A Study of Language Learning Strategies Used by Applied English Major Students in University of Technology Shr-Chau Huang 黃士晁 I-Chen Chen 陳怡真 Southern Taiwan University of Technology Abstract Studies in the area of language learning strategies are blooming in Taiwan; however, little attention has been paid to students majoring in applied English departments. In view of this, this study was undertaken to understand applied English major students’ language learning strategy use and to investigate how factors such as academic level and gender were associated with their language learning strategy use. A total of 267 students at a university of technology in southern Taiwan participated in this study to answer a widely used language learning strategy questionnaire, Oxford’s (1990) Strategy Inventory for Language learning (SILL) during their regular classes. Descriptive statistics was conducted to understand students’ tendency in using language learning strategies whereas one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to examine whether gender and academic level had any significant effects on their strategy use. The results indicated students in this study used learning strategies in the medium range. The most frequently used strategies were social strategies followed by metacognitive, compensation, cognitive, affective and memory strategies. As far as the effects of gender and academic level on learning strategy use, gender made no significant differences in overall strategy use and in any of the six strategy categories whereas academic level made significant differences in overall strategy use, cognitive strategy use, memory strategy use and compensation strategy use. Finally, based on the results of this study, implications for learning strategy training in classroom are generated and discussed. Keywords: Learning strategy, Applied English majors, gender, academic level Introduction In the past few decades, the ESL / EFL education has shifted its focus from the teachers' teaching to the learners' learning. This shift has increased numbers of studies investigating learners’ perspectives, especially, on language learning strategies. In addition, more teachers and educators have realized that the application of language learning strategy training into regular classroom activities will enhance students’ learning. Influenced by these changes, teachers tend to encourage students to learn in a self-directing way so as to realize how to learn a language autonomously rather than learning a language relying on the teacher's lectures. 1 Learning strategies as defined by Oxford (1990) were specific actions taken by the learner to make learning easier, faster, more enjoyable, more self-directed, more effective and more transferable to new situations. These strategies involved a wide range of learning behaviors that helped learners become autonomous, self-regulated, and goal-oriented in their learning, which in turn enhanced their academic achievement through developing language skills. Since language learning strategies were considered as actions taken by learners to facilitate their learning, it is hoped that this study will give English teachers valuable information on how their students process information, plan and select the most suitable strategies to fit in their learning contexts. As a result, teachers can help their students become better language learners by training them in using the appropriate strategies. Although studies on learners' language learning strategy use have received much attention (Chen, 2000; Chung, 2000; Ku, 1995; Yang, 1992), most of these studies were conducted with heterogeneous student samples. Very little attention had been paid to English major students. Given their academic major, these students needed to receive teachers’ attention because their use of learning strategies might be proportionally higher than students majoring in other areas. These students at least received six years of formal English education since our education system decreed English as a required course starting from the first year in junior high school to the freshman year at the university or college. In general, these students had fundamental English skills before entering the college or university for receiving advanced English education. In the past, many English departments usually emphasized on traditional English literature and linguistics training, lacking practical aspects of English skill training in specialized areas such as translation and interpretation, English teaching and business English. In light of the demands for more practical English programs by students, English departments started to offer more practical English courses and some applied English departments were established in recent years to meet students’ need and train English major students as language professionals to be involved in various kinds of careers. Therefore, these students were reasonably expected to experience more language learning strategies than students majoring in other areas. Since few studies on language learning strategy had been conducted with English major students, the present study, therefore, aimed to fill in this gap by investigating English major students at university of technology on their learning strategy use and factors affecting their choice of language learning strategies. This study was organized around the following research questions: 1. What language learning strategies do university English major students report using? And what are their most frequently used language learning strategies? 