Running head: AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA Perception of Affective Musical Elements and the Relation to Delusions in Schizophrenia Rachel Nolan Dr. Sohee Park Vanderbilt University 1 AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 2 ABSTRACT Using prosodic, facial, and musical stimuli, this study probed the extent of emotion recognition deficits in schizophrenic patients. Difficulties in the perception of emotional material have been well documented in the literature. Here, we examined the perception of emotion in music and whether or not schizophrenic patients show the same dysfunction in this domain as they do in the domains of facial and speech emotion. 15 patients, and 15 healthy controls listened to and rated 40 instrumental music clips expressing either a happy, sad, calm, or scary emotion. Participants were also asked to choose which emotion they thought best characterized the song as a whole. We then compared the ratings of each song across groups, as well as overall accuracy on the task. Psychophysiological skin conductance data was collected, but the results were shown to be inconclusive. Patients were significantly impaired in determining the correct emotion of each song, showing specific deficits in recognizing scary songs. In addition, patients significantly overrated the perceived happiness of each song type as compared to controls. Overrating of the happiness of scary songs was highly correlated with each subscale of the PDI in both patients and controls. Performance on the task was also shown to be related to delusion scores in patients, and to schizotypal personality scores in controls. These results suggest that the inability to extract affective information from sounds may result from subjective misinterpretation of auditory cues, and may play a role in increasing delusions. AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 3 INTRODUCTION Schizophrenia is a serious and persistent mental disorder characterized by aberrations in the perception of reality. Though it only affects approximately one percent of the U.S. population, schizophrenia is of vital interest because of the heavy burden placed on patients, families, and society. Only about 28% of schizophrenics are able to live independently, with the rest either homeless, in jail, hospitalized, or in assisted living situations (Torrey, 2006). A total of about $63 million dollars per year spent by families and society make it the most costly mental illness in the United States (Wu et al., 2005). Schizophrenia also has a high cost on the life of the patient. The quality of life of a schizophrenic person is often extremely poor due to debilitating symptoms that make it difficult to function normally in society. The symptoms of schizophrenia fall into two categories; positive symptoms and negative symptoms. Positive symptoms are thoughts and behaviors that normal people rarely experience such as hallucinations, delusions, and bizarre behavior. Negative symptoms are those symptoms that reflect a decrease in normal functions; for example, social dysfunction, anhedonia, poverty of speech, blunted affect, and cognitive deficits (van Os & Kapur, 2009). Negative symptoms are much more pervasive and chronic, and have been shown to have a larger impact on outcome than positive symptoms (Kelley, Haas, & van Kammen, 2008). Positive symptoms can be treated using antipsychotic drugs, but there are few treatments available for negative symptoms, making negative symptoms a great interest for psychological research. Social dysfunction is one of the most common characteristics of schizophrenia. These impaired social skills most likely result from a debilitating mix of positive and negative symptoms. If someone has trouble distinguishing hallucinations from reality, then they are going AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 4 to portray bizarre behavior that violates many social norms. One such trait is a decreased ability to recognize emotional cues. People who cannot encode these cues are going to have major difficulties interacting in social situations; for example, misinterpretation of the intentions or mood of other people. In fact, schizophrenic patients have been shown to experience a deficit in emotional processing in social domains such as facial expression and speech prosody. A study by Norton et al (2009) investigated the ability of schizophrenic patients to recognize facial affect. Subjects were shown two pictures at the same time, one that was neutral and one that showed an emotion. Emotional intensity was manipulated by morphing the neutral face with the emotional face at different percentages (Norton et al., 2009). Schizophrenic patients needed significantly more powerful emotional stimuli than controls in order to discriminate the emotions of fear and happiness in morphed faces. Although these patients showed deficits in the perception of emotion in faces, they did not show any significant deficits in basic facial and visual processing (Norton et al, 2009). Studies have shown that schizophrenic patients are just as good at processing facial features and judging age as controls are (Morris et al, 2009). Therefore these facial emotional processing deficits do not originate from a general perceptual deficit. Similar studies have been done with auditory stimuli from voices. Bach et al (2008) demonstrated that schizophrenic patients have a deficit in recognizing the emotional content of speech without any semantic content. Therefore the difficulty with recognizing emotions from visual cues extends to auditory cues as well in schizophrenia. Although individuals