RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS Running Head: RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia Levels and Fluctuation in Children of Depressed versus Nondepressed Mothers Alexa Curhan Vanderbilt University 1 RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 2 Abstract Previous research has demonstrated that offspring of depressed mothers are at increased risk for developing dysfunctional affect regulation, which is a risk factor for the onset of depression and other psychopathology. One way in which depression may be transmitted from mothers to their children is through dysfunctional neuroregulatory mechanisms, especially those related to affect regulation. Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia (RSA) has been shown to be related to affect regulation, and RSA level and fluctuation index an individual’s autonomic flexibility. The present study investigated whether there were differences in RSA level and fluctuation in offspring of depressed and nondepressed mothers, as well as how RSA levels related to affect. The study consisted of 92 mother-child dyads (37 mothers with a history of depression and 55 nondepressed mothers). Mothers and children completed questionnaires, and RSA data were obtained from children while they watched brief video clips (neutral, negative, and positive). RSA levels or fluctuation did not differ significantly between children of depressed and nondepressed mothers, and RSA levels did not significantly predict children’s affect during the mood induction videos. Exploratory analyses revealed a nonsignificant, trend for child sex and mother’s level of depression symptoms to predict RSA during the mood induction videos. Limitations of the current study and directions for future research are discussed. RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 3 Approximately 1 in 10 adults in the United States has experienced a major depressive episode (MDE) in their lifetime (CDC.gov), which results in about 15 million children growing up in a household with a parent who has had an MDE during their life (Forehand, Thigpen, Parent, Hardcastle, Bettis & Compas, 2012). Children of depressed parents are 3 to 4 times more likely to experience a depressive episode themselves (Goodman & Gotlib, 1999; Hammen & Brennan, 2003; Weissman, Wickramaratne, Nomura, Warner, Pilowsky & Verdeli, 2006). Offspring of depressed parents also are at increased risk of negative behavioral and emotional outcomes, have increased levels of internalizing and externalizing problems, and experience a narrower range of emotions (Connell, Hughes-Scalise, Klostermann & Azem, 2011 & Goodman, Rouse, Conelll, Broth, Hall, & Heyward, 2011). Maternal depression also has been shown to be associated with problems in affect in both mothers and children. Affect refers to the display or experience of emotions and can be categorized into negative and positive and high or low. Maternal depression has been related to low levels of positive affect (Connell et al., 2011), and low positive affect in mothers has been found to be associated with an increased likelihood of depression in children (Dietz et al., 2008). Low positive affect in mothers also is related to low positive affect in their children, which increases the chances of the children developing depression (Forbes & Dahl, 2005 & Lonigan, Hooe, David & Kistner, 1999). Thus, children of depressed parents are at high risk for developing depression and other negative outcomes. How might depression be transmitted from parent to child? Goodman and Gotlib (1999) proposed four ways in which parents likely transmit depressive symptoms, including genes and exposure to negative maternal cognitions, behavior and affect. A third means of transmission is through dysfunctional neuroregulatory mechanisms. RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 4 Ashamn, Dawson, and Panagiotides (2008) found that offspring of mothers with depression had changes in psychophysiological systems that were related to emotion regulation. They also found that children of depressed mothers had increased heart rate and decreased vagal tone as compared to with offspring of nondepressed mothers. Dysfunctional psychophysiological systems have been found to increase the risk for the onset of psychopathology and may be one possible mechanism underlying affect dysregulation and depression. What is Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia? Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia (RSA) has been used to assess the functioning of underlying systems of affect regulation, which indicate individuals’ ability to adaptively respond to the environment and regulate their affect (Porges, Doussard-Roosevelt & Maiti, 1994). According to Porges and the Polyvagal theory (1994, 1997, 2007), RSA level and fluctuation are indicators of the functioning of the parasympathetic nervous system, and they quantify the effect of the vagus nerve on heart rate. RSA is measured by assessing the extent to which respiration creates variability in heart rate. Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia is divided into an individual’s RSA level and RSA fluctuation; each construct reflects a different aspect of the individual’s autonomic flexibility (Salomon, 2005). Baseline RSA reflects an individual’s parasympathetic activity while at rest, referred to as RSA level. When faced with a stimulus, the vagal pathway combines cardiac autonomic activity with emotional behavioral processes to either augment or suppress parasympathetic activity. When confronted with a new or evocative stimulus, RSA is suppressed in order to mobilize an individuals resources, such as in the fight or flight response. RSA augmentation occurs when the vagal brake is applied to allow for parasympathetic activity and decrease energy expenditure (Porges, 1994, 1997). The augmentation and suppression of RSA presumably index the autonomic flexibility of an individual. RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 5 RSA Level RSA level is a measure of the influence of the vagus nerve on heart rate when an individual is at rest. Past studies have shown that RSA level is related to temperament and emotionality (Ashman et al., 2008; Beauchine, 2001). High resting RSA levels have been found to be associated with social competence, adaptive coping, effective emotion regulation, and resiliency to risks, such as maternal depression (Conell et al., 2011; El-Sheikh Harger & Whitson, 2001; Morales, Beekman, Blandon, Stifter, & Buss, 2014; Rottenberg, 2007). Low resting RSA levels in children are associated with depression, behavioral problems, emotional inflexibility, increased internalizing and externalizing disorders, and dysregulated emotional states (Boyce, Quas, Alkon, Smider, Essex & Kupfer,. 2001; Conell et al., 2011, El-Sheikh et al., 2001; Field , 1995; Pine et al., 1998). Previous research has revealed inconsistent findings regarding RSA level and affect. Oveis, Cohen, Gruber, Shiota, Haidt & Keltner (2009) reported that higher RSA level was associated with higher stability of positive mood, level of positive mood in response to a neutral stimuli, and tonic positive emotionality. Previous research on RSA level and negative emotions has mainly focused on stress and anxiety, but these results also have been mixed (Oevis et al., 2009). Regarding RSA level and sadness, Oevis and colleagues found no relation between RSA level and tonic negative emotionality. Silvia, Jackson & Sopko (2014) compared RSA and a multitude of assessments of dispositional positive emotions across two large data sets but could not replicate any of the results of Oevis et al. and did not find any significant associations between RSA level and positive affect. A third study found that higher baseline RSA was associated with greater positive emotional reactivity and lower negative emotional reactivity RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 6 (Calkins, 1997). One purpose of the current study was to further explore the relation of RSA level to affect in children at risk for depression. RSA Fluctuation RSA fluctuation is the measure of the augmentation and withdrawal of the vagal brake on parasympathetic