Evaluation of Intuition 1 Running Head: AN EMPIRICAL EVALUATION OF INTUITION Should Emotions Have a Stake in Decision-Making? An Empirical Evaluation of Intuition. Skyler G. Jacobs Thesis completed in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the Honors Program in the Psychological Sciences Under the Direction of Dr. Craig Smith and Dr. Leslie Kirby Vanderbilt University April 2011 Evaluation of Intuition 2 Abstract The construct of intuition has gained recent attention in the literature on decision-making and emotion. Of particular interest are measures of decision-making style that claim to assess individual differences in intuitiveness and deliberativeness. Study one analyzed the extent to which these measures assess intuitiveness and deliberativeness, developed a brief, derived measure of decision-making style, and explored suspected dispositional correlates of decisionmaking style. Study two explored the effects of individual differences in intuitiveness and deliberativeness on quality of interpretation in an experiment in which participants provided advice to a fictional person in a moral dilemma under experimentally manipulated time pressure. Key dispositional features of decision-making style as well as key features of intuitiveness and deliberativeness in practice are identified and discussed. Evaluation of Intuition 3 Should Emotions Have a Stake in Decision-Making? An Empirical Evaluation of Intuition. Suppose you plan to get married. You meet someone and marry him/her in the same day. You do not bother to consider anyone else. You meet, spend the day together, and get married that night; it just feels right. You are unsure of your new spouse’s family history, religious beliefs, political inclinations, and views on raising children, but you are not worried. You know that whom you choose to marry will greatly impact your quality of life, but you are comfortable basing your choice on a “feeling” you experience after spending a single day together. Most of us would deem this a poorly made decision. It is widely believed that when faced with an important decision, one should think “hard” about the choice—break it into simpler pieces, analyze each piece individually, and anticipate the consequences of each alternative— only then can an informed decision be made. We commonly employ strategies such as “TCharts” based on the belief that organizing the available information into concrete reasons that can be weighed against one another will provide greater insight into the “right” choice. Because humans are uniquely endowed with the introspective ability to think about our thoughts, we tend to assume that good decisions are the result of careful, conscious deliberation. Intuition v. Reasoning Contrary to popular belief, recent research suggests that reasoning may not be the most effective mechanism for decision-making in all cases and provides support for an alternative, less wieldy approach to decision-making: intuition. Nicknamed the “go with your gut” approach, intuition can be conceptualized as rapid interpretation of low-level feelings of ease or unease. Unlike reasoning, which relies on slow, conscious, serial analyses of limited judgments, intuition is driven by associative processing of affective signals (Smith & Kirby, 2000). Because associative processing allows for fast, automatic, and simultaneous analyses of unlimited Evaluation of Intuition 4 information, recent research suggests that intuition may present a better mechanism for complex, affective decisions in which there are multiple abstract factors, while reasoning may be more suited for simple, utilitarian decisions in which there are few, concrete factors (Dijksterhuis, Bos, Nordgren, & van Baaren, 2006). Without the need for conscious awareness, intuition guards against the threat of suboptimal weighting, has the flexibility of a greater processing capacity that does not rely on semantics, and results in less instances of reduced post-choice satisfaction making it well-suited to many of life’s most nuanced and fundamental decisions (Wilson & Schooler, 1991; Dijksterhuis, 2004; Smith & Kirby, 2000; Wilson & Kraft, 1993; Wilson, Lisle, Schooler, Hodges, Klaaren, and LaFleur, 1993; Dijksterhuis and van Olden, 2006). Suboptimal Weighting. The effectiveness of intuition as a mechanism for complex decision-making is bolstered by the absence of the threat of suboptimal weighting present in the reasoning process (Wilson & Schooler, 1991). Reasoning involves successive, discrete analyses of semantically encoded components that are ascribed a certain value or weight within the holistic decision (Smith & Kirby, 2000). Implicit within this process is great potential for skew or suboptimal weighting of the factors relevant to the decision. Wilson and Schooler (1991) demonstrated the phenomenon of suboptimal weighting in a study that compared participants’ preferences for different brands of strawberry jam with the ratings of trained sensory experts. In the first condition, participants simply tasted each jam and ranked them from best to worst. In the second condition, participants did the same but were provided with the list of criteria used by the sensory experts to consider while deliberating. The presence of ranking criterion in the second group induced reasoning of numerous complex and esoteric factors, while the first group was free to make more preferential decisions based on the Evaluation of Intuition 5 most salient features of the jam. Results indicated that rankings by the first group reflected expert opinion more consistently than rankings by the second group. Wilson and Schooler (1991) argued that forced reasoning in the second condition resulted in less optimal decision-making because participants engaged in suboptimal weighting of the various criteria used by the trained sensory experts. The presence of ranking criteria in the second condition provided participants with additional reasons on which to base their decisions; however, because participants lacked the expertise to successfully evaluate and weight the criteria, these conditions increased the likelihood of suboptimal weighting. Even though the first group also lacked expertise, their own preferences showed greater consistency with the preferences of the experts, because they remained ignorant of criteria they were not qualified to assess and based their decisions on the most relevant features of jam such as taste and texture. By taking a relatively simple decision, jam preference, and imbuing it with complexity, Wilson and Schooler’s jam study establishes the effectiveness of an intuitive approach for complexdecision-making in which we lack expertise. Limited Processing Capacity of Consciousness. The limited processing capacity of consciousness can also explain the threat of suboptimal weighting by restricting the amount of information available for simultaneous consideration during reasoning (Dijksterhuis, 2004). The maximum amount of information that can be consciously considered at one time is about seven (plus or minus two) units, while the unconscious can simultaneously draw upon anywhere from 40-60 units of information per second (Dijksterhuis, 2004). As a consciously mediated process, reasoning limits our consideration to some, but not all, factors relevant to a decision, while intuition, an associatively mediated process, has the unconscious capacity for many relevant factors. Without the ability to simultaneously weight all of the factors relevant to a decision Evaluation of Intuition 6 against one another, reasoning is less suited than intuition for making accurate, holistic assessments of complex decisions. Furthermore, in addition to restricting the number of factors that we can simultaneously evaluate, the limited capacity of consciousness also limits the type of information available for conscious consideration to that which can be semantically encoded (Smith & Kirby, 2000). As an affectively charged process unencumbered by consciousness, intuition accommodates the inclusion of sensations and instincts that cannot be formally articulated into the decision-making process, while reasoning discards or dilutes sensations these feelings for semantics. Yet, it is well established that emotions are both informative and have desirable and adaptive effects on decision-making, suggesting that pure reasoning may eclipse meaningful sensations and lead to less optimal outcomes (Smith & Kirby, 2000). Wilson and Kraft (1993) demonstrated this phenomenon in a study that required participants to list and evaluate reasons for being in romantic relationships. After providing an initial report about how they felt about their relationships and specific partners, one group of participants was asked to provide reasons for their feelings, while the other group was asked to consider an unrelated topic. Participants in the forced-reasoning condition brought to mind thoughts that were inconsistent with their initial reports and changed their attitudes in the direction of those reasons, while participants who reasoned about an unrelated topic demonstrated little attitude change. Wilson and Kraft (1993) argued that participants who were forced to reason through their feelings could not adequately translate the sensations into words, but rather came up with superficial list of reasons that skewed their original feelings about their relationships and partners with enough significance to prompt attitude change in the opposite direction. These findings Evaluation of Intuition 7 suggest that emotions, despite their semantic incongruence, are not only germane to the decisionmaking process, but also often lead to less optimal decision-making when excluded, providing evidence for intuition over reasoning as the best mechanism for decisions that are affective in nature. Reduced Post-Choice Satisfaction. Whether it is the result of suboptimal weighting, the limited capacity of the consciousness, or both, construal of the factors involved in a decision has measurable effects on post-choice satisfaction. Because the quality of our choices is subjective to decision-makers—there is no universal standard that predicts decision quality—post-choice satisfaction is an important operationalized, self-report measure of decision quality. Wilson, Lisle, Schooler, Hodges, Klaaren, and LaFleur (1993) demonstrated an association between reasoning and reduced post-choice satisfaction in a poster study. In the study participants were instructed to select a poster of their choice to keep. In one condition, the group was instructed to quickly select a poster. In the other condition the group was instructed to provide reasons for their selection (Wilson et al., 1993). Upon follow-up with the participants three weeks later, Wilson et al. found that those who had reasoned their choice demonstrated less post-choice satisfaction than those who had simply chosen a poster (Wilson et al., 1993). Dijksterhuis and van Olden (2006) confirmed and extended the findings of Wilson et al. (1993) by demonstrating that variation in time of post-choice evaluation did not mediate satisfaction. Whether participants took a test of post-satisfaction a few weeks or a few months after selecting their poster they still demonstrated less post-choice satisfaction than the control group, maintaining that reasoning increases the likelihood of reduced post-choice satisfaction. Additionally, Dijksterhuis and van Olden (2006) added another condition to their study in which participants viewed the posters, were distracted by an unrelated task, and then instructed to Evaluation of Intuition 8 immediately choose a poster. Result indicated that this third group of unconscious deliberators demonstrated greater post-choice satisfaction of any condition. Dijksterhuis and van Olden argued that unconscious deliberation led to higher rates of post-choice satisfaction, because the unrelated distraction task allowed participants to unconsciously mull over each choice in order to inform their eventual decision, provides support that unconscious deliberation increases postchoice satisfaction. Together the findings of Wilson et al. (1993) and Dijksterhuis & van Olden (2006) suggest that