with schizophrenia exhibit significantly diminished expressions of emotion, patients actually report an emotional experience that is similar to that of controls (Earnst and Kring, 1999). Additionally, researchers have found that physiological measures of emotion in schizophrenic patients are similar to those of controls (Hempel et al, 2007). Hempel AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 5 et al. displayed emotion-eliciting pictures to schizophrenic patients and controls while simultaneously measuring heart rate, skin conductance, blood pressure, and breathing rate. They also had the subjects rate the valance and arousal of each of the photos. The researchers found no significant differences between the two groups. Perhaps schizophrenic patients are capable of experiencing a wide range of emotions both physically and subjectively, but simply lack the tools to express it; however more research is needed on this topic. In controls, experience of emotion and expression of emotion are very closely linked which is why we often have trouble concealing our emotions. In schizophrenia, experience and expression of emotion may be dissociated, which could partly explain why schizophrenic patients have such difficulty recognizing emotion from social cues. However, it remains to be seen if schizophrenic patients have difficulty recognizing, experiencing and expressing emotion in all forms of stimuli and expression, for example in regards to music. Although the definition of music will change depending on whom you ask, the most useful definition for our purposes is an artistic arrangement of tones and rhythms. Music is more than just patterns of noise. Music is capable of producing a large variety of emotions, something that most other sounds fail to elicit (Gomez, 2004). This characteristic of music makes it a topic of great interest in regards to the study of emotional processing. There is no denying that music can suggest and affect our state of mind. However, it is difficult to study music's emotional power because the emotional content of music can be very subjective. A piece of music may be irrefutably emotional yet still be experienced in different ways. The emotion felt by a piece of music may be affected by numerous factors including; memories associated with that piece, the mood of the person listening and their personality, the environment it is being played in, and even the culture they were raised in (Juslin, 2008). Under such circumstances, it is AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 6 extremely difficult to discover what it is about music that makes it so emotional. Despite these limitations, numerous studies have investigated the emotional properties of music. Studies have shown that healthy people are very good at identifying emotion in music without lyrics (Vieillard et al, 2008). Even more interesting, researchers have found there to be a universal recognition of happy, sad, and fearful emotions in western and native African music (Fritz et al, 2009). Fritz and his colleagues performed a cross-cultural study with subjects from a native African population that were naïve to western music, and a western population that were naïve to African music. They found that both westerners and the native African population recognized happy, sad, and fearful expressions in music at rates that were above chance (Fritz et al., 2009). This universal recognition in novel pieces of music leads us to believe that there are certain structures in music that we innately recognize for emotional content. Perhaps similar acoustic cues are used to present emotion in both speech and music and we may be evolutionarily adapted to notice them (Fritz et al., 2009). Additional studies have been done with music examining musical structures and their relationship to emotional content. Some of the major findings were that; major mode was associated with negative high arousal songs and minor mode was associated with positive high arousal songs (Gomez, 2007). Minor mode is one that uses a minor scale pattern of whole notes and half notes: whole, half, whole, whole, whole, half, whole. A major mode is one that uses the major scale pattern of whole steps and half steps: whole, half, whole, whole, half, whole, whole. Additionally, they found that consonance was highest for calming songs but it was lowest f or threatening songs (Gomez, 2007). Consonant music was typically rated as pleasant, while dissonant music was rated as unpleasant (Sammler, 2007). Additionally when paired with physiological measures, music rated to be unpleasant evoked a significant decrease in heart rate AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 7 (Sammler, 2007). Preliminary data suggests that schizophrenic patients show no significant preference for consonant music over dissonant music (Honea, Borden, Park, 2003). This is significant because nearly everyone prefers consonant music to dissonant music (Sammler, 2007). In a study examining preference for consonance of music in 2-4 month old children, the infants quickly learned that when they turned their head in a certain direction consonant music would play, and when they turned their head in the opposite direction dissonant music would play. Once the infants learned how to turn on the music, they would consistently choose to turn on consonant music over dissonant music (Zentner et al, 2008). These results suggest an innate preference for consonant music over dissonant music. So why don’t schizophrenic patients show this preference? A recent experiment looked into physiological responses to dissonance (Blood, 1999). Music of varying