Stress Responses 1 Running head: STRESS RESPONSES Stress Responses and Worries of Women at Risk for Breast Cancer Czarina E. Sánchez Vanderbilt University Stress Responses 2 ABSTRACT The physiological impact of a breast cancer discussion between mothers and their daughters was analyzed in relation to other anxiety and concern measures. Fifty-eight mothers with varied breast cancer histories participated. Measures of general anxiety (BAI), concern about breast cancer (IES), perceived risk, and observational data were correlated to salivary cortisol and norepinephrine measured by alpha-amylase. Analysis revealed that cancer specific worries but not general anxiety were positively correlated to cortisol levels, specifically IES-intrusion subscale scores, but not IES-avoidance. Observed behavioral anxiety and avoidance during the interaction did not show significant correlations. However, exploratory analyses revealed whine/complain behavior to be positively correlated with cortisol levels. Stress Responses 3 Stress Responses and Worries of Women at Risk for Breast Cancer INTRODUCTION The relationship between stress and cancer development has not been firmly established. Large scale meta-analyses have found only a weak connection between the two. However, stress, especially in the minds of many women with breast cancer, seems to have a direct connection with the development of cancer. For example, stress was the most attributed cause of breast cancer in a survey of 400 women with the disease (Steward, Cheung, Duff, Wong, McQuestion, Cheung, et al., 2001). Because of the mixed findings in the literature with respect to the effect of psychological factors in the progression of breast cancer, it is imperative to seek out information to clarify the relationship. By better understanding stress reactivity in women in relation to a breast cancer related task, this study will shed light on this debated topic and offer some clarification to issues of the mind and body connection. The prevalence and effects of breast cancer make it important to better understand the role of emotions and psychological stress reactions in women at risk. Breast Cancer The chance for a woman in the United States of developing breast cancer during her lifetime is one in eight. Breast cancer is the most common cancer among women besides skin cancer (American Cancer Society, 2007). It is also the second leading cause of cancer death in women. In 2007, an estimated 178,480 women in the United States will be diagnosed with invasive breast cancer. Further, about 40,460 women will die from breast cancer in this year alone. The incidence of breast cancer has continually increased since 1980 although the rate of increase of new cases has leveled off in recent years. Stress Responses 4 Cancer is the abnormal growth of cells due to damaged DNA that develops from inherited abnormalities or environmental triggers. Most breast cancers begin with the development of a malignant tumor at the cells of the glands or ducts in the breast (American Cancer Society, 2007). The diagnosis of breast cancer is carried out by analysis of imaging tests and biopsies. The analysis is necessary for the complex process known as staging which means determining the size of the tumor and extent of spread of the cancer. Staging is essential because treatment options are based on the stage of the cancer. The more widespread the cancer is, the lower the relative survival rate, which can be as low as 20% for the more advanced cases. The treatment options are divided into two categories: local and systemic. Local treatment is designed to treat the cancer without affecting the rest of the body through surgery or radiation (American Cancer Society, 2007). The purpose of surgery is to directly remove the cancer cells. Surgery types range from lumpectomy which is the removal of only the tumor, to radical mastectomy, the removal of the whole breast, underarm lymph nodes, and chest muscles. These options can lead to increasing disfigurement and side effects. Radiation uses high-energy rays from external sources or from radioactive materials placed directly on the tumor to kill cancer cells. Systemic treatments are given by mouth or injected into the bloodstream to reach cancer that is more spread out in the body (American Cancer Society, 2007). Some examples of systemic treatments are chemotherapy and hormone therapy. In chemotherapy, anticancer drugs are introduced into the system to kill the cancer cells. However, because chemotherapeutic agents are designed to identify and kill all rapidly growing cells, many normal cells are also killed in the process causing side effects of fatigue, hair loss, nausea, vomiting, and sometimes permanent changes in menstrual cycle. After chemotherapy many women also experience painful sensations and some loss of physical functioning. Hormone therapy is used in cases where Stress Responses 5 estrogen is the promoter of cancel cell growth and blocking this hormone can be a treatment option. However, the treatment can cause the spread of cancer into the lining of the uterus and alterations to the woman’s cycle. It is clear from this brief summary that the diagnosis of breast cancer and the consequent treatments are accompanied by serious physical trials. However, it is not only the physical effects of breast cancer that have a dramatic influence on the women’s wellbeing. Cancer can also be accompanied by significant emotional toil. Depression and anxiety are often experienced by women faced with cancer (e.g., Epping-Jordan, Compas, & Howell, 1994). These feelings can be the result of changes in body image, the inability to work or care for the family, and fears that develop from the cancer diagnosis. Cancer is also closely associated with the threat of death. All these feelings can have a large psychological impact on a woman’s life. Risk Factors for Breast Cancer There are different risk factors for breast cancer and even though they do not mean a certainty of cancer diagnosis, they indicate an increased chance of developing the disease. Several of the risk factors cannot be changed. For example, the main risk factor is gender. Simply being a woman drastically increases the chances of breast cancer. Age is also a factor with the risk of breast cancer increasing significantly with age. About 77% of cancer diagnoses are for women over the age of 50 (American Cancer Society, 2007). The risk of breast cancer is also higher for women with a history of breast cancer in their family. A woman’s risk doubles if a first degree relative such as a mother, sister, or daughter has had breast cancer and is even greater if the cancer occurred before menopause. BRCA1 and BRCA2 are two genes have been identified to take part in the development of cancer (American Cancer Society, 2007). They are normally tumor suppressor genes but they can trigger cancer Stress Responses 6 growth when mutated. These mutations can be inherited and 5-10% of cancer cases are due to hereditary gene mutations of these genes. Women with inherited mutations have a risk of developing breast