intuition, a minimally conscious process, may be a more successful, optimal approach to decision-making than reasoning (Dijksterhuis & van Olden, 2006). As human beings we will all encounter complex, affective decisions in which we lack the necessary expertise to identify and evaluate all of the relevant factors. In fact, many of life’s most impactful decisions, such as mate selection or deciding to have children, involve an implicit amount of uncertainty and reliance on gut; leaps of faith. Even if we were able to anticipate all of factors relevant to such decisions, the limited processing capacity of the consciousness makes it unlikely that we could accurately determine how much influence or weight each factor should carry within the decision unless we have sufficient experience. Yet, the majority of us will not choose a spouse or have enough children to make these types of decisions with enough frequency to develop expertise. Furthermore, emotions, an undeniably significant source of motivation in decision-making of this nature, must be adequately accounted for and considered. Taken as a whole, the current literature on intuition and reasoning suggest that many of life’s most nuanced and pivotal decisions, those of which are undeniably complex and affective in nature, benefit from a more intuitive approach, and that applying reasoning to such decisions may result in less optimal outcomes. Evaluation of Intuition 9 Individual Differences in Decision-Making Regardless of the approach, the inclination to base our decision-making on a specific deliberative process affects the information that we use, how we consider that information, and the satisfaction we experience after our decision has been made (Betsch, 2008). Recent interest in individual differences in decision-making has prompted the development of several scales to measure individual differences in decsion-making (Pacini & Epstein, 1999; Betsch, 2008; Scott & Bruce, 1995; Burns & D’Zurilla, 1999). Most of the current decision-making style scales include subscales that identify intuitiveness and deliberativeness, while some also include additional constructs. Because subscales for intuitiveness and deliberativeness are separate, and are not mutually exclusive, it is possible to demonstrate high or low preference for both intuitiveness and deliberativeness, just one, or neither. Four of the most prominent decisionmaking style inventories include: The Rational-Experiential Inventory (REI) (Pacini & Epstein, 1999), the Preference For Intuition or Deliberation Scale (PID) (Betsch, 2008), the General Decision-Making Scale (GDMS) (Scott & Bruce, 1995), and the Perceived Modes of Processing Inventory (PID) (Burns & D’Zurilla, 1999). Rational Experiential Inventory (REI). Pacini and Epstein’s (1999) RationalExperiential Inventory (REI) consists of 40 items rated on a five-point Likert scale designed to assess preferences for rationality and experientiality information processing. The measure proposes that people process information on two, parallel, interactive systems. The rationality subscale, measured by an adapted Need for Cognition scale emphasizes a conscious, analytical approach (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982). While the experientiality subscale, measured by the Faith in Intuition (FI) scale, emphasizes a pre-conscious, affective, holistic approach (Pacini & Epstein, 1999). The REI differs from other decision-making style scales in that it assesses individuals’ Evaluation of Intuition 10 beliefs about their ability to successfully use their preferred mode in addition to individual preference. (Betsch & Iannello, 2010). Preference for Intuition or Deliberation Scale (PID). Betsch’s (2008) Preference for Intuition or Deliberation Scale (PID) consists of 18 items rated on a five-point Likert scale that evaluates intuitiveness with its subscale: preference for intuition, and deliberativeness with its subscale: preference for deliberation. The PID differentiates between intuition and deliberation by the information source. Intuition is explicitly stated to rely on affect and implicit knowledge, while deliberation relies on explicit knowledge and are considered in a rule-governed way (Betsch, 2008). While most other decision-making style scales measure information processing in general, the PID differs from other scales in its explicit assessment of strategy preference in decision-making situations (Betsch & Iannello, 2010). General Decision-making Style Scale (GDMS). Scott and Bruce’s (1995) General Decision-making Scale (GDMS) consists of 25 items rated on a five-point Likert scale. The items are distributed among five subscales of decision-making style: rational style, intuitive style, dependent style, avoidant style, and spontaneous style. Rational style is characterized by logical analysis and evaluation of alternates while, intuitive style is characterized by attention to detail in information flow (Scott & Bruce, 1995). The GDMS measures decision-making style in a much broader sense than other decision-making scales, going beyond simply intuition and reasoning with three additional subscales that are not found in the other decision-making style measures (Betsch & Iannello, 2010). Perceived Modes of Processing Inventory (PMPI). Burns and D’Zurilla’s (1999) Perceived Modes of Processing Inventory (PMPI) consists of 18 items rated on a five-point Likert scale. The PMPI is used to assess a person’s awareness and perception of his/her dominant Evaluation of Intuition 11 mode of information processing. The scale measures three different processing styles: rational processing, emotional processing, and automatic processing (Burns & D’Zurilla, 1999). Unlike other decision-making style scales, the PMPI claims to measure coping-style. However, the PMPI’s automatic and emotional processing subscales measure intuition in a uniquely nuanced way by distinguishing between the affective and cognitive features of intuition, while reasoning is measured more directly by the rational processing subscale (Betsch & Iannello, 2010). Though slightly varied, each measures of decision-making style measures individual differences in intuitiveness and deliberativeness. Identification of individuals’ decision-making style would allow people to seek conditions that promote decisional fit in order to optimize the outcome of their decisions (Betsch & Kunz, 2008). Additionally, pairing knowledge of decisionmaking style with an understanding of optimal decision-making mechanisms by case—intuition in the case of complex, affective decisions and reasoning in the case of simple, utilitarian decisions—would allow individuals to anticipate the circumstances in which their characteristic decision-making style is most or least beneficial. In theory, this knowledge should provide individuals with the ability to optimize the outcome of their decisions by maintaining or modifying their characteristic tendencies by case as needed. Research Aims This research aimed to further our understanding of intuition as both a general construct and preferred mode of decision-making. The first study determined the degree to which the different measures of decision-making style truly assessed intuitiveness and deliberativeness by examining intercorrelation across scales. The first study also assessed the relationship between intuition and various correlates. Findings from the first study contribute to our understanding of how intuition is conceptualized and measured within the literature. An additional aim of the first Evaluation of Intuition 12 study was to generate a brief, composite measure of decision-making style, based on the four scales, to use in the second study. The second study compared the nature of interpretations made by intuitives (those who demonstrate high levels of intuitiveness) and deliberatives (those who demonstrate high levels of deliberativeness). Participants responded to a moral dilemma that favored intuitive style under conditions that either promoted or inhibited deliberative style by manipulating time pressure. Findings from the second study contribute to our understanding of individual differences in decision-making and their effects on interpretation quality. Study 1 The following study is a comparative analysis of four major decision-making style inventories: the Rational-Experiential Inventory (REI) (Pacini & Epstein, 1999), the Preference For Intuition or Deliberation Scale (PID) (Betsch, 2008), the General Decision-Making Scale (GDMS) (Scott & Bruce, 1995), and the Perceived Modes of Processing Inventory (PID) (Burns & D’Zurilla, 1999), all of which claim to measure both intuitiveness and deliberativeness. The following study explores how well each scale measures these constructs and to what extent intuitiveness and deliberativeness are conceptualized similarly across the scales. Additionally, conceptual analysis of the four scales and face validity of their individual items, allowed us to generate a brief, composite measures of decision-making style for use in study two. The present study also assessed the construct of intuition as it relates to other variables such as emotional intelligence, mindfulness, outcome-related measures, personality, and appraisal style. As an affectively charged process, it is likely that individual differences in preference for intuition might exist based on individual differences in sensitivity to emotional signals. Thus, we expected intuitiveness to be positively associated with emotional intelligence, and deliberativeness to negatively associated with emotional intelligence. Salovey and Mayer Evaluation of Intuition 13 (1990) define emotional intelligence as “a subset of social intelligence that involves the ability to monitor one’s own and others’ feelings and emotions, to discriminate among them, and use this information to guide one’s thinking and actions. Individuals classified as high on emotional intelligence are thought to be sensitive to the emotional signals their body generates, and to be adept at using these signals in guiding their behavior. Thus, successful reliance on affective processing should reinforce emotionally intelligent individuals’ intuitiveness over time. Based on the same principle, we expected deliberativeness to be positively associated with mindfulness and Alexithymia, and intuitiveness to be negatively associated with mindfulness and Alexithymia. Mindfulness is bringing one’s complete attention to the experiences occurring in the present moment in a non-judging and accepting way, while Alexithymia is a personality trait indicating deficiency in understanding, processing, and describing emotions (Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006; Bagby, Parker, & Taylor, 1994). Individuals high on mindfulness and Alexithymia are adept at viewing things objectively and without affect or attachment, such that their emotional feelings do not influence their perceptions. Thus, successful filtering of emotional signals should reinforce mindful and Alexithymic individuals’ deliberativeness over time. Furthermore, based on the hypothesized relationship between intuitiveness and the use of emotional signals, we are able to make further inferences about the relationship between decision-making style and various outcome-related measures. Like emotional intelligence, we expected intuitiveness to be positively associated with self-esteem and satisfaction with life, and negatively correlated with state-trait anxiety (Salovey & Mayer, 1990). We also assessed the relationship between decision-making style and personality. Our treatment of these analyses was more exploratory, and we did not have any concrete expectations. Evaluation of Intuition 14 Finally, because emotional signals play such a prominent role in intuition, we suspected that intuitiveness was probably also related to appraisal. Appraisals are meaning analyses that serve as the cognitive antecedents of emotions, such that given appraisals predict given emotional responses (Smith & Lazarus, 1990). At the same time, two individuals may appraise the same situation differently, and therefore may exhibit characteristic appraisal styles (Smith & Lazarus, 1990). We expected