Experimental Tests of Endocrine Function in Breeding and Nonbreeding Raptors Julio Blas1,* Fabrizio Sergio1 John C. Wingfield2,† Fernando Hiraldo1 1 Department of Conservation Biology, Estación Biológica de Doñ ana, Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Cientı́ficas, Américo Vespucio s/n E-41092, Sevilla, Spain; 2Department of Biology, Box 351800, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington 98195 to stress in young floaters compared with breeders). Contrary to the hypothesis of sexual immaturity, our results suggest that floating males are physiologically capable of reproducing. The reported differences in adrenocortical function support the idea that floaters are socially subordinate to breeders, and corticosterone responses reflect the sex-specific roles during competition in socially monogamous species. Introduction ABSTRACT Many long-lived avian species defer reproduction for several years, often displaying a “floating” behavior characterized by the lack of mates and exclusive territories. Understanding the proximate mechanisms regulating floating behavior is a relevant topic of research for physiologists, behavioral ecologists, and population biologists because a prolonged period of nonbreeding can negatively affect lifetime fitness and change population dynamics. Here we tested two hypotheses linking endocrine function to floating status: (a) floaters undergo a period of sexual immaturity characterized by lower gonadal function (hypothesis of sexual immaturity), and (b) floating status is socially imposed by dominant conspecifics and revealed by the adrenocortical response to stress (hypothesis of social subordination). The two hypotheses were tested in a population of freeliving black kites Milvus migrans in Doñ ana National Park (southwest Spain), where breeders coexist with young floaters that defer reproduction for 3–7 yr. Hypophysial-gonadal function, estimated as androgen production in response to experimental challenge with gonadotropin-releasing hormone (cGnRH-I), was similar in magnitude and timing between floating and breeding males. The same treatment was, however, unable to elicit any response in terms of increasing estradiol or total androgen levels in females regardless of their breeding status. Following experimental capture and restraint, the adrenocortical response to stress (estimated as circulating corticosterone levels) was higher in floating than in breeding males, while females showed the opposite pattern (i.e., lower response * Corresponding author; e-mail: julioblas@ebd.csic.es. † Present address: Department of Neurobiology, Physiology and Behavior, University of California, Davis, California 95616. Avian populations are frequently composed of breeding individuals coexisting with nonbreeders, which are generally young birds that defer reproduction for a variable number of years. Depending on the species and population, nonbreeders may adopt different behavioral strategies, such as “helping” a reproductive pair (i.e., cooperative breeding systems; Emlem 1982) or “floating” without establishing bonds to a particular territory (i.e., “floaters”; Smith 1978; Zack and Stutchbury 1992; Rohner 1997). Floaters are widespread in wild avian populations (e.g., seabirds: Hector et al. 1990; Williams 1992; Frederiksen and Bregnballe 2001; songbirds: Studd and Robertson 1985; raptors: Newton 1992; Sergio et al. 2009; Blas and Hiraldo 2010), and the length of time that an individual remains a nonbreeder can have important effects on individual fitness and population dynamics (Kokko and Sutherland 1998; Penteriani et al. 2005). For these reasons, understanding the proximate mechanisms underlying such a pervasive strategy constitutes a relevant topic of research with potential applications to managing wild and captive endangered species (e.g., raptors: Ferrer and Hiraldo 1991; Penteriani et al. 2005). The physiological basis of floating remains largely unstudied, especially among long-lived species. This may be due to the need for extensive long-term programs employing capture and recapture and sampling and monitoring of individually marked birds. This study tests whether two complementary nonmutually exclusive hypotheses of proximate causation, namely “sexual immaturity” and “social subordination,” explain floating behavior in a long-lived raptor, the black kite Milvus migrans. The hypothesis of sexual immaturity (Hall et al. 1987; Hector et al. 1990; Williams 1992) posits a decreased sexual function in young floaters and is largely assumed to apply to Accipitridae birds, where young floaters are normally termed “immature” individuals. However, recent studies have shown that the seasonal patterns of circulating sex-steroid hormones in young nonbreeding black kites are similar to those of reproductively active birds (Blas and Hiraldo 2010), with elevated titers of androgens and estradiol characterizing the early stages of the breeding season in males and females, respectively. The latter study suggests that the endocrine system of breeders and floaters responds to the environmental cues signaling the start of the breeding season (namely, “initial predictive information”; Wingfield 1980; Wingfield and Farner 1993), such as photoperiod. However, when compared with breeders, floater males tended to have lower absolute levels of circulating testosterone, whereas floater females had statistically lower estradiol levels (Blas and Hiraldo 2010). This could be explained by either (1) the lack of exposure to additional stimulatory cues, such as within-pair interactions (Wingfield 1980; Wingfield and Farner 1993; Setiawan et al. 2007); or (2) an immature hypothalamuspituitary-gonadal (HPG) axis unable to trigger the adequate hormonal responses to such signals. An experimental test of the maturity of the HPG axis consists of the administration of a standard exogenous dose of gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH) followed by the collection of blood samples to determine changes in circulating sex-steroid hormone levels. GnRH is produced in the hypothalamus and stimulates the pituitary to release luteinizing hormone, which triggers the production of sex steroids in the gonads (Williams 1992; Moore et al. 2002). According to the hypothesis of sexual immaturity, floaters are expected to show little or no production of gonadal steroids following experimental treatment with GnRH compared with breeding birds. With regard to the hypothesis of social subordination, if nonbreeding status is socially imposed by dominant individuals, breeders and nonbreeders may differ in their adrenocortical response to stress (see Mays et al. 1991; Schoech et al. 1997; Young et al. 2006 for examples in cooperative breeding species). Social conflicts normally increase allostatic load (i.e., the energy requirements to perform daily routines; see McEwen and Wingfield 2003 for details), and this is