activity, and allows individuals to flexibly respond to changing environmental demands. Beauchine (2001) suggested that previous research has shown that RSA fluctuation may reflect an individual’s attention regulation, emotion regulation, and mood. Adaptive fluctuations reflect an individual’s ability to physiologically respond in a way that allows sufficient, but not excessive, mobilization of resources to meet the demands of the stressor. Having an appropriate degree of RSA fluctuation in response to stressors can reduce the likelihood of the onset psychopathology (Morales et al., 2014; Rottenberg, 2007), as well as increase individuals’ ability to adaptively regulate their emotions (Blandon, Calkins, Keane & O’Brien, 2008). In contrast, maladaptive RSA fluctuation manifests itself in either insufficient or excessive reactivity. Insufficient RSA fluctuation indicates that an individual did not suppress parasympathetic activity enough, which in turn, would not allow for sufficient sympathetic activity to help prepare the body to deal with the stressor. Excessive RSA fluctuation results in the body suppressing parasympathetic activity too much, which would result in the body spending a disproportionate amount of energy on a stressor. Insufficient or excessive RSA fluctuation is associated with higher levels of anxiety, hostility, alexithymia, impulse control disorders, internalizing disorders, Major Depressive Disorder and other psychopathology (Boyce et al., 2001; Bylsma, Salomon, Taylor-Clift, Morris & Rottenberg, 2014; Friedman & Thayer, 1998; Morales et al., 2014; Nederhof, Marceau, Shirtcliff, Hastings, & Oldehinkell, 2014; RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 7 Rottenberg, 2007). A recent meta-analysis demonstrated that there was a moderate association between lower levels of RSA suppression in response to stress and increased internalizing problems (Hastings, Klimes-Dougan, Kendziora, Brand & Zahn-Waxler, 2014). The current study focused on RSA fluctuation during film clips that elicit sadness and happiness. Previous research has shown that during film clips that induce sadness, the typical and adaptive response is moderate levels of RSA augmentation, which results in an increase in heart rate variability (Hastings et al., 2014). Responses to sad stimuli that deviate from this patterns pose a risk for dysfunctional emotional regulation, either in excessive or insufficient autonomic reactivity. Studies have yielded mixed results for what the adaptive response to a happy stimulus should be. In an earlier meta-analysis, Cacioppo, Berntson, Larsen, Pohlmann & Ito (2000) concluded that positive stimuli evoke less fluctuation than negative stimuli. In contrast, Shiota, Neufeld, Yeung, Moser, & Perea (2011) described a series of studies in which positive stimuli evoked an increase in RSA, and attempted to clarify the previous inconsistent findings on RSA fluctuation in response to positive stimuli. They found that RSA level decreased significantly from baseline for the positive emotions of enthusiasm, attachment love, nurturing, love, and awe, but they did not look at happiness. Another purpose of the current study was to further study children’s responses to a happy stimulus. RSA in Offspring of Depressed Mothers RSA is particularly important in offspring of depressed mothers because one major risk factor for depression is physiological inflexibility and poor affect regulation. Findings have been mixed regarding RSA fluctuation in offspring of depressed mothers. Ashman et al. (2008) reported that offspring of chronically depressed mothers exhibited increased RSA fluctuation in response to happy and sad mood induction videos when compared to offspring of healthy RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 8 controls. In contrast, Forbes, Fox, Cohn, Galles & Kovacs (2006) found that offspring of mother’s with a history of depression had reduced heart period flexibility in response to mood induction tasks compared to children of healthy mothers. Another study found no significant difference in either RSA level or fluctuation between offspring of depressed and nondepressed mothers (Yaraslovsky, Rottenberg & Kovacs, 2014). Interestingly within a sample of children of depressed mothers, Blandon et al. (2008) found that children with higher levels of RSA had less ability to regulate their emotions as compared to children with lower RSA levels. Others have found that higher RSA was related to positive outcomes, such that children with higher RSA levels and adaptive levels of RSA fluctuation were more likely to be buffered against the negative effects of marital conflict between their parents (Katz & Gottman, 1997). Moreover, lower levels of RSA in children exposed to marital hostility was associated with higher levels of behavioral problems. A meta-analysis by Rottenberg, Clift, Bolden & Salomon (2007) found that adults with Major Depressive Disorder exhibited lower levels of RSA. This finding is particularly important given that one hypothesized mode of transmission of depression from mothers to children is through dysfunctional neuroregulatory mechanisms (Goodman & Gotlib, 1999). Thus, it is possible that depressed mothers’ low levels of RSA is somehow transmitted to their offspring. This literature review highlights four areas for which additional research is needed. First, due to the small amount of studies of RSA in offspring of depressed mothers, further research is needed to detect if there are differences in RSA while at rest between children of depressed and nondepressed mothers. Second, research on RSA fluctuation has shown that maladaptive, insufficient or excessive RSA fluctuation is related to negative outcomes, yet it is unclear whether offspring of depressed mothers have atypical RSA fluctuation. Third, the research on RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 9 RSA fluctuation in response to a positive stimuli has been unclear, so further study of whether problems in affect regulation also occur in response to positive stimuli is needed. Understanding the underlying neuroregulatory mechanisms in affect regulation, especially in offspring of depressed mothers, is important because it points to possible ways of decreasing the risk of developing psychopathology. In addition, previous research has mainly focused on very young children, so studying RSA in at risk children at an older age is important, especially during the years leading up to the common age of onset for MDD. In addition, previous studies have examined how RSA level relates to different emotions, but little is known about how RSA level relates to affect in offspring of depressed mothers, both at a trait level and in response to stimuli. To address these issues, the current study examined RSA level and fluctuation in children of depressed versus nondepressed mothers. The following hypotheses were tested: (1) children of depressed mothers will have lower resting RSA at baseline than children of nondepressed mothers; (2) children of depressed mothers will have reduced RSA reactivity compared to children of nondepressed mothers during the mood induction videos; (3) higher levels of resting RSA will be significantly associated with higher levels of positive affect and lower levels of negative affect. Method Participants Participants were 92 mothers-child dyads. Mothers either met criteria for a depressive episode during their child’s lifetime (n=37) or had no history of a depressive disorder during their child’s life (n=55). Children were ages 8, 9, or 10 years old (mean age = 9.39, SD = .83); if the mother had more than one child in this age range, one child was chosen at random. The sample of children was 55% female; 57% of the children were Caucasian, 25% African RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 10 American, 14% other, and 4% unreported. The mean income of the sample was $90,000$100,000. Mothers’ education levels were as follows: 52% completed all or part of graduate school, 33% graduated college, and 15% completed part of college or less. Mothers’ marital status was as follows: 80% married or co-habiting, 6.6% divorced, 13.4% other. Table 1 presents the demographic