intuitiveness to be positively correlated with emotion-focused coping potential, appraisal of one’s ability to emotionally adjust to one’s circumstances, such that people who demonstrated high levels of intuitiveness would appraise their emotion-focused coping potential to be high. Our treatment of the other aspects of appraisal style—problemfocused coping potential, motivational relevance, and motivation congruence—was more exploratory, and we did not have any concrete expectations. Method Participants and Procedure Online users participated in a large-scale survey used to validate measures of intuitiveness. Participants accessed the survey from websites in which they could volunteer to participate in ongoing psychological research. Participants were 388 online users, 85 of which were male and 303 of which were female. Participants’ ages ranged from 18-63 with a mean age of 25. The majorities were Caucasian, currently located in North America, and classified themselves as students. Participants completed the survey online at a time and pace of their choosing. Measures The survey included measures of decision-making style and suspected correlates of intuitiveness and deliberativeness. Evaluation of Intuition 15 Decision-Making Style. Four separate scales measured decision-making style. Each measure was evaluated on a five-point Likert scale in which 1= not at all true of me, 2= slightly true of me, 3= moderately true of me, 4= very true of me, and 5= extremely true of me. The first scale was the 40-item Rational-Experiential Inventory (REI) (Pacini & Epstein, 1999). The REI has two subscales: 1) rationality and 2) experientiality. In the present study the internal consistencies (alpha) for rationality and experientiality was .87 and .92, respectively. Construct validity of the REI has been verified through correlations with the NEO-FFI (Koele & Dietvorst, 2010). The REI’s rationality subscale correlates positively with the NEO-FFI subscales of openness to experience (.44) and conscientiousness (.32), and correlates negatively with the subscale of neuroticism (-.38) (Koele & Dietvorst, 2010). However, the REI’s experientiality subscale shows no substantial correlations with any of the NEO-FFI factors (Koele & Dietvorst, 2010). The second scale included as a measure of decision-making style was the 17-item Preference For Intuition or Deliberation Scale (PID) (Betsch, 2008). The PID has two subscales: 1) preference for intuition and 2) preference for deliberation. In the present study internal consistencies (alpha) for preference for intuition and preference for deliberation was.78 and .79, respectively. Construct validity for the PID has been verified by correlations between the PID and REI in which the preference for deliberation subscale of the PID and the rationality subscale of the REI demonstrate moderate correlation (.51), and the preference for intuition subscale of the PID and experientiality subscale of the REI demonstrate strong correlation (.84) (Koele & Dietvorst, 2010). The third scale included as a measure of decision-making style was the 24-item General Decision-Making Scale (GDMS) (Scott & Bruce, 1995). The GDMS has five subscales: 1) Evaluation of Intuition 16 rational style, 2) intuitive style, 3) dependent style, 4) avoidant style, and 5) spontaneous style. In the present study the internal consistencies (alpha) for rational style, intuitive style, dependent style, avoidant style, and spontaneous style was .76, .78, .90, .90, and .85, respectively. Little has been concluded about the construct validity of the GMDS (Koele & Dietvorst, 2010). The fourth and final scale that measured decision-making style was the 32-item Perceived Modes of Processing Inventory (PID) (Burns & D’Zurilla, 1999). The PMPI has three subscales: 1) rational processing, 2) emotional processing, and 3) automatic processing. In the present study the internal consistencies (alpha) for rational processing, emotional processing, and automatic processing was .89, .88, and .83, respectively. Construct validity for the PMPI has been verified by correlations between the PMPI and REI in which the rational processing subscale of the PMPI was positively correlated with the rational subscale of the REI, while the automatic processing subscale of the PMPI was positively correlated with the experiential subscale of the REI (Koele & Dietvorst, 2010). Finally, based on a conceptual analysis of the four decision-making style scales and the face validity of their individual items, we selected a subset of 18 items, drawing on a couple of items from each scale, to generate a brief, composite measure of decision-making style to use in study two. The brief measure’s individual items and their associated scales of origin are listed in Appendix A. The brief measure includes two subscales: 1) intuitiveness and 2) deliberativeness. Nine items were used to measure intuitiveness: two items from the experiential subscale of the REI, three items from the preference for intuition subscale of the PID, two items from the intuitive style subscale of the GDMS, and two items from the emotional processing subscale of the PMPI. In the present study the internal consistency (alpha) of the brief intuitiveness measure was .87. Nine items were also used to measure deliberativeness: two items from the rational Evaluation of Intuition 17 subscale of the REI, three items from the preference for deliberation subscale of the PID, two items from the rational style subscale of the GDMS, and two measures from the rational processing subscale of the PMPI. In the present study the internal consistency (alpha) of the brief deliberativeness measure was .82. Correlates of Intuitiveness and Deliberativeness. Hypothesized correlates of decisionmaking style included emotional intelligence, mindfulness, Alexithymia, outcome-related measures, personality, and appraisal style. The 48-item Trait Meta Mood Scale (TMMS) was used to measure emotional intelligence (Goldman, Kraemer, & Salovey, 1996). The TMMS has three subscales: 1) attention to mood, 2) clarity in discrimination of feelings, and 3) mood repair. In the present study the internal consistencies (alpha) for attention to mood, clarity in the discrimination of feelings, and mood repair was .88, .91, and .86, respectively. The Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS) was used to measure Alexithymia (Bagby, Parker, & Taylor, 1994). Only subscales from the TAS determined to be most relevant to decisionmaking style were included in the survey, totaling to 12 items. The TAS subscales selected for the survey were 1) difficulty identifying feelings and 2) difficulty describing feelings. In the present study the internal consistencies (alpha) for difficulty identifying feelings and difficulty describing feelings was .86 and .83 respectively. The Five Facet Mindfulness Inventory (FFMI) was used to measure mindfulness (Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006). Only FFMI subscales determined to be most relevant to decision-making style were included in the survey, totaling to 15 items, The FFMI subscales included in the survey were 1) non-judging of inner experience and 2) non-reactivity to inner experience. In the present study the internal consistencies (alpha) for non-judging of inner experience and non-reactivity to inner experience were .91 and .85, respectively. Evaluation of Intuition 18 Three separate scales measured outcome-related variables. The first scale used to measure outcome-related variable was the 10-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965). In the present study the internal consistency (alpha) for the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale was .89. The second scale used to measure outcome-related variables was the 5-item Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWL) (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985). In the present study the internal consistency (alpha) for the Satisfaction with Life Scale was .89. The third scale used to measure outcome-related variable was the 20-item trait version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970). In the present study the internal consistency (alpha) for the STAI was .91. The 60-item NEO-Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) was used to measure features of personality (Costa & McCrae, 1985). The NEO-FFI has five subscales: 1) openness to experience, which includes traits like having wide interests, being imaginative, or insightful, 2) conscientiousness, which includes traits like organized, thorough, and prepared, 3) extraversion, which includes traits like talkative, energetic, and assertive, 4) agreeableness, which includes traits like traits like sympathetic, kind, and affectionate and 5) neuroticism, which includes traits like tense, moody, and anxious. In the present study the internal consistencies (alpha) for openness to experience, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism was .78, .88, .86, .82, and .89, respectively. Finally, the Cognitive Appraisal Style Questionnaire (CAS), which includes 12 scenarios, each with 4 corresponding items, totaling to 48 items, was used to measure appraisal style (David, Kirby, & Smith, 2007). The CAS predicts characteristic appraisals based on personality. The CAS, as used in the present study, has four subscales: 1) problem-focused coping, which measures an individual’s belief that he/she can improve his/her current situation, 2) emotion- Evaluation of Intuition 19 focused coping, which measures an individual’s belief that he/she can accept his/her current situation, 3) motivational relevance, which measures an individual’s belief about the relative importance of his/her current situation, and 4) motivational congruence, which measures an individual’s belief about the desirability of his/her current situation (David, Kirby, & Smith, 2007). In the present study the internal consistencies (alpha) for emotion-focused coping, problem-focused coping, motivation relevance, and motivational congruence was .88, .89, .90, and .73, respectively. Results and Discussion Decision-Making Style Scales Intuitiveness and deliberativeness subscales were analyzed separately to determine intercorrelation among the four decision-making scales and our brief measure. Results for intuitiveness are reported in Table 1. From Table 1, we can see that the intuitiveness subscales were highly intercorrelated with one another. These findings suggest that intuitiveness is conceptualized similarly across scales. Additionally, Table 1 also demonstrates the especially high correlation between the four scales and our brief intuitiveness measure, suggesting that our derived scale is a reliable representation of intuitiveness. Results for deliberativeness are reported in Table 2. From Table 2, we can see that most of the deliberativeness subscales were highly intercorrelated with one another and our brief deliberativeness scale, except for the REI’s rationality subscale. These findings suggest that deliberativeness is conceptualized similarly across most scales with the exception of the REI, which resulted in notably weaker correlation. Additionally, these findings demonstrate the reliability of our derived scale as a dependable representation of deliberativeness. The success of our brief intuitiveness and deliberativeness Evaluation of Intuition 20 scale justifies its use in study two, while the implications of the intercorrelation among the four measures of decision-making style are further discussed in the general discussion section. Subscales were also analyzed together to determine intercorrelation among the constructs of intuitiveness and deliberativeness (-.342**) and reported in Table 3. From Table 3, we can see that intuitiveness and deliberativeness is significantly negatively correlated, suggesting that intuitiveness and deliberativeness, though not polar opposites, are somewhat distinct dimensions. These findings are critical to the analyses used in study two. Correlates of Intuitiveness and Deliberativeness Correlations among our brief intuitiveness and deliberativeness scale and suspected correlates were analyzed to determine the relationship between