Running head: EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY Implications of Effortful Control and Negative Affectivity in the Persistence and Recovery of Stuttering Stephanie Hitti Vanderbilt University This study was completed in partial fulfillment of the requirements for Honors in Psychology at Vanderbilt University under the direction of Tedra A. Walden. EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 2 Abstract The present study investigated the differences in Negative Affectivity and Effortful Control in the presence and absence of stuttering. A Speech Language Pathologist (SLP) measured the stuttering-like disfluencies (SLDs) of 92 preschool-aged participants at two separate time points (i.e. time point 1 and time point 2). The participants were then divided into four stutter groups: children who did not stutter at either time point (CWNS), children who stuttered at both time points (Persistent), children who stuttered at time point 1 but did not stutter at time point 2 (Recovered), and children who did not stutter at time point 1 but stuttered at time point 2 (Transitional). Results indicated that at time point 1, both the Recovered and the Persistent group had significantly lower scores of Effortful Control on the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ) than the CWNS group. Additionally, both the Persistent and the Transitional groups had significantly higher Negative Affectivity scores on the CBQ at time point 1 than at time point 2, but the other groups did not. Keywords: stuttering, temperament, effortful control, negative affectivity EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 3 This study explored the relation between effortful control (EC), negative affectivity (NA), and the persistence and recovery of stuttering. Stuttering, a communication disorder that affects fluency of speech, tends to begin during childhood and, in about 1% of people, can last a lifetime. It is characterized by disfluencies, such as repetitions and prolongations of sounds, which may impede communication. Approximately 5% of all children stutter at some point in their lives, usually beginning between the ages of 2 and 5, as their language skills begin to develop (National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, 2015). Moreover, there are about 3 to 4 times more boys than girls who stutter. While no single cause for stuttering has been established, there are some speculations that the influences of genetics, speech and motor factors, and temperament/emotions may be related (Andrews, 1991; Kent, 2001; Jones, Choi, Conture, & Walden, 2014). Recently researchers have been investigating the relation between elements of temperament, such as inhibitory control and attention regulation, and stuttering (Eggers, De Nil, & Van den Bergh, 2010). However, although data suggest that both variables are indeed related to stuttering, most research focuses on correlations, not causality (Conture, Kelly, & Walden, 2013). The first step in obtaining evidence of causality would be, ideally, to measure temperament before stuttering commences since random assignment of groups is not possible. In order to obtain such data, a very large sample would be necesary. Therefore, to the present authors’ knowledge, there is no study that has measured temperament in preschool-aged children before the onset of stuttering. Stuttering is diagnosed by speech-language pathologists (SLPs) who are trained to detect language disfluencies such as whole word repetitions (WWR), single syllable repetitions (SSR), prolongations, interjections, and revisions. Most SLPs diagnose stuttering by considering both the frequency of stuttering-like disfluencies per 100 words and the Stuttering Severity Instrument EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 4 (SSI) (e.g., Yari, 1981; Yari & Ambrose, 1992b; Yari & Lewis, 1984). A child is usually considered to stutter if he has 3 or more disfluencies per 100 words and has a score of 11 or higher on the SSI (Karrass et al., 2003). However, some researchers solely rely on the frequency of stuttering-like disfluencies per 100 words to diagnose stuttering (Conture & Caruso, 1987; Riley, 1980). SLPs can also predict whether or not the child is likely to continue to stutter and whether his or her parents should consider treatment. Although there is no single factor that can be used to predict the continuation of stuttering, family history and presence of other speech or language disorders can help SLPs make that decision (National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, 2015). Previous literature has found that aspects of temperament may influence communication in children who do not stutter. For example, Dixon and Shore (1997) found a predictive relation between early temperamental aspects (e.g. soothability) and later linguistic style, further establishing the link between language and temperament. Their findings indicated that greater adaptability, increased positive mood, and greater persistence at 13 months predicted advanced language productivity at 20 months. Data also suggest that there is a relation between temperament characteristics and communication disorders such as developmental stuttering (Anderson, Pellowski, Conture, & Kelly, 2003; Lewis & Goldberg, 1997; Embrechts, Ebben, Franke, & van de Poel, 2000). Specifically, researchers have proposed that stuttering, as well as other speech-language disorders, such as late talking and specific language impairment, are directly influenced by the temperamental dimensions of NA and EC (Salley & Dixon, 2007; Johnson, Walden, Conture, & Karrass, 2010; Ntourou, Conture, & Walden, 2013; Felsenfeld, van Beijsterveldt, & Boomsma, 2010; Embrechts et al., 2000). The present study will focus on the scales of NA and EC of the EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 5 Children’s Behavior Questionnaire (Rothbart et al., 2001) and will explore their relation to childhood stuttering. NA is a temperament characteristic that refers to “the stable tendency to experience negative emotions” (Watson & Clark, 1984). People high in NA tend to report consistent negative moods. NA envelops a range of negative mood states: discomfort, sadness, fear and anger or frustration, and belongs to the broader concept of emotional reactivity, or “the tendency to experience frequent and intense emotional arousal” (Karrass et al., 2006, p. 403). Salley and Dixon (2007, p.137) expressed