Running head: CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY Category Specific Spatial Frequency Adaptation with Faces and Cars Yoseph Ali Beki Thesis completed in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the Honors Program in Psychological Sciences Under the Direction of Dr. Isabel Gauthier, PhD and Olivia Cheung, Graduate Student Vanderbilt University April 6, 2011 Approved Date _____________________________ _______________ 1 CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 2 Abstract This experiment establishes new grounds in the theoretical debate of object perception—between the domain-specific hypothesis and the expertise hypothesis. Using spatial frequency adaptation, the results from this study will provide more evidence in support of the expertise hypothesis. The results will test whether expertise processing (here, face processing) is significantly different from object processing. Participants were adapted to a range of spatial frequencies (high/low) and an image category (face/car) to determine if adaptation effect would remain within the image category. Also, bird and car expertise was measured to analyze the interaction between expertise and category specific spatial frequency adaptation. While the correlations between adaptation and expertise were not significant, some adaptation effects were significant and support the need for further research. CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 3 Category Specific Spatial Frequency Adaptation with Faces and Cars Over the past few decades, vision research, in particular the study of face processing, has grown in sophistication and complexity. Scientists first studied how faces are processed in comparison to other “non-face” objects, such as tools or houses. They noticed that people differentiate and recognize individual faces even when faces are similar. All faces have two eyes, a nose, and a mouth, yet this uniformity does not hinder the identification of a specific face amongst others. Also, faces are remembered very well in comparison to other objects. With these observations in mind, psychologists focused on how faces were processed as opposed to other objects. Through experimentation, they revealed holistic processing, where one processes an image wholly as opposed to processing it in parts, and the face inversion effect, where recognition times of studied faces decrease when they are inverted while recognition times of studied objects are not as affected by inversion (Davidoff & Donnelley, 1990; Tanaka & Farah, 1993; Valentine, 1988; Yin 1969; Young, Hellawell, & Hay, 1987; Yovel, Paller, & Levy, 2005). As technology advanced to the point of analyzing functions of the brain, the fusiform face area (FFA), within the fusiform gyrus, was discovered. Through brain imaging, such as fMRI, this area of the brain was found to react more to faces than to other objects (Kanwisher, McCermott, & Chun, 1997; Tong, Mascovitch, Weinrib, & Kanwisher, 2000). With so many differences between face processing and object processing, psychologists began to create theories to explain how faces and objects are processed in two distinct ways. CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 4 There were two theories that emerged over others. The first is the domain-specific hypothesis, which states that faces are processed by a specific area in the brain (the FFA), set apart from areas that process objects (McKone, Kanwisher, & Duchaine, 2006; Kanwisher, 2000). The hypothesis proposes that there is an area that is specialized for processing faces that is separate from areas that process objects. Within this hypothesis, different categories of visual stimuli are processed in separate areas of the brain. Supporters of this hypothesis used the phenomenon of holistic processing as proof for their hypothesis. Holistic processing is the idea that one processes an image as a whole as opposed to processing it in parts. Studies showed that when trying to focus on one part of an image, participants were better at ignoring other parts of objects than other parts of faces (Tanaka & Farah, 1993; Davidoff & Donnelley, 1990; Young, Hellawell, & Hay, 1987; Yovel, Paller, & Levy, 2005). The argument used to support the domain specific hypothesis that arises from this observation is that holistic processing is specific to upright faces, so a specific face module must exist within the brain. The domain-specific supporters also used the face inversion effect as evidence. The face inversion effect is a phenomenon in which previously studied faces are recognized faster and more accurately than previously studied objects, but when the same faces and objects are inverted during testing, objects are recognized faster and more accurately than faces (Yin, 1969; Kanwisher, Tong, & Nakayama, 1998; Valentine, 1988). The difference in results because of inversion proved the domain-specific hypothesis to its supporters. They believe that the particular mode of face processing was hindered by inversion, while the particular mode of object processing was not affected. Support for the domain-specific hypothesis was also found using brain imaging to test whether the brain processes faces separately from objects. Experimenters found that a part of the CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 5 fusiform gyrus activated when participants were presented with a face and not when they were presented with an object (Kanwisher, McCermott, & Chun, 