decision-making style and various factors. Results related to mindfulness, Alexithymia, and emotional intelligence are reported in Table 4. Reliable effects were observed for the FFMI subscale: non-judging of inner experience, the FMMI subscale: non-reactivity to inner experience, the TAS subscale: difficulty identifying feelings, and the TMMS subscale: attention to mood, while the TAS subscale: difficulty describing feelings, the TMMS subscale: mood repair, and the TMMS subscale: clarity in discrimination of feelings did not yield any significant effects. Significant positive associations between deliberativeness and the FFMI subscales: nonjudging of inner experience (.138*) and non-reactivity to inner experience (.394**), as well as the significant negative association of intuitiveness with the FFMI subscale: non-reactivity to inner experience (-.127*) confirmed our expectation that mindfulness would be positively associated with deliberativeness. The significant positive association between intuitiveness and the TAS subscale: difficulty-identifying feelings (.176*), ran counter to our expectation that intuitiveness would be negatively associated with Alexithymia. The significant positive Evaluation of Intuition 21 association between intuitiveness and the TMMS subscale: attention to mood (.465**) and the significant negative association between deliberativeness and the TMMS subscale: attention to mood (-.208**), confirmed our expectation that intuitiveness would be positively associated with emotional intelligence. Implications of these significant correlations and speculation about the insignificant correlations are explored further in the general discussion section. Results related to outcome-related variables, personality, and appraisal style are reported in Table 5. Reliable effects were observed for NEO-FFI subscale: neuroticism, the NEO-FFI subscale: openness to experience, the NEO-FFI subscale: agreeableness, and the NEO-FFI subscale: conscientiousness, while the STAI, SWL, Rosenberg’s Self-Esteem Scale, NEO-FFI subscale: extroversion, the CAS subscale: emotion-focused coping potential, the CAS subscale: problem-focused coping potential, the CAS subscale: motivational relevance, and the CAS subscale: motivational congruence did not yield any significant effects. Implications of the significant associations between intuitiveness and the NEO-FFI subscale: neuroticism (.214**), NEO-FFI subscale: openness to experience (.170*), NEO-FFI subscale: agreeableness (-.155*), and the NEO-FFI subscale: conscientiousness (-.254**), and between deliberativeness and the NEO-FFI subscale: openness to experience (-.153*) and NEO-FFI subscale: conscientiousness (.380**), as well as speculation about insignificant correlations are explored further in the general discussion section. Study 2 The following experiment explored how individual differences in decision-making style may contribute to the nature of interpretations by assessing the impact of intuitiveness or deliberativeness on affective reasoning under conditions that either promoted or inhibited deliberative style. The experiment required participants to use affective judgment to interpret a Evaluation of Intuition 22 morally charged dilemma in the presence or absence of time pressure. The use of time pressure to manipulate intuition and deliberation is documented in the literature (Hortsman, Hausmann, & Ryf, 2010). Nature of interpretation was judged by linguistic inquiry and word count, coherence, and sophistication. Perceived quality of interpretation was also measured with questions that deduced participants’ confidence the advice they gave. We expected deliberativeness to result in impaired activity under time pressure; however we expected no effect of the time pressure manipulation on intuitiveness. In other words, we anticipated that intuitives (those who demonstrate high levels of intuitiveness) would demonstrate more complex interpretation regardless of time pressure, while deliberatives (those who demonstrate high levels of deliberativeness) in the neutral condition would demonstrate more complex interpretations than deliberatives in the forced-time condition. We also expected similar trends in terms of perceived quality of interpretations: intuitives would demonstrate high confidence in the perceived quality of their interpretations both in the neutral and forced-time conditions, while deliberatives in the neutral condition would demonstrate greater confidence in the perceived quality of their interpretations than deliberatives in the forced-time condition. Method Participants Vanderbilt undergraduates participated in a lab experiment for academic credit. Participants were 75 students, 12 of which were male and 63 of which were female. The vast majority of students were Caucasian with a mean age of 20 years. 33 students took the SAT and had an average composite score of 1409 placing them in the 95th percentile, while 43 took the ACT with an average composite score of 31 placing them in the 97th percentile. Evaluation of Intuition 23 Procedure Participants completed the experiment on the computer. The experiment was divided into two parts: scenario-based questions and survey-based questions. First, participants were instructed to read and familiarize themselves with the scenario. The scenario, a moral judgment task adapted from Kohlberg’s moral interview was as follows: Judy was a twelve-year-old girl. Her mother promised her that she could go to a special rock concert coming to their town if she saved up from baby-sitting and lunch money to buy a ticket. She managed to save up the sixty dollars the ticket cost plus another five dollars. But then her mother changed her mind because she found out that the kids were going to be unsupervised at the concert and she became very worried about the types of people who were going to be there. She told Judy that it would be better to spend the money on school supplies. Judy was disappointed and decided to go to the concert anyway. She bought a ticket and told her mother that she had only been able to save five dollars. That Saturday she went to the performance and told her mother that she was spending the day with a friend. A week passed without her mother finding out. Judy then told her older sister, Louise, that she had gone to the concert and had lied to her mother about it. Louise wonders whether to tell their mother what Judy did (Kohlberg, 1963). After reading the scenario participants responded to four open-ended scenario-based questions while imagining that Louise, Judy's older sister, was their good friend and had come to them for advice: 1) What would you advise Louise to do, and why? Please explain your reasons in as much detail as possible. 2) Even though you might have mentioned them already, please list the reasons why Evaluation of Intuition 24 Louise should tell her mother. Please list as many reasons as you can. 3) Even though you might have mentioned them already, please list the reasons why Louise should not tell her mother. Please list as many reasons as you can. 4) If you were Louise, would you do anything different than what you've advised Louise to do and why? Please explain in as much detail as possible. The manner in which participants responded to the open-ended questions was dictated by their condition assignment. In the time-pressure condition, participants were only given one minute to respond to each question, with a total of four minutes to answer four questions. In the neutral condition, participants were not given any explicit instructions and therefore, answered the questions at their own pace. Following the open-ended questions, the procedure for participants in both conditions was identical. First, participants respond to close-ended, scenariobased questions of confidence, and then to a short survey that was unrelated to the scenario. Measures Measures for the scenario-based portion of the experiment were used to determine complexity of interpretation and perceived quality of interpretation, while measures used for the survey-based portion of the experiment were used to determine decision-making style verify construct validity. Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC). Open-ended responses were run through the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC) analysis to determine the complexity of interpretations (Pennebaker, Chung, Ireland, Gonzales, & Booth, 2007). LIWC is a computerized text analysis tool that analyzes the various emotional, cognitive, and structural components present in individuals’ verbal and written speech samples. The application uses an internal default dictionary to group words that tap into a particular domain, such as causative words, Evaluation of Intuition 25 within a target text. Of all the domains LIWC offers for analysis, we selected those that deduce complexity and depth of cognitive engagement. These domains included: 1) total word count, 2) words per sentence, 3) words related to cognitive mechanisms, and 4) the various subcategories of words related to cognitive mechanisms: a) insight words (e.g., think, know, consider, etc.), b) causation words (e.g., because, effect, hence, etc.), c) discrepancy words (e.g., should, would, could, etc), d) tentative words (e.g., maybe, perhaps, guess, etc.), e) certainty words (e.g., Always, never, etc.), f) inhibition words (e.g., block, constrain, stop, etc.), g) inclusive words (and, with, include, etc.), and h) exclusion words (but, without, exclude, etc.). Because we were interested in the complexity of participants’ interpretations we not only reviewed the total word count, but also looked at the number of words per sentence, based on the assumption that longer sentences should reflect more complicated thinking. Additionally, we focused on words reflecting cognitive processes in order assess nuance and depth of interpretation. The LIWC has been used on a variety of different text sources and extensively validated by its developers (Pennebaker et al., 2007). Coherence and Sophistication. Original coding schemes for coherence and sophistication were also used to measure the complexity of interpretations. Six research assistants coded for coherence and sophistication to evaluate the complexity of participants’ interpretations. Both measures were rated on a 7-point Likert scale in which 1=not at all, 4=moderately, and 7=extremely. Coherence was operationalized by the following questions: Is the argument well written? Does one sentence flow to the next? Were all parts of the question answered? Was the question answered in complete sentences? Modifications were made for the last sentence of each response in the forced-time condition, which may have been incomplete Evaluation of Intuition 26 due to the procedure. The average internal consistency (alpha) for coherence, across the six raters, in the present study was .87. Sophistication was also measured. Sophistication was operationalized by the following questions: How sophisticated is the argument? Is the argument nuanced? Does the argument take multiple factors or perspectives into account? Is the argument mature? The average internal consistency (alpha) for sophistication, across the six raters, in the present study was .88. Confidence Items. Additionally, confidence items were used to measure participants’ perceived quality of interpretation. After writing their open-ended responses, participants responded to the following closed-ended scenario-based confidence items using a 7-point Likert scale in which 1=not at all, 4=moderately, and 7=extremely: 1. How confident are you that what you would advise Louise to do is the best course to take in this situation? 2. If you were Louise, how sure are you about what you would do in this situation? 