that, “when children are very high in NA, a relatively greater burden is placed on their behavioral control systems, which must regulate this NA. The end result is fewer resources available for linguistically relevant activities such as paying attention to word-referent associations when learning novel labels.” Others have also found that CWS are more negative in affect and emotions (Howell, Davis, Bartrip, & Wormald, 2004; Johnson, Walden, Conture, & Karrass, 2010; Ntourou, Conture, & Walden, 2013). For example, Ntourou, Conture, and Walden’s (2013) findings indicate that CWS displayed significantly more negative emotions and more self-speech than their CWNS peers when completing a frustrating task, namely the “Attractive Toy in a Transparent Box” (ATTB) task from Goldsmith, Reilly, Lemery, Longley, and Prescott (1999). These results are consistent with the idea that preschool-aged CWS are more emotionally reactive than CWNS and that stuttering is related to NA. Moreover, the literature has consistently found that CWS are more nervous, anxious, shy, and withdrawn than CWNS (Fowlie & Cooper, 1978). In particular, Glanser et al (1949) found that CWS are more self-conscious and anxious when it comes to speaking than their CWNS peers. This anxiety or frustration was apparent as the participants would cry, whisper “I can’t EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 6 talk,” stomp their feet, and/or walk away without completing their sentences. These findings, again, indicate that stuttering may be associated with the NA factor of the CBQ, specifically the Fear and Anger/Frustration characteristics of temperament. Theorists have also found that CWS have a tendency to be more sensitive and possess more vulnerable temperaments (i.e. fearful/shy), when compared to CWNS (Anderson et al., 2003). This indicates that there might be a relation between stuttering and temperamental sadness, “the amount of negative affect and lowered mood and energy related to exposure to suffering, disappointment, and object loss” (Van den Bergh and Ackx, 2003). Johnson, Walden, Conture, and Karrass (2010) investigated emotional regulation in CWS and CWNS when receiving either a desirable or an undesirable gift. Their findings demonstrated that, while CWS and CWNS displayed no difference in positive emotion upon receiving the desirable gift, CWS displayed significantly more negative emotion upon receiving the undesirable gift. This, again, provides support to the possibility of a relation between the temperamental characteristic of “sadness” and stuttering. Others have also implied that such sensitivity and vulnerability may contribute to the onset or continuation of developmental stuttering (Conture, 1991;2001; Guitar, 1998; Zebrowski & Conture ,1998). Rothbart and Rueda (2005) defined EC as the "efficiency of executive attention, including the ability to inhibit a dominant response and/or to activate a subdominant response, to plan, and to detect errors." In other words, EC is one’s ability to independently manage attention, inhibit undesirable behavior, and activate desirable behavior (Eisenberg, 2012). Represented in the concept of EC are attentional focusing, attentional shifting, and inhibitory control; characteristics that have been repeatedly found to be lower in preschool-aged CWS (Felsenfeld, van Beijsterveldt, & Boomsma, 2010; Embrechts et al., 2000; Kraft, Ambrose, & Chon, 2014; EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 7 Reilly, Onslow, & Packman, 2013; Schwenk, Conture, & Walden, 2007). Specifically, Rothbart et al. (2001) defined attentional focusing as the “tendency to maintain attentional focus upon task-related channels,” attentional shifting as the ability to change one’s focus from one item to another, and inhibitory control as “the capacity to plan and to suppress inappropriate approach responses under instructions or in novel or uncertain situations.” Effective EC includes the ability to regulate one’s emotion, therefore, the fact that researchers have found a relation between emotional regulation and stuttering (Dixon & Smitth, 2000; Kefalianos, Onslow, Block, Menzies, and Reilly, 2012) suggests that there should be a relation between EC and stuttering. A common regulatory strategy is attention regulation, or the ability to control one’s attention in order to limit the emotional effect a stimulus may arouse (Ahdahi & Rothbart, 1994). Attention regulation has consistently found to be related to childhood stuttering (e.g., Schwenk, Conture, & Walden, 2007). Karrass et al. (2006) investigated the relation between emotional reactivity and emotional regulation to childhood stuttering. CWS were more emotionally reactive to environmental stimuli and less able to regulate their emotions than their fluent peers. Likewise, Embrechts et al.’s (2000) findings suggest that CWS are less able to regulate their attention and adjust to their environment. Several other studies, such as Kefalianos et al. (2012), found that CWS tend to have lower attention spans and persistence as well as a more negative quality of mood. Similarly, Rothbart and Rueda (2005) found that among U.S. children, EC is inversely correlated with NA. Some researchers have found that CWS have a more difficult time disengaging their attention when asked to do so or when it becomes appropriate to do so (Bush, 2006), as well as difficulty shifting their attention when the need arises (Eggers et al., 2010). Moreover, Felsenfeld, van Beijsterveldt & Boomsma (2010) found that caregivers of CWS are more likely EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 8 to report more attention problems on parent questionnaires. Theorists have also reported that CWS can be more reactive to environmental stimuli, making them less able to keep focused on a particular task (Wakaba, 1998). However, there have been some disparities in the literature. Johnson, Conture, and Walden (2012) did not find that CWS were less apt at regulating their attention than CWNS. This divergence could be explained by the fact that most other studies used data from parental reports, whereas Johnson et al. (2013) examined response speed and accuracy during an experimental task intending to measure attention. Anderson, Pellowski, Conture and Kelly (2003) reported that CWS were more hypervigilant, or less likely to be distracted than CWNS peers. This study is particularly inconsistent with that of Embrechts et al. (2000) who