1997). The discovery of the FFA in the fusiform gyrus gave domain-specific supporters an exclusive area in the brain for processing faces that could prove their hypothesis. Along with showing activation in the FFA, domain-specific supporters used brain imaging to show that prosopagnosics, people who cannot recognize faces or are poorly able to identify faces, have damage to the FFA (Farah, Wilson, Drain, & Tanaka, 1995). They propose that because of the brain damage or deterioration of the FFA, these patients were not able to recognize faces, which further supported the exclusive use of the FFA for faces and the domainspecific hypothesis. The second face processing hypothesis that is currently supported by psychologists is the expertise hypothesis, which proposes that people have become experts at identifying faces, and because of this level of expertise, faces are processed in a different way than other objects. To prove this hypothesis, experimenters have focused on achieving effects exclusive to face processing for other objects, such as birds and cars. They believe that if face expertise causes phenomena such as holistic processing, the face inversion effect, and activation of the FFA, then other types of expertise (bird or car expertise) would cause these phenomena as well. In support of the expertise hypothesis, dog experts were shown to hold inversion effects with dogs in comparison to dog novices, so the faces inversion effect was proved not to be only for faces, but for objects of expertise (Diamond & Carey, 1986). Also, researchers showed that the FFA was activated in bird and car experts when they were presented with objects of their expertise (Gauthier, Skudlarski, Gore, & Anderson, 2000a). With these results, the FFA was CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 6 found not to be exclusive for face processing; the area was used for object processing as well, as long as the participant was an expert with the given object. Furthermore, experiments with novel objects (Greebles) found that with recognition training, participants were able to show activation in the FFA (Gauthier & Tarr, 1997; Gauthier, Williams, Tarr, & Tanaka, 1998). Novices who had never seen a Greeble before the experiment showed activation of the FFA when presented with a Greeble after they had gained a level of expertise with them through training. Along with FFA activation, these Greeble experts also showed holistic processing and inversion effects with the novel objects (Gauthier & Tarr, 1997; Gauthier, Williams, Tarr, & Tanaka, 1998). This study along with other similar studies demonstrated that when people became experts with a certain object, they will process them in the same way as they process faces (Gauthier & Tarr, 1997; Gauthier et al., 2000b; Tanaka & Taylor, 1991). With two prominent hypotheses fighting for validity in the field of face processing, debates between supporters of each side and critiques of each hypothesis were presented. Proponents of the domain-specific hypothesis state that there have not been any significant behavioral effects or imaging results that prove the expertise hypothesis (Kanwisher, 2000; McKone, Kanwisher, & Duchaine, 2006; McKone & Kanwisher, 2005; Robbins & McKone 2006). Furthermore, they point out that there have been expertise experiments that have failed (McKone & Kanwisher, 2005; Tanaka et al., 1996). Also, they were able to show that Greeble training was able to increase performance in categorization and naming tasks in a prosopagnosiac, which led to the conclusion that expertise with faces did not correlate with expertise with objects (Duchaine, Dingle, Butterworth, & Nakayama, 2004). Domain-specific CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 7 supporters used the separation between Greeble/novel object expertise and face expertise to nullify the experiments using Greeble training and to support their hypothesis. However, supporters of the expertise hypothesis suggest that their data give validity to their hypothesis. They also argue that the experiments run by domain-specific hypothesis supporters are biased toward domain specificity (Gauthier, Anderson, Tarr, Skudlarski, & Gore, 1997). These biased studies do not take the level of categorization into account when ruling out object expertise. These experiments use the effects of objects in basic categories such as car or dog to contrast against the effects of individual faces of Tim or Bob. Cars and dogs are more distinct from each other than two individual faces. A car has four wheels and a trunk, and a dog has four legs and a tail. These objects are differentiated from one another by recognizing specific parts or configuration of parts within them. The difference in these parts leads to basic categorization. (Gauthier, Behrmann, & Tarr, 1999). Faces, however, are more closely related to each other. They have the same parts in them, and the parts are configured in the same manner. All faces have two eyes, a nose and a mouth, and they are located on faces in a uniform manner. To differentiate between faces, viewers must use a higher, or subordinate, level of categorization, and they must use more subtle visual cues such as color, size, and shape (Bruce & Humphreys, 1994; Diamond & Carey, 1986; Rhodes, 1988; Tanaka & Taylor, 1991). In comparison to basic level categorization that novices are able to accomplish, subordinate level categorization requires a level of mastery, experience, or expertise to