2. What are the differences between female and male students in the use of learning strategy categories in the SILL? 3. What are the differences among students at academic level one, two, three and four in the use of learning strategy categories in the SILL? 2 Specially, this study was directed at testing the following null hypotheses: Hypothesis 1: There is no significant difference between female and male students in the use of learning strategy categories in the SILL. Hypothesis 2: There is no significant difference among students at academic level one, two, three and four in the use of learning strategy categories in the SILL. Limitations of This Study The sample population involved in this study was limited to English majors at a university of technology in southern Taiwan. As a result, the generalization of the results was confined to the population with a homogeneous nature and did not apply to populations with different educational, cultural, or language backgrounds. Literature Review Research into language learning strategies began in the 1960s due to the development in the cognitive psychology arousing attentions to language learning strategy studies. In most of the studies on language learning strategies, the primary concerns were on the identification of what good language learners reported how they were learning a second language (Rubin, 1987). This type of research focused on what a good language learner did and which language learning strategies they chose in language learning (Naiman, Frohlich, Stern, & Todesco, 1978; Rubin, 1975; Stern, 1975). Their assumptions were learning strategies could be identified and once successful learning strategies were identified, these strategies could be taught to less successful learners to make their learning become more effective and efficient. For example, O’Malley and other researchers (1985) interviewed beginning and intermediate level ESL students and finally they identified twenty-six language learning strategies. Like general learning strategies, language learning strategies involved techniques that learners used to remember what they had learnt; that was, their storage and retrieval of new information (Rubin, 1987). Also, language learning strategies included receptive strategies dealing with receiving the message and productive strategies related to communication (Brown, 1994; Chamot & Kupper, 1989). In terms of the classification, language learning strategies had been classified into several ways. O’Malley et al (1985) categorized strategies into three categories: cognitive, metacognitive and social-affective. Later Oxford (1990) developed O’Malley et al’s classification into a learning strategy system in which she divided learning strategies into direct and indirect strategies. Direct strategies were considered as a “performer” in a stage play working with the language itself in a variety of tasks and situations including memory strategies for remembering and retrieving new information, cognitive strategies for understanding and producing the language, and 3 compensation strategies for using the language despite knowledge gap. Indirect strategies were considered as a “director” of a play dealing with the general management of learning including metacognitive strategies for coordinating the learning process, affective strategies for regulating emotions, and social strategies for learning with others. Both direct and indirect strategies supported and helped one another for the best of possible learning outcomes. Investigations on language learners often showed that successful learners intended to use appropriate learning strategies leading to improvement, use strategies suitable to the learning tasks and even to use more and better strategies than the poor learners (Chamot & Kupper, 1989; O' Malley & Chamot, 1990; Oxford, 1993b). According to Rubin and Thompson (1994), good language learners could find their own way in charge of their learning, organizing their language information and creating their own opportunities for practicing the language. In addition, they are more purposeful in their approach to a language task. They used linguistic knowledge and contextual clues to help them comprehend while working on a language task. Therefore, it seemed good language learners possessed abilities to succeed while others lacked these abilities. Research had also shown factors other than language proficiency had influences on the strategies language learners used. For example, gender was one factor explored by many researchers (Ehrman, 1989; Green & Oxford, 1995; Oxford & Nyikos, 1989) and in many EFL contexts, studies on language learning strategy use in which gender was involved, the results usually showed female learners tended to use learning strategies more frequently than did the male learners (Oxford, 1993a). Other factors such as culture background (Oxford & Burry-Stock, 1995), learning styles (Sheorey, 1998), and years of studying English (Chiang & Liao, 2002) had also been researched and proposed as mediators of language strategy use. Method Instrumentation Information on strategy use was elicited by the Strategy Inventory for Language Learning ESL/EFL Version 7.0 (Oxford, 1990). The SILL was a five point