Learner Resource 1 Using the numbers, place each statement on the agreement continuum below. For each decision you make, you should have a reason/anecdotal evidence to support your theory. 1. Babies may not produce language until nearly two years old. 2. T.V. can ‘teach’ babies how to talk. 3. Children of deaf parents produce their first words sooner than those of hearing parents. 4. A baby, locked in a room with no interaction, will still learn to talk because language is innate. 5. Children learn to speak through imitation (copying). 6. Children learn ‘babytalk’ from their care-givers. 7. Bilingual children learn to talk more slowly than monolingual children. 8. Children cannot communicate clearly using just two words. 9. The amount of talking a baby sees/hears is important. 10. Children’s language acquisition can be made faster. 11. All children are capable, when born, of acquiring any language. 12. Children can only learn languages when they are young. Version 1 Child Language Acquisition 1 Copyright © OCR 2015 Lesson Resource 1 Teacher Notes Below are potential comments to make about each statement. This could be reproduced in the same format as the cards above and could be a matching game alternative to the original task. 1. One of the key things students need to be aware of is the fact that all babies are individual and will acquire language at their own pace. Conceptual models are just that, models. It is also worth highlighting that speech production is just one half of spoken acquisition and learning how to comprehend speech is just as important. Some children don’t speak for two years but this does not mean that acquisition of skills/knowledge hasn’t been taking place. 2. Interaction is key to acquisition. T.V. may be useful in learning of phrases/lexis but meaningful acquisition of grammar, pragmatics, discourse, semantics happens through interaction. 3. When you think about it, this is common sense. It is much easier to gain control of your hands/fingers than it is to manipulate the various parts of the mouth needed to produce intelligible speech. This also suggests that ability to comprehend will often outstrip the ability to produce. Hence, the so-called terrible twos! 4. Innateness is a key acquisition theory but no-one believes acquisition would occur in a linguistic vacuum. As a species we learn what is useful for survival. Communication of ideas verbally is useful in sharing information. That’s why we strive to talk. 5. Imitation is another theoretical approach but research over the last half-a-century suggests that it is not the main driver of acquisition. Think about the child who says “I runned” rather than “I ran”. Presumably this is not by imitation. 6. So-called baby-talk is rooted in the babbling and vocal play stages of pre-verbal children who are playing with their organs of articulation in preparation for becoming verbal. Adults just copied it because it’s cool! (Note, not all cultures around the world exhibit this behaviour). 7. Dual acquisition does tend to be slower which makes sense. If you’re learning two grammatical systems and two lexicons, it will take longer; although, the power of the nascent brain means that is doesn’t take twice as long! 8. Rubbish! I think that was fairly clear from just one word. Adults (especially parents) work hard to decode children’s utterances based on context. 9. As 2. points out, interaction is key to acquisition. The more interaction a baby gets, the faster they will acquire language (up to a point). 10. Well, obviously given that 9. is true. Although the pushy parent should be aware that there is a limit. Version 1 Child Language Acquisition 2 Copyright © OCR 2015 11. Absolutely true. A number of theorists suggest why this is the case but one, Lenneberg, talks of a critical period in which the brain is wired to acquire language. 12. This one is partly true. It’s much easier when they are young but not impossible to learn language later on; although delayed acquisition often leads to incomplete acquisition of deep structures (grammar) OCR Resources: the small print OCR’s resources are provided to support the teaching of OCR specifications, but in no way constitute an endorsed teaching method that is required by the Board, and the decision to use them lies with the individual teacher. Whilst every effort is made to ensure the accuracy of the content, OCR cannot be held responsible for any errors or omissions within these resources. © OCR 2015 - This resource may be freely copied and distributed, as long as the OCR logo and this message remain intact and OCR is acknowledged as the originator of this work. OCR acknowledges the use of the following content: Please get in touch if you want to discuss the accessibility of resources we offer to support delivery of our qualifications: resources.feedback@ocr.org.uk Version 1 Child Language Acquisition 3 Copyright © OCR 2015