accomplish. Therefore, to compare objects and faces, objects must be recognized on the same, subordinate level as faces. More specific, subordinate categories such as Honda Civic or CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 8 Dalmatian need to be used. Expertise hypothesis supporters design their experiments to target subordinate categorization, so that both faces and objects are processed on the same categorical level. Also, in regards to the significance of the experiments ran that suggest expertise, proponents of the hypothesis state that the effects of object expertise is smaller than the effects of faces due to the level of exposure participants have to their object of expertise (Gauthier & Bukach, 2007). In relation to the level of exposure to faces, one sees objects of expertise less than faces. Bird watchers see more people in their lifetime than birds. Therefore, their level of expertise for faces will be higher than their level of expertise for birds, and the effects from viewing faces will be greater than the effects from viewing birds. In response to the supporters of the domain-specific hypothesis that point out failed expertise experiments, expertise supporters stated that, while some experiments fail, they do not rule out the rest of the experiments that succeed (Gauthier & Bukach, 2007). These failed experiments do not nullify the rest of the literature that has shown expertise effects; they only call attention to situations where expertise effects do not happen and promote further inquiry into the area. In light of the existing controversy between the two sides of face perception research, new forms of testing each hypothesis need to be developed. Supporters of each view of face perception use similar experiments and analyze their results in a way to favor their hypothesis. To open a new path of discovery within the field, more innovative experiments and methods should be used. This experiment will apply spatial frequency and adaptation methods in an attempt to find a new facet in the face processing theoretical debate. CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 9 Adaptation methods in visual processing can use visual stimuli containing different properties that are separated in the visual system. An example of adaptation using visual stimuli is red-green phenomenon in color perception, where one views a red stimulus for a period of time and sees green when the red stimulus is removed. Another example is motion aftereffects in motion perception, in which a person perceives upward motion in a still object after viewing downward motion for a period of time (Addams, 1834). In adaptation experiments, a person is exposed to a type of stimulus for a long period of time, which causes one’s perception to be biased against that stimulus. Adaptation effects occur because neurons that respond to a type of stimulus fatigue after prolonged exposure to the stimulus, and neurons that usually compete with the fatigued neurons activate with ease. For example, the red-green phenomenon occurs because neurons that respond to the color green compete with neurons that respond to the color red, and draining the red neurons of energy by looking at the red stimulus causes one to see green (Cohen, 1946). While the examples of visual adaptation discussed above use low-level visual properties, higher-level visual adaptations are seen with face identity or category judgments (Webster, Kaping, Mizokami, & Duhamel, 2004). For example, face adaptation studies have been effective when using higher-level categorizations, such as gender, race, and emotion. The connection between these high-level adaptation studies and the low-level adaptation studies discussed earlier is that the stimuli within each experiment are similar—they are colors, movement, or faces. Adaptation studies that cross categories, such as a study with faces and cars, have not been attempted. The problem with cross-category adaptation is that the shared properties of stimuli decrease as one moves from within-category to cross-category. Therefore, adaptation CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 10 experiments that attempt to use multiple categories should use properties that remain shared across categories. A property that is shared across higher-level categories that can be used in cross-category adaptation studies is the separation of high and low spatial frequencies throughout the visual system. All images contain a spectrum of spatial frequencies. Experimenters can filter out sections of this spectrum from images, leaving certain parts of the spectrum intact within the image. For example, one can filter the middle and high spatial frequencies out of an image to retain only low spatial frequencies (LSFs), which contain very coarse information of an image that give one basic properties of an image. With faces images, LSFs give one the general shape of the face. An image containing LSFs looks as if it is blurred (Figure 1). One can also retain only high spatial frequencies (HSFs), which look like sketches of images and very detailed (Figure 2). With HSFs, one can determine the detailed information of the face, such as wrinkles and scars (Schyns & Oliva, 1999; Gauthier, Curby, Skudlarski, & Epstein, 2005). Schyns & Oliva (1999) found that face processing and categorical judgments, such as gender and expression identification, depend on different ranges of spatial frequencies. These studies show that a low-level