Likert-scale questionnaire containing 50 statements related to learning strategies used by language learners. It had been widely used in learning strategy research to investigate the relationships of strategy use with various learner variables such as language learner’s level of proficiency, achievement, gender, nationality, or motivation, just to name a few. According to Green and Oxford (1995), the SILL had been used among 8000 learners in worldwide studies and had been extensively checked for high levels of reliability and validity (Oxford, 1996). However, the questionnaire, which was originally created to investigate western learners, might be 4 culturally biased when used in different social contexts and might not transfer well from one setting to another. Especially, when the questionnaire was translated and used in other cultures, the respondents in different settings might differ in how they interpreted the items. In order to make the measure accurate, avoid any misunderstanding and answer the questions easily in the instrument, the researcher translated the SILL into Chinese but kept the same total number of items, the arrangement of items, and the essential meaning of each item. Then, the researcher checked the translated questionnaire by consulting his advisor, a translation instructor, a linguist and a few graduate students in the same setting to make sure each item remained its essential meaning and the translation was easily understood. Based on their valuable suggestions and comments, the final questionnaire was made. Data Collection The data collection of this study included a pilot study and a formal study. The results of the pilot study served as the basis for fine-tuning the questionnaire and improving the test procedures, including checking the needed length of testing time, the clarity of the testing instructions, the distribution of questionnaire and so on. Pilot Study Before the formal study, the researcher obtained the permission of the instructor of an English class to conduct a pilot study during the students’ regular class session. The questionnaire was pilot tested with a class of thirty junior English students. Five cases were unusable and hence discarded because of incomplete responses to the questionnaire resulting in a final sample of twenty five cases. The reasons for choosing junior students as pilot study subjects were due to their similar educational background with the intended participants; that is, they had language programs identical to those of the intended participants. As a result of discussions and comments with these pilot subjects, some minor changes were made in test procedures. After the pilot study, the internal consistency reliability of the SILL questionnaire was measured by Cronbach’s alpha yielding a reliability coefficient of 0.93. Also, the reliability coefficients for the six strategy categories in the SILL questionnaire were all above 0.70. These values were acceptable based on a criterion of 0.70 as a minimally acceptable alpha value (Bobko, 2001; Litwin, 1995; Nunally, 1994). Table 1 showed the internal consistency reliability coefficients for the six strategy categories in the SILL questionnaire. 5 Table 1: Reliability Coefficients for the SILL Questionnaire in the Pilot Study Strategy Category Reliability Coefficients Memory strategies ( item 1 to item 9) Cognitive strategies ( item 10 to item 23) Compensation strategies ( item 24 to item 29) Metacognitive strategies ( item 30 to item 38) Affective strategies ( item 39 to item 44) Social strategies (item 45 to item 50) Overall strategy 0.76 0.87 0.72 0.84 0.72 0.81 0.93 Formal Study The formal study was conducted at the same site as the pilot study but a different sample from the one used for the pilot study was used in the formal study. A total of 8 classes of English majors, or 285 students, participated in the formal study. Eighteen cases were discarded because of incomplete or illegible responses. Thus the final sample was 267 cases, 52 were male and 215 were female. Among these students, 63 were the first year students; 60 came from the second year class; 63 came from the third year class; and the other 81 were in their fourth year of study. Table 2 indicated the internal consistency reliability coefficients of the SILL questionnaire in the formal study. For the entire questionnaire, Cronbach’s alpha achieved a coefficient of 0.926. Also, the coefficients of the six strategy categories in the SILL questionnaire were all above 0.70. Table 2: Reliability Coefficients for the SILL Questionnaire in the Formal Study Strategy Category Reliability Coefficients Memory strategies ( item 1 to item 9) Cognitive strategies ( item 10 to item 23) Compensation strategies ( item 24 to item 29) Metacognitive strategies ( item 30 to item 38) Affective strategies ( item 39 to item 44) Social strategies (item 45 to item 50) 0.72 0.86 0.71 0.87 0.70 0.75 Overall strategies 0.93 Data Analysis The SPSS (Statistical Package for the Social Science) version 10.0 (Pallant, 2001) was used to score the data and answer the research questions. The significance level was set at p< 0.05 for all statistical analysis. Descriptive statistics and one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) were statistical analysis methods used in this study: 6 1. Descriptive statistics such as mean scores, standard derivations, and frequencies were computed to summarize the learners’ responses to the SILL with a view to presenting the strategy use profiles of these applied English major students and hence to address the first research question. 