OECD, July 6th What makes the French so unhappy? The Role of Mentality and Culture Claudia Senik Paris School of Economics Beyond GDP … Happiness indicators • Stiglitz-Sen-Fitoussi Commission (2009): Measure Well-Being beyond GDP Inter alia: use Happiness indicators as target for economic policy • De facto: Important magnitude of country fixed-effects in happiness estimates • What is the composition of these fixed effects? Do governments have leverage on them? What is the relevant sphere of intervention? • National cultures of happiness? Income per Capita and Happiness: Country Differences ESS data 20022008 (Happiness, 0-10 Scale) and Penn World Tables (GDP per capita) Cultural Clusters of Happiness Source: Inglehart, Foa, Peterson, Welzel (2008), p.269 Average Happiness and the HDI (GDI = Income per head + Life Expectancy + Educational Attainment) ESS data 2002-2008 (happiness 0-10 scale) and UNDP HDI Cultural Differences Over Time Life Satisfaction 0-4 scale Cultural Differences Over Time Life Satisfaction 0-10 scale The Utility of Income is not the Same for all Countries Life Satisfaction 0-10 scale Estimates of Happiness and Country Fixed-Effects What this paper does: • Show evidence of the important role of Mentality and Culture in Happiness By decomposing country fixed effects • Focus on the case of France as an illustration Disentangle several possible factors explaining international heterogeneity in happiness: External circumstances external institutions, regulations and general living conditions that inhabitants of a country are confronted with “Mentality”, defined as an internal, psychological idiosyncrasy set of specific attitudes, beliefs, ideals and ways of transforming events into happiness that individuals engrain during their infancy and teenage in education and socialization instances such as school, firms and organizations Persistent “Mentality” = Culture long-run persistent attitudes, beliefs and values that characterize groups of people, following the terminology of (among others) Bisin and Verdier (2001, 2011), Fernandez and Fogli (2006, 2007, 2009), Fernandez (2008, 2011), Guiso et al. (2006), Algan et al. (2007, 2010). Assume to start that these dimensions are separable (relax this assumption later) Empirical exercise • European Social Survey of 13 different countries (ESS, waves 1 to 4; 2002-2008) • I contrast the happiness of natives to that of immigrants of the first and second generation in Europe. For example, to the extent in which happiness is due to external circumstances, the pattern of happiness of immigrants in Europe should be the same as that of natives. Assumptions: • Circumstances of a country j are experienced by all its inhabitants in the same way, independently of their geographical origin. • Natives and 2nd generation immigrants of country j share the same socialization experience, hence the same mentality. • 1st generation immigrants have been socialized in a different system. • Immigrants of the first and second generation still share at least part of the culture of their origin country k (k≠j ), while the natives of country j share the culture of that country j. Why Culture, why 2 generations? • Cultural inertia as a stock that has the same value for immigrants of the first and second general, and disappears after the second generation. • Cut-point imposed by the survey, which reports the origin of individuals and of their parents, but not further. • This convention probably correspond to the idea is that cultural differences take time to dissipate (in the case of the culture of origin) or to acquire (in the case of the culture of the destination country), and vanishes after two generations. Using happiness differences of natives and migrants to identify the role of circumstances/mentality/culture • Hence, 1st generation immigrants are taken to differ from natives by their “culture” and “mentality”; • 2nd generation immigrants only differ from natives by their “culture” • 1st generation immigrants differ from 2nd generation immigrants by their “mentality”. • I use these difference and double differences (between countries) to identify the share of the country fixed-effects that can be attributed to Circumstances/ Mentality/Culture. Estimation and Identification These parameters measure the weight of Circumstances, Mentality and Culture in each country’s idiosyncratic happiness. Focusing on the French mal-être • France obtains high scores in negative dimensions of mental health, such as psychological distress and mental disorders, as measured by the International Classification of Disease (ICD10) or the American DSM IV (see Eugloreh, 2007). • Exceptionally high consumption of psychotropes (especially anti-depression) by European standards See ESEMeD, a study of the general population, run in 