cancer as high as 80%. The gene mutations can also develop from environmental triggers. The processes however are yet to be identified. Cancer, Cancer Risk, and Stress The risk of developing cancer is two to three times greater for women with a positive history of breast cancer than for women without first degree relatives who had breast cancer (Gil, Mendez, Sirgo, Llort,Blanco, & Cortes-Funes, 2003). The increased risk due to a familial history of breast cancer can become a chronic psychological stressor. Women with a history of cancer in their families live under the constant threat of developing the disease themselves and suffering the consequences aforementioned. For example, a study by Trask, Paterson, Wang, Satoru, Milliron, and Blumberg (2001) found that two-thirds of women at high risk for breast cancer, out of a sample of 197, had cancer related worries that interfered with their lives. These women also reported higher levels of anxiety and a general decrease in mental health. Another important factor is the presence of intrusive thoughts, defined as unwanted images and strong waves of feelings in response to a specific stressor. Intrusive thoughts are characteristic of a post-traumatic stress response to a cancer diagnosis or the treatment of the disease. These uncontrollable worries are closely tied to unsuccessful efforts to avoid or suppress unwanted thoughts or images about the disease. Studies have also shown that avoidant thoughts about one’s cancer are related to poorer disease outcomes (Epping-Jordan, Compas, & Howell, 1994). These authors found that cancer patients who were high in avoidance were 2.5 times more likely to suffer a recurrence of cancer or to die from the disease one year later, even after statistically controlling for all relevant medical variables. Intrusive thoughts and avoidance Stress Responses 7 may also be important processes to study in women at risk for cancer as well as in patients who have been diagnosed with the disease. Biology of Stress In order to understand the potential role of stress in cancer, it is important to have a basic understanding of the biological processes involved in the human stress response. The body’s reaction to an acute stressor is set in order to prepare the body for an adequate response (Sapolsky, 2004). One important aspect of this response is the rapid mobilization of energy throughout the body. In response to stress, glucose production is increased while its storage in fat cells is decreased. This is done in order to make glucose a readily available energy source for the muscles needing it to react. Heart rate, breathing rate, and blood pressure also increase to transport and provide the glucose and oxygen necessary throughout the body. To best allocate these energy resources, the coordination of different systems takes place. Areas of less critical importance are inhibited so that most resources go to the muscles, heart, and brain which will coordinate the reaction to the stressor. The organs involved in digestion and reproduction receive decreased circulation and fewer materials than under normal circumstances. In this way the body focuses the energy supply on responding to the threat while long term processes are momentarily halted for the more general safety of the body. The immune system is also inhibited under stress, limiting the protection against other body aggressors. Cognitive changes are also part of the stress response. They include decreased sensitivity to pain and increased perception of the surroundings. All these reactions are known as the flight or fight response because they prepare the body to react to the stressor in either of those ways. The physiological activation of the stress response is mediated by two pathways in the body (Sapolsky, 2004). Stress Responses 8 Sympathetic Adrenal Medullary Axis. One of these pathways begins with the activation of the sympathetic branch of the autonomic nervous system. The stressor is translated as an emergency situation. The nerves of the sympathetic nervous system branch from the spinal column out to the body. The nerve endings release epinephrine and norepinephrine which are neurotransmitters that signal the organs to take the appropriate action necessary for the best use of the energy resources. Epinephrine and norepinephrine are catecholamines made in the medulla of the adrenal gland. The parasympathetic system is the other half of the autonomic nervous system and acts in opposition to the sympathetic to suppress the stress response reactions when adequate. It slows heart rate, breathing rate and directs flow to the digestive organs. Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis. The other way the body mediates the stress response is through hormones of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal axis (HPA axis). The hormones are secreted from different glands in the body and travel in the circulation to the organs where they elicit certain reactions. The hypothalamus releases hormones that signal the anterior pituitary gland to also release hormones. These in turn signal to other parts of the body such as the cortex of the adrenal gland to release other hormones that will elicit the desired response. When an individual senses a stressor, the hypothalamus at the base of the brain is activated and releases CRH (corticotropin releasing hormone) along with other hormones with similar purpose. The hormones travel in the circulatory system between the hypothalamus and pituitary and activate the anterior pituitary gland. It in turn releases ACTH (adrenocorticotropin hormone) into the blood stream. Within a few minutes ACTH reaches and activates the adrenal gland which rests on top of the kidneys. The adrenal cortex is stimulated to release cortisol. Stress Responses 9 Cortisol has similar effects to epinephrine and belongs to the category of hormones known as glucocorticoids, steroid hormones made in the adrenal cortex (Padgett &Glaser, 2003). The two systems work in combination to bring about the fight or flight response. However, the stress response is not a clear cut picture of fight or flight, as there are varied responses to stress and gender differences in the physiology and psychology of stress (Sapolsky, 2004). There are further complications even when the same pathways are activated. Any stressor can activate the same pathway, but they can be activated to different degrees and the hormones released in different proportions. Variability in the response can also be brought about by altering the hormone sensitivity of the different tissues. Furthermore, in times when the stressor can neither be fought nor escaped, damage to the body can be caused by the over and prolonged activation of the fight or flight response. Chronic stress can therefore cause problems by the chronic activation of the cardiovascular system. The decrease in resources available for the repair and growth mechanisms of the body leads to an increased likelihood of disease. The energy that is taken up by the stress response means decreased energy resources for use in other