2. One way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to determine whether there were significant differences in the use of the overall language learning strategies and in the use of each strategy category due to the two factors: gender and academic level. When necessary, Scheffé's post-hoc test was utilized to determine the differences of mean scores of strategy use among students at different academic levels since Scheffé 's post-hoc test was generally used when sample groups contain unequal numbers. Results Research Question One What language learning strategies do university English major students report using? And what are their most frequently used language learning strategies? Descriptive analysis of the SILL items was performed to answer the research question one. Table 3 listed the mean scores and standard derivations of all SILL items. Generally, an item with mean score greater than 3.5 indicated that the language learning strategy which had been described in the item was in the high frequency of use; an item with mean score between 2.5 to 3.4 indicated the strategy was in the medium frequency of use; and an item with mean score smaller than 2.4 indicated a low frequency of use of that particular strategy (Oxford, 1990). As showed in table 3, item 32 (M= 3.97), item 29 (M= 3.88), item 12 (M= 3.83), item 45 (M= 3.78), item 33 (M= 3.76), item 42 (M= 3.68), item 31 (M= 3.63), item 15 (M= 3.61), item 11 (M= 3.53), item 10 (M=3.50) were strategies in the high frequency of use, which indicated that students used these strategies more often than the other strategies. The other item mean scores in Table 3 ranged from 3.49 to 2.40 and were in the medium frequency of use. Only the mean score of item 43 (M= 1.81) was below 2.4 and was in the low frequency of use. Table 3: Mean Scores and Standard Derivations for the SILL Items Item description N M 1.學習英文時,我會把新學的東西與已學過的部分做聯想。 267 3.30 2.我會用新學的單字造句,以加深記憶。 267 2.70 3.我會把英文單字的發音與其相關的形象或圖形聯想,以幫助記 267 3.09 憶。 4.我記新的英文單字時 會想像在什麼情況下會使用到這個字。 267 3.08 5.我利用相類式的發音(例如 rice 和 ice; no 和 know)或押韻來記新 267 2.40 的英文單字 (Ex: The rain in Spain stays mainly in the plain.)。 7 SD 0.90 0.91 1.05 0.99 1.07 267 2.72 1.06 7.我會用肢體語言來幫助記憶單字。譬如說當我剛認得 Hit 是 267 2.60 “打”的意思,我就會邊念 Hit,邊用手比劃一下 “打”的動作。 8.我會時常複習英文功課。 267 2.94 9.我會靠英文單字或片語出現在書上,黑板上,或標誌上的位置來 267 2.89 記憶。 10.我會反覆練習說或寫英文生字。 267 3.50 11.我嘗試說得像以英語為母語的人一樣。 267 3.53 12.我練習英語發音。 267 3.83 13.我以不同的方式練習我所學的英文。 267 3.19 14.我會盡量以英語與他人交談。 267 2.74 1.06 267 3.61 267 2.92 267 3.04 0.95 18.閱讀英文時,我會先很快瀏覽英文片段,然後再回頭仔細研讀。267 3.39 19.我看到英文單字時會想一想中文裏哪一個字有類似的意思。 267 3.33 20.我在閱讀英文時,會分析英文的句型以增進了解。 267 3.16 21.我會把一個英文生字分解成幾個我認得的部份(如字首或字 267 3.25 根),以找出它的意義。 22.在理解英文時,我會避免逐字翻譯。 267 3.21 23.我會將我所聽到的和讀到的英文作成摘要筆記。 267 3.00 0.92 24.我遇到不熟悉的英文字時,我會猜一猜它的意思。 267 3.47 25.在英語會話中,若我想不起某個字,我會使用手勢或動作來表 267 3.35 達。 26.當我不知道適切的英文字時,我會自己造字(發明新的單字)來 267 2.84 表達(如用 air ball 來表達汽球 balloon)。 27.在閱讀英文遇到新的單字時,我會每個字都去查字典。 267 3.19 28.用英語交談時,我會去猜測別人下一句要說的英文。 267 2.86 29.當我想不出某個英文字時,我會使用意義相通的字或辭。 267 3.88 30.我會盡其所能地尋找各種方式來運用我所學的英文。 267 3.39 0.89 267 3.63 267 3.97 267 3.76 267 2.63 267 2.99 267 3.15 267 3.06 267 3.16 0.85 6.我會使用單字卡來背英文生字。 15.我會看英語發音的電視節目或電影,或收聽英語廣播。 16.我閱讀英文書刊以作為娛樂、消遣。 17.我用英文來通訊、寫筆記、書信、或做報告。 31.我會留意自己的英語錯誤,並利用它來改進我的英文程度。 32.當別人說英語時,我會留意聽。 33.我會試著找出如何學好英語的方法。 34.我會訂立作息表,以使自己有足夠的時間學習英語。 35.我會留心尋訪我可以用英語交談的對象。 36.我會尋覓時機多閱讀英文。 37.對於增進我的英語能力,我會訂立明確的目標。 38.我會自我檢討自己學習英語進步的情形。 8 0.88 1.09 0.86 0.99 0.89 0.99 0.88 0.98 0.93 0.98 0.99 1.02 0.97 0.97 1.08 1.14 0.99 1.05 0.83 0.92 0.79 0.85 0.98 1.09 0.94 0.93 0.93 267 3.38 1.13 40.即使畏懼犯錯,我仍會鼓勵自己說英語。 267 2.92 41.每當我的英語表現良好,我會獎勵自己。 267 2.61 42.當我讀英文或說英語的時候,我會注意自己是否會緊張。 267 3.68 43.我會和別人討論自己學英語的感受。 267 1.81 44.我會在筆記本上寫下自己學習英語的心得。 267 3.12 45.假如在英語會話中,我有聽不懂的地方,我會要求對方說慢一 267 3.78 點或重說一遍。 46.說英語時,我會要求對方改正我的錯誤。 267 3.14 47.我會與其他同學練習英文。 267 3.13 48.我會向講英文的人求助。 267 3.45 1.07 49.我會發問以澄清及證實英文上的問題。 267 3.17 50.我會試著學習瞭解英語系國家的文化(以說英語為主的國家)。 267 3.49 0.95 39.每當我感到害怕使用英語時,我會設法先放鬆自己心情。 1.27 1.19 0.92 1.32 0.95 1.03 0.98 0.99 1.06 Table 4 presented the rank order of the six strategy categories according to their frequencies of use. Based on the average use of the six strategies, table 4 revealed that social strategies (M= 3.36) had the highest mean score followed by metacognitive strategies (M= 3.30), compensation strategies (M= 3.27), cognitive strategies (M= 3.26), affective strategies (M= 2.92), while memory strategies (M= 2.86) ranked the lowest mean. It was also noticed that the six strategies categories fell in the medium use range. Table 4: Mean Scores and Standard Derivations of SILL Categories. Strategy Category N M SD Rank Social strategies Metacognitive strategies Compensation strategies Cognitive strategies Affective strategies Memory strategies 267 267 267 267 267 267 3.36 3.30 3.27 3.26 2.92 2.86 0.66537 0.64067 0.63871 0.56212 0.73282 0.55923 1 2 3 4 5 6 Research Question Two What are the differences between female and male students in the use of learning strategy categories in the SILL? Null Hypothesis One There is no significant difference between female and male students in the use of learning strategy categories in the SILL. One-way analysis of variance was conducted to determine any significant difference between male and female students in the use of overall