3. If you were Louise, how conflicted would you feel about what to do? 4. If you were Louise, how difficult would it be for you to decide what to do? 5. If you were Louise, once you had made your decision, how comfortable would you be carrying it out? Survey Measures. A short survey that included similar measures of decision-making style and correlates as the survey in study one followed the scenario-based questions. The brief intuitiveness and deliberativeness scales derived in study one were included in the present study and demonstrated internal consistencies (alpha) of .74 and .80, respectively. The TAS was included in the present study and demonstrated internal consistencies (alpha) among its three subscales: 1) identifying emotions, 2) describing emotions, and 3) externally-oriented thinking of Evaluation of Intuition 27 .79, .84, and .59, respectively. The Rosenberg self-esteem scale was also included in the present study and demonstrated an internal consistency (alpha) of .89. Finally, a 33-item scale of motivational orientation was included in the present study’s survey (Griner & Smith, 2000). The measure of motivational orientation has three subscales: 1) performance orientation, 2) learning orientation, and 3) affiliate orientation. This scale was included in the present study, but not in study one, to control for any effects of affiliative tendency on decision-making elicited by the interpersonal nature of the moral dilemma. Thus, by controlling for affiliative orientation we could assess the effects of intuitiveness more clearly. In the present study the internal consistencies (alpha) of the performance orientation subscale, learning orientation subscale, and affiliate orientation subscale was .90, .87, and .83, respectively. Results & Discussion Overview of Analyses Because intuitiveness and deliberativeness are not polar opposites, but negatively correlated (-.342) somewhat distinct dimensions, we analyzed their effects separately. Both intuitiveness and deliberativeness were measured as continuous variables on a 5-point Likert scale. Unlike with analysis of variance, the use of multiple regression allowed us to look at intuitiveness and deliberativeness as continuous scores. Measures of intuitiveness and deliberativeness were centered and the data was analyzed using a two-step hierarchical regression. The first step examined main effects of the time pressure variable and either intuitiveness or deliberativeness by using the time-pressure condition variable (absence of time pressure coded as= -1 and presence of time pressure coded as= 1) and either the intuitiveness variables or deliberativeness variables as predictors. The second step examined the interaction Evaluation of Intuition 28 between time pressure and either intuitiveness or deliberativeness by adding their cross products to the equation and yielding means representative of the interaction (Aiken & West, 1991). For cases in which the regression indicated significant main effects or interactions, representative means were computed by solving the appropriate regression equation using representative values of time pressure and either intuitiveness or deliberativeness factors. The values used were: -1 for absence of time pressure, 1 for time pressure, and plus or minus one standard deviation to denote high or low intuitiveness (st. deviation= .48) or deliberativeness (st. deviation= .59), respectively. Additionally, as noted in the methods, one issue we examined was the effect of affiliative orientation on decision-making style. Preliminary analyses revealed that affiliative orientation was not significantly correlated with intuitiveness or deliberativeness, nor was it correlated with any of the outcome- related variables analyzed in this study. Thus, in no case were the results of analyses in which affiliative orientation was included as a covariate substantively different from analyses in which the covariate was not included. Therefore, for simplicity, only the results of the analyses not involving affiliative orientation are reported. Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC) LIWC analysis yielded various significant results, trends, and interactions related to total word count, words per sentence, and use of causation words, tentative words, and exclusion words. No significant results for total cognitive mechanisms, or use of insight words, discrepancy words, certainty words, inhibition words, or inclusion words were observed. Implications of the following findings related to LIWC analysis are discussed further in the general discussion section. Evaluation of Intuition 29 Total Word Count. A main effect of time pressure on total word count, b= -.44.64, t (72) = -3.45, p < .01, was observed with representative means of 318.96 words used under time pressure and 408.24 words used in the absence of time pressure, indicating the use of fewer words under time pressure. These results were expected given that the absence of time-pressure allowed participants to write at their own pace and did not force them to cut their answers short. A main effect of deliberativeness, b= 41.99, t (72) = 2.11, p < .05, on total word count was observed. Taken together, the main effect of time pressure and deliberativeness demonstrated a significant interaction, b= -.4767, t (71) = -2.16, p < .05. To evaluate this interaction, we solved the resulting regression equation including the interaction term for representative values of both the time-pressure manipulation and the deliberativeness scale. The results of the interaction between time pressure and deliberativeness on total word count are depicted in Figure 1. From the figure, we can see that in the absence of time pressure, high deliberatives used significantly more words than low deliberatives with representative means of 474.79 words and 368.99 words, respectively. However, in the presence of time pressure the representative means of high and low deliberatives, 314.33 words and 321.05, respectively, did not vary with any significance. Overall, participants wrote more in the absence of time pressure, however, this was significantly true of high deliberatives. While these findings are in line with our prediction that time pressure would impair the complexity of highly deliberative participants, we did not expect high deliberativeness to be an advantage in the absence of time pressure. Rather, we predicted no effect of deliberativeness in the absence of time pressure. Additionally, a tendency for high intuitives to use more overall words than low intuitives, b= 44.23, t (72) = 1.65, p= .10, was also observed, with estimated representative means of 384.87 words for high intuitives and 343.44 words for low intuitives. This tendency did Evaluation of Intuition 30 not interact with time pressure, b= -4.85, t (71) < 1, ns. Furthermore, the tendency for high intuitives to use more words than low intuitives lends support for our hypothesis that intuitiveness predicts greater complexity of interpretation regardless of time pressure. Words per sentence. A tendency for high deliberatives to use more words per sentence than low deliberatives, b= 1.82, t (72) = 1.82, p= .07, was observed with representative means of 21.44 words per sentence for high deliberatives and 19.30 words per sentence for low deliberatives. No effects of time pressure b= -.02, t (72) < 1,ns, or intuitiveness, b= -.06, t (72) < 1, ns, on use of words per sentence were observed. These findings run counter to our hypotheses, suggesting that deliberativeness is associated with more complex thinking, regardless of timepressure. Given our original predictions, we would have expected to see this association with intuitiveness not deliberativeness. Causation words. Main effects of intuitiveness on use of causation words, b= -.74, t (72) ƒ= -2.72, p < .01, were observed with representative means of 4.09 causation words used for high intuitives and 4.81 causation words used for low intuitives. There was no main effect of time pressure on use of causation words, b= .16, t (72) <1, ns, nor was there an interaction between time pressure and intuitiveness, b= -.58, t (71) <1, ns. Additionally, a tendency for high deliberatives to use fewer causation words than low deliberatives, b= -.41, t (72) = -1.73, p= .09, was also observed and did not interact with time pressure, b= .05 t (71) <1, ns. Overall, high intuitives used significantly fewer causation words than low intuitives, while high deliberatives demonstrated a tendency to use fewer causation words than low deliberatives. As an indication of complexity, the findings related to causation words run counter to our hypotheses. Whereas we expected intuitiveness would be associated with more complex interpretations, and therefore greater use of causation words, it appears that low intuitives used significantly more causation Evaluation of Intuition 31 words than high intuitives. Additionally, the tendency for low deliberatives to use more causation words than high deliberatives also runs counter to our hypotheses in a similar manner. Tentative Words. A main effect of deliberativeness on the use of tentative words, b= .54, t (72) = -2.41, p < .05, was observed with representative means of 5.30 tentative words for high deliberatives and 5.94 tentative words for low deliberatives. A tendency to use more tentative words in the presence of time pressure, b= .23, t (72) = 1.74, p= .09, was also observed with representative means of 4.85 tentative words used under time pressure and 4.39 tentative words used in the absence of time pressure. Overall, these findings indicate that high deliberatives use significantly fewer tentative words than low deliberatives, while both intuitives and deliberatives demonstrate a tendency to use more tentative words in the presence of time pressure. As an indication of decreased complexity, these findings related to tentative words, validate the time pressure manipulation, suggesting that it was sufficient enough to disrupt conscious deliberation, such that the interpretations of both intuitives and deliberatives were worded tentatively, as opposed to concretely. However, the effect of time pressure was somewhat unexpected, because we would not have expected intuitives to be affected. Additionally, we did not expect deliberativeness to predict complexity of interpretation regardless of time pressure. Rather, we expected an interaction in which complexity of interpretations would be associated with deliberativeness in the absence of time pressure, but would weaken in the presence of time pressure; however, there was no evidence that time pressure hurt deliberatives complexity of interpretation. Exclusion Words. A tendency for high intuitives to use more exclusion words than low intuitives, b= .37, t (72) = 1.69, p= .10, was observed, with estimated representative means of 3.57 exclusion words for high intuitives and 3.21 exclusion words for low intuitives. There were Evaluation of Intuition 32 no effects of time pressure, b= -.06, t (72) < 1, ns, or deliberativeness, b= .01 t (72) <1, ns, on use of exclusion words. Exclusion words (such as but, although, and however) indicate indecisiveness or tentativeness, which indicates lesser complexity. This tendency runs counter to our expectation that intuitives would demonstrate greater complexity and certainty. Coherence Coded values for coherence were analyzed separately for the influence of deliberativeness and intuitiveness using the two-step hierarchical regression. Coherence scores were found to vary systematically with regard to experimentally manipulated time pressure, b= .52, t (72) = -4.37, p < .001, and deliberativeness, b= .43, t (72) = 2.09, p < .05. Beyond the significant effect of time pressure on coherence, no main effects of intuitiveness on coherence, b=.23, t (72) <1 ns, or interactions involving intuitiveness were observed. Overall, participants’ responses were more coherent in the absence of time-pressure regardless of demonstrated decision-making style, coherence was also positively associated with deliberativeness regardless of time-pressure, and intuitiveness was simply unrelated to coherence. While we expected an interaction in which coherence would be associated with deliberativeness in the absence of time pressure, but would weaken in the presence of time-pressure, there was no evidence that time pressure hurt deliberatives ability to write coherently. Implications of these findings are further explored in the general discussion section. Sophistication Coded values for sophistication were analyzed separately for deliberativeness and intuitiveness using the two-step