found that CWS are more distractible and less able to focus than CWNS. Anderson et al. (2003) suggested that these inconsistencies may be due to the fact that Embrechts et al.’s (2000) study was conducted with Dutch children and there may be a cultural difference. Moreover, Embrechts et al. (2000) used the CBQ whereas Anderson et al. (2003) used the Behavioral Style Questionnaire (BSQ). Anderson et al. proposed that it would be wise to use both the BSQ and the CBQ in future studies. On the other hand, Johnson, Conture, Walden & Karrass (2012) theorized that attention regulation in CWS may vary depending on stuttering severity. Their findings indicate that children who have a mild stutter may be more able to regulate their emotions than children who have a moderate to severe stutter. Thus, it may be that the studies mentioned above did not find differences in attention regulation between CWS and CWNS because they did not account for stuttering severity. Research hypotheses EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 9 Therefore, there is sufficient evidence in the literature to suggest that there might be a relation between NA and EC in CWS and CWNS. With the use of the CBQ, the present study explored this relation in the context of four different stutter groups at two time points separated, in most cases, by a period of eight months. The groups were: children who did not stutter at either time point (CWNS), children who stuttered at both time points (Persistent), children who stuttered at time point 1 but did not stutter at time point 2 (Recovered), and children who did not stutter at time point 1 but stuttered at time point 2 (Transitional). First, it was hypothesized that the two groups that stuttered at time point 1, Persistent and Recovered, would have (1) lower scores on EC and (2) higher scores on NA than the two groups that did not stutter at time point 1, CWNS and Transitional. Similarly, it was hypothesized that the two groups that stuttered at time point 2, Persistent and Transitional, would have (1) lower scores on EC and (2) higher scores on NA than the two groups that did not stutter at time point 2, CWNS and Transitional. Second, it was predicted that the participants in the Transitional group would have (1) higher scores of EC and (2) lower scores of NAat time point 1 than at time point 2. Third, it was hypothesized that the participants in the Recovered group would have (1) lower scores of EC and (2) higher scores of NA at time point 1 than at time point 2. Methods Participants Participants consisted of 60 preschool aged boys and 32 preschool aged girls, all native speakers of American English. Participants were involved in a several studies conducted by Vanderbilt University's Developmental Stuttering Project. Participants were divided into four groups according to whether or not they stuttered at time point 1 and at time point 2: CWNS (15 girls and 15 boys), Persistent, (15 boys and 2 girls), EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 10 Recovered (23 boys and 7 girls), and Transitional (7 boys and 8 girls). For the majority of the participants (85), the time points were separated by a period of 8 months. However, due to missed appointments, 6 participants had 15-17 months between visits, and one had 25 months between time point 1 and time point 2. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) reveled significant differences in gender (F(3,88) = 4.00, p = .01), which was expected due to the fact that three to four times more boys than girls who stutter. Participants were between the ages of 2;11 (years; months) and 6;4 at time point 1 (CWNS: M = 48.87, SD = 7.87; Persistent: M = 46.53, SD = 6.75; Recovered: M = 49.43, SD = 10.02; Transitional: M = 47.93, SD = 11.13) with no significant difference in age, F(3, 88) = .41, p = .74, and between the ages of 3;7 and 7;0 at time point 2 (CWNS: M = 57.43, SD = 7.80; Persistent: M = 57.24, SD = 8.76; ; Recovered: M = 58.80, SD = 10.72; Transitional: M = 56.53, SD = 10.79) with no significant difference in age, F(3,88) = .23, p = .88. All participants were paid volunteers referred to the Vanderbilt Bill Wilkerson Center by their caregivers. Recruitment occurred through (a) advertisements in a free parent-orientated magazine based in Middle Tennessee (e.g. the “Nashville Parent), (b) referrals from Nashville area speech-language pathologists, health care providers, researchers, and day cares, (c) or self or professional referral for initial assessment for childhood stuttering. Some participants reported having received formal or structured treatment for stuttering. Participants had no known or reported developmental, academic, hearing, intellectual, neurological, or emotional problems. Each participant’s caregiver signed informed consent and each participant assented. Classification Children who do not stutter (CWNS). A child was considered a CWNS if he or she displayed less than three stuttering-like disfluencies (i.e., sound prolongations, sound/syllable EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 11 repetitions, monosyllabic whole-word repetitions, and broken words) per 100 words during a play-based conversation (from a 300-word sample; Conture, 2001) at both time point 1 (M = 1.25, SD = .71) and time point 2 (M = 1.19, SD = .74). Persistent. A child was considered Persistent if he or she displayed three or more stuttering-like disfluencies per 100 words during a play-based conversation (from a 300 word sample) at both time point 1 (M = 9.22, SD = 4.98) and at time point 2 (M = 7.40, SD = 4.88). Recovered. A child was considered Recovered if he or she displayed three or more stuttering-like disfluencies per 100 words during a play-based conversation (from a 300 word sample) at time point 1 (M = 5.89, SD = 3.35) but displayed less than three stuttering-like disfluencies per 100 words of a play-based conversation (from a 300 word sample) at time point 2 (M = 1.40, SD = .74). Transitional. A child was considered Transitional if he or she displayed less than three stuttering-like disfluencies per 100 words during a play-based conversation (from a 300 word sample) at time point 1 (M = 2.0, SD = .85) but displayed three or more stuttering-like disfluencies per 100 words of a play-based conversation (from a 300 word sample) at time point 2 (M = 4.56, SD = 2.86). Excluded Participants From an initial group of 147 participants, 55 participants were excluded (43 CWNS, 