data by group. Participants were recruited through listservs, medical clinics, and birth databases at Vanderbilt University and Emory University. Possible participants completed a phone screen to assess eligibility and to determine whether the mother had a history of depression during the child’s lifetime. Exclusion criteria were maternal psychosis, Bipolar I Disorder, Schizophrenia or current substance abuse, or child developmental disabilities, autism, or psychosis. Procedure for Psychophysiological Assessment The current study was part of a larger study about positive affect in children of depressed and nondepressed mothers. Mother-child dyads came into the lab for a 3-hour session. Upon arrival, the mother and child were consented and assented, respectively. Children completed questionnaires with a research assistant, while the mother completed questionnaires in a separate room. Children went to a different lab to complete the mood task while psychophysiological measures were recorded. The researchers connected three cardiovascular electrodes to the child and a respiration belt, which were all connected to a BioLog that recorded ECG output. Two electrodes were placed on either side of the rib cage at heart level, and the third was placed centrally just under the collar bone. The child was seated in a chair facing a video screen, and the researcher sat beside the child to monitor the BioLog. Children were told that they would hear and watch a RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 11 series of audio and video clips, and were instructed to focus on their feelings while they viewed the video segments. The video segments were viewed in a fixed order and created six 4-minute sections: baseline, negative mood, recovery, positive mood, recovery, and final baseline. First, a baseline audio story that detailed facts about plants was played for four minutes in order to assess resting RSA. Second, a negative video clip, from either Bambi, Lion King, or Land before Time (i.e., the negative mood condition) was played for four minutes during which RSA was monitored. Third, a neutral video (fish swimming) was played for four minutes to assess RSA recovery. Fourth, a positive video clip, from either Happy Feet, Lion King, or The Princess and the Frog (i.e., the positive mood condition), was played for four minutes during which RSA was measured. Fifth, a different four minute neutral fish video was played to assess RSA recovery. Finally, a fourminute audio story detailing facts about birds was played. Immediately following each clip, children were asked how they were feeling. The researcher placed an event marker on the BioLog at the beginning and end of every video clip to signify the different conditions. The electrodes were then removed from the child, and the child was reunited with their mother. At the end of the study, mothers were compensated for participating and children were given the opportunity to select a small toy from a “treasure chest.” Computation of RSA Scores for both RSA level and fluctuation were calculated at Emory University using the CardioEdit and CardioBatch programs (Porges, 1985 & Porges & Bohrer, 1990). RSA level during the audio story, neutral fish video, and mood induction videos was calculated by splitting the data into 30-second epochs and taking the average score of all of the epochs in order to fully capture what occurred during each four minute segment. RSA fluctuation for the negative video RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 12 was calculated by subtracting RSA level during the baseline audio story from the RSA level during the mood induction video, and the positive fluctuation score was calculated by subtracting RSA level during the neutral fish video from RSA level during the positive video. Measures Child Measures The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule for Children (PANAS-C; Laurent et al., 2009) is a 30-item child report measure of positive and negative affectivity. The scale was administered twice to the child with several other measures between administrations of the trait and state versions of the PANAS. The trait PANAS asked children how often they feel each adjective in general. The state version of the PANAS asked children how much they felt each adjective at the present moment. Children rated on a 5-point Likert scale how much they felt that way, ranging from 1 (not much or not at all) to 5 (a lot). Laurent et al. (2009) found that the PANAS-C yielded high internal consistency as well as good convergent and divergent validity of the positive and negative affect scale scores with reports of anxiety and depressive symptoms. In the present study, internal consistencies for the PA and NA scales were .88 and .91, respectively, for the trait questionnaire, and .92 and .70, respectively for the state questionnaire. Smiley Faces Questionnaire measures children’s mood at the moment. First, children were asked whether they felt happy/good or unhappy/bad during the previous four-minute segment. Children then were asked whether they felt that way a lot, somewhere in between, or a little, using a 3-point Likert scale. Each mood score could range from 1 to 6. Feelings and Me Questionnaire (Kovacs, Rottenberg, & George, 2009) is a 67-item questionnaire about emotion regulation. The measure asks children how true each statement is for them using a 3-point Likert scale ranging from “Not true of me,” “Sometimes true of me” to RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 13 “Many times true of me.” Of the 67 items, 55 ask children about times when they are sad or upset, which contains statements such as “When I feel sad or upset, I throw, kick or hit things” and “When I feel sad or upset, I think about everything being my fault.” Four items ask children about when they are angry or mad, which include statements such as “When I feel angry or mad I break or kick things” and “When I feel angry or mad I have a fight with my family or friends.” Four items ask children about when they feel scared or nervous, which includes statements such as “When I feel scared or nervous, I try to take deep breaths or breathe slowly” and “When I feel scared or nervous, I get mom or dad to hold me.” Four items ask children about when they are happy, and includes “When I feel happy, I play or have fun with friends” and “When I feel happy, I think about how good I feel.” The internal consistency for each subscale were .92, .72, .62, and .66, respectively. Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (MFQ-Child Self-Report Short Version; Angold, Costello, Messer, Pickles, Winder & Silver 1995) is a 13-item measure of how they were feeling or acting over the past two weeks. Children report how true each statement was on a 3-point Likert scale ranging from “Not true,” “Sometimes true,” to “True.” Items include “I felt unhappy or miserable,” “I cried a lot,” and “I hated myself.” Internal consistency was .76. Screen for Child Anxiety Related Disorders (SCARED-Child Version; Birmaher, Brent, Chiappetta, Bridge, Monga & Baugher, 1995) is a 41-item child report measure of anxiety symptoms. The measure asks the child how true each statement was over the past three months. Each item is on a 3-point Likert scale ranging from “Not true or Hardly ever true,” “Sometimes true or Somewhat true” to “Very true or Often true.” The items include statements such as “When I feel frightened, it is hard to breathe” and “I get headaches when I am at school”. Parent Measures RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 14 Parent Perception of Child Temperament is a 20-item measure of parents’ report about their child’s temperament. Mothers reported how well each statement described their child on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from “Not at all like your child” to “Describes your child very well”. Items include statements such as “My child tends to be shy” and “My child cries easily”. The Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CESD; Radloff 1977) is a 20item measure of depressive symptoms over the past week. Mothers reported how often they felt each item using a 4-point Likert ranging from “Rarely or none of the time/Less