hierarchical regression. Sophistication scores were found to vary systematically with regard to both experimentally manipulated time pressure, b= -.38, t (72) = -3.11, p < .01, and deliberativeness, b= .53, t (72) = 2.49, p < .05. However, both of these Evaluation of Intuition 33 effects were qualified by a significant interaction of deliberativeness and time pressure, b= .423, t (71) = -2.05, p < .05. To evaluate this interaction, we solved the resulting regression equation including the interaction term for representative values of both the time-pressure manipulation and the deliberativeness scale. The results of the interaction are depicted in Figure 2. From Figure 2, we can see that in the absence of time pressure, high deliberatives demonstrated significantly greater levels of sophistication than low deliberatives with representative means of 5.55 and 4.49, respectively. However, in the presence of time pressure the representative means of high and low deliberatives, 4.31 and 4.25, respectively, did not vary with any significance. Beyond the significant effect of time pressure on sophistication that was already reported with regard to the analyses involving deliberativeness, no main effects of intuitiveness on sophistication, b= .40, t (72) <1 ns, or interactions involving intuitiveness were observed. Overall, participants’ responses were more sophisticated in the absence of time-pressure, deliberativeness was positively associated with sophistication when controlling for time-pressure, and intuitiveness was unrelated to sophistication. While these findings are in line with our prediction that time pressure would impair the sophistication of highly deliberative participants, we did not expect deliberativeness to be an advantage in the absence of time pressure. Rather, we predicted no effect of deliberativeness in the absence of time pressure. Implications of these findings are further discussed in the general discussion section. Confidence Items Five closed-ended items of confidence that assessed: confidence in advised course of action, certainty about advised course of action, confliction over course of action, decision difficulty, and comfort with advised course of action were analyzed using the two-step Evaluation of Intuition 34 hierarchical regression. Reliable effects were observed for certainty about advised course of action and decision difficulty, while confidence in advised course of action, confliction over course of action, and comfort with advised course of action did not yield any significant effects. Certainty. Certainty about the advised course of action was found to vary systematically with regard to intuitiveness, b= .74, t (72) = 2.38, p < .05, but not deliberativeness, b=.18, t (72) <1 ns. High intuitives indicated significantly higher ratings of certainty than low intuitiveness with representative estimated means of 5.35 and 5.09, respectively. Overall, intuitiveness was positively associated with one’s certainty about the advised course of action regardless of time pressure. While these findings do not demonstrate any effect of deliberativeness, they confirm our expectation that intuitiveness would be positively associated with perceived quality of advice, regardless of time pressure. Though perceived quality certainly does not translate to objective quality of interpretations, these findings have interesting implications concerning decision-making confidence discussed in the following general discussion. Decision Difficulty. Decision difficulty was also found to vary systematically with regard to a significant interaction between time pressure and intuitiveness, b= -.82, t (71) = 2.26 p < .05. To evaluate this interaction, we solved the resulting regression equation including the interaction term for representative values of both the time-pressure manipulation (in which the two conditions were coded as -1 and 1 for absence and presence of time pressure, respectively) and the intuitiveness scale. For the intuitiveness scale, the equation was solved for values representing participants who were one standard deviation above and below the mean, respectively (Aiken & West, 1991). The resulting predicted means are depicted in Figure 3. From Figure 3, we can see that low intuitives provided significantly greater ratings of decision difficulty than high intuitives in the presence of time pressure, with representative Evaluation of Intuition 35 means of 4.39 and 3.20, respectively. However, in the absence of time pressure the representative means of low and high intuitives, 3.60 and 4.00, respectively, were not significantly different. These findings are consistent with our hypothesis that high intuitives would experience less difficulty deciding under time pressure, while low intuitives would experience greater difficulty. General Discussion Implications of the results of both studies inform our current understanding of decisionmaking style. Study one provided us with a conceptual understanding of intuitiveness and deliberativeness and allowed us to begin identifying key, dispositional features associated with decision-making style, while study two furthered our understanding of intuition by allowing us to identify key, features of intuitiveness and deliberativeness in practice. Because both the construct of intuitiveness and deliberativeness were found to be similarly conceptualized and consistently measured across all four decision-making style scales, we were able to use our brief, derived measure to assess the relationship of various measures and individual differences in decisionmaking style. The following discussion will examine the collective results of both studies and draw connections within and between both sets of analyses. Major Findings and Implications Treatment of Emotional Signals. The relative treatment of emotional signals by intuitives and deliberatives—where intuitives pay greater attention to and use significantly more affective cues to inform their judgments, and deliberatives pay less attention to and use significantly less affective cues to inform their judgments—is a defining feature that differentiates decision-making style. For example, attention to mood, a subscale of our measure of emotional intelligence (TMMS), was found to be significantly positively associated with Evaluation of Intuition 36 intuitiveness and significantly negatively associated with deliberativeness, while non-judging of inner experience and non-reactivity of inner experience, subscales of our measure of mindfulness (FFMQ), were found to be significantly positively associated with deliberativeness. However, while intuitives allot greater attention to their emotions, there is evidence to suggest that they are less adept at interpreting emotional signals, indicated by a significant positive association between difficulty identifying feelings, a subscale of our measure of Alexithymia (TAS), and intuitiveness. When this association is considered in conjunction with the significant negative association between non-reactivity to inner experience, a subscale of the FFMQ, and intuitiveness, these results suggest that intuitives are not only less adept at interpreting emotions, but also at regulating their responses to internal emotional stimuli. Finally, while we expected to see similar associations between intuitiveness and the remaining subscales of the TMMS, mood repair and clarity of discrimination of feelings, neither demonstrated significant association with intuitiveness nor deliberativeness, suggesting that the tendency for intuitives to pay greater attention to emotional signals may be lacking in overall utility. Further, while we expected emotional intelligence to provide intuitives with the advantage of judgments enriched with useful emotional cues, it appears that without the faculty to use these emotional cues in meaningful ways, intuitiveness may, in fact, hinder the deliberative process, while deliberativeness seems to provide an unanticipated, defensive advantage. In light of these results, we might speculate that deliberativeness, by nature of inattention to emotional signals, protects against the inclusion of mood in the decision-making process, since neither intuitives nor deliberatives demonstrate significant ability to utilize emotional cues. Evaluation of Intuition 37 Associations among decision-making style and personality may also reflect the relationship between use of emotional signals and intuitiveness and deliberativeness. Significant positive associations of intuitiveness with neuroticism and openness to experience, and significant negative associations of intuitiveness with agreeableness and conscientiousness were observed. While we may have expected characteristics of openness to experience, such as insightfulness, to be positively correlated with intuitiveness and negatively correlated with deliberativeness, we did not anticipate an association between intuitiveness and characteristics of neuroticism, such as moodiness. However, in light of the recent discussion, perhaps this association of intuitiveness with neuroticism is related to the association between intuitiveness and awareness of mood despite the absence of an association between intuitiveness and the ability to utilize emotional signals. Without utility, intuitives’ awareness of mood may translate to stable moodiness or increased feelings of anxiety that are characteristic of neuroticism. Similarly, while we might have expected characteristics of conscientiousness, such as being thorough, to be negatively associated with intuitiveness and positively associated with deliberativeness, we did not anticipate a negative association between intuitiveness and characteristics of agreeableness, such as being sympathetic. Perhaps this association is also related to the relationship between intuitiveness and awareness of emotional signals in spite of the relationship between intuitiveness and difficulty identifying feelings, such that perpetual awareness of one’s own moods without the faculty to interpret or manage their effects may interact with ones ability to demonstrate sympathy towards others. Whether or not these effects could have reinforced themselves over time to become stable facets of personality is debatable; however, speculations about the relationship between personality factors and use of emotional signals seem plausible in light of the current findings. Evaluation of Intuition 38 While the association between intuitiveness and neuroticism certainly provides a rationale for the expectation of a relationship between state-trait anxiety and intuitiveness, especially at the state level, no significant effects were observed. Similarly, significant effects were neither observed for satisfaction with life nor for self-esteem, suggesting that decisionmaking style has insignificant effects on outcome-related variables that indicate at quality of life. Additionally, no effects were observed for appraisal style. While we expected intuitiveness to be associated with emotion-focused coping potential, such that intuitives would appraisal their ability to emotionally adjust to their circumstances as high, the insignificance of this effect may be related to the aforementioned association between intuitiveness and awareness of mood yet insignificant ability regulate and repair mood. In light of this important finding and its relevance to various associations, we might now expect emotion-focused coping potential to be negatively associated with intuitiveness, given that intuitives appear to be demonstrating a pattern in which they are aware of their feelings, but unable to interpret or utilize them with any significance. In sum, while intuitives may be more aware of their emotions than deliberatives, there is no evidence to suggest that this awareness informs decision-making in an advantageous way; rather, there is evidence to suggest intuitives have difficulty interpreting, regulating, and utilizing their emotions, implying that deliberatives may possess a protective advantage that keeps them from becoming aware of moods for which they lack the capacity to manage. Nature of Interpretation. The nature of intuitive and deliberative interpretations is also a