property that is shared across categories, such as spatial frequencies, could be an effective property to use in cross-category adaptation studies. Within the experiment, participants were adapted to images of faces or cars in different spatial frequencies to see if aftereffects of adaptation cross between the different object categories. The aftereffects were retrieved by having participants judge hybrid images before and after adaptation. Hybrids that contained two faces with different spatial frequencies were judged as male or female, and hybrid that contained two cars with different spatial frequencies were judged as sedan or convertible. When a participant is adapted to a certain stimulus, the neural CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 11 network that processes that stimulus will be fatigued and will not react to the stimulus as much as it does before adaptation. The decrease in stimulation is known as an aftereffect. If different types of images (faces and cars) do not create aftereffects for one another, then the neurons that process each type of image should be different. With proof that this method shows differentiation between neural networks, researchers can replicate the study with experts of cars or birds to see if they process these objects through the face, or expertise, processing network or the object processing network. Participants were also given an expertise test consisting of same/different tasks with bird pairs and car pairs to retrieve a d-prime for their selections, a measure for expertise. Measuring expertise makes sense, because if expertise processing uses the same areas as face processing, then car experts viewing cars may use the same neural networks as they use when viewing faces. When adapted to either faces or cars, car experts would fatigue neurons that are shared between car and face processing, which would cause cross-adaptation. Therefore, expertise was measured to account for possible cross-adaptation. Furthermore, both car expertise and bird expertise were measured to see if there is an effect of overall expertise instead of an effect of car expertise. By accounting for other types of expertise, the effect of overall expertise can be partialled out during analysis, leaving only the effect of car expertise. By using known spatial frequency and adaptation methods, new, more definitive results can be made in order to propel one hypothesis over the other. With the current knowledge in vision research, there is an abundance of different tasks that can be applied to experiments within this field, and this test can be the deciding factor to which hypothesis will overcome the other in this debate. Method Participants CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 12 This experiment included 106 healthy participants from Vanderbilt University and the surrounding community with normal or corrected vision. These participants were recruited through Vanderbilt’s research sign-up system, SONA. Twenty participants were excluded due to being over the age of 39, not completing the experiment, and/or having a d-prime below -0.5 for bird expertise. D-prime values below this value, according to signal detection theory, were far below chance on a forced-choice task, so data from participants who scored below this value were excluded from analysis. The mean age of the eligible participants was 21.8 years old. Fortyone of these participants were male; forty-five were female. Participants were either paid for their participation or given credit to use to fulfill experimental requirements for undergraduate psychology courses. They were randomly assigned to one of four groups that differed by adaptation condition, which created a between-subjects design. The adaptation conditions were created by making a 2x2 cross with spatial frequency and image type as factors. The four adaptation conditions (number of participants) were: high spatial frequency faces (22), low spatial frequency faces (22), high spatial frequency cars (22), and low spatial frequency cars (21). Apparatus or Measures Images – The experiment used pictures of faces and cars. They were grayscaled and sized with Adobe Photoshop CS5. They were manipulated into low and high spatial frequency images (<8 cycles per image and >32 cycles per image), being consistent with previous spatial frequency filtering studies (Gauthier, Curby, Skudlarski, & Epstein, 2005; Schyns & Oliva, 1999). The images consisted of 40 face pictures and 40 car pictures. The face pictures consisted of 40 individuals (20 males and 20 females), which were selected from the Karolinska face database (Lundqvist, Flykt, & Öhman, 1998). The car pictures consisted of 40 models (20 sedans and 20 CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 13 convertibles) viewed from the side. After these images were split into two images (HSF and LSF), 20 male faces were paired with 20 female faces and 20 sedans were paired with 20 convertibles to make LSF/HSF hybrid images that contained both options within a category (female/male, sedan/convertible), totaling to 40 hybrids within each category (Figure 3 and 4). The face images were chosen through piloting to see which faces were easily categorized as a certain sex. The car images were not piloted because they were shown as reliable stimuli in previous experiments. The other 40 images (20 LSF, 20 HSF) within each category will be used for adaptation stimuli. In the expertise test, there were 224 greyscaled bird images and 224 greyscaled car images (Figures 5 and 6). 