than 1 day” to “Most or all of the time/5-7 days”. The items include statements such as “I was bothered by things that usually don’t bother me” and “I felt depressed.” Results Demographic Characteristics Demographic characteristics are listed in Table 1. The depressed and nondepressed mothers and their children did not differ significantly on demographic variables, except that nondepressed mothers were more likely to be married, and to have a higher income. Missing Data Of the 92 participants, 6 high-risk and 9 low-risk children were missing baseline RSA data (audio story 1); 6 high- and 6 low-risk children were missing RSA data during the negative video, 4 high- and 5 low-risk children were missing data during the neutral video (Fish 1), and 4 high- and 8 low-risk children were missing RSA data during the positive video. Data were missing due to editing problems, data collection errors, and problems with the data file. Do offspring of depressed and nondepressed mothers differ in resting RSA? RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 15 Level between groups. An Independent Samples t-test was run to test whether there was a difference in resting RSA at baseline between offspring of depressed vs. nondepressed mothers (See Table 2 & 3). No significant difference was found between the groups (p=.892). Do offspring of depressed mothers have reduced reactivity to mood induction videos? Negative Video RSA Fluctuation. An Independent Samples t-test was run to test for differences in fluctuation during the negative video between offspring of depressed versus nondepressed mothers (see Tables 2 & 3). The fluctuation score was calculated by subtracting baseline RSA during the baseline neutral stimulus (audio story) from RSA during the negative video. No significant differences were found between the groups (p=.731), indicating that offspring of depressed mothers did not differ significantly in their RSA reactivity in response to a negative video as compared to children of nondepressed mothers. Positive Video RSA Fluctuation. An Independent Samples t-test was run to determine whether there was a difference in fluctuation during the positive video between offspring of depressed versus nondepressed mothers (see Tables 2 & 3). The fluctuation was calculated by subtracting RSA level during the neutral video (fish) from RSA level during the positive video. No significant difference in fluctuation was found (p=.609) indicating that offspring of depressed and nondepressed mothers did not differ significantly in their RSA reactivity in response to the positive video. Is RSA level at baseline associated with affect? Baseline RSA Level and Affect. A Pearson Correlation was run to examine whether RSA level at baseline was correlated with scores on the PANAS (Laurent et al., 2009) and the Smiley Faces Questionnaire (see Table 4). No significant correlations were found between scores on the RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 16 PANAS and baseline RSA levels. That is, baseline RSA levels were not correlated with positive affect or negative affect on either the trait or state PANAS. Pearson Correlations comparing baseline RSA level and the Smiley Faces Questionnaire responses (see Table 4) revealed a significant, negative correlation between the baseline mood rating and resting RSA levels (r=-.314; p=.006). Higher resting RSA levels were associated with more negative mood ratings in response to the audio story. Resting RSA Levels were not significantly correlated with the mood ratings in response to the negative, neutral, or positive videos. Thus, baseline RSA was not correlated with the affect measures, except for the baseline mood rating. Baseline RSA Level predicting Affect. To further explore the relation between baseline RSA levels and affect, linear regression analyses were conducted to examine whether RSA level at baseline predicted scores on the PANAS state and trait (see Tables 5, 6, 7, & 8). RSA levels at baseline did not significantly predict positive or negative affect on either the state or trait PANAS measures. Linear regression analyses also were conducted to determine whether RSA level at baseline predicted mood ratings in response to the baseline, negative, and positive videos. Baseline RSA levels did significantly predict the mood ratings in response to the baseline audio story (see Table 9), the negative video (p=.667), or the positive video (p=.335). Thus, baseline RSA levels did not significantly predict affect when measured by the PANAS or the mood ratings on the Smiley Faces Questionnaire, but baseline RSA level did significantly predict the mood rating in response to the baseline audio story. Relation between RSA fluctuation and children’s affect RSA Fluctuation and Affect. Pearson correlations of RSA fluctuation during the mood induction videos indicated that neither state nor trait PANAS PA and NA were significantly RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 17 correlated with RSA fluctuation during the positive or negative videos (see Table 10). RSA fluctuation during the mood induction videos also was compared to scores on the Smiley Faces Questionnaire. No significant correlation was found with RSA fluctuation during the positive video. The correlation between RSA fluctuation in response to the negative video and scores on the Smiley Faces Questionnaire after the negative video yielded a nonsignificant trend (r=.226; p=.051), indicating that RSA fluctuation during the negative video tended to covary with the mood ratings in response to the negative video. Relation between RSA (levels and fluctuations) and demographic variables Sex. An Independent Samples t-test conducted to examine whether there was a difference in RSA level between males and females revealed no significant sex difference (p=.688). The Independent Samples t-test examining difference in RSA fluctuation between males and females yielded a nonsignificant trend regarding fluctuation during the negative video (t(74)=1.757, p=.083) indicating that girls tended to have reduced RSA reactivity in response to the negative video as compared to boys. No significant sex difference was found for fluctuation during the positive video [t(78)=.708; p=.481]. Children’s Age, Mother’s Depression Level, and Marital Status. Table 11 presents the Pearson correlations among baseline RSA level, RSA fluctuation, and children’s age, mothers’ depression level (CESD), and marital status and indicate no significant correlations among baseline RSA level, RSA fluctuation, and these demographic variables. There was a significant correlation between fluctuation during the negative video and marital status (r=.237; p=.04) such that children of currently married mothers showed greater reactivity during the negative video than children of mothers who were not currently married. A linear regression analysis controlling for the risk (i.e., offspring of depressed vs. nondepressed) demonstrated that marital status RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 18 significantly predicted RSA fluctuation during the negative video (p=.029), such that children of married mothers had higher reactivity during the negative video (See Table 12). RSA fluctuation during the positive video was not significantly correlated with children’s age, mothers’ depression levels or marital status. Relation of RSA (level and fluctuation) to emotion regulation and anxiety A Table 13 presents the Pearson correlations among RSA level, RSA fluctuation, and measures of children’s emotion regulation (FAM-C & SMFQ), anxiety (SCARED), and temperament (Parent Perception of Child Temperament). Neither RSA levels at baseline nor RSA fluctuation in response to the mood induction videos were correlated with children’s emotion regulation, anxiety symptoms, or parent report of the child’s temperament. What factors predicted RSA during the mood induction videos? Table 14 presents the results of a linear regression analysis testing predictors of RSA during the positive video, including baseline RSA level, children’s age and sex, mothers’ CESD total score, the interaction between children’s sex and mothers’ CESD, and the four subscales of the measure of child