purposes. The body can be easily thrown off balance due to the instability caused by the response activation. Effect of Stress on Immune System The susceptibility to disease is also increased due to suppression of the immune response. As described in a review by Padget and Glaser (2003) the chronic activation of the stress response results in chronic production of glucocorticoids and catecholamines. In excess, these hormones interfere with normal immune cell activity. Immune cells have receptors for the neuroendocrine products, the catecholamines, and the glucocorticoids of the stress response axes: Stress Responses 10 the HPA and the SAM axes. Production of these regulates the immune response when necessary, but overproduction can lead to health problems. Stress Biology and Breast Cancer Women at increased risk for breast cancer may be most affected by the emotional and biological consequences of chronic stress. These processes have been addressed in a series of studies by Bovberg and colleagues. Gold, Zakowski, Valdimarsdottir, and Bovberg (2003) compared endocrine responses to a brief psychological stressor in women at familial risk for breast cancer and women at normal risk. Thirty-six women at normal risk and 17 women at familial risk were presented with a laboratory stressor not related to cancer, a speech task and mental arithmetic task that took about 15 minutes. The control group consisted of 30 normal risk women that did not participate in the tasks. Epinephrine, norepinephrine, and cortisol measurements were taken from their blood plasma at baseline level, immediately after the stress test, and 30 and 45 minutes after. The effect of the stress task was confirmed by their heart rate responses. The results found were that women at familial risk for breast cancer had a stronger and prolonged response post-stress task to epinephrine and cortisol as compared to normal risk women. This was measured as the change from the control group’s baseline level. A significant difference was not found in norepinephrine levels. In a second study, James, van Berge-Landry, Valdimarsdottir, Montgomery, and Bovberg (2004) provided further evidence that women at familial risk of breast cancer have an altered response to stress due to the chronic life stressor of breast cancer threat. Urinary epinephrine and norepinephrine excretion rates were measured in women at work, home and during sleep. The comparison was between 73 women with familial risk and 81 women with normal risk of breast cancer. The purpose of the study was to test the results obtained by Gold et al. in a naturalistic Stress Responses 11 setting. It was found that women at familial risk of breast cancer showed increased epinephrine at work likely due to an increased response to the daily stressors. An increase in the level from sleep to work in both epinephrine and norepinephrine was also found. This shows that women at familial risk had a stronger response to the acute stressors because of the chronic stressor of being at increased risk for breast cancer. Another study that gives further evidence to the increased stress reactivity of women with familial history of breast cancer was carried out by Valdimarsdottir, Zakowski, Gerin, Mamakos, Pickering, and Bovberg (2002). Sixteen women with family history of breast cancer and 32 women at normal risk participated in a 15 minute classic laboratory stressor. The task consisted of a 5 minute taped speech that was performed in front of an experimenter and a 5 minute arithmetic task where the participants were told they needed to work faster or harder. Seventeen women at normal risk served as controls and were assigned a non-stressful task of reading two articles and assured they would not be tested in any way. Self-reported distress was assessed before and after. Heart rate and blood pressure were measured before, during, and after the tasks. Natural killer cell activity (NKCA) measures were obtained from blood drawn before and following the tasks. NKCA is an immune measure that seems to be correlated to emotional distress. Valdimarsdottir et al. (2002) found that women participating in the stressful task experienced higher levels of distress and of those, the women with familial history experienced even higher distress levels than women at normal risk. A similar effect was found in heart rate reactivity with the women at familial risk having the highest increases. Blood pressure rose for both types of women in the stressor task showing the task did have an effect. There was no significant difference found between the two groups though. The women with familial history of Stress Responses 12 cancer did show larger increases in NKCA concentration as compared to women at normal risk who in turn had a higher concentration than women in the non-stressor task. The results indicate that the women at familial risk of breast cancer have a heightened psychobiological reactivity to the laboratory stressor. This is further support for the risk of breast cancer working as a chronic life stressor and causing changes in the body’s response to other stressors. The studies by Bovberg and colleagues are important in establishing that women at increased risk for breast cancer show a heightened physiological response to stress. This suggests that the chronic psychological stress associated with being at risk for breast cancer may contribute to greater sensitivity and reactivity to stress. These studies are limited, however, in their focus on immune system responses to the effects of stress hormones. There is presently no clear evidence that the immune system plays a central role in the regulation of the development of cancer cells. Further, these studies examined the effects of general types of stress and did not examine sources of stress that may be specifically related to the threat or risk of developing breast cancer. Breast cancer related stressors may present a particularly important set of demands to women who are at risk for the disease. Purpose of the Present Study The lack of understanding of the relationship between stress and cancer and its importance to the lives of so many women makes increasing the knowledge on the subject a pressing need. Previous research has been limited in several ways. For example, previous researchers have used laboratory stressors that are unrelated to breast cancer. The study will provide a deeper understanding on the perception of breast cancer for women at risk of breast cancer. Specifically, the proposed study will examine the woman’s biological reaction with respect to a breast cancer related stressor, a discussion of breast cancer risk between the woman Stress Responses 13 and her daughter. The effects of this potentially stressful interaction on the activation of the HPA and SAM axes will be evaluated in relation to other anxiety and worry variables. Research Questions 1. What is the relationship between the mothers’ biological stress response and their general level of anxiety? The hypothesis is that women who experience increased overall anxiety will also show an increased biological response because of an over activated stress response system. 