language learning strategies and in each of the strategy categories. The ANOVA results, as showed in 9 table 5, revealed no significant difference between male and female students in the use of language learning strategies as a whole and in any of the six strategy categories. The results implied that male and female subjects did not differ in their frequencies of using language learning strategy as a whole and in any of the six strategy categories. Table 5: ANOVA Results for the Effect of Gender on Strategy Category Use (N=267) Strategy category Memory strategies Cognitive strategies Compensation strategies Metacognitive strategies Affective strategies Social strategies Overall Strategies SS F P Between Groups 0.23 0.75 0.39 Within Groups 82.96 Between Groups 0.001 0.001 0.95 Within Groups 84.05 Between Groups 0.08 0.2 0.65 Within Groups 108.43 Between Groups 0.11 0.27 0.61 Within Groups 109.07 Between Groups 0.31 0.57 0.45 Within Groups 142.54 Between Groups 0.21 0.47 0.49 Within Groups 117.55 Between Groups 0.01 0.05 0.82 Within Groups 57.09 *p<0.05 Table 6 summarized the mean scores and standard derivations of strategy use by male and female students. The mean scores of each strategy category in the SILL and the total mean scores reported by male and female students showed that the mean scores of strategy use of female students were higher than male students in memory, cognitive, metacognitive, and social strategies. Conversely, the mean scores of male students were higher than female students in the use of compensation and affective strategies. However, the differences between their mean scores of strategy use were slight. Table 6: Mean Scores and Standard Derivations of Strategy Category Use by Gender (N=267) Male (N=52) Female (N=215) Strategy category Mean Std. Deviation Mean Std. Derivation Memory strategies Cognitive strategies Compensation strategies Metacognitive strategies 2.8 3.26 3.3 3.26 2.87 3.27 3.26 3.31 0.56 0.54 0.72 0.65 10 0.56 0.57 0.62 0.64 Affective strategies 2.99 0.78 2.9 0.72 Social strategies Overall strategies 3.30 3.15 0.76 0.7 3.38 3.16 0.64 0.66 Research Question Three What are the differences among students at academic level one, two, three and four in the use of learning strategy categories in the SILL? Null Hypothesis Two There is no significant difference among students at academic level one, two, three and four in the use of learning strategy categories in the SILL. To determine the differences in the use of overall language learning strategies and in the use of each strategy category, one way ANOVA was used. The ANOVA results, as showed in table 7, indicated that there were significant differences on the use of the overall language learning strategies and on memory, cognitive and compensation strategies. Although we knew that students at different academic level groups differed in the use of overall language learning strategies and in the use of memory, cognitive and compensation strategies, we did not know where these differences among the groups occurred. We needed to further understand which two of the academic level groups being compared were significantly different from one another in their learning strategy use. Therefore, post-hoc Scheff é test was used to evaluate pair-wise difference among students at academic level one, two, three and four in the use of overall strategies, memory, cognitive and compensation strategies. Table 8 to table 11 illustrated the reports of post-hoc Scheffé tests for the mean differences among students at different academic level groups in overall strategy use and in the use of memory, cognitive, and compensation strategies. In addition, the mean difference that was significant at the 0.05 level was reported with an asterisk on the upper right side of it. The results of Scheffé's post-hoc test revealed there were significant differences between freshmen and juniors in the mean scores of memory and compensation strategies in favor of the freshmen. This indicated freshman students significantly used more memory strategies and compensation strategies than the juniors. Also, the results indicated significant differences between juniors and seniors in mean scores of cognitive strategies and overall strategies in favor of the seniors, which indicated that seniors employed more cognitive strategies and overall strategies than the juniors 11 Table 7: AVOVA Results for the Effect of Academic Level on Strategy Category Use (N=267) Strategy Category Memory strategies Cognitive strategies Compensation strategies Metacognitive strategies Affective strategies Social strategies Overall strategies SS F P Between Groups 3.37 3.70 0.01* Within Groups 79.82 Between Groups 3.35 3.64 0.01* Within Groups 80.70 Between Groups 4.51 3.80 0.01* Within Groups 104.00 Between Groups 2.34 1.92 0.13 Within Groups 106.84 Between Groups 0.23 0.14 0.93 Within Groups 142.61 Between Groups 2.71 2.07 0.10 Within Groups 115.05 Between Groups 2.29 3.66 0.01* Within Groups 54.81 *p<0.05 Table 8: Scheffé's Post-Hoc Test for Mean Difference Between Academic Levels in Memory Strategies Academic Level Freshman Sophomore Junior Senior Freshman Sophomore Junior Senior -0.14250 -0.32275* -0.11973 -0.18025 0.02277 0.14250 0.32275* 0.18025 0.11973 -0.02277 0.20302 -0.20302 Table 9: Scheffé's Post-Hoc Test for Mean Difference Between Academic Levels in Cognitive Strategies Academic Level Freshman Sophomore