dissonance was played for the subjects while their cerebral blood flow was measured. It was found that the varying degrees of dissonance caused increased activity in the paralimbic regions of the brain, which are associated with emotional processes (Blood, 1999). Perhaps schizophrenic patients cannot recognize the negative emotional content of the music. If they cannot recognize the threatening tone of the piece then maybe they do not find it unpleasant. There are numerous unanswered questions in regards to schizophrenia and music. How does music succeed in prompting emotions within us? Why are these emotions often so powerful? The simple answer is that no one knows. We are able to quantify the emotional responses caused by music, but we cannot explain them. We are no closer with schizophrenia. We know that schizophrenic patients often display a deficit in identifying emotion in social situations, however we can only theorize as to why. We do not know if this is because an overall social deficit, or because of an overall perceptual deficit. Schizophrenic patients are able to AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 8 recognize emotion in some forms; for example their own experience. For all we know, schizophrenic patients may actually be able to recognize emotion outside of a social context. Music may be a good domain in which to explore this theory. Music, though written by humans, can be played on a machine and still contain emotional content. If the emotion is separated from the social aspect of processing then perhaps schizophrenic patients will be able to recognize it. If schizophrenic patients are similar to controls in emotional music processing, then perhaps music therapy could be a viable option for patients in the future. METHODS Participants Fifteen clinically stable schizophrenic outpatients were recruited from a local psychiatric hospital. Diagnoses were confirmed through the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis 1 Disorders performed by a trained research staff member (SCID; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1995). One patient was un-medicated, one patient was taking typical antipsychotic drugs, and the remaining patients were taking atypical antipsychotic drugs. The un-medicated participant did not perform any differently from the other patients. Healthy control participants were recruited by advertisements in the community, and then demographically matched with the patients. They were also screened for psychosis-proneness by using the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (Raine, 1991). All participants were given informed consent approved by the Vanderbilt University IRB and were paid twenty dollars for every hour of their participation. Study exclusion criteria were; significant impaired vision, impaired auditory acuity, head trauma, recreational drug use, comorbid axis I psychiatric disorders, and any neurological condition. Clinical and demographic information are included in AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 9 table 1. The patient and control groups did not differ significantly in age, gender, IQ, race, or education. Twenty-seven Vanderbilt Undergraduate students were also used. They were not used for the main part of the study, but rather for piloting purposes. They were recruited through psychology classes and their participation was considered in the calculation of their grade. Their mean age was 19.2, and their mean SPQ score was 19.04. Measures All participants filled out a number of questionnaires assessing their functioning including the Social Functioning Scale interview (Birchwood et al., 1990), the Positive Affect Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) (Watson et al., 1988), the Peters et al. Delusions Inventory (PDI) (Peters et al., 1999), and the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 2001) which assesses theory of mind. IQ was assessed using the National Adult Reading Test (NART; Nelson, 1982). Handedness was assessed using the Edinburgh Handedness inventory. For patients, clinical symptoms were assessed using the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS; Overall & Gorham, 1962), the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS; Andreasen & Olsen, 1982), and the Scale for Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS; Andreasen & Olsen, 1982). Control participants and Vanderbilt undergraduates also completed the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) (Raine, 1991). DANVA: Diagnostic analysis of nonverbal behavior The Emory Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Behavior uses facial and prosodic stimuli to assess emotion recognition ability. The adult facial expression task was created by having participants were read a story that elicited a particular emotion, and then taking a photograph AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 10 during which they were asked to make a facial expression that was appropriate for that emotion (Nowicki & Carton, 1993). The prosodic task was created by having two professional actors speak the neutral sentence "I am going out of the room now but I'll be back later" to reflect the appropriate emotion at different levels of intensity (Baum & Nowicki, 1997). Participants were presented with 24 faces and 24 voice clips an iMac desktop computer. The participants had to choose whether each stimulus was expressing happy, sad, angry, or fearful emotion and to then circle their choice on a pre-printed answer sheet. Music emotion recognition task The stimuli used, were 40 instrumental music clips that have been selected through