2. What is the relationship between the mothers’ biological stress response and breast cancer specific concern? As seen in previous studies (Gold et al. 2003, James et al. 2004) women at increased risk for breast cancer have elevated responses to stressors. It is hypothesized that women with increased breast cancer concern will also have an increased stress response. For women who are more concerned about breast cancer, experiencing a breast cancer specific stressor will incur a larger stress reaction from their systems. 3. What is the relationship between the mothers’ biological stress response and their perceived breast cancer risk? Similarly as with concern, it is hypothesized that women, who perceived their risk to be higher, will also have increased biological stress response to the task, which will appear in their cortisol and alpha-amylase levels. 4. What is the relationship between the mother’s biological stress response and their observed anxiety and avoidance behavior during the breast cancer discussion? If a person is showing anxiety and distress, it would be expected that the stress will also be detected in a physiological measure. The hypothesis therefore is that women who show more anxiety and/or avoidance during the interaction will also show an increased biological stress response in their cortisol and alpha-amylase levels. Stress Responses 14 METHOD Participants This study included a total of 58 mothers with ages ranging ranged from 32 to 60 years of age with a mean of 48 and a standard deviation of 6.3. The mothers participated in the task with their daughters, whose age ranged from 11 to 32 years with a mean of 19 and a standard deviation of 5.6. This particular study however focuses only on the mothers’ data. The number of years of education for the mothers ranged from 9 to 22 with a mean of 16 years and a standard deviation of 2.2. Out of the 58 mothers, 33 had been diagnosed with breast cancer. The racial distribution of the sample based on self-report questions was as follows out of the 58 women: 41 were Caucasian (71%), 11 African American (19%), 2 other (3%), and 4 did not respond (7%). Measures Generalized Anxiety. The Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) is a widely used measure of symptoms of anxiety in non-psychiatric samples (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996). It is a self-report questionnaire which asks the participant to respond about their anxiety symptoms experienced in the past week, such as “unable to relax” and “fear of dying”. The 4-point Likert scale ranges from 0 (not at all) to 3 (severely, I could barely stand it). It consists of 21 items with higher scores indicating a greater severity of anxiety. For this study, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.86 which based on accepted levels presents a very good to excellent reliability. Avoidance and Intrusive thoughts. The Impact of Events Scale (IES) is a measure used to assess avoidance and intrusive thoughts with respect to a specific stressful event, in this case, breast cancer. The IES is a 15-item self-report questionnaire that measures current degree of impact of the specific stressful event (Horowitz, Wilner, & Alvarez, 1979). Participants were asked to indicate how frequently each item had been true with respect to breast cancer in the 7 Stress Responses 15 days preceding the interview. The responses were made on a 4-point scale ranging from not at all true to often true. The IES is subdivided into two subscales: Avoidance with eight items and a score range of 0-40 and Intrusion with seven items and a score range of 0-35. Examples for the Avoidance subscale are “I try to remove it from my memory,” “I try not to talk about it,” and “I try not to think about it.” Examples for the Intrusion subscale include “I think about it when I don’t mean to,” and “Pictures about it pop into my mind.” The IES has been widely used in the psycho-oncology literature as a measure of cancer-related anxiety. Thewes, Meiser, and Hickie, (2001) suggest good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha=0.84–0.91), and satisfactory testretest reliability (IES-Total r=0.80). For this study the Cronbach’s alpha for the IES scale of avoidance was 0.81 and for intrusion 0.75. These are adequate to good based on accepted values. Perceived Risk. Because of the lack of an established measure of perceived risk, the assessment of perceived risk was based on a self-report assessment of risk question. The participants were asked to respond to the question: “How likely are you to get breast cancer?” on a four-point Likert scale ranging from 1 very unlikely to 4 very likely. Physiological stress. Saliva samples were collected once before the mother-daughter interaction and three times after the breast cancer discussion to analyze levels of cortisol and alpha-amylase as a measure of norepinephrine. Extensive research has established that salivary levels of -amylase serve as a reliable proxy for levels of norepinephrine, which cannot be extracted from saliva (e.g., Chatterton et al., 1996; Skosink et al., 2000). Saliva collection was chosen for determination of cortisol levels because it is simple, non-aversive to the subject, and can be collected repeatedly throughout the study. The saliva samples were obtained with a salivette. Participants were asked to chew on a piece of cotton for a minute and get as much saliva into it as possible. After this, the cotton was Stress Responses 16 then placed back in the salivette. The samples were then stored at 0ºC before being sent off to Salimetrics at Pennsylvania State University to for analysis. (Details for shipping and method for conducting assays are available at the website for the laboratory at www.salimetrics.com/testing services.) These data points would allow for study of reactivity to and recovery from stress. The discussions were scheduled in the afternoon between the hours of 4 and 6:30pm to control for diurnal fluctuations in cortisol. The analysis was conducted in duplicate and the mean level of the two tests was used for the data analyses Salivary cortisol concentrations are independent of flow rate, and reflect unbound “free” cortisol levels in plasma. Because the level of cortisol in saliva is lower than level in the circulation, a standard curve developed to capture the range of values expected in plasma samples was used. The assays conducted in Dr. Granger’s lab have been designed to specifically address the following three problems that have been observed in the use of salivary cortisol analyses. First, the majority of available immunoassays for saliva cortisol are modifications of protocols developed for the use with serum/plasma. The calibrators used in those assay kits are suspended in a human serum matrix. Given that the composition of serum is markedly different from saliva, these calibrators are likely to produce results that are influenced by matrix differences. To ensure the most accurate results, this salivary immunoassay is designed using a