often reflected in increased adrenocortical secretion. Traditionally, it has been thought that subordinate individuals exhibit greater adrenocortical responsiveness than dominants (reviewed in von Holst 1998), although this is not always the case (e.g., Rohwer and Wingfield 1981; Creel et al. 1996; Creel 2001). Only recently, Goymann and Wingfield (2004) reconciled the conflicting evidence and proposed that breeding systems are only crude predictors of how social status affects glucocorticoid concentrations and that dominants or subordinates express higher or lower adrenocortical responsiveness to stress depending on the relative allostatic load of social status. If subordinates do not compete with dominants, they will likely suffer minimal agonistic interactions, their allostatic load may be kept low, and their adrenocortical levels would be similar to or lower than dominant breeders. Examples of the latter scenario include species where dominance over breeding resources is inherited, achieved via a queuing convention or when animals die or disappear. However, when the access to and control of resources differs substantially between dominants and subordinates and dominance status is mainly acquired through overt aggression, subordinates may experience higher allostatic load and thus higher sensitivity to stress compared with breeders (Goymann and Wingfield 2004). The latter competitive scenario is consistent with our study model, where dominant breeders occupy and defend exclusive territories and floaters attempt violent territory takeovers, which sometimes succeed (Sergio et al. 2007a, 2009). Territorial confrontations can be extremely violent, with birds locking talons in the air, falling dangerously on the ground while battling and losing up to several flight feathers (Sergio et al. 2007a; see Fig. S1 in Sergio et al. 2011). Territory trespassing can be frequent, especially in preincubation, and is also aimed at stealing food originally caught by the owners. Dominant breeders signal their social status and fighting ability through bodily and nonbodily signals (i.e., degree of nest decoration; Sergio et al. 2011) and benefit from signaling by lower rates of attacks on trespassers and less time spent in aggressive interactions. As a consequence, we may expect a relatively higher allostatic load in subordinate floaters than in dominant breeders, and we predict higher levels of stress hormones and higher HPA-axis responsiveness to stressors in subordinate nonbreeders (Tarlow et al. 2001; Øverli et al. 2004; Oyegbile and Marler 2006; Sorato and Kotrschal 2006). The physiological response to stress can be assessed in wild birds by means of determining the circulating levels of corticosterone in blood samples collected under controlled conditions (see, e.g., Wingfield 1994). According to the hypothesis of social subordination, we would expect a higher elevation of circulating corticosterone in nonbreeders than in breeding birds in response to standardized stressful events such as the “capture and restraint” protocol. Methods Study Model and Behavioral Observations Black kites are medium-sized socially monogamous migratory raptors that are present on their European breeding grounds from early March to late August (Cramp and Simmons 1980). In the study area (Doñ ana National Park, southwest Spain), the black kite population is composed of ca. 500 breeding pairs plus 400–500 nonbreeding individuals (Sergio et al. 2005; Blas et al. 2009). Since 1965, this population has been subjected to scientific monitoring, and from 1986 onward, nestlings have been regularly banded with alphanumerically coded rings, which can be read with spotting scopes without disturbing the birds. Local longevity records show birds up to 25 yr old, and deferred reproduction can last up to 7 yr, with an average age of first breeding of 3.5 yr for both males and females (Blas et al. 2009). Natal dispersal distances are very short (median distance p 4.8 km), and extensive surveys indicate the absence of emigration to other populations (Forero et al. 2002). After first breeding, kites tend to maintain the same territory and mate (Forero et al. 1999). Marked territory holders were located on an annual basis from 1992 by means of continuous field surveys from early March to late June. When a banded bird was detected in a territory, the area was visited several times to locate its nest and check potential mate changes. Rather than defending exclusive home ranges, floaters are gregarious, forage in unde- fended communal areas, and gather at night at six communal ically immobilized as described above. The exact age was known roosts. Monitoring of the communal roosts twice a week al- in 49 birds and ranged from 1 to 13 yr. lowed identification of floaters (details in Sergio et al. 2009). At the end of the breeding season, the intensive monitoring of the population allowed us to classify the GnRH-treated birds as either breeders (N p 26) or floaters (i.e., nonbreeders lacking Bird Captures previous reproductive experience; N p 28). Within the GnRH group, only two males lacked accurate observations to deterBlack kites were captured using cannon nets at the start of the mine their status and thus were only used to assess overall breeding season during March and April 1998. According to gonadal response as compared with control birds. previous studies that reported seasonal changes in sex-steroid levels, circulating testosterone (in males) and estradiol (in females) are elevated during this time in both breeders (Blas et Assessment of the Adrenocortical Response to Stress al. 2010) and floaters (Blas and Hiraldo 2010). By restricting Glucocorticosteroid levels in birds rapidly elevate following bird captures to this part of the season, we covered the fertile capture and restraint (e.g., Wingfield 1994; Baos et al. 2006). period of most breeding pairs in the population, allowing comSecretion patterns are typically monitored in blood samples parisons with nonbreeders at a time of the year when their collected at different times following capture, normally within reproductive axes would be expected to be upregulated. Because a range of 30–45 min. This can be done by means of performing several kites were typically trapped at the same time, to avoid a stress series, where each individual is repeatedly blood saminjury or suffering, all the individuals were first removed from pled at several fixed times postcapture (e.g., 10, 30, and 45 min; the net, physically immobilized with cloth corsets made speBlas et al. 2007). Alternatively, the time-related pattern of glucifically for this purpose, and maintained with their sight decocorticosteroid change can be assessed at the group level by prived by means of falconry hoods. We then checked our means of collecting a single blood sample per individual. Rather records, and based on the previous history, behavioral obserthan