Midterm report NA3C0005 Comparison between “The Development and Validation of a Listening Practice Strategy Questionnaire” and “The Metacognitive Awareness Listening Questionnaire: Development and Validation” “The Development and Validation of a Listening Practice Strategy Questionnaire” (hereafter called article 1) was written by Hui-Ming Lucy Lee, Mong-Na Lo Huang, and Wan-Ping Hung and published in the fall, 2010. The main purposes of article 1 were to develop a valid and reliable questionnaire to assess EFL students’ listening practice strategies and to investigate listeners’ use of practice outside of class. The other article “The Metacognitive Awareness Listening Questionnaire: Development and Validation” (hereafter called article 2) was written by Vandergrift et al. and published in September, 2006. The main purposes of article 2 were to develop a valid listening questionnaire to assess second language (L2) listeners’ metacognitive awareness and to perceive use of strategies while listening to oral texts. As reading the titles of these 2 articles and comparing the purposes of them, there should be a highly relevant between them. Obviously, the authors all tried to develop a valid and reliable questionnaire to assess listening comprehension strategies. Nevertheless, they constructed questionnaires in terms of different theories. The design of the questionnaire in article 1 was based on theoretical models of listening comprehension theory and practice theory. On the other hand, the design of the questionnaire in article 2 was based on theoretical model of metacognition. Based on their similarities, I presume that article 1 should be derived from or inspired by article 2. Quality of LR Comparison In order to construct a valid and reliable questionnaire, the authors adopted theories to develop their questionnaires. The parts of their literature review focused on relevant theories about the directions of assessment. They also found there is a insufficient for some assessment instruments. In article 1, the authors pointed out that it is unclear how learners practice listening comprehension and how often they practice listening. Moreover, there is a lack of a reliable and valid survey instrument to assess listeners’ practice behaviors. First, the researchers reviewed the listening comprehension theories. In this part, they cited Lundsteen’s definition of listening. Next, they quoted Anderson’s three stages of language comprehension: perceptual processing, parsing, and utilization. Through analysis of comprehension process, it showed listeners processed phrases and sentences immediately for meaningful representations. Furthermore, they illustrated three major kinds of listening processing: bottom-up, top-down, and interactive processing. Second, they reviewed the practice theories. In order to develop an expert performance, learners used extensive practice to acquire skills. According to Anderson, there are three stages in skill acquisition: the cognitive, the associative, and the autonomous stages. Next, they reviewed Newell and Rosenbloom’s power law of practice, or log-log linear learning law, performance improves as a power function of practice. In other view of expert performance, deliberate practice is the most effective way of improving the current level of performance. Practice is a learning strategy in SLA and second language listening comprehension. From a conceptual standpoint, practice is classified as a cognitive strategy. From an information-processing perspective, the routinization of cognitive skills is characterized as a function of practice. Third, they exemplified some empirical research on practice. The first one is that Ericsson et al. compared current and past levels of deliberate practice in three main groups of adult violinists to examine the effects of deliberate practice on performance. The second one is that a seminal study was conducted by Bialystok to measure language learning strategies and the effects of strategy use on language achievement. The third one is that Yang reported a study to investigate 505 Taiwanese EFL university students’ beliefs about language learning and their use of language learning strategies. The critical effects of practice on expert performance have been supported by the reviewed empirical research in the area of cognitive psychology. However, the authors found that in the field of EFL listening research, there is a lack of empirical research on the role of practice in the acquisition of EFL listening skill. They attempted to explore EFL listeners’ practice behavior by developing a valid and reliable questionnaire and by measuring the frequency of their listening practice strategy use. In article 2, this questionnaire is designed to investigate the metacognitive awareness listening of a second language learner. Therefore, the authors compared the literatures and used Flavell’s model of metacognitive knowledge. Three categories of metacognitive knowledge, person knowledge, task knowledge, strategy knowledge, represented key components in the process of cognitive self-appraisal. In the field of education and L2 learning, metacognition plays an important role in enhancing thinking and comprehension. Learners with high degrees of metacognitive awareness are better at processing and storing new information, finding the best ways to practice and reinforce what they have learned. Next, the authors reviewed the research of metacognitive knowledge about listening. To elicit learners’ metacognitive knowledge about listening, various procedures have been used, most commonly diaries, interviews, and questionnaires. The hypothesized relationships among metacognition, motivation, and listening proficiency were further explored by Vandergrift. However, none of these earlier self-report measures had been subjected to rigorous validation procedures. Research Methods Comparison Both research had recruited a large sample of respondents. Article 1 had recruited 306 university students in Taiwan. Article 2 had recruited 1,478 university and high school students in various countries. In order to validate the questionnaires, they administered a pilot study and a formal study. During data analysis, they used an exploratory factor analysis and a confirmatory factor analysis to test the validity and reliability of the factors. However, in article 1, the authors focused on analysis the items of each factor. In article 2, the authors focused on analysis how many factors model is suitable. Participants In article 1, 306 university students in Taiwan were recruited. The development of the questionnaire involved 3 phases of a pilot study and a formal study. Since this study was conducted in Taiwan, the authors used 2 language versions to verify the validity of the questionnaire. This research method is more robust than the study in article 2. During the first phase of the pilot study, 16 English major students were recruited for exploratory interviews. The second phase of the pilot study attempted to assess the clarity between English version and Chinese version of the LPSQ. 10 students from the previous interview were chosen. The third phase of the pilot study, 94 students were recruited and encouraged to ask questions about the questionnaire during the administration process. The formal study was conducted