temperament (PPCT). The interaction between the children’s sex and mothers’ CES-D yielded a nonsignificant trend toward predicting RSA during the positive video (p=.061). Girls whose mothers had higher scores on the CES-D tended to have lower RSA levels during the positive video. Also, the shyness subscale on the PPCT had a nonsignificant trend toward predicting RSA level during the positive video, where children with higher scores of shyness had lower levels of RSA during the video (p=.051). Table 15 presents the results of a linear regression analysis testing predictors of RSA during the negative video, including baseline RSA level, children’s age and sex, mothers’ CESD total score, the interaction between children’s sex and mothers’ CESD, and the subscales of the RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 19 PPCT. The interaction between the children’s sex and mothers’ CES-D yielded a nonsignificant trend toward predicting RSA during the negative video (p=.074), indicating that girls tended to have lower RSA levels than boys during the negative video. Finally, because of the observed sex differences in RSA during both the negative and positive video, further analyses were conducted to see if the mood ratings in response to the mood induction videos we conducted independent samples t-tests to determine whether males and females differed in their mood ratings in response to the mood induction videos. A significant difference was found between males and females in their mood rating in response to the positive video (p=.016), where females rated the video as significantly more positive than males (see Table 16). A nonsignificant trend was found for mood ratings in response to the negative video (p=.063), where girls rated the negative video as more negative than boys. Thus, girls showed greater reactivity to the mood induction videos than did boys. Discussion Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia level and fluctuation have been shown to be an index of parasympathetic autonomic flexibility. High resting RSA level is associated with positive outcomes such as social competence, adaptive coping, and resiliency, whereas low resting RSA is associated with negative outcomes such as behavioral problems and emotion dysregulation (Boyce et al., 2001; Conell et al., 2011; El-Sheikh et al., 2001; Field, 1995; Morales et al., 2014; Pine et al., 1998; Rottenberg, 2007). Atypical RSA fluctuation also may be associated with increased risk for the onset of MDD, anxiety disorders, and higher levels of internalizing problems and impulse controls disorders (Boyce et al., 2001; Bylsma et al., 2014; Friedman & Thayer, 1998; Morales et al., 2014; Nederhof et al., 2014; Rottenberg, 2007). Few studies, however, have examined the link between RSA level/fluctuation and affect in offspring of RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 20 mothers with depression. The current study looked at differences in RSA in offspring of depressed and nondepressed mothers, as well as how RSA related to affect. The first hypothesis posited that offspring of depressed and nondepressed mothers differ in their baseline RSA level was not supported. One possible explanation for the failure to find group differences is the relatively low sample size, resulting in low power to detect effects. Moreover, the 15 participants who were missing data further reduced the already small sample of children of depressed mothers. The second hypothesis of reduced fluctuation in response to both positive and negative mood induction videos in children of depressed versus nondepressed mothers also was not supported. That is, there were not significant differences in RSA fluctuation between the two groups during the mood induction videos. Regarding the fluctuation during the negative video, the lack of significant findings may have occurred because children did not find the negative mood video to be in fact negative. The average mood rating after the negative video was 2.97, which represents the rating “Just a little sad”. In order to accurately assess RSA fluctuation in response to a stressor, the stimulus must effectively induce the desired mood state. Second, as noted with regard to baseline RSA, the lack of group differences may have been due to the small sample size and missing data. The third hypothesis proposed that participants with higher levels of RSA at baseline would have higher levels of positive affect and lower levels of negative affect. This hypothesis was tested with regard to child’s state and trait levels of positive and negative affect as measured by the PANAS, and with affect during the mood induction videos as measured by the Smiley Faces Questionnaire. No significant associations were found between baseline RSA and state or trait negative or positive affect (PANAS), or the mood ratings in response to both the positive RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 21 and negative videos. It may be that respiratory sinus arrhythmia is not associated with children’s positive and negative affect. It also is possible that our operationalization of PA and NA in children was not adequate here (discussed further below). A significant association was found between the baseline RSA level and the mood rating in response to the baseline audio story. This association showed that children who had higher levels of baseline RSA reported feeling higher levels of negativity during the baseline audio story. This finding is contrary to previous research showing that higher levels of resting RSA are associated with positive outcomes, such as adaptive coping and resiliency, whereas these results indicate that higher RSA level was associated with more negativity. One explanation may be that there is a “disconnect” between the physiological reaction to a stimulus and individuals’ selfreport of mood in response to that stimulus. Exploratory analyses also were conducted to examine whether RSA level or fluctuation were related to demographic factors. No sex differences were found for baseline RSA level or RSA fluctuation during the positive video. A nonsignificant trend was found for fluctuation during the negative video where females were more likely to have reduced RSA reactivity in response to the negative video. No significant associations were found between children’s age and RSA, but given the small age range, significant age differences may not have been present. It also is possible that RSA as an indicator of risk may not appear until later in development (e.g., after puberty. In addition, no significant associations were found between children’s RSA and mother’s depression severity, as measured by the CES-D. Although the sample included mothers with histories of depression, very few mothers were currently depressed or had high scores on the RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 22 CES-D (mean = 7.8; SD = 6.4). Thus, the sample had a limited range on the dimensional measure of depression. There was a significant relation between mothers’ marital status and children’s RSA fluctuation during the negative video. That is, having a currently married mothers significantly predicted increased reactivity during the negative video. Thus, children coming from “intact” families may develop more adaptive RSA fluctuation in response to stress. Exploratory analyses also found no significant relations between RSA level and fluctuation and children’s emotion regulation (FAM-C, SMFQ), anxiety (SCARED), and temperament on the Parent Perception of Child Temperament. We next explored possible predictors of RSA during the mood induction videos and found a nonsignificant trend for girls of mothers with higher depression scores (CES-D) to have lower levels of RSA during the positive video. Being female and having a mother with higher levels of depressive symptoms are both risk factors for negative outcomes, such as developing depression. Lower RSA in response to positive stimuli may be one factor contributing to this increased risk. Higher scores on the shyness subscale of the Parent Perception of Child Temperament questionnaire significantly predicted reduced RSA during the positive video. Previous research has demonstrated that