each individual being sampled at fixed times, the indivations accumulated before capture, and sex determination viduals are sampled once along a continuum that ranges, for through molecular analyses (see below), individuals were classexample, 30 or 45 min following capture (e.g., Mays et al. 1991; ified into either (1) an experimental group for GnRH challenge Sockman and Schwabl 2001; Wada et al. 2009). The latter proor for assessment of stress levels (see below), or (2) a nonexcedure is typically the method of choice when logistic conperimental group that was subjected to banding and/or restraints do not allow within-individual handling and restraint cording of biometric data before release. These activities were series. Examples of such scenarios include studies where the performed by two separate teams of 2–3 people working conindividuals are too small to be repeatedly bled (e.g., Wada et currently with the aim of speeding up the process and reducing al. 2009), when many individuals are captured simultaneously the time to release the birds. Our field methods did not cause (e.g., Sockman and Schwabl 2001), or when the lapse time from casualties or damage to the birds, all the sampling protocols capture to blood sampling is logistically difficult to control were performed according to Spanish laws, and we prioritized because of the capture methods employed (e.g., Mays et al. ethical considerations over scientific goals. 1991). The nature of our study settings, where groups of black kites (rather than individual birds) are generally captured at the exact same time, led us to choose the latter method (i.e., GnRH Challenges one sample per individual). Thus, we performed a continuum For experimental birds (31 males and 40 females), a preinjec- sampling of different individuals within a given interval of time tion blood sample was collected from the brachial vein into a that ranged from 1 to 45 min postcapture. In order to stanheparinized tube (average time from trapping to blood sam- dardize the handling and restraint procedures, all the birds were pling: 17.1 ± 9.9 min). Kites were then challenged with an in- maintained with their sight deprived and physically immobitravenous injection of either chicken gonadotropin-hormone lized from capture to blood sampling. The selection of indireleasing hormone-I (c-GnRH-I; Sigma-Aldrich L0637; N p viduals (51 birds; 22 males and 29 females) was done based on 56 birds) or a control saline solution (0.9% NaCl; N p 15 the criteria explained above (i.e., previous history of records, birds). All the injections were performed within 5 min following behavioral observations accumulated until the time of capture, collection of the first blood sample; otherwise, the individuals and sex determination through molecular analyses) and to obwere discarded from the experiment. Experimental injections tain a well-distributed sample within the previously defined were prepared from a stock solution of 200 mg GnRH/mL saline, interval of 45 min. Some of these samples, but not all, correwhich was preserved in frozen aliquots until the moment of sponded to specimens used in the GnRH experiment. In the capture. The selected dose was 20 mg GnRH/kg body mass latter case, only preinjection samples were used for corticostefollowing Williams and Sharp (1978) or the same mass-cor- rone analyses, with the aim of avoiding a potentially confoundrected volume of saline (i.e., 100 mL/kg body mass). Following ing effect that the injections could exert on glucocorticoid seinjections, we collected three blood samples at fixed times (i.e., cretion. The behavioral observations allowed us to classify 10, 30, and 60 min postchallenge). Between sampling episodes, individuals as either breeders (N p 30) or floaters (N p 21) at kites were maintained with their sight deprived and were phys- the end of the breeding season. Blood Processing, Molecular Sexing, and Hormone Analyses Blood samples were maintained in coolers and centrifuged the same day for separation of plasma, which was stored frozen (—80°C) until making determinations of hormone levels (see below). Because male and female black kites look alike, when sex was not known, the cellular fraction was preserved in ethanol and subjected to molecular analyses for sex determination (Ellegren 1996). Plasma steroids concentrations were determined through radioimmunoassay (RIA) at the University of Washington (Seattle) following a slight modification in the method described in Wingfield and Farner (1975) and Wingfield et al. (1991; i.e., samples were assayed without column chromatography; see Hau et al. 2000). Because the testosterone antibody used in the RIA displayed 60% cross-reactivity with 5-alpha-DHT and 6% cross-reactivity with 5-beta-DHT, all further references will be to total plasma androgen levels. For statistical purposes, nondetectable samples were assumed to have steroid concentrations equal to the detection limits of the RIAs (i.e., 100 pg/mL androgens, 60 pg/mL estradiol, and 1 ng/mL corticosterone). Intra- and interassay coefficients of variation were below 4%, 19%, and 3% for androgens, estradiol, and corticosterone RIAs, respectively. Statistical Comparisons Endocrine responses to GnRH challenges were analyzed through generalized linear mixed models (GLMM) using the PROC GLIMMIX package of SAS (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). This procedure allowed us to incorporate bird identity as a random factor in the models and thus control the potential pseudoreplication associated with the use of several observations from the same individuals. The GLMMs followed a normal distribution of errors and identity link functions and were constructed using a backward selection procedure (McCullagh and Nelder 1989; Crawley 1993). From initial saturated models containing as dependent variables the time postinjection (min), treatment (experimental vs. control), status (breeder vs. floater), and their interactions, the least-significant terms were sequentially removed until obtaining a minimum adequate model that only retained significant effects at 15% rejection probability. Because the time from capture to initial blood sampling (i.e., “delay time”) can potentially modify sex-steroid levels, we tested this effect in a subset of experimental birds whose exact preinjection processing times were recorded in the field (i.e., 21 of 31 males and 17 of 23 females for androgens and estradiol, respectively). The effects of delay time on preinjection sex-steroid levels were tested by means of Pearson’s correlations, and the effects on postinjection levels were tested by incorporating this variable in the final GLMM models. In addition to the tests above, one-way ANOVAs were performed to test for differences in (a) preinjection titer, (b) maximum postinjection levels, (c) absolute difference between preinjection sex-steroid levels and the maximum values recorded