in 2001 (Is it a mistype? Or this article was published in 2010, using the result in 2001 seems far away from the reality.) Another question is the authors did not mention the total participants in formal students. If they recruited overlapped students in this research, is it adequate for the research result? In article 2, the draft version of the MALQ was field tested with 966 respondents in various countries, including Canada, Singapore, and The Netherlands. During this phase, instructors administer the questionnaire after the class of listening activity and all respondents completed the questionnaire in English. Respondents were encouraged to comment on any item that was unclear to them. However, not all of the respondents’ mother languages are English. Is it adequate to use English version questionnaire in this phase? Next, a confirmatory phase of the study was conducted with another large sample of respondents. In order to test whether there was a relationship between the listening behaviors reported in the MALQ and actual listening performance, a listening comprehension test was administered. This resulted in a reduced, but still relatively large sample of 341 students, including 115 EFL learners from Iran and 236 FSL learners from Canada. The questionnaire used in confirmatory phase was translated for the Iranian participants. Data Collection and Analysis In article 1, all the quantitative pilot data analyses were computed using version 16.0 of SPSS. First, an intraclass correlation was employed to examine the relationships between the English and Chinese versions of the questionnaires. Second, the Pearson product-moment correlation was employed to assess the validity of the LPSQ. Third, Cronbach’s alpha was used to examine the LPSQ’s internal consistency reliability. Exploratory factor analysis using principal component analysis (PCA) with oblique Promax rotation with Kaiser Normalization was performed using SPSS to identify underlying constructs for the fifty-one practice strategy items on the LPSQ for formal study. One unsatisfactory variable was removed at a time. First, a corrected item-total correlation analysis was used to remove items with correlation coefficients of 0.1 or less. Second, items with main loadings had absolute values of less than∣.30∣were deleted. Third, crossloading items with differences in factor loading of less than 0.1 were dropped. Cronbach’s alpha was used to examine the reliability of the scale. Finally, descriptive statistics were computed using SPSS to summarize the students’ responses to the questionnaires. In article 2, during exploratory factor analysis, an investigation of the MALQ factor structure was conducted through SPSS to determine whether there was empirical support for separate factors pertaining to metacognitive awareness of L2 listening and to identify any items that might be removed from the questionnaire. To estimate the maximum number of factors that might be interesting began with a principal component analysis (PCA) on the 51 items of the first version of the MALQ. For reasons of parsimony and meaningfulness, the scree test of eigenvalues plotted against factors was examined. Principal-axis factor analysis (PFA) was chosen to find the optimal number of factors. Promax rotation with Kaiser Normalization allows for intercorrelation among factors. The 51 items then underwent two PFA runs specifying five and six factors. A five-factor solution met the goals of interpretability, and was preferable in terms of comprehensibility. Next, to examine whether there were any unsatisfactory items. All items were evaluated for any observed low factor loadings, complex loadings, and/or reduced reliabilities. Using an iterative PFA analyses with Promax rotation, 39 items yielded five factors. A confirmatory factor analysis was conducted based on data collected from the second sample, using LISREL 8.72. Based on the results of the exploratory factor analysis, the authors thought there should be a further comparison of the goodness-if-fit among the four-, five-, and six-factor model. They employed maximum likelihood (ML) estimation. Several widely accepted goodness-of-fit indexes were computed, including CFI, TLI, PNFI, RMSEA. Based on the goodness-of-fit indexes of the confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), combined with the results of the exploratory factor analysis, the five-factor model was selected as the model of choice. Finally, in order to establish a relationship between the listening behaviors reported in the MALQ and actual listening behaviors, the authors correlated the data from the MALQ with the participants’ scores on a listening comprehension test. The correlation coefficient confirmed the relationship between listening comprehension ability and metacognitive awareness of the processes underlying successful L2 listening. To further reinforce the strength of this relationship, they regressed the scores of listening test on the MALQ scores. That indicated that about 13% of the variance in listening performance could be explained by metacognition. Findings Comparison According to the data analysis and discussion, the authors of 2 articles believed they constructed valid and reliable questionnaires as useful tools to assess their desired focal points. In article 1, the authors constructed a listening practice strategy questionnaire. There were 5 practice factors: problem-solving, language processing, deliberate practice, aural immersion, and English-comprehension practice in this questionnaire. The questionnaire used a 10-point scale. The five-factor model accounted for 46.9% of the total variance. A Cronbach’s alpha of .92 indicated high reliability. Regarding the frequency of strategy use, the aural immersion factor was sometimes used and ranked first among the five factors, followed by the problem-solving factor, which was also sometimes used. English-comprehension practice was least used among the five factors. Finally, the adequate validity and reliability of the questionnaire could make it a useful tool for listening strategy training. In article 2, the authors constructed a metacognitive awareness listening questionnaire. There were 5 factors in this questionnaire. However, they were different from factors in article 1. The 5 factors were problem-solving, planning and evaluation, translation, person knowledge, and directed attention. Not only was the significant relationship between the behaviors that listeners report using MALQ scores and actual listening performance listening test scores, but the regression analysis further verified a meaningful relationship between metacognition and listening comprehension success. Correlation between the scores accounts for about 13% of the variability in listening performance. However, this questionnaire is a self-report instrument. Instructors should consider the MALQ as one source of information about the current level of student awareness of L2 listening and strategic competence in processing L2 oral texts. Using the MALQ can enable L2 learners to become self-regulated listeners who can better capitalize on the aural input. By increasing their awareness of the listening process, students can learn how to become better listeners which will enable them to learn another language more quickly and more efficiently.