RSA is related to temperament; this result suggests that shyness may be associated with a maladaptive parasympathetic regulatory pattern. We next explored predictors of RSA during the negative video. There was a nonsignificant trend for being female predicting lower levels of RSA during the negative video. With a larger sample, the association among these variables might reach significance. One implication is that interventions that improve parasympathetic flexibility in females may be one means of reducing their risk of depression. RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 23 Interestingly, girls also reported that their mood was significantly more positive during the positive video in comparison with boys, and there was a nonsignificant trend for girls to report more negative mood than boys during the negative mood induction video. Thus, girls reported greater emotional reactivity but less physiological reactivity to the mood induction videos. Future studies should explore possible explanations for this apparent discrepancy between the physiological level and the self-report of mood level. Limitations and Future Directions There were several limitations to the current study. First, the negative video during the mood induction videos may not have been an effective way of inducing a negative mood due to the high ratings on the mood rating scale after the video, which would mean that RSA fluctuation during the negative video was not an accurate index of RSA in response to a stressor. Second, the use of two different media forms for the two different baselines, an audio story and a video, may not be a consistent baseline. This difference also may have complicated the fluctuation scores since the RSA fluctuation during the negative video was calculated using the audio story, and the RSA fluctuation during the positive video was calculated using the neutral video. A third limitation of the study was that the video order was not counterbalanced, which would have resolved the issue of having the mood induction videos following each type of baseline (audio and neutral fish). In addition, the children may have became fatigued during the session, which could have influenced their mood ratings. Fourth, the timing of the mothers’ depressive episodes was not taken into account, nor were the duration or frequency of the episodes. A fifth limitation was that the Smiley Faces Questionnaire was on a 6-point Likert scale, and children could only choose one answer as their mood rating. A possibly better way of assessing affect during the mood induction videos would have been to ask the children about RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 24 both their positive and negative affect during the film, as well as to increase the number of points on the scale to allow for more variability. Also, at times the report of the child’s mood during the video was influenced by whether or not the child expressed positive or negative feelings towards the movie as a whole if they had seen the movie at a previous time. In the future, having the child report whether or not they had seen the movie and their previous feelings about the movie may be useful. Sixth, certain characteristics that may have influenced RSA were not taken into account such as the child’s BMI, current medications, and any other illness that may affect RSA, such as asthma. Finally, as noted previously, the small sample size, the difference in sample size between depressed and nondepressed mothers, and the missing RSA data also were limitations. Future research that addressed these various limitations is needed. With a larger sample, researchers may be able to find significant differences between the groups, as well as explore other predictors of RSA. Given the lack of significant findings with the negative mood induction video, future studies should use an alternative way of inducing a negative mood that can better capture the parasympathetic flexibility of children. The current study observed some sex differences in RSA fluctuation that should be examined further. Also, the discrepancy found in girls between physiological reactivity and selfreported mood needs further study. This discrepancy also was found in both sexes with resting RSA at baseline and self-reported mood at baseline, which were inversely correlated. The sex differences in RSA might have implications for intervention. Programs to increase adaptive RSA reactivity could be targeted at children of depressed mother, especially girls. In addition, given that having a married mother significantly predicted RSA reactivity, collecting more information about the home environment may provide hints regarding possible mechanisms through which marital status and children’s RSA are related. This research could RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 25 lead to possible interventions focused on improving mother-child interactions, which in turn may increase resting RSA level and create more adaptive RSA fluctuation in the child. Improved RSA in at-risk children could serve as a buffer against the onset of psychopathology and other problems later on in life. RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 26 References America Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: APA. 4th ed., text rev. Angold, A., Costello, E. J., Messer, S. C., Pickles, A., Winder, F., & Silver, D. (1995). The development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 5, 237 - 249. Ashman, S. B., Dawson, G., & Panagiotides, H. (2008). Trajectories of maternal depression over 7 years: Relations with child psychophysiology and behavior and role of contextual risks. Development and Psychopathology, 20(1), 55-77. Beauchaine, T. (2001). Vagal tone, development, and Gray's motivational theory: Toward an integrated model of autonomic nervous system functioning in psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 13(02), 183-214. Berntson, G. G., Bigger, J. T., Eckberg, D. L., Grossman, P., Kaufmann, P. G., Malik, M., ... & Van Der Molen, M. W. (1997). Heart rate variability: Origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology, 34, 623-648. Birmaher, B., Brent, D. A., Chiappetta, L., Bridge, J., Monga, S., & Baugher, M. (1999). Psychometric properties of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED): a replication study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(10), 1230–1236. Blandon, A. Y., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., & O'Brien, M. (2008). Individual differences in trajectories of emotion regulation processes: The effects of maternal depressive RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 27 symptomatology and children's physiological regulation. Developmental Psychology, 44(4), 1110. Boyce, W. T., Quas, J., Alkon, A., Smider, N. A., Essex, M. J., & Kupfer, D. J. (2001). Autonomic reactivity and psychopathology in middle childhood. British Journal of Psychiatry, 179(2), 144-150. Brennan, P. A., Hammen, C., Katz, A. R., & Le Brocque, R. M. (2002). Maternal depression, paternal psychopathology, and adolescent diagnostic outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(5), 1075-1085. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022006X.70.5.1075 Bylsma, L. M., Salomon, K., Taylor-Clift, A., Morris, B. H., & Rottenberg, J. (2014). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity in current and remitted major depressive disorder. Psychosomatic Medicine, 76(1), 66-73. Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Larsen, J. T., Pohlmann, K. M., & Ito, T. A. (2000). The psychophysiology of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 173–191). New York: Guilford Press. Calkins, S. D. (1997). Cardiac vagal tone indices of temperamental reactivity and behavioral regulation in young children. Developmental Psychobiology, 31(2), 125-135. CardioBatch software. Brain-Body Center, University of Illinois at Chicago. 2007. CardioEdit software. Brain-Body Center, University of Illinois at Chicago. 