Junior Senior Freshman Sophomore Junior Senior 0.02421 -0.22676 0.06173 -0.25096 0.03752 -0.02421 0.22676 0.25096 -0.06173 -0.03752 0.28849* -0.28849* Table 10: Scheffé's Post-Hoc Test for Mean Difference Between Academic Levels in Compensation Strategies Academic Level Freshman Sophomore Freshman Sophomore Junior Senior -0.14749 -0.37566* -0.17166 -0.22817 -0.02418 0.14749 12 Junior 0.37566* 0.22817 Senior 0.17166 0.02418 0.20400 -0.20400 Table 11: Scheffé's Post-Hoc Test for Mean Difference Between Academic Levels in Overall Strategies Academic Level Freshman Sophomore Junior Senior Freshman Sophomore Junior Senior -0.01724 -0.22691 -0.01044 -0.20966 0.00681 0.01724 0.22691 0.20966 0.01044 -0.00681 0.21647* -0.21647* Table 12 summarized the mean scores and standard deviations of strategy category use by the subjects at four different academic level groups. It was noted that, across all the academic levels, except academic one, the order of the six strategy categories is unchanged. The first was social strategies followed by metacognitive strategies and cognitive strategies. The fourth category was always compensation strategies. In the fifth place was affective strategies followed by memory strategies. Moreover, the total mean scores of strategy use reported by students at four academic levels indicated that, in overall strategy use, the mean scores of strategy use of students at academic level one were higher than students at the other academic levels. Table 12: Mean Scores and Standard Deviations of Strategy Category Use by Students at Four Academic Levels (N= 267) Level 1 Strategy Category Memory strategies Cognitive strategies Compensation strategies Metacognitive strategies Affective strategies Social strategies Overall strategies (N=63) Level 2 (N=60) Level 3 (N=63) Level 4 (N=81) M SD M SD M SD M SD 3.00 0.50 2.86 0.57 2.68 0.45 2.88 0.64 3.29 0.53 3.32 0.58 3.07 0.44 3.36 0.62 3.44 0.60 3.29 0.67 3.06 0.57 3.27 0.66 3.28 0.58 3.39 0.72 3.15 0.60 3.38 0.65 2.94 0.77 2.92 0.78 2.87 0.71 2.95 0.69 3.42 0.67 3.44 0.57 3.18 0.57 3.40 0.78 3.23 0.56 3.20 0.56 3.00 0.73 3.20 0.67 Discussion Since the subjects of this study were applied English major students at a university of technology, the results of this study might not be totally consistent with previous language learning strategy studies but their frequencies of strategy use were greater than those of other student population (i.e. high school and non-English majors) participating in similar previous studies (Chen, 2000; Chung, 2000; Ku, 1995; Yang, 1992). 13 A close look at the frequency use of learning strategies of these students revealed that applied English major students’ learning strategy use, on the average, was of medium use (M= 3.16) with social strategies used more frequently (M= 3.36), which was in contrast to the stereotype that Chinese students were shy and resist partaking in social interaction to improve their language skills. Since social strategies referred to the ways in which learners managed interacting with other learners or native speakers to facilitate their learning including skills such as asking questions for clarification, cooperating with peers, and developing cultural understanding, these strategies, in general, helped learners work with others to get input and practice. The high use of social strategies may be due to the teaching objectives and curriculum design in the applied English program that usually emphasize training students in practical and communicative aspects of English skills. For example, teachers in the applied English program often adopted the communicative approach to motivate students to learn the language. Teachers often engaged their students in learning English by partnering up the students in pairs or small groups to complete learning tasks through communicative activities. As a result, applied English majors were encouraged to employ social strategies more often than other strategies. In addition, the high use of social strategies is opposed to the learning behavior study by Polizer and McGroarty (1985) suggesting that Asian students seemed to prefer strategies involving rote memorization of language rules, rather than communicative strategies. Also, the findings of social strategies used more often than other strategies were different from the studies of Ku (1995) and Yang (1992) reporting that college students used compensation strategies most frequently among the six strategy categories in the SILL. With regard to the effect of gender on strategy use, many studies had consistent findings that females appeared to use a far wider range of language learning strategies than did males (Belen M, 1995; Green & Oxford, 1995; Oxford, Lavine, & Crookall, 1989; Oxford & Nyikos, 1989). However, this study did not discover distinct gender differences in strategy use just as Ehrman and Oxford’ study (1990) failed to discover any evidence of differing language learning strategy use between the genders. It might be concluded, perhaps, that although females and males did not always demonstrate differences in language learning strategy use, women were found to use more language learning strategies than men. In terms of the effect of academic level on the use of language learning strategies, this study showed, in the use of overall language learning strategy use, senior students were found to be using significantly more strategies than junior