1 Transitional, 3 Persistent, and 8 Recovered). The main reason for exclusion was an incomplete CBQ at either time point, (23 participants; 11 CWNS, 1 Transitional, 3 Persistent, and 8 Recovered). Thus, 124 participants had a complete CBQ at both time point 1 and time point 2 (62 CWNS, 15 Transitional, 17 Persistent, and 30 Recovered). All 15 Transitional, 17 Persistent, EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 12 and 30 Recovered participants were included in the study. However, to make the groups roughly equivalent in number, 15 girls and 15 boys were randomly selected from the 62 CWNS. Standardized speech-language tests and hearing screening To participate in this study, each participant must have scored above the 16th percentile or higher on a series of standardized speech-language tests: (a) Peabody Picture Vocabulary TestThird or Fourth Edition (PPVT-III or IV A or B; Dunn & Dunn, 1997, 2007), a measure of receptive vocabulary, (b) Expressive Vocabulary Test First or Second Edition (EVT or EVT-2; Williams, 1997, 2007), a measure of expressive vocabulary, (c) Test of Early Language Development-3 (TELD-3; Hresko, Reid, & Hamill, 1999), a measure of receptive and expressive language, and (d) “Sounds in Words” subtest of the Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2 (GFTA-2; Goldman & Fristoe, 2000). Additionally, each child passed a bilateral pure tone hearing screening and tympanometric screening. All tests were conducted at the Vanderbilt Bill Wilkerson Center. Race The participants’ race was obtained through a parent interview. The CWNS group included 23 Caucasian, 4 African American, and 2 biracial participants; the Persistent group included 12 Caucasian, 2 African American, 1 biracial, and 1 Asian participant; the Recovered group included 26 Caucasian, and 4 biracial participants; and the Transitional group included 14 Caucasian and 1 African American participant. Three participants, 2 CWNS and 1 from the Persistent group, opted to not disclose their race. Measures The Children’s Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ) assesses temperament of preschool-aged children (ages 3 to 7) along 15 temperamental characteristics or scales. The temperamental scales EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 13 make up three composite higher order factors: Effortful Control (EC), Negative Affectivity (NA), and Extraversion/Surgency. Detail is provided in Table 1. This study focused on the higher order factors of EC and NA (excluding Extraversion/Surgency) because several participants lacked scores in the Extraversion/Surgency scales as their parents completed the short version of the CBQ (70 questions) instead of the longer version (237 questions). Parents rated each item from 1 (“extremely untrue of your child”) to 7 (“extremely true of your child”). Scale scores were calculated by averaging the item scores. Examples of EC items include “is hard to get her/his attention when s/he is concentrating on something” and “can easily shift from one activity to another.” Examples of NA items include “has temper tantrums when s/he doesn’t get what s/he wants” and “tends to feel ‘down’ at the end of an exciting day.” Procedures Parents completed an online questionnaire (CBQ) and speech language pathologists (SLPs) measured the participants’ stuttering-like disfluencies (SLDs) at two different time points. At each time point, SLPs listened to a total of 300 words in a play-based conversation and recorded the SLDs. If the participant had 3 or more SLDs per 100 words, s/he was considered to stutter; if s/he had less than 3 SLDs per 100 words, s/he was considered to not stutter. Each visit also included a parental interview, the language and vocabulary tests (i.e. PPVT, EVT, TELD) and a hearing test. Each visit took place at the Bill Wilkerson Center at Vanderbilt’s Medical Center and lasted approximately 2 hours. Parents completed the questionnaires before arriving and were given information about their children's performance on the speech, language, and temperament assessments at the end of the visit. Data Analyses EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 14 Group differences in gender and age, at both time point 1 and time point 2, were assessed using t-tests. Statistical tests of the hypotheses are described below. Between-groups comparisons. A One Way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was conducted across all groups to determine whether NA and EC, as well as all their respective scales, differed among the groups at either time point 1 and time point 2. The independent variable was the stutter group (i.e. CWNS, Persistent, Recovered, or Transitional) and the dependent variable was the score each participant received in each variable of the CBQ. Next, pairwise t-tests for significant differences determined which groups differed. Within-groups comparisons. Repeated Measures ANOVA and ANCOVA, with gender as a covariate, assessed whether the groups differed at time point 1 and time point 2, while controlling for gender. The independent variables were the stutter groups and time points (i.e. time point 1 or time point 2). The dependent variable, again, was the score each participant received in each variable of the CBQ. Results Descriptive Statistics Levene’s test of equality of error variances was conducted in order to ensure equality of variance among groups. Additionally, to ensure homogeneity of variance, frequency distribution graphs were created. Group means for NA and EC scores are in Table 2. Scores all showed normality and homogeneity of variance. Additionally, Levene’s test of equality of error variance reveled no significance on EC at time point 1 (F = .57, p = .64), EC at time point 2 (F = .30, p = .83), NA at time point 1 (F = .63, p = .42), or NA at time point 2 (F = .42, p = .74). Pearson’s correlations evaluated the relation between EC and NA at both time points. There was significant negative correlations between NA at time point 1 with EC at time point 1 EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 15 (r(92) = -.22, p = .03), and positive correlation between NA at time point 1 with NA at time point 2 (r(92) = .56, p < .001), and EC at time point 1 with EC at time point 2 (r(92) = .65, p < .001). Between-group differences ANOVA with gender as a covariate revealed between group differences in EC at both time point 1 (F(4,87) = 2.55, p = .04) and time point 2 (F(4, 87) = 