defining feature that differentiates decision-making style. Analysis of intuitives’ interpretations reveled a significant negative correlation between intuitiveness and use of causative words, a tendency for intuitives to use more exclusion words, and various insignificant results related to other measures of complexity, all of which were not qualified by any significant interaction of Evaluation of Intuition 39 intuitiveness and time pressure. Because use of causative words is associated with greater complexity and use of exclusion words is associated with reduced complexity, these findings establish the interpretations of high intuitives as significantly less complex than the interpretations of low intuitives. Thus, we might expect to see more straightforward and simple answers from high intuitives. These findings suggest that intuitiveness may present a disadvantage when it comes to decision-making, such that is based on interpretations that may be too superficial to yield favorable results. On the other hand, analysis of deliberatives’ interpretations revealed significant associations among deliberativeness and various measures of complexity, both in the presence and absence of time pressure. For example, in the absence of time pressure, deliberativeness was significantly positively associated with both sophistication and total word count, demonstrating that conditions that enabled deliberativeness resulted in more complex interpretations. At the same time, conditions that inhibited deliberativeness, namely time pressure, also yielded significant associations between deliberativeness and complexity. For example, deliberativeness demonstrated significant positive association with coherence and significant negative association with use tentative words regardless of time-pressure. It seems likely that these relationships may be related to the significant positive association between deliberativeness and words per sentence, such that deliberatives were able to overcome the impairment of time pressure by sacrificing their total word count, for fewer, more well-developed, concrete sentences. Taken as a whole, these findings suggest that deliberativeness may possess a certain advantage of resiliency that intuitiveness lacks. In fact, the only case in which intuitives demonstrated significant desirable effects and deliberatives did not, were related to ease arriving at a decision, indicated by a significant Evaluation of Intuition 40 negative association between intuitiveness and decision difficulty, and confidence regarding the decision, indicated by a significant positive association between intuitiveness and certainty of advised course of action. However, because these measures are intended to assess perceived quality of interpretation, as opposed to objective quality, these findings simply suggest that intuitives experience considerably greater, yet unsubstantiated, confidence in their choices than deliberatives. Furthermore, not only is this confidence unsubstantiated, but also significantly undermined by poor efficacy, demonstrated by the negative association between intuitiveness and complexity of interpretation. Contrastingly, the positive association between deliberativeness and complexity of interpretation provides grounds for deliberatives to exhibit confidence; however, neither ease arriving at a decision nor confidence regarding the decision was significantly associated with deliberativeness, suggesting that deliberatives tend to underestimate their efficacy. Aforementioned associations between decision-making style and various dispositional measures may explain these differences in confidence. For example, the tendency for intuitives to experience overconfidence may also be explained by their use of emotional signals. While intuitives demonstrate a significant association with attention to mood, they also demonstrate a significant negative association with non-reactivity to inner experience and a significant association with difficulty describing feelings. Taken together, these findings suggest that when intuitives experience emotions after making a decision, they are more prone to react to those emotions in spite of the difficulty they experience identifying them. Furthermore, the significant association between intuitiveness and neuroticism may make intuitives more apt to label their feelings as confidence and ease than doubt and difficulty, as a mechanism of protecting themselves and reducing their characteristic feelings of anxiety. Yet, this overestimation of Evaluation of Intuition 41 confidence could have significant negative implications the outcome of a decision is unfavorable, especially because intuitives do not appear to posses the ability to repair their moods, regulate their affect, or adjust their expectations with any significance. Similarly, the tendency for deliberatives to underestimate their efficacy may be related to use of emotional signals and personality. Significant negative correlation between deliberativeness and attention to mood, suggest that deliberatives may lack the awareness to accurately report feelings of confidence, since they are less prone to attend to their moods. Additionally, significant positive correlations between deliberativeness and non-judging of inner experience and non-reactivity to inner experience, imply that even if deliberatives possessed awareness of mood they prevent themselves from analyzing or reacting to these feelings. Furthermore, this pattern in use of emotional signals may be reinforced or qualified by the significant positive association between conscientiousness and deliberativeness, such that deliberatives are significantly more likely to prepare themselves for the worst. These speculations, though most likely less innocuous than the intuitive tendency to overestimate confidence that is unjustified, suggest that deliberatives may demonstrate unnecessary prudence given the significant association of their interpretations with complexity. In sum, the stable relationship between deliberativeness and increased complexity indicates that deliberativeness may be a more adaptive and advantageous mechanism for decision-making than previously expected. Deliberatives significantly outperformed intuitives in the absence of time pressure, demonstrating greater complexity of interpretation. Even in cases where deliberatives were unable to overcome the effect of time pressure, their performance was simply on par with the performance of intuitives, never inferior. Furthermore, deliberatives demonstrations of confidence, though somewhat inaccurate, pose less threat than intuitives Evaluation of Intuition 42 demonstrations of confidence, providing additional support that deliberativeness is a more durable and beneficial mechanism for decision-making. Limitations A notable limitation of this study was that the advice given by participants could not be analyzed for objective quality. While LIWC analyses, and coding for coherence and sophistication indicated an association between intuitiveness and decreased complexity as well as an association between deliberativeness and increased complexity, complexity is simply an indication of, but does not equate with, objective quality. By determining the objective quality of the advice given in the moral dilemma, one could indicate direct relationships between individual differences in decision-making style and decision-making quality. The implications of these associations would be both interesting and of significant value to the current literature on decision-making, emotion, and personality. Another limitation of this research may have been the robustness of the confidence items. The fact that intuitiveness demonstrated significant associations with certainty and decision difficulty; however no associations among intuitiveness and confidence in advised course of action, confliction over course of action, or comfort with advised course of action were observed calls into questions the robustness of the items as they are intended to measure slight variations in what is more or a less a larger domain of perceived quality. This discrepancy, suggests that the effects may be fragile or specific and perhaps not that indicative or broad. Future Directions Many of the aforementioned findings are deserving of further attention and examination. For example, explanations for why the REI rationality subscale demonstrated notably weaker intercorrelation with the subscales of deliberativeness than any other deliberative scale may Evaluation of Intuition 43 inform the literature of the REI’s efficacy considering that it is currently one of the most widely used measures of decision-making. Additionally, given the observed positive association between neuroticism and intuitiveness, the relationship between intuitiveness and state-trait anxiety, which was insignificant in the present research, probably deserves re-examination. Most importantly, the fact that many of our predictions about intuitiveness were unfounded, suggests that the relationship between emotion and intuition deserves considerable attention. Specifically, assuming that the robustness of the confidence items did not present significant limitation, the fact that intuitives were more apt to label their emotions positively, as confidence and ease, than negatively, as doubt and difficulty, than deliberatives in spite of the significant association between intuitiveness and difficulty identifying feelings deserves greater attention. One explanation might be that intuitives genuinely experience more positive emotions after making decision, because they are able to decide and resume whatever they were doing prior to the choice with considerable speed. At the same time, if this is the case intuitives probably would have had to have been in a positive mood prior to the choice to demonstrate positive mood after the choice. While this does little to account for intuitives who may have been in a negative mood prior to the task, an experiment that assesses confidence in decision-making and includes an emotion manipulation in which intuitives are made to feel positively or negatively prior to the task, may provide greater insight. In conclusion, while the combination of both studies shed light on the nature of intuitiveness and deliberativeness, they also raised at least as many questions about the nature of these individual differences as they resolved in the process. Evaluation of Intuition 44 References Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Baer, B., Smith, G., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 27- 45. Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D., & Taylor, G. J. (1994). The twenty-item toronto alexithymia scaleI. item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 23- 32. Betsch, C. (2008). Chronic preferences for intuition and deliberation in decision-making: lessons learned about intuition from an individual differences approach. In Plessner, Henning; Betsch, Cornelia; Betsch, Tilmann (Ed.), Intuition in judgment and decision-making. (pp. 231-248). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Betsch, C. & Iannello, P. (2010). Measuring individual differences in intuitive and deliberative decision-making styles. In Glöckner, Andreas, Witteman, Cilia (Ed.). Foundations for tracing intuition: Challenges and methods. (pp. 219-237). New York, NY, US: Psychology Press. Betsch, C. & Kunz, J. (2008). Individual strategy preferences and decisional fit. Journal of Behavioral Decision-making, 21, 532-555. Burns, L.R. & D’Zurilla, T.J. (1999). Individual differences in perceived information- processing styles in stress and coping situations: development and validation of the perceived modes of processing inventory. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 23, 345-371. Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116-131. Evaluation of Intuition 45 Costa, P.T., & McCrae, R.R. (1985). The neo personality inventory manual. Odessa (Fla): Psychology Assessment Resources. David, B., Kirby, L. D., & Smith, C. A. (January, 2007). Introducing a multidimensional measure of appraisal style. 