students, which echoed Ehrman’s (1990) study of students in the Foreign Service Institute where she found that students with more years of learning tended to employ more strategies than those with fewer years of learning experience. As for the use of the six strategy categories, it was found that senior students tended to use more cognitive strategies than junior students. The reason for senior students using more cognitive strategies than junior students could be the requirement of the school that students need to achieve a specified English proficiency level before they could 14 graduate. Students need to prove their English proficiency by taking various forms of standardized tests such as the TOEFL, TOEIC, GEPT or English tests developed and administered by the school itself. Before taking these standardized tests, school has set specific preparation courses to help students pass a wide range of English tests and these students usually take these specific courses in their fourth year of study in the applied English program. In these specific courses, students had to practice a lot of sample exam questions as part of their preparation for a real test and hence cognitive strategies (strategies for practicing, analyzing, and summarizing) such as repetition, translation, and note-taking of what they heard or read in English were being employed to enhance their learning. Consequently, these students used cognitive strategies more often than their peers at the third year of study in the applied English program. Freshman students were found to be using memory and compensation strategies more often than junior students. The possible explanation could be that when freshman students just entered a new school, they have not adjusted themselves to the new learning environment. These freshmen were still used to memorization, drill and constant tests, which were the most common methods used in high school classroom for learning academic subjects. Also, they perhaps could not fit in the classroom where teacher lectured in English throughout the class. As a result, these students were trying to use compensation strategies (strategies for using the language despite gaps in knowledge) for overcoming their deficiency in English class and tended to learn English by memorizing grammatical rules, vocabulary and idioms and neglected to use the English they learn functionally. After passing the initial stages of adjustment to the new learning contexts, these new students may feel at ease and use memory and compensation strategies less frequently as they progress to higher academic level. Implications Since the present research has generated findings pertaining to applied English major students’ language learning strategies, the most important implication of this study would be to provide students with more chances to use learning strategies more frequently through integrating language learning strategy training into regular classroom activities because all the strategy categories reported by the subjects fell in the medium range. Recent research suggested “explicit training interwoven into normal classroom activities, spread out over a long period of time with plenty of practice and transfer opportunities” (Yang, 1996, p.73) in both informal and formal second language settings. Therefore, the teachers’ role in strategy training is an important one. English teachers could design their classroom activities so that students might have opportunities to listen, speak, read and write in English and try various learning strategies to help students accomplish their learning tasks. Also, teachers could inform students of the purposes and values of the strategies they already used and then 15 presented and modeled new or unfamiliar strategies for these students followed by providing them with extensive practice to expand their repertoire of learning strategies. In addition to providing strategy training to the students, the language teacher should have also analyzed his/her textbook, lesson plan and teaching method to see whether his/her teaching allows students to approach the learning tasks in different ways or not so that the teacher could become better prepared to focus on language learning strategies and strategy training throughout his/her teaching. Conclusion This study aimed at investigating the language learning strategies of applied English major students studying at a university of technology and how factors such as academic level and gender were associated with their language learning strategy use. Probably owing to their educational background and English learning contexts, the students in this study reported using learning strategies that were somewhat different from previous studies conducted with student groups. It seemed applied English majors preferred to use social strategies more often than other strategies. In addition, this study did not find any significant difference in strategy use as far as the effect of gender is concerned. However, significant differences were detected among students at different academic levels in their use of language learning strategies. Although this research was based on a particular student sample and the generalization of the results from this setting to another may be limited, it is hoped that this research can serve as a basis for future research to explore language learning strategies used by other populations to better understand their learning strategies and help them learn language more effectively. 