The Acquisition of Word Structure, Collocations, Word-class, and Meaning Based mainly on: Clark, E. (1995). The lexicon in acquisition Ellis, Nick. chap. 2.2, in Vocabulary description, acquisition and pedagogy Sequencing (Ellis, 2004) • Language is sequential. • Language is patterned. • Some sequences/patterns are more likely to appear, that is more predictable, than others. – “the” vs. “thx” – “the dog” vs. “book dog” www.themegallery.com Language:Logic vs. Probability (Ellis, 2004) • Logic-based – Rule-governed – Open system • Probability-based – Habitual – Idiomatic – Collocated, patterned – Closed system www.themegallery.com Word Groups / Phrases (E. Clark, 1995) • Words may be combined as idioms, where the meanings of the parts do not add up to the meaning of the whole. – – – – – to belt up, to be in a flap, to hit the sack, to keep tabs on, to blow one's own trumpet. Idioms like these are typically restricted in syntax, so some, for instance, may not appear in the passive (compare He blew his own trumpet vs. *His own trumpet was blown by himself). • Words may be combined in short phrases that act as if they were single words. – – – – by and large, in short, happy go lucky, once upon a time. These forms are fixed: their word order is frozen, and many are used only in restricted contexts. Once upon a time, for example, serves to introduce fairy tales and rarely occurs outside that context. www.themegallery.com Word Groups / Phrases (E. Clark, 1995) • Words can combine, following the syntactic rules of a language, to form an indefinite number of noun phrases (e.g., the lone skier, a red fox, three children) and verb phrases (e.g., raced down the hill, crossed the road, climbed over the gate) in each language. • Words, fixed phrases, and idioms, then, all contribute to the construction of clauses (e.g., Justin changed gear, Rod plotted the data, Sophie is practicing her flute). • Speakers build their utterances (both clauses and combinations of clauses) from words, fixed phrases, and idioms. And these are also the lexical units identified by listeners as they parse and interpret utterances heard from others. www.themegallery.com Word Structure & Meaning (E. Clark, 1995) • Lexical items "clothe" syntactic structure. They are what exemplify a relative clause (Duncan pointed at the man who was running), an adverbial clause (Duncan went outside when he heard the car, or a verb complement (They wanted to climb the hill). • Without words, there is no way for the syntax of a language to be realized. Equally, without words, there is no way to exemplify the phonological system or the morphology of language, in inflections (e.g., in jump-s, race-d, wait-ing, know-n; cup-s, toy-s, cat-'s), derivations (e.g., watch -er, violin-ist, construct-ion, silver-y, green-ish, palm -ate), or compound words (e.g., snow-plough, key-hole, piledriver, writing desk). • In using language, children reveal what they know about it. And when children start to talk, they start with words--word forms and word meanings. www.themegallery.com Word Structure, Meaning, & Word Class (E. Clark, 1995) • Lexical and syntactic development go hand in hand. • Children learn the syntactic forms that go with specific lexical items, and gradually accumulate sets of words that can act the same way syntactically. • It is unclear when (or whether) young children learn rules of syntax, but there is growing evidence that they learn syntactic properties specific to individual lexical items. As they learn more lexical items, they become more likely to act consistently in the syntactic patterns they produce. (Whether this consistency is best described in terms of rules or strategies remains an open question.) www.themegallery.com Lexical/Vocabulary Acquisition (E. Clark, 1995) • Children build up a vocabulary (isolating forms from the stream of speech, constructing hypotheses about possible meanings, and mapping those meanings onto forms). • Children must also elaborate semantic fields, linking words whose meanings are related; analyze word structures, so they are able to identify stems and affixes and their relative contributions to meaning; and coin new words to express meanings where they lack the relevant conventional forms. www.themegallery.com Lexical/Vocabulary Acquisition (E. Clark, 1995) www.themegallery.com Lexical/Vocabulary Acquisition (E. Clark, 1995) www.themegallery.com Chunking (Ellis, 2004) Newell (1990, p.7): • “A chuck is a unit of memory organisation, formed by bringing together a set of already formed chunks in memory and welding them together into a larger unit. Chunking implied the ability to build up such structures recursively, thus leading to a hierarchical organisation of memory. Chunking appears to be a ubiquitous feature of human memory. Conceivably, it could form the basis ofr an equally ubiquitous law of practice.” www.themegallery.com Memory • Short Term Memory (STM) • Long Term Memory (LTM) • STM LTM www.themegallery.com Language Chunks (Ellis, 2004) • Smaller, lower level (sublexical) chunks appear more frequently than larger, higher-level (lexical) chunks. • Sublexical > lexical > supralexical • th > the • the / ir > their • their > their home • their home > their home in Tainan www.themegallery.com Chunks (Ellis, 2004) • Various terms for chunks: – lexical phrase, holophrase, prefabricated routines and patterns, formulaic speech, memorized