8th Annual meeting of the Society of Personality and Social Psychology. Memphis, TN. Diener, E., Emmons, R., Larsen, R., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71-75. Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). Think different: The merits of unconscious thought in preference development and decision-making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 586–598. Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M., Nordgren, L., & van Baaren, R. (2006). On making the right choice: the deliberation-without-attention effect, Science, 311. Dijksterhuis, A. & Nordgren, L. (2009). The devil is in the deliberation: Thinking too much reduces preference consistency. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 39-46. Dijksterhuis, A. & van Olden, Z. (2006). On the benefits of thinking unconsciously: Unconscious thought can increase post-choice satisfaction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,42, 627–631. Goldman, D., Kraemer, D., & Salovey, P. (1996). Beliefs about mood moderate the relationship of stress to illness and symptom reporting. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 41, 115128. Griner, L. A., & Smith, C. A. (2000). Contributions of motivational orientation to appraisal and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 727-740. Hodgkinson, G., Langan-Fox, J., & Sadler-Smith, E. (2008). Intuition: a fundamental bridging Evaluation of Intuition 46 construct in the behavioural sciences. British Journal of Psychology, 99, 1-27. Hortsman, N., Hausmann, D., & Ryf, S. (2010). Methods for inducing intuitive and deliberative processing modes. In Glöckner, Andreas, Witteman, Cilia (Ed.). Foundations for tracing intuition: Challenges and methods. (pp. 219-237). New York, NY, US: Psychology Press. Koele, P. & Dietvorst, G. (2010). The internal validity of self-report measures for intuitive and rational decision-making style. In Glöckner, Andreas, Witteman, Cilia (Ed.). Foundations for tracing intuition: Challenges and methods. (pp. 219-237). New York, NY, US: Psychology Press. Kohlberg, L. (1963). The development of children's orientations toward a moral order: I. sequence in the development of moral thought. Vita Humana, 6, 11-33. Pacini, R. & Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of rational and experiential information processing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 972-987. Pennebaker, J.W., Chung, C.K., Ireland, M., Gonzales, A., & Booth, R.J. (2007). The development and psychometric properties if liwc2007. Liwc2007 Manual. Retrieved from: http://www.liwc.net Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Salovey, P. & Mayer, J. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 9, 185-211. Scott, S. G. & Bruce, R. A. (1995). Decision-making style: the development and assessment of a new measure. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 55, 818-831. Smith, C. A., & Kirby, L. D. (2000). Consequences require antecedents: Toward a process model Evaluation of Intuition 47 of emotion elicitation. In J. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 83-106). New York: Cambridge University Press. Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1990). Emotion and adaptation. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 609-637). New York: Guilford Press. Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. (1970). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory: stai (“self-evaluation questionnaire”). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Wilson, T. & Kraft, D. (1993). Why do I love thee? Effects of repeated introspections about a dating relationship on attitudes toward the relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 409-418. Wilson, T., Lisle, D., Schooler, J., Hodges, S., Klaaren, K., & LaFleur, S. (1993). Introspecting about reasons can reduce post-choice satisfaction. Personality and Psychology Bulletin, 19, 331-339. Wilson, T. & Schooler, J. (1991). Thinking too much: Introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 181-192. Evaluation of Intuition 48 Table 1. Intercorrelation of Intuitiveness Measures PMPI: Emotional Processing REI: Experientiali ty PID: Preference For Intuition GDMS: Intuitive Style Brief Intuitiveness Scale Pearson Correlation N Sig. (2-tailed) Pearson Correlation N Sig. (2-tailed) Pearson Correlation N Sig. (2-tailed) Pearson Correlation N Sig. (2-tailed) Pearson Correlation N Sig. (2-tailed) PMPI: Emotional Processin g 1 REI: Experiential ity GDMS: Brief Intuitiv Intuitivenes e Style s scale .527** PID: Preferenc e for Intuition .584** .741** .823** 276 276 0 1 275 0 .565** 240 0 .761** 276 0 .729** 276 0 .584** 276 275 0 1 240 0 .700** 276 0 .760** 275 0 .741** 275 0 .761** 275 240 0 1 275 0 .848** 240 0 .823** 240 0 .729** 240 0 .760** 240 240 0 1 276 0 276 0 275 0 240 0 .527** .565** **. Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed). *. Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed). .700** .848** 276 Evaluation of Intuition 49 Table 2. Intercorrelation of Deliberativeness Measures PMPI: Rational Processin g PMPI: Rational Processing Pearson Correlation N Sig. (2-tailed) REI: Pearson Rationality Correlation N Sig. (2-tailed) PID: Pearson Preference For Correlation Deliberation N Sig. (2-tailed) GDMS: Pearson Rational Style Correlation N Sig. (2-tailed) Brief Pearson Deliberativene Correlation ss Scale N Sig. (2-tailed) REI: PID: Rationalit Preference y for Deliberati on .207** .613** GDMS: Brief Rationa Deliberativ l Style e-ness scale .644** .677** 276 0.001 1 276 0 .333** 276 0 .272** 276 0 .416** 276 0.001 .613** 276 276 0 1 276 0 .756** 276 0 .834** 276 0 .644** 276 0 .272** 276 276 0 1 276 0 .874** 276 0 .677** 276 0 .416** 276 0 .834** 277 277 0 1 276 0 276 0 276 0 277 0 1 276 .207** .333** **. Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed). *. Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed). .756** .874** 277 Evaluation of Intuition 50 Table 3. Intercorrelation of Intuitive and Deliberative Measures. PMPI: REI: Rational Rationality Processing PMPI: Emotional Processing REI: Experientiality PID: Preference For Intuition GDMS: Intuitive Style Brief Intuitiveness Scale Pearson Correlation N Sig. (2-tailed) Pearson Correlation N Sig. (2-tailed) Pearson Correlation N Sig. (2-tailed) Pearson Correlation N Sig. (2-tailed) Pearson Correlation N Sig. (2-tailed) -.222** -.330** PID: Preference for Deliberation -.267** 276 .000 -.269** 276 .000 -.026 276 .000 -.280** 276 .000 -.183** 276 .000 -.300** 276 .000 -.074 276 .661 -.113 276 .000 -.036 276 .002 -.058 276 .000 -.077 275 .220 -.243** 275 .061 -.206** 275 .557 -.287** 275 .341 -.208** 275 .202 -.310** 240 .000 -.172 240 .001 -.227** 240 .000 -.209** 240 .001 -.171** 240 .000 -.251** 276 .004 276 .000 276 .000 276 .004 276 .000 **. Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed). *. Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed). GDMS: Rational Style Brief Deliberative -ness scale -.259** -.319** Evaluation of Intuition 51 Table 4. Correlates of Intuitiveness and Deliberativeness: Mindfulness, Alexithymia, and Emotional Intelligence FFMQ: Non-Judging of Inner Experience FFMQ: Non-Reactivity to Inner Experience TAS: Difficulty Indentifying Feelings TAS: Difficulty Describing Feelings TMMS: Attention to Mood TMMS: Mood Repair TMMS: Clarity in Discrimination of Feelings Pearson Correlation Sig. (2-Tailed) N Pearson Correlation Sig. (2-Tailed) N Pearson Correlation Sig. (2-Tailed) N Pearson Correlation Sig. (2-Tailed) N Pearson Correlation Sig. (2-Tailed) N Pearson Correlation Sig. (2-Tailed) N Pearson Correlation Sig. (2-Tailed) N Brief Intuitiveness Scale .077 .202 276 -.127* .042 257 .176* .015 189 -.013 .854 189 .465** .000 189 .041 .578 187 -.085 .248 185 *. Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed). **. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). Brief Deliberativeness Scale .138* .022 276 .394** .000 257 -.072 .323 189 -.039 .596 189 -.208** .004 189 -.052 .483 187 .124 .094 185 Evaluation of Intuition 52 Table 5. Correlates of Intuitiveness and Deliberativeness: Outcome-Related Variables, Personality, and Appraisal Style Brief Intuitiveness Scale STAI: State-Trait Pearson Correlation .103 Anxiety Inventory Sig. (2-Tailed) .171 N 178 Rosenberg Self-Esteem Pearson Correlation -.124 Scale Sig. (2-Tailed) .100 N 178 Satisfaction with Life Pearson Correlation .029 Scale Sig. (2-Tailed) .698 N 178 NEO-FFI: Neuroticism Pearson Correlation .214** Sig. (2-Tailed) .004 N 176 NEO-FFI: Extraversion Pearson Correlation .074 Sig. (2-Tailed) .329 N 176 NEO-FFI: Openness to Pearson Correlation .170* Experience Sig. (2-Tailed) .024 N 176 NEO-FFI: Pearson Correlation -.155* Agreeableness Sig. (2-Tailed) .040 N 176 NEO-FFI: Pearson Correlation -.254** Conscientiousness Sig. (2-Tailed) .001 N 176 CAS: Emotion-Focused Pearson Correlation .042 Coping Potential Sig. (2-Tailed) .595 N 159 CAS: Problem-Focused Pearson Correlation -.038 Coping Potential Sig. (2-Tailed) .634 N 159 CAS: Motivational Pearson Correlation .060 Relevance Sig. (2-Tailed) .449 N 159 CAS: Motivational Pearson Correlation .153 Congruence Sig. (2-Tailed) .054 N 159 *. Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed). **. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). Brief Deliberativeness Scale -.027 .717 178 .030 .694 178 -.015 .841 178 -.029 .707 176 -.110 .146 176 -.153* .042 176 -.054 .477 176 .380** .000 176 -.033 .683 159 .008 .917 159 .023 .771 159 -.029 .720 159 Evaluation of Intuition 53 Figure 1. Effect of Interaction between Time Pressure and Deliberativeness on Total Word Count 500 Average Total Word Count 450 474.79 400 350 321.05 368.99 300 314.33 250 200 Low Deliberatives 150 High Deliberatives 100 50 0 No Time Pressure Time Pressure Evaluation of Intuition 54 Figure 2. Effect of Interaction between Time Pressure and Deliberativeness on Sophistication 6 5.55 5 4.31 Sophistication 4.49 4 4.25 3 Low Deliberatives 2 High Deliberatives 1 0 No Time Pressure Time-Pressure Evaluation of Intuition 55 Figure 3. Effect of Interaction between Time Pressure and Intuitiveness on Difficulty Deciding 5 4.39 Difficulty Deciding 4 4 3.6 3.2 3 2 Low Intuitives High Intuitives 1 0 No Time Pressure Time Pressure Evaluation of Intuition 56 Appendix A Brief Intuitiveness and Deliberativeness Scale Items Brief Intuitiveness Scale (N=9) 1) When making decisions, I rely upon my instincts. (GDMS: Intuitive Style subscale, Item 1) 2) When making decisions, I tend to rely on my intuition. (GDMS: Intuitive Style subscale, Item 2) 3) To cope, I usually go with my instincts rather than trying to reason things out. (PMPI: Emotional Processing subscale, Item 1) 4) My feelings usually determine how I will cope. (PMPI: Emotional Processing subscale, Item 2) 5) I like to rely on my intuitive impressions. (REI: Experientiality subscale, Item 1) 6) I believe in trusting my hunches. (REI: Experientiality subscale, Item 3) 7) I listen carefully to my deepest feelings. (PID: Preference for Intuition subscale, Item 1) 8) My feelings play an important role in my decisions. (PID: Preference for Intuition subscale, Item 5) 9) I am a very intuitive person. (PID: Preference for Intuition subscale, Item 7) Brief Deliberativeness Scale (N=9) 1) My decision-making requires careful thought. (GDMS: Rational Style subscale, Item 3) 2) I make decisions in a logical and systematic way. (GDMS: Rational Style subscale, Item 2) 3) I usually think of as many alternative ways of coping as possible before I decide what I am going to do. (PMPI: Rational Processing subscale, Item 3) 4) I usually try to get all the facts that I can before deciding how to cope. (PMPI: Rational Processing subscale, Item 5) 5) I enjoy solving problems that require hard thinking. (REI: Rationality subscale, Item 3) 6) I am much better at figuring things out logically than most people. (REI: Rationality subscale, Item 6) 7) Before making decisions I first think them through. (PID: Preference for Deliberation subscale, Item 1) 8) When I have a problem I first analyze the facts and details before I decide. (PID: Preference for Deliberation subscale, Item 7) 9) I prefer making detailed plans rather than leaving things to chance. (PID: Preference for Deliberation subscale, Item 4)