112 bird images were separated into 56 pairs of images, each pair showing a species of birds. The images in each pair show the species of birds at a different perspective. The other 112 bird images are separated into 56 pairs, each pair showing two different species of birds. 112 car images are separated into 56 pairs of images, each pair showing a car model. The images in each pair show the model at a different perspective and can differ by year of production. The other 112 car images are separated into 56 pairs, each pair showing two different car models. Lastly, a grayscale mask containing the whole spatial frequency spectrum (Figure 7) was used during testing. Design Participants were randomly placed into one of four groups: High spatial frequency face adaptation, low spatial frequency face adaptation, high spatial frequency car adaptation, and low spatial frequency car adaptation. Depending on their group, participants were adapted to a certain type of image. There were 5 phases to the adaptation experiment: Originals phase, Practice phase, Pre-adaptation phase, Adaptation phase, and Post-adaptation phase. The pre-adaptation CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 14 choices for face and car hybrids were compared with the post-adaptation choices to show differences caused by adaptation. Lastly, participants were given an expertise test that consisted of same/different judgment of bird pairs and car pairs. Expertise ratings for birds and cars were used to make correlations between expertise and adaptation effects. Procedures After giving informed consent, participants went through the five phases of the experiment. In the originals phase, participants saw the images similar to what they would see during testing. They saw 10 unfiltered images of cars and 10 unfiltered images of faces. This phase allowed participants to become familiar with the stimuli they would be judging. The images were presented in a steady, continuous stream that was separated by blocks of five images. Each block contained only one type of image (for example, female faces) and began with a notification of what would be seen during the block. Participants did not have to make judgments on these images. In the practice phase, the participants saw 5 spatial frequency filtered faces and 5 spatial frequency cars, and they had to decide if they were male faces, female faces, convertibles, or sedans. This phase accustomed the participants to the judgment task. The image categories (faces/cars) were separated by block, randomized within each block, and there were four blocks. Each image was shown twice during this phase. The keys used in this phase (as well as in the other phases with judgments—pre- and post-adaptation) were “n” for sedans, “m” for convertibles, “z” female, and “x” for male. In the pre-adaptation phase, participants saw all of the face hybrids and car hybrids (80 total) which were presented, and they had to decide if they saw male faces, female faces, convertibles, or sedans. The image categories were separated by block, randomized within each CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 15 block, and there were four blocks. Each hybrid image was presented twice. A fixation point was presented before each trial and followed by a 50 ms presentation of a hybrid image which was then covered with a mask for 50 ms. Participants had an indefinite amount of time to judge the faces In the adaptation phase, participants were shown images in regards to their adaptation condition. They were prompted to pay close attention to the images, and they were shown all of the adapting stimuli for their adaptation condition. The images were presented continuously in one block. They were randomized and presented for 2 s each. There was a 200 ms interval between each stimulus as well. In the post-adaption phase, they saw all of the face hybrids and car hybrids, and they had to decide if they saw male faces, female faces, convertibles, or sedans—just as they did in the pre-adaptation phase. However, between the fixation cross and each trial image, adapting images within a participant’s adaptation condition were presented for six seconds as a form of readaptation. Within each re-adaptation, four images were presented in the fashion of the adaptation phase, lasting about six seconds. Each trial was started by the participant pressing the space bar. The expertise test asked participants to judge bird image pairs as the same species or not and to judge car image pairs as the same model or not, irrespective of the production year. The test consisted of 8 blocks (4 with bird pairs, 4 with car pairs), each containing 28 pairs of images. The sequence of the image pairs was randomized. Starting with a fixation cross, the images within a pair were presented sequentially, and participants made a same/different judgment on the image pair. They used the “s” key to respond “same” and the “d” key to respond “different”. Results CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 16 Data Analysis Each participant’s pre- and post-adaptation phases were compared to test whether adaptation affected the perception of spatial frequency. The data were reduced to averages across participants, using the change in LSF responses from pre- to post-adaptation both within their adaptation image category and outside their adaptation image category. The pre-adaptation averages