sentences and lexicalised stem, lexical chunk, formula • Examples: – Excuse me. – How do you do? – I have a headache. www.themegallery.com Speaking natively? (Ellis, 2004) • • • • “I wished to be wedded to you.” “Your marrying me is desired by me.” “My becoming your spouse is what I want.” “I want to marry you.” • “Speaking natively is speaking idiomatically using frequent and familiar collocations, and the job of the language learner is to learn these familiar word sequences.” (Nick Ellis, p.129) www.themegallery.com Language acquisition (Ellis, 2004) • “An important index of native-like competence is that the learner uses idioms fluently. So language learning involves learning sequences of words (frequent collocations, phrases, and idioms) was well as sequences within words.” (Nick Ellis, p.130). www.themegallery.com Computational model of English word-class acquisition (Ellis, p.131) • A computer with an input of 15,000word corpus from mother-baby talks. • A associative learning program which establishes links between words and their contexts (immediate successor). • A classification learning program which classifies words with similar leftcontext. • The computer generated clusters of words which seemed to be based wordclass. www.themegallery.com Computational model of English word-class acquisition (Ellis, p.131) Computer generated clusters of words: • Noun-like words: hen, sheep, pig, farmer, cow, house, horse • Verb-like words: can, are, do, think, see • Adjective-like words: little, big, nice • pronoun-like words: this, he, that, it, you, I • Article-like words: a, the www.themegallery.com Human implicit learning of artificial grammar (Ellis, 2004, p.131) • Human subjects in the experiments with the artificial language (and grammar) are able to make judgments at significantly better than chance levels without being able to articulate detailed information about what the rules governing the letter strings are, or which one they were using in guiding their decisions. www.themegallery.com Acquisition of language form (Ellis, 2004, p.132) • Input phonological memory analysis of language data acquisition of – – – – – phonotactic patterns, word form, formulas, phrases and idioms, word collocation information, grammatical word-class information • Acquisition can be speeded by making the underlying patterns more salient as a result of explicit instruction or consciousness raising. www.themegallery.com Memory and language acquisition (Ellis, 2004, p.133) • STM is a reliable predictor of long-term acquisition of L1 vocabulary and syntax. • Phonological STM is a reliable predictor of later vocabulary acquisition in both L1 and L2. • Dyslexic children with verbal STM deficiency have poor syntactic development in both L1 and L2. www.themegallery.com L2 Vocabulary acquisition (Ijaz, 1986, cited in Ellis, 2004, p.134) • “…the second language learners essentially relied on a semantic equivalence hypothesis. This hypothesis facilitates the acquisition of lexical meanings in the L2 in that it reduces it to the relabelling of concepts already learned in the L1. It confounds and complicates vocabulary acquisition in the L2 by ignoring crosslinguistic differences in conceptual classification and differences in the semantic boundaries of seemingly corresponding words in the L1 and L2.” (p.443 in Ijaz, 1986) www.themegallery.com Reading & Vocabulary acquisition (Sternberg, 1987, cited in Ellis, 2004, p.134) • People who read more know more vocabulary. • Moderate-to-low-frequency words differentiate individuals of high vocabulary size from those of low vocabulary size. These words are more likely to appear in print than in speech. • In reading, the reader has opportunities to study the context, to analyze the forms and meanings of the fixed words on the page. www.themegallery.com Strategies in learning word meaning (Sternberg, 1987, cited in Ellis, 2004, p.135) • Selective encoding: Separating relevant from irrelevant information for the purposes of formulating a definition. • Selective combination: Combining relevant cues into a workable definition. • Selective comparison: relating new information to old information already stored in LTM. www.themegallery.com Cues and variables in word learning (Sternberg, 1987, cited in Ellis, 2004, p.135) • The number of occurrences of the unknown word. • The variability of contexts in which multiple occurrences of the unknown word appear. • The importance of the unknown word to understanding the text. • the helpfulness of the surrounding context in understanding the meaning of the unknown word. • The density of unknown words in the text. www.themegallery.com Mnemonic strategies in word learning (Sternberg, 1987, cited in Ellis, 2004, p.136) • Learning the meanings of words is a conscious process. • Imagery > Sentence generation > Sentence reading > Repetition www.themegallery.com Mnemonic strategies in word learning (Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986, cited in Ellis, 2004, p.138) • Vocab. instruction is a useful adjunct to natural learning from context. • Learning both the definition and use (context) were more effective. • Several exposures were more beneficial for drill-and-practice methods. • Keyword methods were more effective than others. • Learning multiple aspects of a word was more helpful for future understanding of texts using those words in context. www.themegallery.com