amazon.com, songpeddler.com, sonnyboo.com, and incompetech.com. Music was chosen that was thought to be unfamiliar to western listeners in order to eliminate the potential confound of emotional experience with a song. Music clips were selected that were thought to fit into the emotional categories of happy, sad, scary, and calm. Once collected, multiple experimenters rated the song clips in order to ensure agreement on the emotional content of each song. In a piloting phase, thirty Vanderbilt University undergraduates listened to each music clip, and then were asked to choose whether each song was happy, sad, calm, or scary. There was no time limit imposed for responding to each question. The mean accuracy on this task was determined to be 82.4%. This accuracy matched previous findings in the literature suggesting emotion recognition in music to be 80%, so the task was deemed to be of appropriate difficulty for use with patients and controls. The healthy controls, and the schizophrenic patients completed a similar task using the same music clips. The music clips were all ten second long aiff. Files that have been edited with AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 11 Audacity sound editing software. First the ten-second music clip was presented. Then the participants were also asked to rate on a scale of 1 to 7 how happy, sad, scary, and calm they perceived each piece to be, with 1 being neutral and 7 being very emotional. After rating the songs, participants were also asked to choose which of those four emotions they thought best represented the song as a whole. A final question asked participants if they had ever heard the song before. In this task, participants were only given 7 seconds to answer each of the 6 questions. This was to ensure proper time synchronization for the acquisition of physiological data. All music listening tasks were presented in a quiet room on a 12-inch Mac Book Pro laptop, using PsyScope X B57 software. All participants used headphones, and the computer volume was set to 6. Psychophysiological recording While the participants were listening to the music, physiological measures were also being recorded. Skin conductance data was collected using Biopac System Inc. apparatus, with the Gain levels set at 20 ’s/10 Hzs/ DC power linked to a Dell PC computer. The data was recorded using AcqKnowledge software III for MP100WS with a sample rate of 200 samples/second. Electrodes of the TDE-426-Y-ZZ-S Ag-AgCl type was used with isotonic electrode paste. The sites in which the sensors were applied were scrubbed with dry gauze for 20 seconds to remove any moisture. The white conductance paste was applied to the center of the sensors, so that the paste was level with the surface of the sensor. The sensors were then applied and taped to the left hand using medical tape; one sensor was placed at the bony part of the palm underneath the pinky finger, and the other was placed on the fleshy part of the palm near the base of the thumb. During the music task, acquisition of skin conductance data was taken only during the ten-second interval in which each song was playing. In an attempt to avoid skin conductance AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 12 changes due to movement, participants were explicitly told to try to keep their left hand as still as possible during the task. The mean, minimum, and maximum SCL frequency was extracted for each ten-second period of time in which the participant was listening to a song. For each song, the frequency of the mean was compared to the frequency of the max in order to investigate emotional activation during this time period. Emotional activation was classified as the range between the mean frequency and the maximum frequency. Participants were characterized as either normal or low responders, and the two groups were analyzed separately A skin conductance response scored for a magnitude increase greater than 5 was classified as a normal responder. Statistical Analysis Accuracy on the face, voice, and music tasks was defined by percent correct out of total answered. Emotional ratings were consolidated by song type and type of rating for each song, and then averaged for each participant. The number of false responses for each type of emotion was also calculated for each participant, as was total accuracy for each type of emotion. Levene’s test for equality of variances was done, and between group differences for accuracy and ratings were assessed using a one way ANOVA. Significance was determined individually for each measure depending on whether or not equal variance was found. Relationships between measures were assessed with Spearman’s rho bivariate measure of correlation. The groups were compared on demographic and clinical variables using an independent samples T-test for equality of means, with categorical variables made using chi-square analysis. A significance of .05 was used for all statistical tests, and all tests were carried out with version 17.0 SPSS statistical package. AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 13 Skin conductance data was consolidated for each type of emotion and compared between groups using mixed design analysis of variances (ANOVA). Friedman repeated measures analysis of variance (RM-ANOVA) was done to examine effect of emotional category. RESULTS Ratings of emotion in music The results show significant group differences in the emotional ratings of the songs. As a whole, schizophrenic patients were shown to overrate the positive emotions on all types of songs, and to underrate the negative emotions for the negative valance songs. In particular patients were