Current Event: Constructivism CURRENT EVENT: CONSTRUCTIVISM Evaluation of a Constructivism Research Article Encouraging conceptual change: the use of bridging analogies John C. Olson California State University, Northridge 1 Current Event: Constructivism 2 Abstract The qualitative study critiqued in this paper addressed the use of bridging analogies in a constructivist framework to see if the use of analogies would help the subjects in the study come to their own correct conclusions regarding the ‘at rest’ condition of Newton’s third law. The study group was made up of twenty one 15 year old students. Each student was questioned during an interview as the student studied graphic analogies. The objective was to see if the questioning would lead the students to correctly identify the forces working in Newton’s third law. The paper summarizes the content of the study in terms of design and methodology. The strengths and weaknesses of the study are addressed, covering the topics of study group size, the interview schedule, and data coding. The paper also discusses ideas from the study that have applications for our own action research project. Current Event: Constructivism 3 Evaluation of a Constructivism Research Article Encouraging conceptual change: the use of bridging analogies Summary of the study This project was designed to study the “effectiveness of bridging analogies intended to bring about conceptual change” (Bryce & MacMillan, 2005, p.737). The goal was to discover if using these analogies “as part of a constructivist approach,” (2005, p.737) would help students synthesize their own correct understanding of the action-reaction force and the at rest condition. The study used open-ended coding, incorporating the “categories of fruitfulness, intelligibility and plausibility” (Bryce & MacMillan, 2005, p.742) taken from previous research studies. These categories were deemed to be “of fundamental importance in the search for evidence of conceptual change” (2005, p.742). During the study, researchers showed a series of analogies to the participants during “semi-structured ‘think-aloud’ interviews” (Bryce & MacMillan, 2005, p.742). The interviews were conducted with 21 volunteers. These volunteers were third-year secondary physics students from one school. With a few carefully chosen questions, the researchers first attempted to draw out the preconceptions the subjects had of the action-reaction force, then to guide them through a series of analogies which were intended to help the subjects come to their own correct conclusions and beliefs about what forces are actually at work (Bryce & MacMillan, 2005). Strengths The interview schedule The design of the interview schedule was carefully reviewed before the study was started. The researchers stated that “An initial interview schedule was devised and then piloted. The piloting process involved formally recording, transcribing and analyzing Current Event: Constructivism 4 interviews with two students, as well as more informal trials with five other students (from the same age group)” (Bryce & MacMillan, 2005, p.743). Once the transcripts of these interviews had been recorded and studied, the researchers made adjustments to the original schedule. Their findings “resulted in a number of alterations…which made the questions more open and improved the way the students were guided through the process” (2005, p.743). The careful design and modification of their methodology gave this study a great deal of credibility. They were very careful to make sure that they did not tell students the answer “at any stage in the process” (2005, p.743). The whole purpose of the study was to see if the students by using the analogies given could “construct the law for themselves and propose a cause which seemed plausible” (2005, p.743). Data Coding The data coding for this study was very systematic. The authors stated that “Evidence of the analogies making the existence and the cause of the reaction force from the table more intelligible and plausible was looked for” (Bryce & MacMillan, 2005, p.746). The interviews were transcribed and then were analyzed using methods from previously researcher. In the transcripts, “an individuals responses were coded and recorded in order to allow the tracking of individual students’ thinking to be carried out as the analogies were worked through” (Bryce & MacMillan, 2005, p.746). The coding was designed specifically to look for “categories of pre-conceptions; evidence of conceptual change at each of the four analogical stages; evidence of students demonstrating a cyclical thought process,…” and “…categories of responses to the alternative scenarios” (2005, p.746). Current Event: Constructivism 5 Study group The researchers were careful to balance the subjects within their study group. The fact that “the sample size matched the numbers used in the study conducted by Brown” (Bryce & MacMillan, 2005, p.742), one of the studies they based their research on, added to the credibility of their work, because they were using a previous study’s techniques in the design of their own study. Subgroups identified in the study included: “Group 1: previously taught Newton’s Third Law,” (2005, p.746), “Group 2: previously taught about forces only,” (2005, p.748), and “Group 3: No prior teaching” (2005, p.750). The quality of work in identifying and balancing the study group, as well as the methods used to compare the results, demonstrated a high level of professionalism in the design and implementation of this study. Weaknesses Study group The size of the study group does not lend itself to make any strong, valid claims. The researchers conceded that “on numerical terms it has not provided conclusive evidence that analogical methods are better at causing conceptual change than more conventional teaching methods” (Bryce & MacMillan, 2005, p.761). It would be beneficial to see this same study applied on a larger sampling of students. Interviews Although the design of the study was very careful not to give students answers, (Bryce & MacMillan, 2005), the transcripts of the interviews do indicate a substantial amount of questioning by the interviewers, which may have unintentionally lead the students in the direction the interviewer wanted them to go. Although the line of questioning seemed to avoid directing students, questions such as “so are you saying there Current Event: Constructivism 6 is a force bouncing down and a bouncing up, or are you saying that it is bending on the table?” (Bryce & MacMillan, 2005, p.757) could lead the subject to reconsider their response in terms of what they might think the interviewer is looking for. Applications The use of an interview process to solicit the thought processes of the students involved may be something I will want to consider in my project. I am definitely interested in mining and coding transcripts to see how students interact with each other in discussing and solving the assignments they will be doing in my study, and I may want to include some one-on-one interviews to see what the students are really getting from the materials I will be creating. I also found the process of refining the study with a pilot program of value, and I know I will want to make revisions of my own work based on how the students respond to it. It is evident that it is important to carefully consider the size of the study group when looking at what it is you actually are trying to study and discover. This study also clarifies that one needs to determine the number of classes and students to include in the study to give a balance between useful information and manageable data. Current Event: Constructivism References Bryce, T., & MacMillan, K. (2005). Encouraging conceptual change: The use of bridging analogies in the teaching of action-reaction forces and the "at rest" condition in physics. International Journal of Science Education, 27(6), 737-763. Retrieved April 14, 2007, from ERIC database. 7