3.04, p = .03). Results are illustrated in Figures 1 and 2. No between-group differences were found in NA at either time point 1 (F(4,87) = 1.07, p = .27) or time point 2 (F(4,87) = 3.04, p = .436). These results are in Figures 3 and 4. Fisher’s least significant differences (LSD) were conducted as post hoc tests for EC scores at time point 1 and time point 2. Results are shown in Table 3. As expected, at time point 1 the Persistent and the CWNS groups were significantly different (p = .02). This difference was replicated in time point 2 (p = .003). Finally, at time point 1 there was a significant difference between the Recovered and the CWNS groups (p = 0.02), but no difference at time point 2. This was expected as, at time point 1 the Recovered group stuttered and the CWNS did not stutter, but at time point 2, neither group stuttered. Within-group differences Repeated measures multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA) assessed the hypothesis that the Transitional children would have higher EC at time point 1 than at time point 2 and the hypothesis that the Transitional groups would have lower NA at time point 1 than at time point 2. The effect of time was significant for NA (p < .01), supporting the hypothesis. However, the effect of time was not significant for EC (p = .62). These findings did not support the hypotheses. EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 16 Additionally, the results reflected a significant effect of time on NA in the Persistent group (p = .01). Results are illustrated in Figure 5 for NA and Figure 6 for EC. No other groups differed between time points. Finally, repeated measures analyses with two main effects (group and time) and one interaction term (group x time) indicated that for NA scores collected at time point 1 and time point 2, the effect of time and time x gender were significant: p < .05 and p< .05 respectively, but no differences in EC. While a time x gender effect would call for further analyses, as the Persistent group contains only 2 girls and 15 boys, further analyses are not possible. In the Persistent group, the mean of NA for girls was 2.57 at time point 1 and 4.10 at time point 2 and for boys was 3.79 at time point 1 and 4.29 at time point 2. Thus, while NA increased at time point 2 for both sexes, the increase was more dramatic for girls. Discussion The present study’s primary purpose was to investigate the relation between EC, NA, and developmental stuttering. It extends previous research on the study of temperament and stuttering as it examines persistence and recovery from stuttering instead of solely the presence or lack of stuttering. Researchers have commented on the heterogeneity of the CWS group (Blood, Ridenour, Qualls, & Hammer, 2003), yet few studies explore differences within the CWS group. By creating subgroups of CWS group (i.e. Persistent, Recovered, and Transitional), this study goes beyond the CWNS and CWS comparison, increasing the current understanding of the relation between temperament and the persistence and recovery from stuttering. Main findings: An overview The study resulted in three main findings. First, at time point 1, children in the Recovered and Persistent groups differed in EC from the CWNS group. At time point 1 both the Recovered and Persistent groups stuttered, suggesting that children who stutter differ from children who do EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 17 not stutter in EC. This is consistent with previous literature and supports the present study’s first hypothesis. Second, at time point 2 only the Persistent group differed from the CWNS group in EC. That is, when the Recovered group stopped stuttering, they no longer differed from the CWNS group. Third, findings indicate that both the Persistent and Transitional groups scored higher in Negative at time point 2 than at time point 1. While it was hypothesized that the Transitional group would have higher NA at time point 2 than at time point 1, this was not hypothesized for the Persistent group. Differences in Effortful Control While EC improves significantly throughout childhood, there are large individual differences in this dimension of temperament. These differences may be influenced by both biological and environmental factors. EC, or self-regulation, is related to the control of thoughts and feelings, as well as the ability to resolve conflicts (Rothbart & Bates, 2006). This aspect of temperament is, thus, important, as children who are not able to regulate themselves tend to stimulate negative reactions from others (Posner & Rothbart, 2007). Additionally, the skills involved in EC are important for learning and social competence (Eisenberg, Valiente, & Eggum, 2010; Eisenberg, Eggum, Vaughan, & Edwards, 2010). Between-group differences. While at time point 1 the Persistent and the Recovered group differed from the CWNS group in EC, they did not differ from the Transitional group. One explanation for this could be that the participants in the transitional group were in the process of developing a stutter and, thus, were not different from the groups who did stutter (i.e. the Persistent and Recovered groups). Additionally, while at time point 2 the Persistent group differed from the CWNS group in EC, it did not differ from the Recovered group. It was also hypothesized that the Transitional group would differ from both the Recovered and the CWNS EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 18 groups at time point 2 and findings did not support this hypothesis. This may be due to the instability of both the Recovered and the Transitional groups. The exact time that the participants in these groups stopped or began to stutter was unknown. Thus, it is possible that a participant who had stopped or began to stutter 7 months prior would differ in EC from a participant who had only just stopped or began to stutter. Within-group differences. While it was hypothesized that the Transitional group would have higher EC at time point 1 than at time point 2, the results of the present study did not support this. The results also did not support the hypothesis that the Recovered group would have lower EC at time point 1 than at time point 2. A possible explanation for these findings may be that the Recovered and the Transitional groups were in the process of change and, thus, highly unstable. Additionally, EC was obtained through caregiver report and may be influenced by