found to significantly overrate the happiness of all song types as well as the calmness of scary songs. The means were compared using a one way ANOVA, and are summarized in table 2 along with the standard deviation, F, and p-values. The degrees of freedom for the between groups analysis of this test was 1. A graph of these rating differences is presented in figure 1. In addition, a number of emotion ratings seemed to be correlated with both positive and negative symptoms ratings, PDI, Social functioning, and SPQ (in controls only). Nonparametric correlations were assessed using Spearman’s rho. These results are summarized in tables 3-6. Emotion recognition Patients were shown to be significantly impaired in recognizing emotion in music as compared to controls, with specific deficits shown for the scary stimuli. Control participants demonstrated an overall accuracy of 75.12%, while patients demonstrated an accuracy of 61.83% (p=.013). These results of the one way ANOVA are summarized in table 7, and visualized in AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 14 figure 2. Interestingly, no significant group differences were found for accuracy on the DANVA faces or voices task. Accuracy on the music task was also shown be correlated to a number of additional measures summarized below. Of particular note, performance on the task was highly negatively correlated with a number of delusional subscales including paranoia, paranormal beliefs, and negative self. The test was done using Spearman’s rho correlation. These relationships are summarized in tables 8-12. Although patients were not found to be impaired on either DANVA task, performance on the faces task was shown to be correlated with certain measures summarized in table 13, and accuracy on the prosodic task was shown to be negatively correlated to PDI negative self (r=.568, p=.034). Psychophysiological results The skin conductance data did not appear to differ significantly between controls and patients. We were only able to analyze data from 9 controls, and 12 patients, and the results were also spilt into two types of responders; high and low, so more participants may be needed in order to increase statistical power. Overall the results were inconclusive DISCUSSION Our findings have both confirmed and extended knowledge of emotion recognition deficits in schizophrenia. Patients performed significantly worse than controls in identifying the emotional content of songs. In addition, a number of interesting relationships between total accuracy, as well as accuracy for each individual emotion were apparent in patients (tables 7-12). AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 15 In patients, a higher level of paranormal belief delusions, and delusions of negative self were related to poor overall performance (table 8). In addition, poor performance on happiness recognition was related to a higher symptoms rating for SAPS formal thought disorder, and SANS alogia (table 10). Sadness inaccuracy was related to paranoia delusions, thought disturbance delusions, negative self delusions, and paranormal beliefs (table 11). Scariness inaccuracy was also related to PDI thought disturbance, and PDI paranormal beliefs (table 12). We were unable to replicate prior findings of affect recognition deficits in faces and voices. Perhaps the DANVA faces and voices task was not difficult enough to produce significant group differences or our group of patients were exceptionally high-functioning but still showed differences in our music task. Evidence of group difference in the emotional ratings of songs, may suggest that patients are experiencing a unique subjective perception of music (Table 2). A previous study, using a pitch perception task (Hooker & Park, 2002) has shown that patients do not demonstrate impairments in tonal perception. However, patients have been shown to perform significantly worse than controls on a distorted tunes task (Leitman et al., 2007), and patients show no preference for consonant music over dissonant music (Honea, Borden, Park, 2003). It appears that these differences stem from subjective, rather than perceptual impairments. Patients appear to have a higher threshold for dissonance and distortion in music. My results also support this explanation, as patients were most impaired in the recognition of scary music (table 7), which was more likely to contain dissonant chords and uneven rhythms. Future studies should aim to further dissect this subjective peculiarity, perhaps by quantitatively altering the rhythm, tonality, and/or tempo of music to look at the effect of subjective emotionality. AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 16 Of particular interest; is the subjective emotional ratings made by controls and their relationship to SPQ (table 6). It seems that the tendency to overrate the positive emotions is related to a number of schizotypal traits. It seems that the happiness rating for scary songs presents the strongest and most numerous correlations. This evidence provides additional support that schizotypy may be associated with abnormal subjective experience of affective emotional cues. Perhaps controls with schizotypal traits also demonstrate a higher threshold for dissonant or unnerving music. Overall, the results of this study suggest an affect recognition deficit that may be highly dependent on subjective preference. Patients may be missing auditory cues for affective information because they are not encoding the perceptual characteristics (dissonance, rhythm, tempo) in the same way as controls. This could also explain why performance on the task was related to measures