matrix that matches saliva. Second, the level of cortisol in saliva is significantly lower than levels in the general circulation. The use of a standard curve developed to capture the range of values expected in serum/plasma samples is often not sensitive enough to capture the complete range of individual differences in the level expected in saliva. This assay is designed to capture the full range of salivary cortisol levels while using only 25 µl of saliva per test. Third, the pH of saliva is easily lowered or raised by the consumption of food or drink. Performance of immunoassays becomes Stress Responses 17 compromised as the pH of samples to be tested drops below 4. This results in artificially inflated levels. This assay system is designed to be very sensitive to the effects of interference caused by collection techniques that affect pH. In addition, a built-in pH indicator warns the user of acidic or basic samples. Behavioral observation. The communication styles were determined through behavioral observation of the taped discussion on breast cancer. The observation procedure used was based on the methods developed by Conger and colleagues in studies of families coping with stress (Reuter 1995). The mother-daughter pairs spent 15min discussing their feelings and concerns about breast cancer. The following were the questions prompting discussion for the issue of disagreement: What is the disagreement we seem to have about _______________? When do we have this disagreement and what usually happens? How do you feel about this issue? What can we do to solve this problem? For the breast cancer topic, these were the questions on the card: What kinds of feelings do we each have about breast cancer and the chance that we might get breast cancer? How often do we talk about our feelings about breast cancer? If we don’t talk about it, then why not? What prevents us from talking about it? What is it about breast cancer that has most affected our lives? What is the most emotional or difficult time in our family regarding breast cancer? Do we feel that we have any control over the chance of getting breast cancer? Mom: Do you worry about your daughter and her risk of breast cancer? Daughter: Do you worry about your mom and her risk of breast cancer? Stress Responses 18 Coding of Mother-Daughter Interactions Both discussions were videotaped. The Iowa Family Interaction Rating Scales (IFRS, Melby & Conger 2001) were used as the manual for coding emotion and communication in the mother-daughter interactions. The IFRS are designed to measure behavioral characteristics of individuals and the exchange between the two as the pair conveys information both through verbal and nonverbal communication. This is a macrosystem with a total of 19 scales. These include 14 general scales to describe the individual behaviors and characteristics of mothers and daughters: Sadness, Anxiety, Positive Mood, Externalized Negative, Lecture/Moralize, Whine/Complain, Hostility, Denial, Warmth/Support, Listener Responsiveness, Communication, Prosocial, Antisocial, Avoidance; two used for scoring behaviors of the mothers in their relationship with their daughters: Parental Influence, Sensitive/Child Centered; two for daughters in their relationship with their mothers: Instrumental Caretaking, Emotional Caretaking; and one as the subjective opinion of the rater: Rater’s Response. This analysis focused on Anxiety and Avoidance as two scales that measured the behavior representative of anxiety and distress associated with discussing the topic of breast cancer. Anxiety (AX). The anxiety scale measures how much the participant’s verbal and nonverbal behaviors communicate emotional distress conveyed as anxiety, nervousness, fear, tension, stress, worry, concern, and embarrassment. The participant may appear tense, fearful, uncomfortable, and/or self-conscious. Some examples of anxiety: “I am really concerned about this” (said with tense voice), “I’m really worried,” and nervous fidgeting. Stress Responses 19 Avoidance (AV). The scale measures the extent to which the participant averts her gaze and/or orients her body (head, arms, shoulder, or torso) away from the other person in such a manner as to avoid interaction. This avoidant manner conveys rejection, withdrawal, and evasion of the other person. Examples of Avoidance (italicized): Mother discloses information about their family history of breast cancer and becomes teary talking about her own mothers’ battle with the illness in her early 40’s. Daughter turns away and stares down. Daughter says “I don’t know if it’s that, it’s that I’ve got other things to deal with, I mean, I don’t NEED another problem to think about…and it’s not like I really need to think about it”. Mother immediately looks away. Mother says “So how do we usually get off the topic? What happens”? Daughter frowns, rolls her eyes, looks down and says, “I just say- ‘I don’t want to talk about it’… “I DON’T care.” Even though these two scales were the original focus of the study, additional exploratory analysis also included two other behaviors: Whine and Complain. This scale assesses the degree the participant demonstrates dissatisfaction by means of whining and whiny complaining. The expressions convey the sense the focal is an innocent victim, that things are not fair, and that she has been mistreated or misunderstood. Note the use of “poor me” tone of voice with a sing-song nasal quality. Examples: “I never get to go anywhere” and “You are ruining my life” Positive Mood. This scale measures the degree the participant appears content, happy, and optimistic and/or demonstrates positive behavior toward self, others or things in general. Both nonverbal communication such as facial expression and the content of the statements are taken into account. Examples: “John is a good friend,” and “We can do this!” Stress Responses 20 The scales range from 1 (the behavior was not at all characteristic of the person) to nine (the behavior was extremely characteristic of the person). The ratings were given by observers trained in the scales and used to derive scores for the mother-daughter pairs on behavioral dimensions. To become a coder, the person had to work through several steps. First he had to study the manual with the description and definitions of the scales. Next a test that asked for the name, definition, and two examples of all the scales had to be passed with a score of 80% or better. Once the person was familiar with the coding system, he would code interactions previously coded by reliable coders. The person would be considered a reliable coder when rating 80-85% of the scales within 2 points of the reliable coders. All the interactions were coded by two reliable coders. In codes where there was a difference, it was discussed until a consensus code was decided on for those scales. The consensus codes were used as the data. Procedures Recruitment Participants were recruited through the Vanderbilt Breast Center and at Meharry Medical College Breast Center. Women seen at the Breast Center were approached about their possible