16 References Belen M, S. (1995). Gender difference, perceptions on foreign language learning and language learning strategies. Paper presented at the twelfth conference on English teaching and learning in the Republic of China, Fu Jen Catholic University, Taiwan. Bobko, P. (2001). Correlation and regression (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, California: Sage Publications. Brown, H. (1994). Principles of language learning teaching (3rd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall Regents. Chamot, A. U., & Kupper, L. (1989). Learning strategies in foreign language instruction. Foreign Language Annals, 22, 13-24. Chen, J. Y. (2000). Junior high school students' English learning strategies. Paper presented at the ninth international symposium on English teaching, Taipei. Chiang, M., & Liao, P. (2002). The study of learning strategies used by applied English majors in junior college. The Journal of Chungchou, 16, 309-325. Chung, Y. T. (2000). The motivation and language learning strategies of students in high school: A site study. Paper presented at the ninth international symposium on English teaching, Taipei. Ehrman, M. E. (1989). Effects of sex differences, career choice, and psychological type on adult language learning strategies. The Modern Language Journal, 73(1), 1-13. Ehrman, M. E. (1990). The role of personality type in adult language learning: An ongoing investigation. In S. T. Parry & C. W. Standfield (Eds.), Language aptitude reconsidered (pp. 126-178). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Ehrman, M. E., & Oxford, L. R. (1990). Adult language learning styles and strategies in an intensive training setting. The Modern Language Journal, 74(3), 311-327. Green, J. M., & Oxford, L. R. (1995). A closer look at learning strategies, L2 proficiency, and gender. TESOL Quarterly, 29(2), 261-297. Ku, P. Y. (1995). Strategies associated with proficiency and predictors of strategy choice: A study on language learning strategies of EFL students at three educational level in Taiwan. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Indiana, Bloomington. Litwin, M. S. (1995). How to Measure Survey Reliability and Validity. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Naiman, N., Frohlich, M., Stern, H. H., & Todesco, A. (1978). The good language learner. Toronto: The Ontario Institute for Studies in Education. Nunally, J. C. (1994). Psychometric theory (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. 17 O' Malley, J. M., & Chamot, A. U. (1990). Learning Strategies in Second Language Acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press. O' Malley, J. M., Chamot, A. U., Stewner-Manzanares, G., Russo, R. P., & Kupper, L. (1985). Learning strategy applications with students of English as a second language. TESOL Quarterly, 19, 557-584. Oxford, L. R. (1990). Language learning strategies: What every teacher should know. New York: Newbury House/Harper Collins. Oxford, L. R. (1993a). Instructional implications of gender differences in language learning styles and strategies. Applied Language Learning, 4, 65-94. Oxford, L. R. (1993b). Research on second language learning strategies. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 13, 175-187. Oxford, L. R. (1996). Employing a questionnaire to assess the use of language learning strategies. Applied Language Learning, 7, 25-45. Oxford, L. R., & Burry-Stock, J. A. (1995). Assessing the use of language learning strategies worldwide with the ESL/EFL version of the strategy inventory for language learning (SILL). System, 23(2), 153-175. Oxford, L. R., Lavine, R. Z., & Crookall, D. (1989). Language learning strategies, the communicative approach, and their classroom implications. Foreign Language Annals, 22, 29-39. Oxford, L. R., & Nyikos, M. (1989). Variables affecting choice of language learning strategies by university students. Modern Language Journal, 73, 291-300. Pallant, J. (2001). SPSS survival manual : A step-by-step guide to data analysis using SPSS for Windows (Version 10) (1st ed.). Crows Nest, N.S.W.: Open University Press. Politzer, R. L., & McGroarty, M. (1985). An exploratory study of learning behaviors and their relationship to gains in linguistic and communicative competence. TESOL Quarterly, 19, 103-123. Rubin, J. (1975). What the good language learner can teach us. TESOL Quarterly, 9, 41-51. Rubin, J. (1987). Learner strategies: Theoretical assumptions, research history and typology. EngleWood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Rubin, J., & I., T. (1994). How to be a more successful language learner (2nd ed.). Boston: Heinel&Heinel. Sheorey, R. (1998). The state of English and English language teaching in India. TESOL Matters, 8(4), 1-19. Stern, H. H. (1975). What can we learn from the good language learner? Canadian Modern Language Review, 31, 304-318. 18 Yang, N. D. (1992). Second language learners' beliefs about language learning and their use of learning strategies: A study of college students of English in Taiwan. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Texas at Austin, Austin. Yang, N. D. (1996). A study of factors affecting college EFL students' use of learning strategies. Paper presented at the eleventh conference on English teaching and learning, Fu Jen Catholic University, Taiwan. 19