2001-2003 over 21 425 individuals aged 18 and over, see also CAS (2010). • France is also one of the rare Western European countries where the prevalence of suicide as a cause of death is higher than 13 for 100 000 it was of 16,3 for 100 000 inhabitants in 2007, i.e. 10 000 suicide deaths per year. This is much higher than any of the “old European countries” except Finland (World Health Organization). • In France, suicide is the second cause of mortality among the 15-44 years old (after road accident), and the first cause among the 30-39 years old (CAS, 2010). By contrast, the rate of suicide death is low in Italy, Portugal and Germany, as well as the consumption of psychotropes. Probit estimate of declaring oneself very happy (810 on a 0-10scale). Marginal effects of living in country X rather than in France Other controls: age, age square, gender, marital status, log(income), unemployed, year FE, cluster(country). Potential explanations of the French unhapiness • Algan and Cahuc (2007): role of the vicious heavy state regulation - low trust low happiness nexus. cultural dimension of trust and happiness in cross-country comparisons. • High rate of unemployment (see below) • Poor quality of employment relationship (Mueller and Philippon, 2006; Blanchard and Philippon, 2006; • Role of lost colonial grandeur (that France shares with Italy and Germany), • Anti-capitalist preferences (Saint Paul, 2010), • Contradictory values (egalitarianism and aristocratism) exacerbated by the very elitist school system (d’Iribarne, 1989) • … Related literature • Culture identified using migrants: Luttmer and Singhal (2011), Algan, Cahuc et al. (2007, 2010, 2011), Alesina and Giuliano (2011), Fernández and Fogli (2006, 2007, 2009) • Inter-generational transmission of cultural traits Portes and Zhou (1993), Bisin and Verdier (2001, 2001), and Bisin et al. (2004). • Happiness and Migration Bartram (2011) Safi (2010), Baltascu (2007), De Jong et al. (2002). • Discrimination of immigrants … • Differential Item Functioning (DIF), Frame Of Reference Biases (FORB), Vignettes King et al. (2004), King and Wand (2006), Beegle et al.( 2009), Kapteyn et al. (2009), Angelini et al. (2009), Hopkins and King (2010). Data: European Social Survey, 4 waves: 2002-2008 • Regression sample: Countries that are present in the survey at each wave: 13 countries with about 1000 (Slovenia) to 2300 (Germany) observations per wave. Composition of migration flows to Europe Region of Origin of Migrants to Europe OLS Estimate of Happiness (0-10 scale) Country Fixed-Effects by Migration Status Whereas French natives score 22% lower on happiness than the average Europeans, first generation immigrants in France are in the average in terms of happiness. Second generation immigrants in France are about 19% lower than the average second generation immigrants. Derivation of parameters based on the former estimates Robustness • Controlling for main effects and interactions of macroeconomic circumstances with migration status does not alter the basic findings (the structure of parameters) • Share of government expenditure/GDP, national rate of unemployment, GDP growth rate • Migration status*country of destination • Region of origin*country of destination • Migration status*country of destination*gender/age/income/unemployed/ transfer recipient OLS Estimates of Happiness. Subsample of 1st generation immigrants. Immigrants who went to school in France before the age of 10 are less happy Other controls: age, age square, income, unemployed, marital status, regions of origin, year fixed-effects. Cluster(country) Probit estimate of happiness. Sub-sample of 1st generation immigrants Immigrants who have lived in France for more than 20 years are less happy Other controls: age, age square, gender, log(income), marital status, unemployed, Region of origin, year fixed-effects. Cluster(country) French migrants are less happy Sub-sample of migrants from the 13 European countries of the ESS OLS Estimate of Happiness Other controls: age, age square, gender, log(income), marital status, unemployed, Region of origin, year fixedeffects. Cluster(country). Only waves 2-4 because no information about country of origin of immigrants in ESS wave 1. Replicating Luttmer and Singhal (2010) OLS Estimates of Happiness Sub-sample of migrants from countries of the ESS The happiness level of immigrants is correlated with the average happiness of residents in their country of origin Probit estimate to be very happy. Sample of residents in Belgium. Marginal effects displayed French-speaking Belgians are less happy Probit estimate to be very happy. Sample of residents in Switzerland. Marginal effects displayed French-speaking Swiss are not less happy than Germanophones or Italianophones It is not a purely nominal language effect