until Sid finished elaborating an answer and John acknowledged receiving one. Evidence that a conversational outcome is accomplished – by Sid’s answering a challenging question in ways that satisfy both Sid and John - occurs between lines 23 and 32. Why should this level of conversational detail matter to our topic of hijacking? My answer is that it points to a responsive dimension of conversation important to how counselors customize their communications when working with clients. Like a dancer trying to introduce a new dance step into a dance that is already under way, great sensitivity is required not only in advancing something unfamiliar to the dance partner, but in responding to how the partner responds. Counselors proposing something different from what has been already worked out have comparable important challenges: suppose the client doesn’t want to do the dance step, suppose the initial response back isn’t going as hoped for, suppose the client proposes a different dance step? Developments inside counseling are not much different, although counselors can take ‘conversational hostages’ when they shut down the kind of responsiveness shown by John above. Looking closely at good counseling has shown me how much counselors try to customize a language and a conversational process together with clients (e.g., Strong, in press). Creating Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 23 common ground in shared understandings and actions takes a lot of negotiating, to accomplish understandings and actions that clients can carry forward beyond counseling. Negotiations as forward moving collaborations? Psychology has a poor vocabulary to account for how individuals collaborate or negotiate common understandings and actions. More common are notions like groupthink, enmeshment, surrendering to logic – to me, forms of conversational hijacking. How is it that people talking and listening come to influence each other? Are they tripping cognitive or affective switches with their words? Elsewhere they strike up business partnerships and work out other joint projects, even between relative strangers, in ways not unlike John and Sid. The best partnerships show partners highly attentive to new developments either proposes and with conversational work great things can emerge. No hostages are taken, no coercive power is required, two people’s intentions get ‘wordsmithed’ (Strong, 2006) into a new language at least one could usefully take forward. What’s that about, if not a transfer of expert knowledge? My answer is that it is about the responsive, resourceful and flexible ways counselors engage clients (and themselves) in developing a shared language, one that is continually up for negotiation, so as to remain perspicuous for the client. But, counselors are typically trained to have particular kinds of conversations with clients, those consistent with their theoretical orientations and training. Stephen Tyler (as cited in Hoffman, 1995), once described an anthropologist entering a hitherto unknown culture with textbooks taped to his eyes. This is an extreme caricature of what can happen to counselors as they learn to selectively engage with and respond to clients. Our clients don't usually come to counseling conceptually packaging their understandings in the discourses of Freud, Aaron Back or Michael White. But, our conversations Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 24 can sure take them there. Bill O'Hanlon (workshop comments, 2002) pokes a lot of fun at counselors "marrying" their clinical hypotheses and models. A few years ago I became interested in a conversational metaphor I first learned from Lois Shawver on her Postmodern Therapies listserv (PMTH@MAELSTROM.STJOHNS.EDU). The metaphor was that of a dialogic borderzone and I began looking at how good dialogue requires a sense that it is the 'dwelling' (in Heidegger's sense) of both speakers (Strong, 2002). Counseling typically starts with a conversation between two relative strangers. They may share a common mother tongue, but this can blind them to each other's unique uses and understandings of language. The discursively aware counselor, recognizing this, faces a choice as to how to welcome the client’s understandings and ways of talking without accepting them as the sole language of counseling, or hijacking the client’s language as an alternative. A borderzone implies that client and counselor have some common ground to work out in their communications. This metaphor bears some family resemblances to the work of Imelda McCarthy and her colleagues (e.g., Byrne & McCarthy, 1988) in creating conversational spaces where new forms of talking and listening are possible. Clients and counselors are, in a sense, on linguistic foreign ground each time they talk, or negotiate, their way through unfamiliar developments between them. I say “in a sense” here because neither creates or comes to such a borderzone without prior understandings or ways of talking. Indeed, part of what a borderzone can offer is a purposefully co-constructed context in which to unpack significant differences in how client and counselor use language, as part of talking their