participation in the study. Interested women were provided with a brochure describing the study and asked for their permission to be contacted by a member of the research team. Recruitment brochures were also distributed at Vanderbilt-Ingram Cancer Center events as well as Komen Foundation, Greater Nashville Affiliate breast cancer risk awareness events. The foundation also printed announcements on their newsletters. Advertisement through the Medical Center Communications Office email list distributed an email describing the study to VUMC personnel. Stress Responses 21 The women who filled out an information sheet were then contacted by a member of the staff, explained the experiment, and asked if they would like to participate. If they accepted, they were asked to schedule an appointment for both mother and daughter to come to the lab. Questionnaires Then they were mailed the first set of questionnaires and asked to return them as soon as possible. Lab visit Before the arrival of the participants, the laboratory was set for the experiment: the TVs and video recorder were set up and the salivettes were labeled with the participants’ ids and the letters A-E for the sequential gathering of saliva samples. A coin was also flipped to determine which topic, whether breast cancer or an issue of disagreement, would be discussed first. The participants were met by one of the investigators and escorted into the lab. The nature of the study was again described to the participants and they were given the consent forms to sign unless they had been already returned with the initial questionnaires. The first saliva sample for baseline reading was collected by asking each person to chew on a cotton ball for about a minute and then return it to the salivette. Next a short questionnaire the Family Issues Selection Form was given to the pair to identify from a list of possible issues, one on which they disagreed. Based on this questionnaire the other topic of discussion was determined. The mother and daughter were seated beside each other on couches in a small room. The camcorder was in front of them. Depending on the order established by the coin toss, they were given a card with discussion questions either on the issue of disagreement or breast cancer. They were asked to ignore the camera and simply talk to each other as they normally would. The investigator would leave the room, close the door, and not return until the 15min Stress Responses 22 were up. After the 15min the investigator would return and give them the second card with the other discussion topic. The same instructions were given to discuss the topic for 15min. The investigator would again leave and return after 15min. Upon return a second saliva sample was taken on both mother and daughter and a timer started. The mother was taken to a separate area for the recovery period. Both mother and daughter were asked to watch a relaxing video on nature called “Denali”. Saliva samples on each were taken again 15 and 30min later. Following the interaction, a research assistant administered a semi-structured interview to the daughters. While daughters participated in the interview, the mothers were given the BDI-II, BAI, and either the CBCL or the YABCL to complete. Upon the daughters’ completion of the semi-structured interviews, mothers were privately interviewed by the research assistant while their daughters complete either the YASR or the YAR depending on their ages. Once they were finished, they were debriefed and asked what the experience was like for them and given check request forms. The mother-daughter pairs were each paid $10 for an initial questionnaire, $15 for the experiment, and $10 for a follow-up questionnaire. The salivettes were then taken to a freezer for storage. Six months later, follow-up questionnaires were mailed to the pair. RESULTS Descriptive Statistics Mean and standard deviations for all variables are presented in Table I. The means and standard deviations show there is enough variability within the data to carry out analyses to test the research questions and hypotheses. The standard deviations for the cortisol measurements however are larger than the means. This indicated the data is skewed toward 0 showing a lack of sensitivity of the measurement or a lack of sympathetic arousal in the task. Stress Responses 23 The mean of the BAI scores for the women is within the range of normal anxiety according to Beck and Sterr (1990). For the IES, the intrusion and avoidance scores are lower than women newly diagnosed with breast cancer (Epping-Jordan, Compas, & Howell, 1994). Perceived risk cannot be compared to normative samples, but the average of 2.85 means most women saw their risk as somewhat likely. The levels of cortisol were quite low in comparison to standard laboratory stress test such as the Trier Stress Test levels of 10-15µg/dL reported in the literature. The levels of alpha-amylase seem comparable to standard laboratory stressor results. Research Question 1: Stress Biology and General Anxiety The relationship between stress biology and general levels of anxiety was tested by analyzing the correlations between salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase and BAI scores. As presented in Table II, a significant correlation was not found between BAI and cortisol (r = .09 to .24). For alpha-amylase, the first time point was significantly correlated r= .39 at p<0.05. The correlations of alpha-amylase for the last 4 time points (r= -.09 to .21) were not significantly correlated with BAI scores. Alpha-amylase is significantly correlated with itself as shown in Table III (r =.56 to 0.85; p < 0.01). Table III also shows that alpha-amylase and cortisol are not significantly correlated with each other except at alpha-amylase 3 with cortisol 3, 4, and 5 (r = .36 to .42; p < 0.05). Except for the one significant result of alpha-amylase 1, the results were contrary to the initial hypothesis. Research Question 2: Stress Biology and Breast Cancer Specific Worries To study the relationship between stress biology and worries and concern about breast cancer, the correlations between salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase and IES avoidance and intrusion scales were tested. The results are presented in Table II. There was no significant correlation shown between alpha-amylase and either IES scale (with IES avoidance r = -.24 to Stress Responses 24 .01; with IES intrusion r = -.01to 0.34). No significant correlation was found between cortisol and IES avoidance (r = 0.09 to 0.24). However, a significant correlation (r = 0.49 to .57) was found between cortisol and IES intrusion scale, with p < 0.01 for all five time points. It is not surprising that all five time points would be significant since, as Table III shows, cortisol was significantly correlated at the five time points (r = .91 to 0.98, p < 0.01). It was predicted that both stress biology markers would correlate with both scales of the IES. However, this was not the case and only IES-intrusion and cortisol levels were significantly correlated. Research