were analyzed to determine if there were any pre-existing spatial frequency biases within image type. The adaptation data (post-adaptation changes from baseline) were compared between and across groups. Also, the change in LSF response for each participant was matched with the participant’s results from the expertise test to make scatter plots connecting expertise to adaptation effects. Both car expertise and bird expertise was measured and analyzed with the participant’s results using zero-order Pearson-Product correlations and multiple regressions. Results The pre-test responses were near chance (as shown in Figures 8 and 9). People adapted to HSF faces, when tested with faces responded “LSF” 57.4% of the time (SD = 7.3%). When tested with cars, they responded “LSF” 39.5% of the time (SD = 7.85%). People adapted to LSF faces, when tested with faces responded “LSF” 54.0% of the time (SD = 8.44%). When tested with cars, they responded “LSF” 39.9% of the time (SD = 9.00%). People adapted to HSF cars, when tested with faces responded “LSF” 56.4% of the time (SD = 6.40%). When tested with cars, they responded “LSF” 37.4% of the time (SD = 11.5%). People adapted to LSF cars, when tested with faces responded “LSF” 55.0% of the time (SD = 9.19%). When tested with cars, they responded “LSF” 41.1% of the time (SD = 8.98%). The differences between the pre-test and the post-test are shown in Figures10 and 11. Using a single sample t-test for each combination of adaptation condition and test condition (for CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 17 example, adaptation to HSF faces and test faces), two cases were found to be significant. There was a significant difference from zero when participants were adapted to HSF faces and tested with face hybrids, t(21) = -3.175, p = 0.005. There was also a significant difference from zero when participants were adapted to HSF cars and tested with car hybrids, t(20) = 2.777, p = 0.01. The t-test data for the rest of the combinations of adaptation conditions and test conditions are shown in Table 1. Lastly, when these data are combined with expertise measures, the correlations are not significant (shown in Table 1). Even after partialling out bird expertise in order to rule out effect of overall expertise, the results from the adaptation experiment did not hold significant correlations with car expertise. Discussion In this study, there was not adaptation across image type in some of the adaptation conditions of the experiment. Some differences in LSF responses changed in the intended direction, but most were not significant changes. Two test conditions produced significant results. The first significant result occurred when participants were adapted to HSF cars and tested on car hybrids. These participants reported seeing more LSF cars in the post-test. When the participants within this adaptation condition were tested with faces, there was no significant change between pre- and post-test. These results suggest that spatial frequency adaptation was category specific, because there was an adaptation effect within for cars when adapted to cars, and this effect did not cross over to faces. The adaptation effect remained within the image category to which participants were adapted. In other situations, there was evidence for cross-adapation, but the evidence was not significant. One case is when participants were adapted to LSF faces and tested on car hybrids. CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 18 They reported seeing fewer LSF cars in the post-test, which is something that would happen due to cross-adaptation. Lastly, in some conditions (one being the second significant result), the opposite of what was expected occurred. For example, when participants were adapted to HSF faces and tested with face hybrids, they saw fewer LSF faces during the post-test. The expected result in this adaptation condition was an increase in the reporting of LSF faces after adaptation, and the actual result showed the opposite. The same participants did not show adaptation effects for cars, so even though the adaptation effect was in the opposite direction of what was expected, there was no adaptation that crossed over image category. The two significant results show there is no cross-category spatial frequency adaptation. However, the adaptation effects went in both directions. This conflict of results suggests that cars and faces may be processed differently. For example, while being adapted to HSF cars causes an effect in the predicted direction (making more LSF responses when viewing car hybrids), being adapted to HSF faces causes an effect in the opposite direction. Therefore, while car processing (or object processing in general) follows traditional adaptation effects, face processing does not. This difference may be due to levels of expertise. Changes from object processing to expertise processing has been shown with other effects, such as the inversion effect and holistic processing, and adaptation may be another case where faces are processed differently from other non-expertise objects. This difference in processing may cause the non-congruent adaptation effect for faces and cars. Along with most of the changes in LSF responses by adaptation condition, the correlations found from the data were not significant. Therefore, expertise did not significantly affect the results found in the adaptation experiment. Going along with the earlier suggestion that CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 19 faces are processed differently than cars (or non-expertise objects), the correlations show insignificant trends that connect car expertise with different adaptation effects. For example, when participants are adapted to LSF cars and tested with car hybrids, the opposite of an adaptation effect occurs with increasing levels of expertise. This suggests that car experts process LSF cars as they do faces, which leads to the opposite of an adaptation effect. Also, when participants are adapted to faces in either SF, adaptation effects increase as car expertise increases. Therefore, when one is a car expert, one experiences a traditional adaptation effect with faces, which is opposite from what was discussed earlier. While insignificant, these results suggest that when one is an expert with an object, the object is processed differently, and if expertise processing is within the FFA, then there may be competition between different objects of expertise, causing, for example, faces to be processed as non-expertise objects in car experts. These suggestions are speculative, in that they assume that behavioral results parallel brain activity, but the results highlight a trend that future studies should follow. While some significant results did arise from this study, future experiments of this nature should revise the design in a few ways. First, the adapting stimuli should include noise from the other SF. For example, if an adapting stimulus is a HSF face, there should be LSF noise added to it. This addition would rule out the argument that only lower-level visual areas are being adapted in this design, meaning that the adaptation effects never reach the category selective areas. By adding the other SF, participants will have the lower level areas adapted to both SFs while the higher level area will be adapted to one SF. Another way to improve this design is to test for face expertise. By testing expertise for faces, experimenters can gauge participants against each other and make correlations similar to CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 20 what was found with car expertise. If correlations were found using face expertise, they may coincide with the speculations about SF biases in expertise, as discussed earlier. Furthermore, testing expertise for multiple categories of objects may be beneficial. In this study, bird expertise was used to rule out overall expertise in the correlations, and one may find interactions between different types of expertise if one tests for expertise in multiple object categories. Continuing research in this path may further differentiate between the domain-specific hypothesis and the expertise. CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 21 References Addams, R. (1834). An account of a peculiar optical phaenomenon seen after having looked at a moving body. London and Edinburgh Philosophical Magazine and Journal of Science, 5, 373-374. Bruce, V, & Humphreys, G. (1994). Recognizing objects and faces. Visual Cognition, 1, 141180. Cohen, J. (1946). Color adaptation of the human eye. The American Journal of Psychology, 59, 84-110. Davidoff, J. & Donnelley, N. (1990). Object superiority: A comparison of complete and part probes. Acta Psychologica, 73, 225-243. Diamond, R. & Carey, S. (1986). Why faces are and are not special: An effect of expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 115, 107-117. Duchaine, B. C., Dingle, K., Butterworth, E., & Nakayama, K. (2004). Normal greeble learning in a severe case of developmental prosopagnosia, Neuron. 2004, 43(4):469-73. Farah, M.J., Wilson, K.D., Drain, H.M., & Tanaka, J.R. (1995). The inverted face inversion effect in prosopagnosia: Evidence for mandatory face-specific perceptual mechanisms. Vision Research, 35, 2089-2093. Gauthier, I., Anderson, A. W., Tarr, M. J., Skudlarski, P., & Gore, J. C. (1997). Levels of categorization in visual recognition studied using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Current Biology, 7, 645-651. Gauthier, I., Behrmann, M., & Tarr, M. J. (1999). Can face recognition really be dissociated from object recognition? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 349-370. Gauthier, I. & Bukach, C. (2007). Should we reject the expertise hypothesis? Cognition, 103, CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 22 322-330. Gauthier, I., Curby, K. M., Skudlarski, P., & Epstein, R. (2005). Activity in the human fusiform gyrus reflects separate spatial frequency channels. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 222-234. Gauthier, I., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J.C., Anderson, A.W. (2000a). Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition. Nature America, 3, 191-197. Gauthier, I. & Tarr, M.J. (1997) Becoming a ‘Greeble’ expert: Exploring the face recognition mechanisms. Vision Research, 37, 1673–1682. Gauthier, I., Tarr, M.J., Moylan, J., Anderson, A.W., Skudlarski, P., & Gore, J.C. (2000b). Does visual subordinate-level categorization engage the functionally defined fusiform face area? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 143-163. Gauthier, I., Williams, P., Tarr, M. J. & Tanaka, J. W. (1998). Training ‘Greeble’ experts: A framework for studying expert object recognition processes. Vision Research, 38, 2401– 2428. Kanwisher, N. (2000). Domain specificity in face perception. Nature, 3, 759-763. Kanwisher, N., McCermott, J., & Chun, M.M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 4302-4311. Kanwisher, N., Tong, F., & Nakayama, K. (1998). The effect of face inversion on the human fusiform face area. Cognition, 68, 1-11. Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces – KDEF, CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet, ISBN 91-630-7164-9. CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 23 McKone, E. & Kanwisher, N. (2005). Does the human brain process objects of expertise like faces? A review of the evidence. In: Dehaene S, Duhamel JR, Hauser M, and Rizzolatti, editors: From Monkey Brain to Human Brain. MIT Press. McKone, E., Kanwisher, N., & Duchaine, B.C. (2006). Can generic expertise explain special processing for faces? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 8-15. Rhodes, G. (1988). Looking at faces: First-order and second-order features as determinants of facial appearance. Perception, 17, 43–63. Robbins, R. A., & McKone, E. (2007). No face-like processing for objects-of-expertise in three behavioural tasks. Cognition, 103, 322-330. Schyns, P., & Oliva, A. (1999). Dr. Angry and Mr. Smile: when categorization flexibly modifies the perception of faces in rapid visual presentations. Cognition, 69, 243-265. Tanaka, J.W., & Farah, M.J. (1993). Parts and wholes in face recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 225-245. Tanaka, J. W., Kay, J. B., Grinnell, E., Stansfield, B., & Szechter, L. (1996). Face recognition in young children: When the whole is greater than the sum of its parts. Visual Cognition, 5, 479-496. Tanaka, J.W., & Taylor, M. (1991). Object categories and expertise: Is the basic level in the eye of the beholder? Cognitive Psychology, 23, 457-482. Tong, F., Nakayama, K., Moscovitch, M., Weinrib, O., & Kanwisher, N. (2000). Response properties of the human fusiform face area. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 257-279. Valentine, T. (1988). Upside-down faces: A review of the effect of inversion upon face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 471-491. Webster, M. A., Kaping, D., Mizokami, Y., & Duhamel, P. (2004). Adaptation to natural face CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 24 categories. Nature, 428, 557-561. Yin, Robert K. (1969). Looking at upside-down faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 141-145. Young, A.W., Hellawell, D., & Hay, D.C. (1987). Configurational information in face perception. Perception, 16, 747-759. Yovel, G., Paller, K.A., Levy, J. (2005). A whole face is more than the sum of its halves: Interactive processing in face perception. Visual Cognition, 12, 337-352. CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 25 Tables Adapt Image Adapt SF Test Type T-test (df) T-test p-value Zeroorder r Partial r Partial p-value -0.147 Zeroorder p-value 0.52 Car HSF Car 0.01 Car HSF Face Car LSF Car Car LSF Face Face HSF Car Face HSF Face Face LSF Car Face LSF Face (20) 2.777 (20) -0.434 (20) 0.239 (20) 0.998 (21) -0.108 (21) -3.175 (21) -1.05 (21) 0.69 -0.07 0.76 0.669 -0.186 0.42 -0.131 0.57 0.81 0.202 0.38 0.231 0.32 0.33 0.074 0.749 0.076 0.749 0.92 -0.181 0.42 -0.132 0.56 0.005 0.232 0.299 0.243 0.276 0.31 0.208 0.35 0.167 0.458 0.51 -0.01 0.96 -0.214 0.338 Table 1 CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 26 Figure Captions Figure 1 – Example of a low spatial frequency image Figure 2 – Example of a high spatial frequency image Figure 3 – Example of a face hybrid Figure 4 – Example of a car hybrid Figure 5 – Example of a grayscale bird Figure 6 – Example of a grayscale car Figure 7 – Mask used during testing Figure 8 – Column graph of % LSF face responses by adaptation condition in the pre-test (includes standard deviation) Figure 9 – Column graph of % LSF car responses by adaptation condition in the pre-test (includes standard deviation) Figure 10 –Column graph of % change in LSF face responses by adaptation condition in the pretest (includes standard error) Figure 11 – Column graph of % change in LSF car responses by adaptation condition in the pretest (includes standard error) CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY Figures Figure 1 27 CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY Figure 2 28 CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY Figure 3 29 CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY Figure 4 30 CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY Figure 5 31 CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY Figure 6 32 CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY Figure 7 33 CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 34 % LSF Face Response by Adaptation Condition during Pre-Test 65% 60% % LSF Reponse 55% 50% 45% 40% 35% 30% HSF Car LSF Car HSF Face Adaptation Condition Figure 8 LSF Face CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 35 % LSF Car Response by Adaptation Condition during Pre-Test 65% 60% % LSF Response 55% 50% 45% 40% 35% 30% 25% 20% HSF Car LSF Car HSF Face Adaptation Condition Figure 9 LSF Face CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 36 % Change in LSF Face Response by Adaptation Condition 10% % Change LSF Face Response 8% 6% 4% 2% 0% -2% -4% -6% -8% -10% HSF Car LSF Car HSF Face Adaptation Condition Figure 10 LSF Face CATEGORY SPECIFIC SPATIAL FREQUENCY 37 % Change in LSF Car Response by Adaptation Condition 10% 8% % Change LSF Car Reponse 6% 4% 2% 0% -2% -4% -6% -8% -10% HSF Car LSF Car HSF Face Adaptation Condition Figure 11 LSF Face