way to common ground. Milking this metaphor further, there are lots of junctures where clients and counselors depart from common ground, taking relational risks by reflecting on the already spoken, by proposing and working out new language for how they will conversationally move forward Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 25 together (Strong & Tomm, 2007). Needed is a language that clients find fitting in understanding and acting on concerns they brought to counseling, something I suggest happens through this unpacking and negotiating we have been discussing. But, I’m not confident that this can happen when clients are conversationally hijacked by the already spoken, or by simply mimicking the counselor’s language without customizing it as their own. A borderzone can be a place of conversational hybridity where words and ways of talking can be unpacked, tried on, and customized to clients’ purposes for beyond counseling. Without such a conversational place, well intended counseling can become conversational hijacking occurs for reasons that owe much to outdated views on conversation and language use in counseling, something that concerns me. Hijacking the Final Word: No way! I want to conclude with a little experiment. Throughout this paper I have repeatedly suggested that counseling is a negotiated process which can be hijacked without great sensitivity and responsivity to both clients’ and counselors’ words and ways of talking. My stance has been that collaborative dialogue in counseling involves more than client cooperation and that this is accomplished through developing contexts and ways of talking that promote perspicuous understandings and common ground. But, I have been doing this all along as a monologue, proposing ways of talking and understanding to you in the hopes that we might get to common ground (translators and all). Borrowing from Bakhtin (1981), a collaborative dialogue should ideally take people to a sense of “we” in their communications, a “we” in which understandings and actions are customized so that they are personally fitting. My invitation is for you to see my paper as one turn in a dialogue about where important conversations about counseling could better accomplish that for you. Our conversational negotiations don’t stop because I said so. That would be my way of hijacking your conversation. Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 26 References Amundson, J., Valentine, L. & Stewart, K. (1993) Temptations of power and certainty. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 19, 111-123. Andersen, T. (1996). Language is not innocent. In F. W. Kaslow (Ed.), Handbook of relational diagnosis and dysfunctional family patterns (pp. 119-125). New York: John Wiley & Sons. Anderson, H. (1997). Conversation, language and possibilities. New York : Basic Books. Anderson, H. (2001). Postmodern collaborative and person-centred therapies: what would Carl Rogers say? Journal of Family Therapy 23(4), 339–360. Arminen, I. (1999). Conversation analysis: A quest for order in interaction and language use. Acta Sociologica, 42, 251-257. Bakhtin, M. (1981) The dialogical imagination (M. Holquist, Ed., C. Emerson & M. Holquist Trans). Austin: University of Texas Press. Bateson, G. (1980). Mind and nature: A necessary unity. New York: Bantam Bavelas, J. B., McGee, D., Phillips, B., & Routledge, R. (2000). Microanalysis of communication in psychotherapy. Human Systems: The Journal of Systemic Consultation and Management, 11(1), 3-22. Berger, P., & Luckmann, T. (1967). The social construction of reality. New York: Doubleday. Billig, M. (1996). Arguing and thinking (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press. Bryceland, C., & Stam, H. (2005). Empirical validation and professional codes of ethics: Description or prescription. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 18, 131-155. Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 27 Byrne, N.O., & McCarthy, I. (1988). Moving statues: Requesting ambivalence through ambiguous discourse. Irish Journal of Psychology, 9, 173-182. Buber, M. (1958) I and thou (2nd ed.). New York: Scribner Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. New York: Cambridge University Press. Davis, K. (1986). The process of problem (re)formulation in psychotherapy. Sociology of Health and Illness, 8(1), 44-74. Derrida, J. (1976). Of grammatology. (G.C. Spivak, Trans.). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins Press. Durlak, J. A. (1979). Comparative effectiveness of paraprofessional and professional helpers. Psychological Bulletin, 86(1), 80-92. Edwards, D. (1997). Discourse and cognition. London: Sage. Edwards, D., & Potter, J. (1992). Discursive psychology. London: Sage. Ferrara, K. (1993). Therapeutic ways with words. New York: Oxford University Press. Fisher, J. T., & O’Donohue, W. T. (Eds.) (2006). Practitioner's guide to evidence-based psychotherapy. New York: Springer. Foucault, M. (1972). The archaeology of knowledge (Trans. A.M. Sheridan). New York: Pantheon Books. Foucault, M. (2006). Psychiatric power: Lectures at the College de France 1973-1974 (J. LaGrange, Ed.; G. Burchell, Trans.). New York: Palgrave MacMillan. Freire, P. (1985). Pedagogy of the oppressed (M. B. Ramos, Trans.). New York: Continuum. Fuller, M., & Strong, T. (2001). “Alive” moments and invitations to spiritual discourse. Counselling and Psychotherapy Research, 1 (3), 200-214. Garfinkel, H. (1967). Studies in ethnomethodology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 28 Goffman, E. (1967). Interaction ritual: Essays on face-to-face behavior. New York: Doubleday Anchor. Greenberg, G. (2007). Manufacturing depression: A journey into the economy of melancholy. Harper’s Magazine, 314(1884), 47-57. Haley, J. (1963). Strategies of psychotherapy. New York: The Psychological Corporation Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc. Henriques, J., Hollway, W., Urwin, C., Venn, C., & Walkerdine, V. (1984). Changing the subject: Psychology, social regulation and subjectivity. New York: Methuen & Co. Heron, J. (1996). Cooperative inquiry. London: Sage. Hoffman, L. (1995). Exchanging voices: A collaborative approach to family therapy. London: Karnac. Imber-Black, E. (Ed.) (1989). Rituals in families and family therapy. New York: Norton. Jensen Doss, A. (2005). Evidence-based diagnosis: Incorporating diagnostic instruments into clinical practice. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 947-952. Laing, R. D. (1967). The politics of experience and the bird of paradise. Baltimore: Penguin. Latour, B. (1988). Science in action: How to follow scientists and engineers through society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Levine, D. (1985). The flight from ambiguity: Essays in social and cultural theory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press Maturana, H., & Varela, F. (1988). The tree of knowledge. Boston: Shambhala. McNamee, S., & Gergen, K. (Eds.) (1999). Relational responsibility: Resources for sustainable dialogue. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 29 Miller, G. & Silverman, D. (1995) Troubles talk and counseling discourse: A comparative study. The Sociological Quarterly, 35, 725-747. Newman, F., & Holzman, L. (1996). Unscientific psychology: A cultural performatory approach to understanding human life. New York: Praeger. Norcross, J. C., Beutler, L. E., & Levant, R. F. (Eds.) (2006). Evidence-based practices in mental health: Debate and dialogue on the fundamental questions. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Ochs, E, & Taylor, C. (1992). Family narrative as political activity. Discourse & Society, 3, 301340. Parker, I. (Ed.) (1999). Deconstructing psychotherapy. New York: Routledge. Prilleltensky, I. (1994). The morals and politics of psychology: Psychological discourse and the status quo. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. Ritterman, M. (1983). Using hypnosis in family therapy. San Francisco: Jossey Bass. Rose, N. (1990). Governing the soul. New York: Routledge. Rush, A. J. (2001). Practice guidelines and algorithms. In M.M. Weissman (Ed.). Treatment of depression: Bridging the 21st century (pp. 213–242). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press Inc. Sampson, E. E. (1993). Celebrating the other: A dialogic account of human nature. Boulder, CO: Westview. Shotter, J. (1993). Conversational realities. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Shotter, J. (2006). ‘Getting it’: Withness-thinking and the dialogical in practice. Unpublished manuscript. Hijacked conversations 7/17/2016 30 Strong, T. (2002). Dialogue in therapy’s “borderzone”. The Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 15, 245-262. Strong, T. (2003). Getting curious about meaning-making in counselling. British Journal of Guidance and Counselling, 31, 259-273. Strong, T. (2006). Wordsmithing in counselling? European Journal of Psychotherapy and Counselling, 8, 251-268. Strong, T. (2007). Accomplishments in social constructionist counseling: Micro-analytic and retrospective analyses. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 4(1-2), 85-106. Strong, T., & Tomm, K. (2007). Family therapy as re-coordinating and moving on together. Journal of Systemic Therapies, 26(2), 42-54. Tomm, K. (1991). Beginnings of a “HIPs and PIPs” approach to psychiatric assessment. The Calgary Participator, Spring, 21-24. Toulmin, S. (1990). Cosmopolis: The hidden agenda of modernity. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Turner, V. (1975). Drama, fields, and metaphors. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Vico, G. (1984). The new science. (T. G. Bergin & M. H. Fisch, Trans.) Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. (Original work published in 1744) Vygotsky, L. (1987). The collected works of L.S. Vygotsky, Volume 1. New York: Plenum Wittgenstein, L. (1961). Tractatus Logico-Philosophicus. (Trans. D. F. Pears & B.F. McGuinness, with Introduction by B. Russell). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. (original published in English, 1922, original German Edition, 1921). Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical Investigations (G.E.M. Anscombe,Trans). New York: Macmillan. Zeig, J. K. (1980). A teaching seminar with Milton H. Erickson. New York: Brunner Mazel.