postchallenge, and (d) secretion rate. The latter variable was calculated as in Schoech et al. (1997) following the formula (C ma x — C 0 )/T, where Cmax is the maximum steroid value recorded postchallenge, C0 is the baseline level, and T is the time in minutes to reach maximum titer. The adrenocortical responses to stress were analyzed through general linear models following the same stepwise backward procedure explained above. Initial saturated models considered the effects of handling time, status, body condition, and the number of previous captures (ranging 0–2). After obtaining a minimum adequate model, age was incorporated as an additional dependent term, and its effects were tested in the subsample of birds whose exact age was known (N p 15 and N p 22 for males and females, respectively). Because temporal changes in corticosterone concentrations are expected to follow Figure 1. Male response to GnRH treatment. A, Circulating levels of androgens in breeding (white square, N p 13 ) and floating (black square, N p 16) males before experimental manipulation. B, Androgen levels at 10, 30, and 60 min following injection of either c-GnRH-I (circles, N p 23) or a control saline solution (N p 6). White and black circles represent experimental breeders (N p 10 ) and floaters (N p 13), respectively. Note that all the control birds have been merged into a single postinjection group for clarity of representation. Bars represent means ± 1 SE. Figure 2. Circulating preinjection levels of total androgens in male black kites as a function of the time from capture to blood sampling. a curvilinear response, the recorded time in minutes was log transformed in order to allow linear fitting in the regression analyses. To estimate body condition, for each sex we used the residuals of a regression of mass on body size (hereafter “mass residuals”). Because univariate metrics have been criticized as measures of body size (Freeman and Jackson 1990; McDonald et al. 2005), we estimated size by means of the first axis of a principal components analysis built using tarsus, wing, and tail length (Sergio et al. 2007b, 2011). Results Before experimental manipulation, androgen levels in males were unrelated to status (F1, 27 p 1.81, P p 0.19; Fig. 1A) and independent of the time from capture to blood sampling (r p 0.20, P p 0.39; Fig. 2). The GLMM (Table 1) revealed a significant interaction between time postinjection and treatment that lead us to analyze each experimental group separately. GnRH-treated males showed a pronounced and significant time-related elevation of circulating androgens that was independent of status (GLMM: time: F1, 41 p 36.35, P ! 0.01; status: F1, 41 p 0.45, P p 0.50; time # status: F1, 40 p 0.33, P p 0.56; Fig. 1B). Androgens reached peak levels 60 min following injection, when average titers were more than fourfold higher than pretreatment values (i.e., 320.9 ± 92.2 pg/mL vs. 1416.2 ± 219.7 pg/mL). Saline-injected control males, on the contrary, showed a moderate time-related decrease in circulating androgen levels (GLMM: time: F1, 11 p 4.96, P p 0.04; status: F1, 11 p 0.29, P p 0.59; time # status: F1, 10 p 0.05, P p 0.82; Fig. 1B). The incorporation of delay time (i.e., time from capture to blood sampling) as an additional dependent variable to the models above did not change the results, and its effect was nonsignificant in all cases (P 1 0.24). Maximum titer, absolute increase, and secretion rate did not differ between breeding and floating males (all P ≥ 0.45). In addition, none of the latter three variables were correlated with male age (all P ≥ 0.61). With regard to females, estradiol levels before experimental manipulations ranged 60–100 pg/mL and were unrelated to status (F1, 21 p 0.24, P p 0.63; Fig. 3A) and independent of the time from capture to blood sampling (r p 0.28; P p 0.27). Following GnRH treatment, some breeding individuals elevated estradiol levels up to 310 pg/mL, increasing the variability in circulating titers (range 60–310) in relation to nonbreeders and control birds (see error bars in Fig. 3). However, overall estradiol levels in GnRH-treated females were statistically similar to controls, and there was no effect of time postinjection, status, or any of the interactions we considered (Table 2; Fig. 3B). The time from capture to blood sampling had no effect on postinjection estradiol levels (GLMM: F1, 20 p 0.45, P p 0.51). Restricting the analysis to experimental GnRH-injected females, the GLMM did not detect significant differences in estradiol levels between pre- and postinjection samples (F1, 45 p 1.48, P p 0.22). Peak estradiol levels, absolute increase, and relative increase were not related to reproductive status or associated with female age (all P 1 0.05). The absence of a clear estrogenic response to treatment with c-GnRH-1 led us to analyze plasma androgen levels in a second set of experimental samples corresponding to 17 additional females. The concentration of total androgens, however, was below detection limits in all cases. Circulating corticosterone levels in male black kites were not affected by the number of previous captures (F1, 16 p 0.243, P p 0.63) but showed a significant positive association with handling time (log time: F1, 17 p 35.85, P ! 0.01), a negative association with mass residuals (F1, 17 p 6.01, P p 0.02), and a significant effect of status (F1, 17 p 9.03, P ! 0.01). The latter two significant effects were independent of handling time (log time # mass residuals: F1, 16 p 0.35, P p 0.56; log time # status: F1, 15 p 0.01, P p 0.95). Corticosterone levels were higher in male floaters than in breeders, and both groups showed a parallel time-course pattern of the response to stress (Fig. 4A). When the final model was tested in the subsample of males whose age was known (N p 15), age was unable to explain corticosterone levels (F1, 10 p 1.70, P p 0.22), while the effects of time, status, and mass residuals remained significant. In females, neither mass residuals nor the number of previous Table 1: Results from the mixed linear model analyzing circulating changes in androgen levels of male black kites after injection of either c-GnRH-I or a control saline solution Effect Test Value P Time* Treatment* Status Time # treatment* Time # status Treatment # status Time # treatment # status F1, 52 F1, 52 F1, 52 F1, 52 F1, 51 F1, 51 F1, 50 !.01 p p p p p p p 9.33 .01 .40 11.97 .32 .17 .08 .94 .53 !.01 .57 .68 .77 Note. Test values and associated probabilities are shown for the terms retained by the final model (indicated with an asterisk) and the nonsignificant effects when excluded during the backward procedure. Figure 3. Female response to GnRH treatment. A, Circulating levels of estradiol in breeding (white square, N p 12 ) and floating (black square, N p 11) females before experimental manipulation. B, Estradiol levels at 10, 30, and 60 min following injection of either