Question 3: Stress Biology and Perceived Risk for Breast Cancer The relationship between stress biology and perceived breast cancer risk was tested by analysis of the correlation between alpha-amylase and cortisol levels with the perceived risk score. As Table II shows, a significant correlation was only found between cortisol measurement at time 1 and the perceived risk score (r= -.64; p < 0.05). These results contradicted the expectation that the perception of risk would affect the physiological response of the participants. The results go further against expectations in that the correlation is negative and indicates than more perceived risk correlates with less cortisol. Research Question 4: Stress Biology and Observed Maternal Behavior For further study of stress biology, its relationship with the mothers’ anxiety and avoidance behaviors during the breast cancer discussion was correlated with their levels of cortisol and alpha-amylase. Contrary to the hypothesis, no significant correlation was found. The correlations between alpha-amylase and anxiety ranges were r = -.13 to.10 and avoidance r = -.10 to .12). For cortisol the correlations with observed anxiety ranged from r = .04 to .18 and with avoidance r =.15 to.19. Stress Responses 25 Since no significant results between avoidance and anxiety were found, some exploratory analysis was carried out because of the large amount of behavioral data that was available from the coding of the mother-daughter interaction. Correlations between the sixteen codes for the mothers and cortisol were carried out to view if any of the observed behaviors were significantly correlated. Whine/Complain was significantly correlated with r = .65 to .75; p < 0.001. Positive mood was also significantly correlated r = -.39 to -.49, p < 0.03. Because of the large number of correlations carried out, the p value was decreased to 0.003 for a correlation to be considered significant. With the stricter requirements, the whine/complain correlation remained significant with p < 0.001. DISCUSSION Research Question 1: Stress Biology and General Anxiety General anxiety as measured with the BAI did not show a significant correlation with the biological stress levels of either stress axis. The results go against the hypothesis that women with higher general anxiety would also show an increased biological response. The one positive correlation may indicate that the tests were not sensitive enough, but the biological reactions are in some way related to general anxiety. This pattern is somewhat different from the findings of studies by Bovberg and colleagues who found that biological stress responses were related to general sources of stress in women at risk for breast cancer. To the extent that the BAI reflects general levels of anxiety and stress, the current study did not replicate the findings of Bovberg. Research Question 2: Stress Biology and Breast Cancer Specific Worries On the second research question, a positive correlation was found between cancer specific worries and cortisol levels. It is important to note that although no correlation was found between general anxiety levels and biological stress responses, there was a significant correlation Stress Responses 26 for cancer specific worries. It is an interesting finding that only the HPA axis and not the SAM axis (as reflected in levels of alpha amylase) showed a corresponding reaction during the task with respect to the scores on intrusive thoughts of the IES. Previous research has found avoidance to be associated with poorer outcome in cancer patients (Epping-Jordan, Compas & Howell, 1994). The reasons mentioned for the negative outcomes were the non-compliance and non-adherence to treatment associated with the avoidance. In this study, however, no significant correlation was found between IES avoidance and cortisol. On the other hand IES- intrusion did show a significant correlation. A reason is that intrusion could be considered of shorter term effect. Since the IES asks about symptoms in the past 7 days, women may have been influenced to think about breast cancer because of their participation in the study. Because the thoughts had just begun, perhaps the participants in the current study were not yet so bothered by them to try to avoid them. Research Question 3: Stress Biology and Perceived Risk for Breast Cancer No significant correlation was found between perceived risk and stress biology. An explanation for this is that the measure of perceived risk was a single item measure that did not have reliability. There was also the limitation of sample size because women who had already had breast cancer were instructed to not fill out the assessment of risk question. This caused a decrease in the sample size to 13 women, which in turn decreased the power of the analysis. Another reason for the lack of a significant result however might be that perceived risk is not a direct measure of worry. Women might believe they are very likely to get breast cancer, yet not consider it something to worry about which could explain the lack of correspondence between stress levels and perceived risk levels. Research Question 4: Stress Biology and Observed Maternal Behavior Stress Responses 27 Contrary to the original hypotheses, the mothers’ anxiety or avoidance behaviors were not correlated to either cortisol or alpha-amylase levels. A reason might be that the women are not even realizing their own sensitivity to the topic of breast cancer and their stress was seen in their physiological response but not on their physical behavior. The significant correlation between whine/complain and cortisol levels was an unexpected finding which points to a very specific behavior and stress biology relationship. Importance of the Current Study The findings from this study, point to the importance of breast cancer specific concerns. It is not only general anxiety that will affect the biological stress response but a more specific worry and concern related to breast cancer. This is of importance because of the effects of over activation of the biological stress responses. The evidence in relation to stress and cancer is confusing and the connection between the two has not been revealed. Nevertheless stress has great implications with relation to health. In particular chronic stress has damaging effects on the immune system that decrease the body’s ability to ward off against virus and bacteria making one more susceptible to contagious diseases. The relation between cancer and immunosuppression has been proposed as a link to increased cancer risk via chronic activation of the stress axes. Another possible pathway between chronic stress and breast cancer is through of a family of proteins, cell transcription factors. These directly alter the DNA in cell nuclei and can contribute to the development of cancerous cells. A study by Bierhaus, Wolf, Andrassy, Rohleder, Humpert, Petrov, et al. (2003) suggests one type of cell transcription factor, Nuclear Factor kappa B (NF-kB), as the mediator that forms the connection between the stressor and the effects