of delusion (Table 4, 8-12). Delusions arise from a subjective misinterpretation of environmental information. If performance on the music task is linked to subjective misinterpretation of perceptual information; than it may make sense that participants with high levels of delusion have difficulty extracting affective clues from auditory information. Music is a relatively novel modality in emotion research. We know that it is capable of inducing emotions in listeners, and we know that control populations are very good at detecting the emotional content of songs. However little is known about its effects upon the schizophrenic population. Music is a good domain to explore because it does not have any explicitly social content. The evidence that patients are impaired in a modality lacking any social cues, may suggest an underlying perceptual deficit. In this case, a misinterpretation of the sensory contributions of basic affective stimuli, that in turn affects the interpretation of social cues. It would be interesting to do imaging studies of the auditory and prefrontal cortex during music AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 17 listening to examine any differential patterns of activation. Such studies could help us answer the question of whether the emotion recognition deficit in schizophrenia is due to top-down or bottom-up dysfunction. AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 18 REFERENCES Abercrombie, H. C., Schaefer, S. M., Larson, C. L., Oakes, T. R., Lindgren, K. A., Holden, J. E., Perlman, S. B., Turski, P. A., Krahn, D. D., Benca, R. M., Davidson, R. J., 1998. Metabolic rate in the right amygdale predicts negative affect in depressed patients. NeuroReport, 9:3301-3307. Bach, D.R., Buxtorf, K., Grandjean, D., Strik, W.K., 2008. The influence of emotion clarity on emotional prosody identification in paranoid schizophrenia. Journal of Psychological Medicine, 39:927-938. Blood, A.J., Zatorre, R.J., Bermudez, P., and Evans, A.C. (1999) "Emotional responses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with activity in paralimbic brain regions." Nature Neuroscience, 2, 382-387. Bozikas, V.P., Kosmidis, M.H., Anezoulaki, D., Giannakou, M., Andreou, C., Karavatos, A., 2006. Impaired perception of affective prosody in schizophrenia. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clincial Neuroscience, 18: 81-85. Butler, P.D., Abeles, I.Y., Weiskopf, N.G., Tambini, A., Jalbrzikowski, M., Legatt, M.E., Zemon, V., Loughead, J., Gur, R.C., Javitt, D.C., 2009. Sensory contributions to impaired emotion processing in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35: 1095-1107. Davidson, R.J., 2002. Anxiety and affective style: Role of prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 51:68-80. Earnst, K.S., Kring, A.M., 1999. Emotional responding in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 88, 191–207. AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 19 Fritz, T., Jentschke, S., Gosselin, N., Sammier, D., Peretz, I., Turner, R., Friederici, A.D., Koelsch, S., 2009. Universal recognition of three basic emotions in music. Current Biology, 19:573-576. Gard, D.E., Kring, A.M., 2009. Emotion in the daily lives of schizophrenia patients: Context matters. Journal of Schizophrenia Research. Gomez, P., Danuser, B., 2007. Relationships between musical structure and psychophysiological measures of emotion. Emotion 7(2):377-387. Hempel, R.J., Tulen, J.H.M., van Beveren, N.J.M., Mulder, P.G.H., Hengeveld, M.W., 2007. Subjective and physiological responses to emotion-eliciting pictures in male schizophrenic patients. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 64: 174-183. Herrmann, M. J., Ehlis, A.C., Fallgatter, A.J., 2002. Prefrontal activation through task requirements of emotional induction measured with NIRS. Journal of Biological Psychology, 64:255-263. Hooker, C., Park, S., 2002. Emotion processing and its relationship to social functioning in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 112: 41-50. Kelley, M. E., Haas, G. L., & van Kammen, D. P., 2008. Longitudinal progression of negative symptoms in schizophrenia: A new look at an old problem. Schizophrenia Research, 105, 188–196. Juslin, P.N., Liljestrom, S., Vastfjall, D., Barradas, G., Silva, A., 2008. An experience sampling study of emotional reactions to music: Listener, music, and situation. Emotion, 8(5):668-683. AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 20 Khalfa, S., Isabella, P., Jean-Pierre, B., Manon, R., 2002. Event-related skin conductance responses to music emotions in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 328: 145-149. Kim, J., Andre, E., 2008. Emotion recognition based on physiological changes in music listening. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 30(12): 1-17. Kohler, C.G., Nilker, W., Hagendoorn, M., Gur, R.E., Gur, R.C., 2000. Emotion recognition deficit in schizophrenia: Association with symptomatology and cognition. Society of Biological Psychiatry. 48: 127-136. Leitman, D.J., Hoptman, M.J., Foxe, J.J., Saccente, E., Wylie, G.R., Nierenberg, J., Jalbrzikowski, M., Lim, K.O., Javitt, D.C., 2007. The neural substrates of impaired prosodic detection in schizophrenia and its sensorial antecedents. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 164: 474-482. Leitman, D.I., Foxe, J.J., Butler, P.D., Saperstein, A., Revheim, N., Javitt, D.C., 2005. Sensory contribution to impaired prosodic processing in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 58: 56-61. Leitman, D.I., Luakka, P., Juslin, P.N., Saccente, E., Butler, P., Javitt, D.C., 2010. Getting the cue: Sensory contributions to auditory emotion recognition impairments in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36: 545-556. Miller, G.F., Tal, I.R., 2007. Schizotypy versus openness and intelligence as predictors of creativity. Schizophrenia Research, 93:317-324. Morinaga, K., Akiyoshi, J., Matsushita, H., Ichioka, S., Tanaka, Y., Tsuru, J., Hanada, H., AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 21 2007. Anticipatory anxiety-induced changes in human lateral prefrontal cortex activity. Biological Psychology, 74:34-38. Norton, D., McBain, R., Holt, D.J., Ongur, D., Chen, Y., 2009. Association of impaired facial affects recognition with basic facial and visual processing deficits in schizophrenia. Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 65:1094-1098. Peters, E.R., Joseph, S.A., Garety, P.A., 1999. Measurement of delusional ideation in the normal population: Introducing the PDI (Peters et al. delusions inventory). Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25(3): 553-576. Raine, A., 1991. The SPQ: A scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSMIII-R criteria. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 17(4):555-564. Sammler, D., Grigutsch, M., Fritz, T., Koelsch, S., 2007. Music and Emotion: Electrophysiological correlates of the processing of pleasant and unpleasant music. Psychophysiology, 44(2):293-304. Shea, T.L., Serejew, A.A., Burnham, D., Jones, C., Rossell, S.L., Copolov, D.L., Egan, G.F., 2006. Emotional prosodic processing in auditory hallucinations. Schizophrenia Research, doi:10.1016/j.schres.2006.09.021 Torrey, E.F. (2006). Surviving schizophrenia: A manual for families, patients, and providers (5th ed.). New York: Harper Collins publishers. van Os, J. & Kapur, S. (2009). Schizophrenia. Lancet, 374, 635-645. Vieillard, S., Peretz, I., Gosselin, N., Gagnon, L., Bouchard, B., 2008. Happy, sad, scary and peaceful musical experts for research on emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 22(4):720-752. AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 22 Wu, E.Q., Birnbaum, H.G., Shi, L., Ball, D.E., Kessler, R.C., Moulis, M., Aggarwal, J., 2005. The economic burden of schizophrenia in the United States in 2002. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 66(9):1122-1129. Zentner, M., Grandjean, D., Scherer, K.R., 2008. Emotions evoked by the sound of music: Characterization, classification, and measurement. Emotion 8(4):494-521. AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 23 FIGURES Table 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Healthy Controls and Patients with schizophrenia (Independent samples T-test and chi squared analysis) Table 2. Rating differences for the emotional content of songs (ANOVA) Figure 1. Mean rating differences for the emotional content of songs (ANOVA) Table 3. Significant Spearman’s rho correlations of symptoms with emotion ratings Table 4. Significant Spearman’s rho correlations of PDI with emotion ratings Table 5. Significant Spearman’s rho correlations of social functioning with emotion ratings Table 6. Significant Spearman’s rho correlations of SPQ with emotion ratings Table 7. Group differences in emotion recognition accuracy (ANOVA) Figure 2. A graph of the group differences in emotion recognition accuracy (ANOVA) Table 8. Spearman’s rho correlation: Significant relationships with total accuracy Table 9. Spearman’s rho correlation: Significant relationships with calmness accuracy Table 10. Spearman’s rho correlation: Significant relationships with happiness accuracy. Table 11. Spearman’s rho correlation: Significant relationships with sadness accuracy Table 12. Spearman’s rho correlation: Significant relationships with scariness accuracy Table 13. Spearman’s rho correlation: Significant relationships with DANVA faces AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 24 Table 1. Characteristic Controls (n=15) Patients (n=15) t-test significance Age 37.73+/- 10.546 42.73 +/- 7.824 -1.475 .152 Gender (M/F) : (n=15) 8/7 (n=20 9/6 X2 =.136 .713 IQ 106.96+/- 7.764 102.79+/-11.38 1.169 .252 Years of education 16.13+/-2.031 14.13+/-3.378 1.965 .059 2 Ethnicity (white/black/asian) 11/3/1 7/8/0 X =4.162 .125 Edinburgh 86.33+/- 15.75 51.67+/- 60.46 2.149 .04* CPZ equivalent dosage (mg) 378.74+/-396.5 BPRS total score (n=13) 15.23+/-7.661 SAPS (n=13) 17.69+/- 10.67 SANS total score (n=13) 22.54+/- 12.57 Duration of illness 17.8+/-9.01 PANAS PA 35.4+/- 8.192 31.73+/-7.815 1.254 .22 PANAS NA 19.33+/-8.789 21.13+/-8.262 -.578 .568 AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 25 Table 2. emotion being rated happy sad calm scary happy sad calm scary happy sad calm scary happy sad calm Scary type of song happy happy happy happy sad sad sad sad calm calm calm calm scary scary scary Scary SZ mean rating 6.102 1.408 2.932 1.18 2.702 4.347 4.653 1.971 4.101 3.103 4.927 1.487 3.146 2.773 2.357 4.257 SZ Std. dev. .5344 .4098 1.066 .268 1.088 1.361 .8131 1.024 .9598 .9228 .9415 .5817 1.09 .7982 .8129 1.522 CO mean rating 5.398 1.403 2.343 1.431 1.847 4.492 4.079 2.047 3.066 2.425 4.527 1.341 2.021 2.832 1.736 4.645 CO Std. dev. 1.075 .4261 .9581 .4378 .5354 .8854 1.042 .7527 1.12 .9329 .9881 .4496 .8068 .7222 .5268 .9888 F 5.161 .001 2.535 3.570 7.455 .121 2.83 .053 7.393 4.004 1.291 .585 10.332 .045 6.146 3.824 pvalue .031* .972 .123 .069 .011* .731 .104 .819 .011* .055 .266 .451 .003** .834 .019* .061 AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 26 Figure 1. AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 27 Table 3. SAP hallucinations SAPS bizarre behavior SAPS delusion .033 calm rating for happy p-value .626* r .022 calm