extraneous variables. For example, as children age parents expect them to have greater EC and, if their expectations are not met, their judgment may be affected. Similarly, as many participants were entering the school system at the time of this investigation, more was expected from them. Thus, parents may be judging them to be lower in EC compared to similar-aged peers. Differences in Negative Affectivity Between-group differences. Whereas previous literature has found that children who stutter are more negative in their affect than children who do not stutter (e.g. Howell, Davis, Bartrip, & Wormald, 2004; Johnson, Walden, Conture, & Karrass, 2010; Ntourou, Conture, & Walden, 2013), the results of the present study did not support this. Specifically, it was hypothesized that at time point 1 the Recovered and Persistent groups (who both stuttered at time point 1) would have higher NA than the CWNS and Transitional groups (who did not stutter at time point 1). It was also hypothesized that at time point 2 the Persistent and Transitional groups EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 19 would have higher NA than the CWNS and Recovered groups. However, findings did not support these hypotheses. This, again, may be a result from the instability of the Transitional and the Recovered groups. Moreover, as there were only 15-20 participants per group, the study lacked power and may have failed to detect an effect that may actually exist. Within-group differences. As hypothesized, the Transitional group had higher NA at time point 2 than at time point 1. A possible explanation for these findings is that as participants in this group developed a stutter, they may have become frustrated with their speech and, thus, more negative in affect. Moreover, they may be teased by peers, which would again influence their affect. Findings also indicated that participants in the Persistent group also had higher NA at time point 2 than at time point 1. While this was not hypothesized, these findings may be due to the fact that many participants started school between time point 1 and time point 2. An increase in peer interaction could be stressful and increase negative emotions. Another possible explanation for this finding is that as participants in the Persistent group would have been stuttering for a longer period of time at time point 2 than at time point 1, they may have been more frustrated with their speech and become more negative in affect. Finally, while it was hypothesized that the Recovered group would have lower NA at time point 2 than at time point 1, results did not support this. Perhaps negative emotions that arise from stuttering may persist even after the stuttering is gone. Research has found that children who stutter are more likely to be rejected by their peers and are less popular (Davis, Howll, & Cooke, 2002). The negative affect that arises from this is unlikely to change within months of the termination of stuttering. Limitations and future directions The present study had several limitations. First, as there were only 15-30 participants per group, the study lacked power. Thus, an effect that may have actually existed may not have been EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 20 detected (a type 2 error). Future research should aim to have more participants per group to avoid this. However, to increase the number of Transitional participants (i.e. did not stutter at time point 1 and stuttered at time point 2), a very large sample would be needed, as only 3-5% of the population stutters. Second, the results from this study are based on the report of a single parent or caregiver as well as a single testing instrument, the CBQ. As parents may be influenced by social desirability bias, future research should strive to obtain multiple reports from multiple sources (e.g. parent, clinician, and teacher reports; observations home or in the laboratory; and physiological data such as cortisol levels or heart rate). Third, while most participants’ parents completed the longer version of the CBQ (237 questions), a number of participants completed the shorter version of the CBQ (70 questions). Although both tests are known to assess the same variables of EC and NA, future studies should maintain the version of the CBQ constant. Moreover, participants were said to stutter or not stutter based on the amount of SLDs per 100 words as determined by one SLP. While there is high inter-rater reliability within the SLPs in the study, future research should assess participants in different contexts and use different instruments (e.g. the SSI) to determine whether or not he or she stutters. Using multiple criteria has the advantage of a more accurate diagnosis. However, it has the potential of excluding participants who are in a transitional period and don’t satisfy multiple criteria. Finally, while in most cases, time point 1 and time point 2 was divided by a period of eight months, this period was longer for 7 of the participants. Future research should control for time between time points. Conclusions The present findings indicate that children who stutter differ from children who do not stutter in terms of EC and NA. Moreover, children who persisted stuttering, recovered from stuttering, or are in the process of developing a stutter, differ in EC and NA. These differences EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 21 suggest that there is a relation between aspects of temperament and the persistence and recovery from stuttering. Thus, the findings of the present study are essential in understanding the role that EC and NA play in the development and maintenance of stuttering in preschool-aged children. EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 22 References Ahadi, S. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (1994). Temperament, development, and the Big Five. In C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 189-207). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Anderson, J., Pellowski, M., Conture, E., & Kelly, E. (2003). Temperamental characteristics of young children who stutter. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 46(5), 1221–1233. Andrews, G. (1991). Genetic factors in stuttering confirmed. Archives of General Psychiatry 48, 1034-1035. Blood, G.W., Ridenour, V.J., Qualls, C.D., & Hammer, C.S. (2003). Co-occurring disorders in children who stutter. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36(6), 427-448. Buss, A. & Plomin, R. (1987). Temperament: Early developing personality traits. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Conture, E.G. (1991). Young stutterers' speech production: A critical review. In: H.F.M. Peters, W. Hulstijn, & C.W. Starkweather (Ed), Speech motor control and stuttering (pp. 365384). Amsterdam: Elsevier/Excerpta Medica Conture, E. G. (2001). Stuttering: Its nature diagnosis and treatment. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Conture, E. & Caruso, A. (1987). Assessment and diagnosis of childhood disfluency. In L. Rustin, H. Purser, & D. Rowley, (Eds.), Progress in the treatment of fluency disorders (pp. 57-82). London: Taylor and Francis. Conture, E.G., Kelly, E.M., & Walden, T.A. (2013). Temperament, speech and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 46(2), 125-142. EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 23 Davis, S., Howell, P., & Cooke, F. (2002). Sociodynamic relationships between children who stutter and their non-stuttering classmates. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 939-947. Dixon, W. E., JR., & Shore, C. (1997). Temperamental predictors of linguistic style during multiword acquisition. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 99-103. Dixon, W., & Smith, P. (2000). Links between early temperament and language acquisition. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46(3), 417-440. Dunn, L.M. & Dunn, D.M. (1997). Peabody picture vocabulary test-4. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service Inc. Dunn, L.M. & Dunn, D.M. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary test-4. Bloomington, MN: NCS Pearson Inc. Eggers, K., Nil, L., & Van den Bergh, B. (2010). Temperament dimensions in stuttering and typically developing children. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 35(1) 355-372. Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., & Eggum, N.D. (2010). Self-regulation and school readiness. Early Education and Development, 21, 681-698. Eisenberg, N., Eggum, N., Vaughan, J., & Edwards, A. (2010). Relations of selfregulatory/control capacities to maladjustment, social competence, and emotionality. In: R. Hoyle (Eds.) Handbook of personality and self-regulation. New York: Wiley. Embrechts, M., Ebben H., Franke P., & van de Poel, C. (2000). Temperament: A comparison between children who stutter and children who do not stutter. Bosshardt, H.G., Yaruss, J., Peters, H. (Eds.). Proceedings of the third world congress on fluency disorders: Theory, research, treatment, and self-help. Nijmegen, The Netherlands: University Press Nijmegen. EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 24 Felsenfeld, S., van Beijsterveldt, C.E., & Boomsma, D.I. (2010). Attentional regulation in young twins with probable stuttering, high nonfluency, and typical fluency. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Resarch, 53, 1147–1166. Fowlie, G.M., & Cooper, E.B. (1978). Traits attributed to stuttering and nonstuttering children by their mothers. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 3, 233–246. Glasner, J. (1949). Personality characteristics and emotional problems in stutterers under the Age of Five. Journal of Speech and Language Disorders, 14(1) 135-138. Goldman, R. & Fristoe, M. (2000). Goldman-Fristoe test of articulation-2. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services Inc. Goldsmith, H., Buss, A., Plomin, R., Rothbart, M., Thomas, A., & Chess, S.(1987). Roundtable: What is temperament? Four approaches. Journal of Child Development, 58, 504–529. Goldsmith, H.H., Reilly, J., Lemery, K.S., Longley, S., & Prescott ,A. (1999). The Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery: Preschool Version. University of Wisconsin. Guitar, B. (1998). Stuttering: An integrated approach to its nature and treatment. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. Howell, P., Davis, S., Bartrip, J., & Wormald, L. (2004). Effectiveness of frequency shifted feedback at reducing disfluency for linguistically easy, and difficult, sections of speech (original audio recordings included). Stammering Research : An on-Line Journal Published by the British Stammering Association, 1(3), 309–315. Hresko, W., Reid, D., & Hamill. D. (1999). Test of early language development-3. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Jones, R. M., Choi, D., Conture, E.G., & Walden, T.A. (2014). Temperament, emotion, and childhood stuttering. Seminars in Speech and Lanuguage, 35(2), 114-131. EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 25 Johnson, K. N., Walden, T. A., Conture, E. G., & Karrass, J. (2010). Spontaneous regulation of emotions in preschool children who stutter: Preliminary findings. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 1478-1495. Johnson, K.N., Conture, E.G., & Walden, T.A. (2012). Efficacy of attention regulation in preschool-age children who stutter: A preliminary investigation. Journal of Communication Disorders, 45(4), 263-278. Kagan, J. (1989). Temperamental contributions to social behavior. American Psychologist, 44(4), 668-674. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.4.668 Karrass, J., Walden, T., Conture, E., Graham, C., Arnold, H., Hartfield, K., & Schwenk, K. (2006). Relation of emotional reactivity and regulation to childhood stuttering. Journal of Communication Disorders, 39 (6) 402-423. Kefalianos, E., Onslow, M., Ukoumunne, O., Block, S., & Reilly, S. (2014). Stuttering, temperament, and anxiety: Data from a community cohort ages 2-4 years. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research (Online), 57(4), 1314-1322. Kent, R.D. (2000). Research on speech motor control and its disorders: A review and prospective. Journal of Communication Disorders, 33(5), 391-428. Kraft, S., Ambrose, N., & Chon, H. (2014). Temperament and environmental contributions to stuttering severity in children: the role of effortful control. Seminar Speech Language, 35(2), 80-94. Lewis K.E., & Goldberg L.L. (1997). Measurements of temperament in the identification of children who stutter. European Journal of Disorders of Communication, 32(4), 441–448. National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders. (2015). Stuttering (NIH Pub. No. 10-4232). Retrieved from EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 26 http://www.nidcd.nih.gov/health/voice/pages/stutter.aspx Ntourou, K., Conture E., & Walden, T. (2013). Emotional reactivity and regulation in preschoolage children who stutter. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 38 (3), 260-277. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2013.06.002 Posner, M.I. & Rothbart, M.K. (2007) Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 1-23. Riley, G. (1980). Stuttering severity instrument for children and adults (Rev. ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Riley, G. (1994) Stuttering severity instrument for children and adults-3. SSI-3. (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Reilly, S., Onslow, M., Packman et al. (2013). Natural History of Stuttering to 4 Years of Age: A Prospective Community-Based Study. Pediatrics, 36, 460-467. Rothbart, M.K. (2007). Temperament, development, and personality. Association for Psychological Sciences, 16 207-212. Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: The Children's Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development, 72(5), 1394-1408. Rothbart, M.K. & Bates, J.E. (2006). Temperament. In: N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Social, emotional, and personality development (Vol. 3)(pp. 99-166). W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of Child Psychology. New York, NY: Wiley. Rothbart, M.K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual differences in temperament. Advances in developmental psychology, 1, 37–86. Rothbart, M. K., & Rueda, M. R. (2005). The development of effortful control. In U. Mayr, E. EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 27 Awh, & S. KeeleEndFragment (Eds.), Developing individuality in the human brain: A tribute to Michael I. Posner (167-188). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Salley, B. J., & Dixon, W. E. (2007). Temperamental and joint attentional predictors of language development. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly (Wayne State University. Press), 53(1), 131–154. Schwenk, K., Conture, E.G., & Walden, T.A. (2007). Reaction to background stimulation of preschool children who do and do not stutter. Journal of Communication Disorders, 40,129–141. Thomas, A. & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: Brunner/Mazel Van den Bergh, B., & Ackx, M. (2003). Een Nederlandse versie van Rothbarts ‘Children’s Behavior Questionnaire’. [The Dutch version of Rothbart’s Children’s Behavior Questionnaire] Kind en Adolescent, 24, 77–78. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: The disposition to experience aversive emotional states. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 465-490. Williams, K. (1997). Expressive vocabulary test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services Inc. Williams, K. (2007). Expressive vocabulary test-2. Bloomington, MN: NCS Pearson Inc. Yari, E. (1981). Disfluencies of normally speaking two-year-old children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 24, 490-495. Yari, E. & Ambrose, N. (1992b). A longitudinal study of stuttering in children: A preliminary report. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 755-760. Yari, E. & Lewis, B. (1984). Disfluencies at the onset of stuttering. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 154-159. EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 28 Zebrowski, P., & Conture, E.G. (1998). Influence of non-treatment variables on treatment effectiveness for school-age children who stutter. In: A. Cordes, & R. Ingham (Ed.). Treatment efficacy for stuttering: A search for empirical bases (pp. 293–310). San Diego, CA: Singular 29 EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY Tables and Figures Figure 1 Effortful Control Scores at Time Point 1 Effortful Control score on CBQ 5.1 5 4.9 4.8 4.7 4.6 4.5 4.4 4.3 4.2 4.1 4 Persistent Transitional CWNS Recovered Stutter Group Note: Persistent and Recoverd groups differed from CWNS group Figure 2 Effortful Control Scores at Time Point 2 Effortdul Control Score on CBQ 5 4.9 4.8 4.7 4.6 4.5 4.4 4.3 4.2 4.1 4 Persistent Transitional CWNS Stutter Group Note: Persistent group differed from CWNS group Recovered 30 EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY Figure 3 Negative Affectivity Scores at Time Point 1 Negative Affectivity score on CBQ 4.2 4.1 4 3.9 3.8 3.7 3.6 3.5 3.4 3.3 Persistent- Transitional CWNS Stutter Group Recovered Note: Groups did not differ Figure 4 Negative Affectivity Scores at Time Point 2 Negative Affectivity Score on CBQ 4.4 4.3 4.2 4.1 4 3.9 3.8 3.7 Persistent Transitional CWNS Stutter Group Note: Groups did not differ Recovered 31 EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY Figure 5 Effortful Control Score on CBQ 5 Change in Effortful Control Between Time Point 1 and Time Point 2 4.9 4.8 4.7 4.6 Persistent- 4.5 Transitional 4.4 CWNS 4.3 Recovered 4.2 Before After Timepoint Note: Groups did not differ from time point 1 to time point 2 Figure 6 Negative Affectivity Score on CBQ Change in Negative Affectivity Between Time Point 1 and Time Point 2 4.3 4.2 4.1 4 3.9 3.8 Persistent- 3.7 Transitional 3.6 CWNS 3.5 Recovered 3.4 3.3 Before After Timepoint Note: Persistent and Transitional groups differed from time point 1 to time point 2 32 EFFORTFUL CONTROL AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY Table 1: Dimensions of Temperament Scales that Contribute to Each Higher Order Factor on the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire Extraversion/Surgency Impulsivity High Intensity Pleasure Activity Level Positive Anticipation Negative Affectivity Discomfort Sadness Fear Anger/Frustration Effortful Control Low Intensity Pleasure Inhibitory Control Perceptual Sensitivity Attentional Focusing Smiling/Laughter Soothability/Falling Reactivity (loading negatively) Attentional Shifting Shyness (loading negatively) Excitatory Control Table 2: Group Means for Negative Affectivity and Effortful Control NA time point 1 NA time point 2 EC time point 1 3.65 4.11 4.49* Persistent 3.87 4.23 4.82 Transitional 3.64 3.96 4.93 CWNS 3.98 4.07 4.57* Recovered NOTE: * Significant at p < 05. EC time point 2 4.46* 4.72 4.85 4.66 Table 3: Least Significant Difference Scores for Effortful Control at Time Point 1 and Time Point 2 Groups Persistent Transitional CWNS Recovered Persistent 1 0.089 0.003* 0.115 Transitional 0.113 1 0.335 0.689 CWNS 0.015* 0.56 1 0.097 Recovered 0.654 0.177 0.019* 1 NOTE: above diagonal is time point 1 and below diagonal is time point 2 * Significant at p < 05.