on the body at the cellular level. NF-kB could Stress Responses 28 be implicated in breast cancer tumor development (Dejardin, Bonizzi, Bellahcene, Castronovo, Merville, and Bours, 1995). This study also has implication about dealing with stress. In order to target and attempt to decrease the stress associated with breast cancer, the focus needs to be on breast cancer specific issues, not just general anxiety or perceived risk. This offers an option for psychological intervention in medical treatment. By using the IES, a short measure that reflects breast cancer specific worries, and has been shown to be related to stress reaction, patients can be screened without the requirement for a doctor interview. This is an option that could improve treatment through more comprehensive care. Limitations and Future Directions Future studies could be improved in several ways. A larger sample would undoubtedly make the power of the analysis greater and the effects easier to recognize. Another way would be to have a more focused breast cancer stress task to elicit stronger stress responses. The conversation about breast cancer provided an honest sample of behavior but did not elicit much biological stress reactivity from the participants. By having a breast cancer specific stress task differences in cortisol and nor-epinephrine (via alpha-amylase) could be better picked up. It would also be interesting to further the study of the relationship between the whine/complain behavior and the body’s stress reaction. Stress Responses 29 References American Cancer Society. (2006). Detailed guide: Breast cancer. Retrieved March 31, 2007, from http://www.cancer.org/docroot/CRI/CRI_2_3x.asp?dt=5 Beck, A.T., & Steer, R.A. (1990). Beck Anxiety Inventory manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Bierhaus, A.,Wolf, J., Andrassy, M., Rohleder, N., Humpert, P.M., Petrov, D., et al. (2003). A mechanism converting psychosocial stress into mononuclear cell activation. PNAS, 100, 1920-1925. Chatterton, R.T., Vogelsong, K.M., Lu, Y., Ellman, A.B., & Hudgens, G.A. (1996). Salivary αamylase as a measure of endogenous adrenergic activity. Clinical Physiology, 16, 433-448. Dejardin, F.E., Bonizzi, G., Bellahcene, A., Castronovo, V., Merville, M.P, & Bours, V. (1995). Highly-expressed p100/p52 (NFkB2) sequesters other NF-kB-related proteins in the cytoplasmof human breast cancer cells. Oncogene, 11,1835-1841. Epping-Jordan, J.E., Compas, B.E., & Howell, D.C. (1994). Predictors of cancers progression in young adult men and women: Avoidance, intrusive thoughts, and psychological symptoms. Health Psychology, 13, 539-547. Gil, F., Mendez, I., Sirgo, A., Llort, G.,Blanco, I., & Cortes-Funes, H. (2003). Perception of breast cancer risk and surveillance behaviours of women with family history of breast cancer: A brief report on a Spanish cohort. Psycho-Oncology, 12, 821827. Gold, S.M., Zakowski, S. G., Valdimarsdottir, H.B., & Bovberg, D.H. (2003). Stronger endocrine responses after brief psychosocial stress in women at familial risk of breast cancer. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28, 584-593. Stress Responses 30 Horowitz, M.J., Wilner, N., & Alvarez, W. (1979) Impact of Event Scale: a measure of subjective stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 41, 209-218 James, G.D., van Berge-Landry, H., Valdimarsdottir, H. B., Montgomery, G. H., & Bovberg, D. H. (2004). Urinary catecholamine levels in daily life are elevated in women at familial risk of breast cancer. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 831-838. Padgett, D. A., & Glaser, R. (2003). How stress influences the immune response. TRENDS in Immunology, 24, 444-448. Sapolsky, R.M., (2004). Glands, gooseflesh, and hormones. Why zebras don’t get ulcers. (pp. 19-36). New York: Henry Holt and Company. Skosnik, P.D., Chatterton, R.T., Swisher, T., & Park, S. (2000). Modulation of attentional inhibition by norepinephrine and cortisol after psychological stress. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 36, 59-68. Steward, D.E., Cheung, A.M., Duff, S., Wong, F., McQuestion, M., Cheung, T., et al. (2001). Attributions of cause and recurrence in long-term breast cancer survivors. PsychOncology, 10, 179-183. Thewes, B. Meiser,B., & Hickie, I., B. (2001) Psychometric properties of the impact of event scale amongst women at increased risk for hereditary breast cancer. Psycho-Oncology, 10, 459-468. Trask, P.C., Paterson, A.G., Wang, C., Satoru, H., Milliron, K.J., & Blumberg, L.R. (2001). Cancer-specific worry interference in women attending a breast and ovarian cancer risk evaluation program: Impact on emotional distress and health functioning. Psycho-Oncology 10, 349-360. Valdimarsdottir, H.B., Zakowski, S.G., Gerin, W., Mamakos, J., Pickering, T., & Stress Responses 31 Bovberg, D.H. (2002). Heightened psychobiological reactivity to laboratory stressors in healthy women at familial risk for breast cancer. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 25, 5163. Stress Responses 32 Table I: Mean and Standard Deviations for main measures Measure Mean Standard Deviation BAI 8.23 6.64 IES avoidance 12.11 7.96 IES instrusion 9.09 6.67 Perceived risk 2.85 .86 Cortisol 1 µg/dL .25 .46 Cortisol 2 .17 .25 Cortisol 3 .16 .35 Cortisol 4 .17 .33 Cortisol 5 .24 .76 Alpha amylase 1 33.08 28.87 Alpha amylase 2 36.68 33.25 Alpha amylase 3 35.60 31.28 Alpha amylase 4 38.75 34.88 Alpha amylase 5 38.01 36.70 Stress Responses 33 Table II: Correlations for cortisol with general anxiety, intrusive thoughts, perceived risk, observed behavior BAI IES avoidance IES intrusion Perceived Risk Observed Anxiety Observed Avoidance Cortisol 1 .24 .03 .57** -.64* .18 .16 Cortisol 2 .11 .19 .57** -.39 .08 .15 Cortisol 3 .15 .21 .56** -.15 .04 .19 Cortisol 4 .09 .09 .50** -.18 .07 .16 Cortisol 5 .15 .13 .50** -.15 .08 .17 -amylase 1 .39* -.12 -.01 -.16 .10 -.10 -amylase 2 .21 .01 .13 .06 -.11 -.04 -amylase 3 -.09 -.15 .34 -.03 .01 .125 -amylase 4 -.17 -.24 .18 .21 -.13 .10 -amylase 5 .08 -.20 .18 .05 -.04 .08 * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed) ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) Stress Responses 34 Table III: Correlations of Alpha-amylase (-a) and cortisol (Cort) at five time points Cort 1 Cort 2 Cort 3 Cort 4 Cort 5 -a 1 -a 2 -a 3 -a 4 Cort 1 --- Cort 2 .96** --- Cort 3 .92** .96** --- Cort 4 .91** .97** .92** --- Cort 5 .93** .97** .97** .98** --- -a 1 .03 -.29 -.31 -.17 -.16 --- -a 2 .18 .16 .23 .24 .25 .82** --- -a 3 .31 .33 .42* .36* .38* .62** .69** --- -a 4 .10 .12 .18 .16 .17 .56** .72** .85** --- -a 5 .19 .19 .29 .25 .27 .79** .80** .85** .75** * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed) ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) Table IV: Correlations between psychological measures IES IES Perceived BAI avoidance intrusion Risk BAI -IES --.34* avoidance IES --.32* .28* intrusion Perceived .12 .08 -.41* Risk Observed .11 .08 .04 Anxiety Observed -.07 .08 -.03 Avoidance * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed) Observed Anxiety Observed Avoidance ---.01 --- -.01 .01 ---