c-GnRH-I (circles, N p 19) or a control saline solution (N p 4). White and black circles represent experimental breeders (N p 10) and floaters (N p 9), respectively. Note that all the control birds have been merged into a single postinjection group for clarity of representation. Bars represent means ± 1 SE. results indicate that the pituitary and gonads of male kites positively responded to our chemical challenge. More important for this study, the absolute elevation of androgen levels and the time-course pattern of gonadal response were similar in floating and breeding males, and all of the response parameters that we considered were unrelated to reproductive status or male age. Overall, these results suggest that the reproductive axes of floating males are functionally similar to breeding individuals contrary to the hypothesis of sexual immaturity and consistent with the results in Blas and Hiraldo (2010). In the latter nonexperimental study, circulating levels of testosterone were monitored in breeding and floating male kites throughout the reproductive season, and both groups showed parallel temDiscussion poral changes and statistically similar absolute levels regardless Our experimental treatment with exogenous c-GnRH-I elicited of age and breeding status. Taken together, the results from a marked increase in circulating androgens in male black kites experimental and nonexperimental tests, the reproductive systhat was not observed in control saline-injected males. These tem of floating male kites, similarly to that of breeders, seems to be able to respond not only to long-term naturally occurring Table 2: Results from the mixed linear model environmental signals such as photoperiod but also to shortanalyzing circulating changes in estradiol levels of term hormonal challenges such as those simulated here through female black kites after injection of either c-GnRH-I treatment. The ability to trigger short-term fast elc-GnRH-I or a control saline solution evations of circulating androgens can be particularly important in a context of male-male competition. During periods of social Effect Test Value P instability, when males compete for access to territories and Time F1, 32 p 1.74 .20 mates, testosterone can increase relatively rapidly over the Treatment .45 F1, 32 p .58 short-term, thereby facilitating aggression in intrasexual disStatus .52 F1, 32 p .42 putes (i.e., challenge hypothesis: Wingfield et al. 1987; HirTime # treatment .46 F1, 30 p .56 schenhauser and Oliveira 2006). Such endocrine responses Time # status F1, 31 p 1.38 .25 would be particularly relevant for floating black kites because Treatment # status .80 F1, 30 p .06 they often perform territorial intrusions to challenge breeding Time # treatment # status .57 F1, 29 p .33 conspecifics (Sergio et al. 2011), reinforcing the hypothesis that Note. Test values and associated probabilities are shown for all floaters are behavioral and physiologically prepared for social the terms when excluded during the backward procedure. captures affected corticosterone levels (mass residuals: F1, 25 p 0.17, P p 0.68; previous captures: F1, 24 p 0.247; P p 0.62), but handling time and status exerted significant effects (log time: F1, 26 p 15.63, P ! 0.01; status: F1, 26 p 4.06, P p 0.05). Breeding and floating females showed the same time-course pattern of response to stress (log time # status: F1, 25 p 0.45, P p 0.50), but contrary to males, corticosterone levels were statistically lower in floaters than in breeders (Fig. 4B). When the final model was tested in the subsample of females whose precise age was known (N p 22), age was unable to explain corticosterone levels (F1, 18 p 0.88, P p 0.77), while the effects of time and status remained significant. Figure 4. Adrenocortical responses to stress in male and female black kites. Changes in plasma corticosterone titers throughout the 45-min protocol of capture and restraint (time in log scale) in male (A) and female (B) black kites. Black circles represent floaters and white circles represent breeders. Solid and dashed lines represent predicted corticosterone levels from linear regression models as a function of log time for floating and breeding birds, respectively. competition and that their status is not constrained by maturation of the HPG axis. With regard to female kites, previous studies reported similar patterns of estradiol secretion throughout the reproductive season in breeding and floating birds but lower absolute estradiol levels in the latter group (Blas and Hiraldo 2010). These results suggested that floating females responded to environmental cues and attained some degree of gonadal recrudescence. However, they also suggested that full ovarian development would only be attained on exposure to additional reproductive cues such as intersexual stimulation (e.g., Péczely and Pethes 1979; Setiawan et al. 2007). Contrary to our expectations, treatment of females with exogenous c-GnRH-I did not elicit an overall positive response in terms of increased estradiol or androgen levels. This could be explained by the inability of c-GnRH-I to stimulate the production of gonadotropins in the hypophysis and/or by the lack of gonadal response to gonadotropins; a proper discrimination of hypotheses would require further research. However, the fact that c-GnRH-I injections elicited a strong gonadal response in males may suggest sexual differences in the underlying GnRH control system. Such sexual differences could involve c-GnRH-II, a different form of GnRH present in birds. Although the function of c-GnRH-II is currently unclear (Maney et al. 1997; Stevenson et al. 2008), it shows seasonal and age-related changes similar to c-GnRH-I, and both forms of hormone are potentially involved in the regulation of the HPG axis (Stevenson and MacDougall-Shackleton 2005; but see Meddle et al. 2006). Studies in passerine birds indicate that c-GnRH-II is regulated by the social context and affects female sexual behavior (Maney et al. 1997; Stevenson et al. 2008). The latter findings and the lack of response to c-GnRHI we reported in female kites may suggest that c-GnRH-II is a good candidate hormone for the regulation of breeding and floating status that deserves future investigation. The adrenocortical response to stress of male black kites showed a significant effect of status, with floaters displaying higher corticosterone titers than breeders and a negative effect of mass residuals. The latter finding is widespread among field endocrinology studies (e.g., Kitaysky et al. 2001, 2003) and consistent with the role of corticosterone in the mobilization of endogenous energy stores aimed at coping with food shortages and improving survival (reviewed in Sapolsky et al. 2000). With regard to the effects of status, the male pattern was consistent with the hypothesis of social subordination. Floaters were expected to show higher adrenocortical