2007. Center for Disease Control and Prevention. (March 31, 2011). An estimated 1 in 10 U.S. adults report depression. CDC.gov. Retrieved November, 2013 from http://www.cdc.gov/features/dsdepression/. RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 28 Connell, A. M., Hughes-Scalise, A., Klostermann, S., & Azem, T. (2011). Maternal depression and the heart of parenting: Respiratory sinus arrhythmia and affective dynamics during parent–adolescent interactions. Journal of Family Psychology, 25(5), 653-662. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025225 Dietz, L.J., Birmaher, B., Williamson, D.E., Silk, J.S., Dahl, R.E., Axelson, D.A., Ehmann, M, & Ryan, N.D. (2008). Mother-child interactions in depressed children and children at high risk and low risk for future depression. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(5), 574-582. El‐Sheikh, M., Harger, J., & Whitson, S. M. (2001). Exposure to inter-parental conflict and children's adjustment and physical health: The moderating role of vagal tone. Child Development, 72(6), 1617-1636. Field, T. (1995). Infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development, 18(1), 1-13. Forbes, E. E., Fox, N. A., Cohn, J. F., Galles, S. F., & Kovacs, M. (2006). Children's affect regulation during a disappointment: Psychophysiological responses and relation to parent history of depression. Biological Psychology, 71(3), 264-277. Forehand, R., Thigpen, J. C., Parent, J., Hardcastle, E. J., Bettis, A., & Compas, B. E. (2012). The role of parent depressive symptoms in positive and negative parenting in a preventive intervention. Journal of Family Psychology, 26(4), 532-541. Friedman, B. H., & Thayer, J. F. (1998). Anxiety and autonomic flexibility: A cardiovascular approach. Biological Psychology, 47(3), 243-263. Goodman, S. H., & Gotlib, I. H. (1999). Risk for psychopathology in the children of depressed mothers: A developmental model for understanding mechanisms of RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 29 transmission. Psychological Review, 106(3), 458-490. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033295X.106.3.458 Goodman, S.H., Rouse, M.H., Connell, A.M., Broth, M.R., Hall, C.M., & Heyward, D. (2011). Maternal depression and child psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Springer Science and Business Media, 14, 1-27. Hammen, C. & Brennan, P.A. (2003). Severity, chronicity, and timing of maternal depression and risk for adolescent offspring diagnoses in a community sample. Archives of General Psychology, 60(3):253-258. Hankin, B.L., Abramson, L.Y., Moffitt, T.E., Silva, P.A., McGee, R., & Angell, K.E. (1998). Development of depression from preadolescence to young adulthood: emerging gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107(1), 128-140. Hastings, P. D., Klimes-Dougan, B., Kendziora, K. T., Brand, A., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2014). Regulating sadness and fear from outside and within: Mothers' emotion socialization and adolescents' parasympathetic regulation predict the development of internalizing difficulties. Development and Psychopathology, 26(4), 1369-1384. Katz, L. F., & Gottman, J. M. (1997). Buffering children from marital conflict and dissolution. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 26(2), 157-171. Laurent, J., Catanzaro, S. J., Joiner, T. E., Jr., Rudolph, K. D., Potter, K. I., Lambert. S., et al. (1999). A measure of positive and negative affect for children: Scale development and preliminary validation. Psychological Assessment, 11, 326–338. Lonigan, C. J., Hooe, E. S., David, C. F., & Kistner, J. A. (1999). Positive and negative affectivity in children: Confirmatory factor analysis of a two-factor model and its relation RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 30 to symptoms of anxiety and depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67(3), 374-386. Morales, S., Beekman, C., Blandon, A. Y., Stifter, C. A., & Buss, K. A. (2015). Longitudinal associations between temperament and socioemotional outcomes in young children: The moderating role of RSA and gender. Developmental psychobiology, 57(1), 105-119. Nederhof, E., Marceau, K., Shirtcliff, E. A., Hastings, P. D., & Oldehinkel, A. J. (2014). Autonomic and adrenocortical interactions predict mental late adolescence health in: The TRAILS Study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 1-15. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wolfson, A., Mumme, D., & Guskin, K. (1995). Helplessness in children of depressed and nondepressed mothers. Developmental Psychology, 31(3), 377-387. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.31.3.377 Oveis, C., Cohen, A. B., Gruber, J., Shiota, M. N., Haidt, J., & Keltner, D. (2009). Resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia is associated with tonic positive emotionality. Emotion, 9(2), 265. Pine, D. S., Wasserman, G. A., Miller, L., Coplan, J. D., Bagiella, E., Kovelenku, P., ... & Sloan, R. P. (1998). Heart period variability and psychopathology in urban boys at risk for delinquency. Psychophysiology, 35(05), 521-529. Porges, S.W. Method and Apparatus for Evaluating Rhythmic Oscillations in Aperiodic Physiological Response Systems. Patent Number: 4,510,944. Washington DC: U.S. Patent Office. April 16, 1985. Porges, S.W., & Bohrer, R.E. (1990). Analyses of periodic processes in psychophysiological research. In J.T. Cacioppo and L.G. Tassinary (eds.). Principles of Psychophysiology: Physical, Social, and Inferential Elements. New York: Cambridge University Press, 708-753. RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 31 Porges, S. W. (1995). Cardiac vagal tone: a physiological index of stress. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 19(2), 225-233. Porges, S. W. (1997). Emotion: An evolutionary by‐product of the neural regulation of the autonomic nervous system. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 807(1), 62-77. Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological psychology, 74(2), 116-143. Porges, S. W., Doussard‐Roosevelt, J. A., & Maiti, A. K. (1994). Vagal tone and the physiological regulation of emotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2‐3), 167-186. Radloff, L.S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1:385-401. Rottenberg, J. (2007). Cardiac vagal control in depression: a critical analysis. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 200-211. Rottenberg, J., Clift, A., Bolden, S., & Salomon, K. (2007). RSA fluctuation in major depressive disorder. Psychophysiology, 44(3), 450-458. Salomon, K. (2005). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia during stress predicts resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia 3 years later in a pediatric sample. Health Psychology, 24(1), 68. Shiota, M. N., Neufeld, S. L., Yeung, W. H., Moser, S. E., & Perea, E. F. (2011). Feeling good: autonomic nervous system responding in five positive emotions. Emotion, 11(6), 1368. Silvia, P. J., Jackson, B. A., & Sopko, R. S. (2014). Does baseline heart rate variability reflect stable positive emotionality? Personality and Individual Differences, 70, 183-187. Yaroslavsky, I., Rottenberg, J., & Kovacs, M. (2014). Atypical patterns of respiratory sinus arrhythmia index an endophenotype for depression. Development and Psychopathology, 26(4pt2), 1337-1352. RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 32 Weissman, M.M, Wickramaratne, P., Nomura, Y., Warner, V., Pilowsky, D. & Verdeli, H. (2006). Offspring of depressed parents: 20 years later. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1001-1008. RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 33 Table 1. Demographic Characteristics Offspring of Depressed Offspring of Nondepressed (N=37) (N=55) Age (M,SD) 9.31 (.78) 9.45 (.86) % Caucasian 45% 65% % Female 59% 53% Mother's Education (M,SD) 6.42 (1.65) 6.61 (1.68) Mother's Income (M,SD) 6.64 (4.1) 4.24 (3.4) 67% 87% Variable % Mother Married Note. Education was assessed on an 8-point scale with education level increasing with each number, where a score of 6 represents graduated a four year college. Income was assessed on a 14-point scale with incoming decreasing with each number, with a 4 representing $100,001$110,000 and 6 representing $80,001-$90,000 Table 2. Mean and Standard Deviation of RSA Level and Fluctuation Offspring of Depressed Offspring of Healthy Control Variable (M, SD) (M, SD) RSA Baseline Level 6.43 (1.08) 6.39 (1.19) RSA Negative Video .023 (.365) -.015 (.517) RSA Neutral Video 6.45 (1.03) 6.48 (1.17) RSA Positive Video .117 (.500) .172 (.450) Table 3. Independent Samples T-Test by Risk of RSA at baseline and RSA fluctuations during positive and negative videos 95% Confidence Risk (Offspring of) Interval Depressed Nondepressed Variable t df (N=32) (N=48) Lower Upper Baseline RSA 6.43 (1.08) 6.39 (1.19) .136 75 -.496 .571 RSA Negative .023 (.365) -.015 (.517) .345 74 -.179 .254 RSA Positive .117 (.500) .172 (.450) -.514 78 -.269 .159 RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 34 Table 4. Correlations between Baseline RSA and Affect Measures Risk RSA-Bsl PA-Tr NA-Tr PA-St NA-St MR-Bsl MR-Neg MR-Neu MR-Pos -.016 -.176 -.133 .015 -.094 .109 .039 .120 .106 Risk --.161 -.106 -.102 -.06 -.314** .050 .072 -.112 RSA-Baseline -.006 .371** .091 .143 -.039 .087 .075 PA-Trait --.222* .336** -.193 -.107 -.305** -.198 NA-Trait --.039 .137 .088 .327** .348** PA-State --.158 -.034 -.158 .002 NA-State -.291** .377*** .173 MR-Baseline -.293** -.065 MR-Negative -.269* MR-Neutral -MR-Positive Note. Risk = Mother history of Depression (0 = No; 1 = Yes); RSA-L= RSA level at baseline; PATr= Positive affect score on the PANAS trait questionnaire; NA-Tr = Negative affect score on the PANAS trait questionnaire; PA-St = Positive affect score on the PANAS state questionnaire; NASt= Negative affect score on the PANAS state questionnaire; MR-Bsl= Mood rating at baseline; MR-Neg= Mood rating after negative video; MR-Neu=Mood rating after neutral fish video; MRPos=Mood rating after positive video *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001 Table 5. Linear Regression of Baseline RSA Predicting Positive Affect (PANAS-State) Coefficientsa Model 1 (Constant) Baseline RSA B 3.454 Std. Error .510 Beta t 6.778 Sig. .000 -.069 .079 -.102 -.883 .380 Dependent Variable: PANAS state positive affect Table 6. Linear Regression of Baseline RSA Predicting Positive Affect (PANAS-Trait) Coefficientsa Model 1 (Constant) Baseline RSA B 4.139 Std. Error .320 Beta t 12.936 Sig. .000 -.069 .049 -.161 -1.400 .166 Dependent Variable: PANAS trait positive affect RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 35 Table 7. Linear Regression of Baseline RSA Predicting Negative Affect (PANAS-State) Coefficientsa Model 1 (Constant) Baseline RSA B 1.323 -.015 Std. Error .182 .028 Beta t 7.261 -.546 -.063 Sig. .000 .586 a. Dependent Variable: PANAS state negative Table 8. Linear Regression of Baseline RSA Predicting Negative Affect (PANAS-Trait) Coefficientsa Model 1 (Constant) Baseline RSA B Beta 2.312 Std. Error .430 t 5.376 Sig. .000 -.061 .066 -.106 -.919 .361 a. Dependent Variable: PANAS trait negative Table 9. Linear Regression of Baseline RSA Predicting Baseline Mood Rating Coefficientsa Model 1 (Constant) Baseline RSA B 6.477 Std. Error .711 Beta t 9.107 Sig. .000 -.311 .109 -.314 -2.842 .006 a. Dependent Variable: Baseline Mood Rating RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 36 Table 10. Correlations among RSA and Affect Measures RSA-N RSA-P PA-Trait NA-Trait PA-State NA-State MR-B MR-N MR-F MR-P RSA-N -- RSA-P .115 -- PA-T NA-T PA-S NA-S MR-B MR-N MR-F MR-P -.083 -.020 -- -.126 -.119 .006 -- -.062 .125 .371** -.222* -- -.070 -.100 .091 .336** -.039 -- -.162 -.081 .143 -.193 .137 -.158 -- -.226~ -.066 -.039 -.107 .088 -.034 .291** -- -.056 .140 .087 -.305** .327** -.158 .377*** .293** -- .004 .126 .075 -.198 .348** .002 .173 -.065 .269* -- Note. RSA-N= Fluctuation in RSA from baseline to negative video; RSA-P= Fluctuation in RSA from neutral fish video to positive video; PA-T= Positive affect score on the PANAS trait questionnaire; NA-T= Negative affect score on the PANAS trait questionnaire; PA-S= Positive affect score on the PANAS state questionnaire; NA-S= Negative affect score on the PANAS state questionnaire; MR-B= Mood rating at baseline; MR-N= Mood rating after negative video; MR-F=Mood rating after neutral fish video; MR-P=Mood rating after positive video ~p < .10; *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001 Table 11. Exploratory Correlations between RSA Level/Fluctuation and Demographics RSA-Bsl RSA-Bsl RSA-Neg RSA-Neu RSA-Pos Sex Age CES-D Marital St -- RSA-Neg .000 -- Sex Age CES-D Marital St .930*** RSA-P .104 -.046 .109 .024 .163 .190 .115 -.200 .032 .009 .237* -- -.039 -- -.079 -.080 .107 -.076 .037 -.147 .196 .001 -- -.067 -- .272* -.035 -- -.056 -.108 -.231* -- RSA-F Note. RSA-L= RSA level at baseline; RSA-Neg= Fluctuation in RSA from baseline to negative video; RSA-Neu= RSA level during the neutral fish video; RSA-Pos= Fluctuation in RSA from neutral fish video to positive video; CES-D= Mother’s severity of depression; Marital St= Martial Status (1= Currently Married, 0=Not Currently Married) *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001 Table 12. Linear Regression of Marital Status Predicting RSA Fluctuation during the Negative Video RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 37 Coefficientsa Model B Std. Error Beta 1 (Constant) -.291 .141 Risk Condition .095 .111 .100 Marital Status .310 .139 .262 a. Dependent Variable: RSA Fluctuation during Negative Video t -2.066 .850 2.229 Sig. .042 .398 .029 Table 13. Correlations between RSA Level/Fluctuation and Questionnaires RSA-Bsl RSA-Neg RSA-Neu RSA-Pos SCARED SMFQ FAM-CM FAM-CA PPCT-E PPT-Sh RSABsl -- RSANeg .000 RSA-F SCA RED -.087 SMFQ .93*** RSAP .104 -- .190 .115 -- -.008 FAMCM -.063 FAMCA .-.168 PPC T-E .029 PPCTSh .056 PPCTA -.038 PPCTSo -.010 -.052 .039 .017 -.007 -.069 .186 -.161 -.127 -.039 -.039 -.063 .011 -.155 .074 .060 -.068 -.025 -- -.009 .176 -.027 .157 -.155 -.187 .061 -.099 -- -.489** .539** .321** .145 .043 .124 .086 -- -.566** -.018 -.043 .002 -.157 -.068 -- .092 .178 .175 .028 -.064 -- -.053 -.037 -.052 -.063 -- .324** -.200 -.031 -- -.433** -.458** -- .471** PPCT-A PPCT-A -- Note. RSA-L= RSA level at baseline; RSA-Neg= Fluctuation in RSA from baseline to negative video; RSA-Neu= RSA level during the neutral fish video; RSA-Pos= Fluctuation in RSA from neutral fish video to positive video; SCARED= Screen for Child Anxiety Related Disorders; SMFQ= Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire; FAM= Feelings and Me, Maladaptive Emotion Regulation Subscale; FAMCA= Feelings and Me, Adaptive Emotion Regulation Subscale; PPCT-E= Parent Perception of Child Temperament, Emotionality Subscale; PPCT-Sh=Parent Perception of Child Temperament, Shyness Subscale; PPCT-A=Parent Perception of Child Temperament, Activity Subscale; PPCT-So=Parent Perception of Child Temperament, Sociability Subscale *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001 Table 14. Linear Regression of Variables Predicting RSA during the Positive Video Coefficientsa RSA IN CHILDREN OF DEPRESSED VS. NON DEPRESSED MOTHERS 38 Model B Std. Error 1 (Constant) 1.416 .803 Child Sex .260 .168 Child Age -.024 .060 Mother’s CES-D .027 .019 Sex X CESD -.040 .021 Baseline RSA .958 .049 PPCT-Emotionality -.054 .077 PPCT-Shyness -.181 .091 PPCT-Activity -.019 .111 PPCT-Sociability -.135 .118 Dependent Variable: RSA during Positive Video Beta .122 -.018 .156 -.255 .952 -.035 -.117 -.010 -.068 t 1.764 1.546 -.392 1.449 -1.905 19.642 -.701 -1.987 -.172 -1.145 Table 15. Linear Regression of Variables Predicting RSA during the Negative Video Coefficientsa Model B Std. Error Beta t 1 (Constant) .957 .889 1.076 Child Sex -.337 .185 -.158 -1.818 Child Age .027 .070 .021 .395 Mothers’ CES-D -.014 .021 -.080 -.687 Sex X CESD .021 .023 .129 .898 Baseline RSA .879 .053 .880 16.438 PPCT-Emotionality -.094 .087 -.062 -1.079 PPCT-Shyness .157 .103 .098 1.521 PPCT-Activity -.085 .124 -.043 -.683 PPCT-Sociability .025 .130 .012 .193 Dependent Variable: RSA during Negative Video Sig. .082 .127 .696 .152 .061 .000 .486 .051 .864 .256 Sig. .286 .074 .694 .495 .372 .000 .285 .133 .497 .848 Table 16. Independent Samples t-test of Sex, Positive, and Negative Mood Rating 95% Confidence Sex Interval Variable Positive Mood Rating Negative Mood Rating Female Male t df 5.53 (.793) (N=49) 2.75 (1.20) (N=51) 5.10 (.841) (N=40) 3.26 (1.33) (N=39) 2.46 -1.89 Lower Upper 81.34 .083 .778 77.09 -1.05 .029