rating for scary p-value happy rating for sad p-value r happy for scary p-value r scary rating for happy p-value SANS anhedonia .593* r r SAPS formal thought disorder .595* .032 -.624* .694** .575* .023 .009 .040 -.581* .648* .704** .037 .017 .007 AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 28 Table 4. PDI PDI PDI PDI PDI PDI paranormal thought catastrophic. PDI idea PDI PDI PDI PDI total paranoia religiosity grandiosity beliefs disturbance ideation reference. distress preoccupation conviction calm r rating for happy p-value .428* .390* .405* .366* .439* .490** .018 .033 .026 .046 .015 .006 calm r rating for scary p-value .485** .368* .598** .546** .523** .383* .515** .528** .007 .045 .000 .002 .003 .037 .004 .003 happy r rating for calm p-value .421* .366* .413* .384* .365* .376* .395* .455* .020 .046 .023 .036 .047 .040 .031 .012 happy r rating for sad p-value .599** .484** .000 happy r for scary p-value .403* scary r rating for calm p-value * .007 .027 .408 .025 .373 * .042 .495** .552** .639** .463* .551** .599** .590** .005 .002 .000 .010 .002 .000 .001 .374* .364* .380* .390* .390* .473** .042 .048 .038 .033 .033 .008 * .375* .032 .041 .421 * .020 .381 * .038 .391 AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 29 Table 5. Withdrawal/ social engagement r Interpersonal communication -.361* Prosocial Employment -.364* .050 calm rating for scary p-value .048 -.371* r .044 happy rating for sad p-value -.527 r happy for scary .003 p-value r sad rating for calm p-value ** -.438* -.407* .015 .025 * -.516** .047 .004 -.366 AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 30 Table 6. SPQ SPQ SPQ SPQ SPQ SPQ SPQ SPQ SPQ SPQ SPQ no SPQ constric SPQ positive negative cognitive interperson disorganiz SPQ idea social odd unusual odd close odd ted suspicious cluster cluster perceptual al ation. reference anxiety beliefs perception behavior friends speech affect ness SPQ total .374* r calm rating for happy p-value .042 .405 r calm rating for sad p-value * .026 happy for scary sad rating for calm .037 .414 .042 .450 * .013 * .383 * * .414 * .037 .414 .023 .393 happy rating for sad .383 * .399* .029 .450 * * .361 .473 * .013 .050 .008 * .371 .023 .385 * .414 * * .044 .385 * .420 * * * r p-value .377* .374* .040 .042 .399 * .029 * ** .395* .031 * .384* .022 .036 .409* .491** .441* .411* .406* .413* .469 * .366 .410 * .417 * .032 .023 .036 .023 .036 .021 .446* .472** .389* .472** .389* .458* .024 .032 .006 .481** .497* .009 .047 .024 * .013 .009 .034 .009 .034 .011 .516** .566** .512** .566** .512** .480** .007 .005 .542** .492* .595* * r p-value * .015 .410 .392 .487 r p-value .031 .441 r p-value .394* * r calm rating for scary p-value happy rating for calm .374* .004 .362 * .049 .001 .004 .406 * .026 .001 .004 .406 * .026 .007 .403 .027 .026 .023 .481** .528** .512* .476* .541** .501** * .002 .006 .001 * .025 .006 .015 .024 * .007 .380 * .038 * .003 .004 .008 .412 * .024 .002 .377 * .040 .005 AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 31 Table 7. All emotions happy sad calm scary SZ mean accuracy 0.6183 0.8489 0.4376 0.5861 0.5395 SZ Std. dev .1599 .1855 .2354 .2511 .2966 CO mean accuracy 0.7512 0.937 0.5541 0.7332 0.7643 CO Std. dev. .085 .0884 .1866 .1812 .142 F 07.343.013* 2.76 2.256 3.387 7.006 P-value .011* .108 .144 .076 .023* AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 32 Figure 2. AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 33 Table 8. spearman's rho p-value Meaures all SZ CO all SZ CO IQ 0.447* 0.013* DANVA faces % correct 0.583** .666** 0.001** .009** PDI paranoia PDI paranormal beliefs -475** -.634* .008** .011* PDI negative self -.542* .037* PDI distress -.361* .05* RSF IP .37* .044* SPQ disorg .623* .013* SPQ Odd behavior 0.668** .006** SPQ suspiciousness -.544* .036* AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 34 Table 9. spearman's rho p-value Measures all SZ CO all SZ CO DANVA faces % correct 0.405 0.029 PDI paranormal beliefs -448* -.541* .013* 0.037 RSF IP .43* .018* SPQ Odd behavior .537* 0.039* SPQ suspiciousness -.538* .039* AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 35 Table 10. spearman's rho p-value Measures all SZ CO all SZ CO PANAS .554* .032* PDI paranormal beliefs -.448* .013* RSF rec .384* .589* .036* 0.021 RSF Interpersonal communication .43* .018* SAPS hallucination .67* .012* SAPS formal thought disorder -.62* 0.024* SANS alogia -.658* 0.015* AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 36 Table 11. spearman's rho p-value Meaures all SZ CO all SZ CO IQ .423* .02* PDI total -.617* .014* PDI paranoia -.517* .048* PDI thought disturbance -.544* .036* PDI negative self -.651** .516* .009* .049* PDI paranormal beliefs -.403* -.645** .027* .009* PDI preoccupation -.583* 0.023* PDI distress -.403* -.62* .027* 0.041 PDI conviction -.533* 0.041* SANS attention -.623* .023* RSF IP .43* .018* SPQ Odd behavior .566* .028* SPQ suspiciousness -.538* .039* PANAS PA -.523* .046* AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 37 Table 12. spearman's rho p-value Measures all SZ CO all SZ CO DANVA faces % correct .477** .009** PDI thought disturbance -.362* 0.049 PDI paranormal beliefs -.410* -.582* .024* 0.023 PDI conviction -.391* 0.033 RSF employment .397* .03* RSF withdrawal -.545* .035* SPQ Odd behavior .537* 0.039 SPQ suspiciousness -.538* .039* AFFECTIVE MUSICAL PERCEPTION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 38 Table 13. spearman's rho p-value Measures all SZ CO all SZ CO eyes .406* .036* PDI paranormal beliefs -.373* 0.046 RSF Interpersonal communication .429* .552* .033* RSF prosocial 0.417 .024* SANS total -.557* .048* RSF withdrawal -.691** 0.006