function if they tend to lose contests with dominant breeding conspecifics (e.g., Tarlow et al. 2001; Øverli et al. 2004; Oyegbile and Marler 2006; Sorato and Kotrschal 2006). Among black kites, territorial disputes are frequent and often involve physical fighting (Sergio et al. 2007a, 2011). Age and previous site dominance constitute the major determinants for winning a contest (Sergio et al. 2007a), and therefore floaters—which are smaller, younger, arrive later from migration, and have no experience in territorial defense (Sergio at al. 2009)—tend to be evicted by experienced breeders. Contrary to males, floating females showed a lower stress response than breeders. An explanation consistent with the hypothesis of social subordination may rely on the different roles that males and females play during the processes of territory acquisition and defense. In socially monogamous migratory avian species, disputes over territories are predominantly undertaken by males, which tend to arrive earlier and settle in breeding territories before the arrival of females (Andersson 1994). Females, nonetheless, may also gain from getting involved in territorial defense when they are mated, especially in species establishing long-term pair bonds such as black kites (Forero et al. 1999). Following this reasoning, paired (i.e., breeding) females would be involved in social conflicts more often than single (i.e., floating) females, explaining why the stress response is more pronounced in the former group. This interpretation would support the arguments in Blas and Hiraldo (2010) proposed to explain the evolutionary basis for sexual differences in reproductive maturation in birds. Floating males would benefit from a fully functional reproductive system (as our results suggest) because this facilitates access to breeding resources (e.g., increasing the chances of winning intrasexual competition through testosterone secretion, which triggers aggressive behavior according to the challenge hypothesis; Wingfield et al. 1987; Hirschenhauser and Oliveira 2006). However, a floating male actively searching for a territory would not only have a higher chance of losing contests (because of younger age, less experience, and later arrival dates) but would also be more exposed to social conflicts than a particular resident male defending its breeding ground. This would explain the higher response to stress in floating than in breeding males. Floating females on the other hand may delay maturation until establishing pair bonds with a territorial male (Blas and Hiraldo 2010). A more passive role in territorial disputes would imply less exposure to social conflicts, explaining their lower response to stress than breeding females. Several additional hypotheses (1–3 below) may also predict differences in adrenocortical function between breeders and floaters regardless of social conflicts, but our results did not fully meet the predictions of any of them. For example, (1) glucocorticoids can act as mediators of ontogenetic transitions (Wada 2008). This hypothesis posits that baseline and stressinduced corticosterone endogenously elevates to facilitate the transition between two life-history stages. An increased HPAaxis sensibility in floaters could thus facilitate the transition from nonbreeding to breeding status similar to that which occurs during the transition from nestling to fledging in some avian species (Schwabl 1999; Sockman and Schwabl 2001; Wada et al. 2008; but see Blas et al. 2006; Romero et al. 2006). Although this hypothesis may contribute to explaining the elevated response to stress of nonbreeding males, it does not explain the reported sexual differences: floating males and females are in the same life-history transition but show opposite patterns of corticosterone release compared with breeders. A second hypothesis (2) posits that breeders downmodulate the stress response according to their reproductive investment (Heidinger et al. 2006, 2008; Lendvai et al. 2007). Reduced corticosterone secretion helps to avoid diversion of critical resources away from reproduction. This argument explains, for example, why breeding passerines respond less strongly to a stressor when their clutches are experimentally enlarged (Lendvai et al. 2007) and why the stress response declines with age in breeding seabirds (Heidinger et al. 2006, 2008). Our results, however, do not fully support this hypothesis because breeding females showed a stronger (rather than a weaker) adrenocortical response than floaters, and age did not explain differences in corticosterone levels in either sex. A third hypothesis (3) is that breeders and floaters differ in their response to stress as a consequence of differential habituation to humans. Several studies have shown that repeated exposure to direct human presence and/or repeated handling of the same individuals can result in downmodulation of the response to stress (Love at al. 2003; Walker et al. 2006). Depending on the species and study settings, such an effect may confound results and be a matter of concern. Our research was performed on wild medium-sized raptors living in their natural habitat and subjected to little human pressure, typically showing long flight initiation distances (J. Blas, F. Sergio, and F. Hiraldo, unpublished observations) and seldom captured during their generally long life spans. If black kites downmodulate the stress response as a consequence of habituation to human presence, we would expect corticosterone levels to decrease with age; if they habituate to human handling, we would expect a negative effect of the number of previous captures. Neither age nor the number of previous captures affected the response to stress, and therefore, there is no evidence that habituation occurs in our study model. Despite the fact that other physiological parameters not considered in this study (e.g., clearance rates of circulating corticosterone, concentration of binding globulins, number of receptors) may also play a role in the described patterns, our results concur with a large body of evidence indicating that social competition affects the response to stress. Also important, although our data do not fully meet the predictions of hypotheses 1–2 above, we cannot discard their potential contribution because we are not testing mutually exclusive hypotheses. All of the proposed effects may thus partially underlie the sex and status patterns of response to stress, but such patterns are fully consistent with the hypothesis of social subordination in black kites. Acknowledgments We thank G. Garcı́a, S. Cabezas, R. Baos, A. Sánchez, M. Guerrero, F. J. Vilches, and L. Hillströ m for help in the field; G. Garcı́a, L. Erckman, M. Hau, M. Wikelski, S. Kitaysky, and A. Godoy for providing help and advice during laboratory analyses; S. Young for revising the text; and two anonymous reviewers for comments and suggestions that improved earlier drafts. Funding was provided through the Spanish Ministerio de Educación y Cultura and by the research projects PB960834 of the Direcció n General de Investigació n Cientı́fica y Tecnoló gica, CGL2008-01781/BOS of the Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovació n, JA-58 of the Consejerı́a de Medio Ambiente de la Junta de Andalucı́a, and by Excellence Projects RNM 1790 and RNM 03822 of the Junta de Andalucı́a. J.C.W. acknowledges grant IBN-9905679 from the National Science Foundation, and J.B. acknowledges a “Ramon y Cajal” contract from the Spanish government. Literature Cited Andersson M. 1994. Sexual Selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Baos R., J. Blas, G.R. Bortolotti, T.A. Marchant, and F. Hiraldo. 2006. Adrenocortical response to stress and thyroid hormone status in free-living nestling white storks (Ciconia ciconia) exposed to heavy metal and arsenic contamination. Environ Health Perspect 114:1497–1501. Blas J., R. Baos, G.R. Bortolotti, T.A. Marchant, and F. Hiraldo. 2006. Age-related variation in the adrenocortical response to stress in nestling white storks (Ciconia ciconia) supports the developmental hypothesis. Gen Comp Endocrinol 148:172– 180. Blas J., G.R. Bortolotti, J.L. Tella, R. Baos, and T.A. Marchant. 2007. Stress response during development predicts fitness in a wild, long-lived vertebrate. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104: 8880–8884. Blas J., and F. Hiraldo. 2010. Proximate and ultimate factors explaining deferred breeding in long-lived birds. Horm Behav 57:169–176. Blas J., L. Ló pez, A. Tanferna, F. Sergio, and F. Hiraldo. 2010. Reproductive endocrinology of wild, long-lived raptors. Gen Comp Endocrinol 168:22–28. Blas J., F. Sergio, and F. Hiraldo. 2009. Age-related improvement in reproductive performance in a long-lived raptor: a crosssectional and longitudinal study. Ecography 32:647–657. Cramp S. and K.E.L. Simmons. 1980. The Birds of the Western Palearctic. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Crawley M.J. 1993. GLIM for Ecologists. Blackwell Scientific, Oxford. Creel S. 2001. Social dominance and stress hormones. Trends Ecol Evol 16:491–497. Creel S., N.M. Creel, and S.L. Monfort. 1996. Social stress and dominance. Nature 379:212. Ellegren H. 1996. First gene on the avian W chromosome (CHD) provides a tag for universal sexing of non-ratite birds. Proc R Soc B 263:1635–1641. Emlem S.T. 1982. The evolution of helping. I. An ecological constraint model. Am Nat 119:29–39. Ferrer M. and F. Hiraldo. 1991. Evaluation of management techniques for the Spanish imperial eagle. Wildl Soc Bull 19: 436–442. Forero M.G., J.A. Donázar, J. Blas, and F. Hiraldo. 1999. Causes and consequences of territory change and breeding dispersal distance in the black kite. Ecology 80:1298–1310. Forero M.G., J.A. Doná zar, and F. Hiraldo. 2002. Causes and fitness consequences of natal dispersal in a population of black kites. Ecology 83:858–872. Frederiksen M. and T. Bregnballe. 2001. Conspecific reproductive success affects age of recruitment in a great cormorant, Phalacrocorax carbo sinensis, colony. Proc R Soc B 268:1519–1526. Freeman S. and W.M. Jackson. 1990. Univariate metrics are not adequate to measure avian body size. Auk 107:69–74. Goymann W. and J.C. Wingfield. 2004. Allostatic load, social status and stress hormones: the costs of social status matter. Anim Behav 67:591–602. Hall M.R., E. Gwinner, and M. Bloesch. 1987. Annual cycles in moult, body mass, luteinizing hormone, prolactin and gonadal steroids during the development of sexual maturity in the white stork (Ciconia ciconia). J Zool (Lond) 211:467– 486. Hau M., M. Wikelski, K.K. Soma, and J.C. Wingfield. 2000. Testosterone and year-round territorial aggression in a tropical bird. Gen Comp Endocrinol 117:20–33. Hector J.A.L., S.P.C. Pickering, J.C. Croxall, and B.K. Follett. 1990. The endocrine basis of deferred sexual maturity in the wandering albatross, Diomedea exulans. Funct Ecol 4:59–66. Heidinger B.J., I.C.T. Nisbet, and E.D. Ketterson. 2006. Older parents are less responsive to a stressor in a long-lived seabird: a mechanism for increased reproductive performance with age? Proc R Soc B 273:2227–2231. ———. 2008. Changes in adrenal capacity contribute to a decline in the stress response with age in a long-lived seabird. Gen Comp Endocrinol 156:564–568. Hirschenhauser K. and R.F. Oliveira. 2006. Social modulation of androgens in male vertebrates: meta-analyses of the challenge hypothesis. Anim Behav 71:265–277. Kitaysky A.S., E.V. Kitaiskaia, J.F. Piatt, and J.C. Wingfield. 2003. Benefits and costs of increased levels of corticosterone in seabird chicks. Horm Behav 43:140–149. Kitaysky A.S., E.V. Kitaiskaia, J.C. Wingfield, and J.F. Piatt. 2001. Dietary restriction causes chronic elevation of corticosterone and enhances stress response in red-legged kittiwake chicks. J Comp Physiol B 171:701–709. Kokko H. and W.J. Sutherland. 1998. Optimal foraging and queuing strategies: consequences for density dependence and habitat loss. Am Nat 152:354–366. Lendvai A.Z., M. Giraudeau, and O. Chastel. 2007. Reproduction and modulation of the stress response: an experimental test in the house sparrow. Proc R Soc B 274:391–397. Love O.P., L.J. Shutt, J.S. Silfies, and D.M. Bird. 2003. Repeated restraint and sampling results in reduced corticosterone levels in developing and adult captive American kestrels (Falco sparverius). Physiol Biochem Zool 76:753–761. Maney D.L., R.D. Richardson, and J.C. Wingfield. 1997. Central administration of chicken gonadotropin-releasing hormoneII enhances courtship behavior in a female sparrow. Horm Behav 32:11–18. Mays N.A., C.M. Vleck, and J. Dawson. 1991. Plasma luteinizing hormone, steroid hormones, behavioral role, and nest stage in cooperatively breeding Harris’ hawks (Parabuteo unicinctus). Auk 108:619–637. McCullagh P. and J.A. Nelder. 1989. Generalized Linear Models. 2nd ed. Chapman & Hall, New York. McDonald P.G., P.D. Olsen, and A. Cockburn. 2005. Selection on body size in a raptor with pronounced reversed sexual size dimorphism: are bigger females better? Behav Ecol 16: 48–56. McEwen B.S. and J.C. Wingfield. 2003. The concept of allostasis in biology and biomedicine. Horm Behav 43:2–15. Meddle S.L., S. Bush, P.J. Sharp, R.P. Millar, and J.C. Wingfield. 2006. Hypothalamic pro-GnRH-GAP, GnRH-I and GnRHII during the onset of photorefractoriness in the whitecrowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys gambelii). J Neuroendocrinol 18:217–226. Moore I.T., N. Perfito, H. Wada, T.S. Sperry, and J.C. Wingfield. 2002. Latitudinal variation in plasma testosterone levels in birds of the genus Zonotrichia. Gen Comp Endocrinol 129: 13–19. Newton I. 1992. Experiments on limitation of bird numbers by territorial behaviour. Biol Rev 67:129–173. Øverli O., W.J. Korzan, E. Hoglund, S. Winberg, H. Bollig, M. Watt, G.L. Forster, et al. 2004. Stress coping style predicts aggression and social dominance in rainbow trout. Horm Behav 45:235–241. Oyegbile T.O. and C.A. Marler. 2006. Weak winner effect in a less aggressive mammal: correlations with corticosterone but not testosterone. Physiol Behav 89:171–179. Péczely P. and G. Pethes. 1979. Alterations in plasma steroid concentrations in the collared dove (Streptopelia decaocto) during the sexual maturation and reproduction cycle. Acta Physiol Acad Sci Hung 54:161–170. Penteriani V., F. Otalora, F. Sergio, and M. Ferrer. 2005. Environmental stochasticity in dispersal areas can explain the “mysterious” disappearance of breeding populations. Proc R Soc B 272:1265–1269. Rohner C. 1997. Non-territorial “floaters” in great horned owls: space use during a cyclic peak of snowshoe hares. Anim Behav 53:901–912. Rohwer S. and J.C. Wingfield. 1981. A field study of social dominance, plasma levels of luteinizing hormone and steroid hormones in wintering Harris’ sparrows. Z Tierpsychol 57: 173–183. Romero L.M., D.W. Holt, M. Maples, and J.C. Wingfield. 2006. Corticosterone is not correlated with nest departure in snowy owl chicks (Nyctea scandiaca). Gen Comp Endocrinol 149: 119–123. Sapolsky R.M., L.M. Romero, and A.U. Muck. 2000. How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? integrative, permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocr Rev 21:55–89. Schoech S.J., R.L. Mumme, and J.C. Wingfield. 1997. Corticosterone, reproductive status, and body mass in a cooperative breeder, the Florida scrub-jay (Aphelocoma coerulescens). Physiol Zool 70:68–73. Schwabl H. 1999. Developmental changes and among-sibling variation of corticosterone levels in an altricial avian species. Gen Comp Endocrinol 116:403–408. Sergio F., J. Blas, G. Blanco, A. Tanferna, L. Ló pez, J.A. Lemus, and F. Hiraldo. 2011. Raptor nest decorations are a reliable threat against conspecifics. Science 331:327–330. Sergio F., J. Blas, M.G. Forero, J.A. Donázar, and F. Hiraldo. 2007a. Sequential settlement and site-dependence in a migratory raptor. Behav Ecol 18:811–821. ———. 2007b. Size-related advantages for reproduction in a slightly dimorphic raptor: opposite trends between the sexes. Ethology 113:1141–1150. Sergio F., J. Blas, M.G. Forero, N. Fernández, J.A. Donazar, and F. Hiraldo. 2005. Preservation of wide-ranging top predators by site-protection: black and red kites in Doñ ana National Park. Biol Conserv 125:11–21. Sergio F., J. Blas, and F. Hiraldo. 2009. Predictors of floater status in a long-lived, migratory bird: a cross-sectional and longitudinal test of hypotheses. J Anim Ecol 78:109–118. Setiawan A.N., L.S. Davis, J.T. Darby, P.M. Lokman, G. Young, M.A. Blackberry, B.L. Cannell, and G.B. Martin. 2007. Effects of artificial social stimuli on the reproductive schedule and hormone levels of yellow-eyed penguins (Megadyptes antipodes). Horm Behav 51:46–53. Smith S.M. 1978. The “underworld” in a territorial sparrow: adaptive strategy for floaters. Am Nat 112:571–582. Sockman K.W. and H. Schwabl. 2001. Plasma corticosterone in nestling American kestrels: effects of age, handling stress, yolk androgens, and body condition. Gen Comp Endocrinol 122:205–212. Sorato E. and K. Kotrschal. 2006. Hormonal and behavioural symmetries between the sexes in the northern bald ibis. Gen Comp Endocrinol 146:265–274. Stevenson, T.J., J.E. Bentley, T. Ubuka, L. Arckens, E. Hampson, and S.A. MacDougall-Shackleton. 2008. Effects of social cues on GnRH-I, GnRH-II, and reproductive physiology in female house sparrows (Passer domesticus). Gen Comp Endocrinol 156:385–394. Stevenson T.J. and S.A. MacDougall-Shackleton. 2005. Seasonand age-related variation in neural cGnRH-I and cGnRH-II immunoreactivity in house sparrows (Passer domesticus). Gen Comp Endocrinol 143:33–39. Studd M.V. and R.J. Robertson. 1985. Life span, competition, and delayed plumage maturation in male passerines: the breeding threshold hypothesis. Am Nat 126:101–115. Tarlow E.M., M. Wikelski, and D.J. Anderson. 2001. Hormonal correlates of siblicide in Galá pagos Nazca boobies. Horm Behav 40:14–20. von Holst D. 1998. The concept of stress and its relevance for animal behavior. Adv Stud Behav 27:1–131. Wada H. 2008. Glucocorticoids: mediators of vertebrate ontogenetic transitions. Gen Comp Endocrinol 156:441–453. Wada H., K.G. Salvante, C. Stables, E. Wagner, T.D. Williams, and C.W. Breuner. 2008. Adrenocortical responses in zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata): individual variation, repeatability, and relationship to phenotypic quality. Horm Behav 53:472–480. Wada H., K. Salvante, E. Wagner, T.D. Williams, and C.W. Breuner. 2009. Ontogeny and individual variation in the adrenocortical response of zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) nestlings. Physiol Biochem Zool 82:325–331. Walker B.G., P.D. Boersma, and J.C. Wingfield. 2006. Habituation of adult Magellanic penguins to human visitation as expressed through behavior and corticosterone secretion. Cons Biol 20:146–154. Williams J.B. and P.J. Sharp. 1978. Age-dependent changes in the hypothalamous-pituitary-ovarian axis of the laying hen. J Reprod Fertil 53:141–156. Williams T.D. 1992. Reproductive endocrinology of macaroni (Eudyptes chrysolopus) and gentoo (Pygoscelis papua) penguins. II. Gonadal steroid and LH in immature birds in relation to deferred sexual maturity. Gen Comp Endocrinol 85:241–247. Wingfield J.C. 1980. Fine temporal adjustment of reproductive functions. Pp. 363–389 in A. Epple and M.H. Stetson, eds. Avian Endocrinology. Academic Press, London. ———. 1994. Modulation of the adrenocortical response to stress in birds. Pp. 520–528 in K.G. Davey, R.E. Peter, and S.S. Tober, eds. Perspectives in Comparative Endocrinology. National Research Council of Canada, Ottawa. Wingfield J.C., G. Ball, J. Dufty, R. Hegner, and M. Ramenofsky. 1987. Testosterone and aggression in birds: tests of the “challenge hypothesis.” Am Sci 75:602–608. Wingfield J.C. and D.S. Farner. 1975. The determination of five steroids in avian plasma by radioimnumoassay and competitive protein-binding. Steroids 26:311–327. ———. 1993. Endocrinology of reproduction in wild species. Pp. 163–327 in D.S. Farner, J.R. King, and K.C. Parkes, eds. Avian Biology. Academic Press, New York. Wingfield J.C., R.E. Hegner, and D.M. Lewis. 1991. Circulating levels of LH and steroid hormones in relation to social status in the cooperatively breeding white-browed sparrow weaver, Plocepasser mahali. J Zool (Lond) 225:43–58. Young A.J., A.A. Carlson, S.L. Monfort, A.F. Russell, N.C. Bennett, and T.H. Clutton-Brock. 2006 Stress and the suppression of subordinate reproduction in cooperatively breeding meerkats. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:12005–12010. Zack S. and B.J. Stutchbury. 1992. Delayed breeding in avian social systems: the role of territory quality and “floater” tactics. Behaviour 123:194–219.