Students’ Strategies & Mistakes Using a Map in the Real World Kim A. Kastens MG&G Seminar 8 March 2007 Earth Fertile research area at interface Mind Earth Mind Fertile research area at interface Kim’s Priority List of Research Topics in ROLE/ GEO • Complex systems of the Earth & environment • Geologic time • Learning in the Field • Spatial thinking in Geosciences Earth Mind Fertile research area at interface Kim’s Priority List of Research Topics in ROLE/ GEO • Complex systems of the Earth & environment • Geologic time • Learning in the Field • Spatial thinking in Geosciences versus Field photo from: http://www.ldeo.columbia.edu/users/menke/slides/EESC2200FIELDTRIP04/fieldtrip_3.html Lab photo from: www.ldeo.columbia.edu/edu/DLESE/activities/Galileo/index.html versus • Large scale, internal spatial viewpoint • Small scale, external spatial viewpoint • High level of detail and complexity • Everything on table top is relevant to inquiry • Rarely can do controlled experiments • Focus on controlled experiments • Begin with raw materials of Nature • Begin with inscriptions such as maps, diagrams, graphs, equations • Unfamiliar setting can cause anxiety: fear of getting lost or hurt • Familiar setting Field photo from: http://www.ldeo.columbia.edu/users/menke/slides/EESC2200FIELDTRIP04/fieldtrip_3.html Lab photo from: www.ldeo.columbia.edu/edu/DLESE/activities/Galileo/index.html versus • It doesn’t matter where you are: focus is on that is which is true everywhere, every time. Lab Report Methods: We made our observations at 40° 47' N, 73° 58' W, 72 cm above the floor level, at the front table, in room 558 Schermerhorn. Field photo from: http://www.ldeo.columbia.edu/users/menke/slides/EESC2200FIELDTRIP04/fieldtrip_3.html Lab photo from: www.ldeo.columbia.edu/edu/DLESE/activities/Galileo/index.html versus • It matters crucially where you are. – Situation 1: Location is an independent variable when we are studying variation along a gradient or between field sites. – Situation 2: Location is a confounding factor, because of the heterogeneity of the Earth & environment on all scales. • It doesn’t matter where you are: focus is on that is which is true everywhere, every time. Lab Report Methods: We made our observations at 40° 47' N, 73° 58' W, 72 cm above the floor level, at the front table, in room 558 Schermerhorn. Field photo from: http://www.ldeo.columbia.edu/users/menke/slides/EESC2200FIELDTRIP04/fieldtrip_3.html Lab photo from: www.ldeo.columbia.edu/edu/DLESE/activities/Galileo/index.html A Manual for Deeptow Transponder Navigators A Tool for Understanding and Improving how Children Learn to Use Maps “THE WORLD IN SPATIAL TERMS… The geographically informed person knows and understands... • How to use maps and other geographic representations, tools, and technologies to acquire, process, and report information from a spatial perspective… By the end of the fourth grade, the student knows and understands: … • How to display spatial information on maps and other geographic representations.” Knowing how to use a map is important for everyone who wants to be able to travel to new places. Field-based investigations of children’s map skills: • Baseline Group • Self-Explaining Group • Verbal Description Group The “Baseline” Group The “Baseline” Group SK A T PLACE (" CHILD REPRESE (ma The “Baseline” Group SK A T PLACE (" CHILD REPRESE (ma The “Baseline” Group “Put the blue sticker on the map to show where you think the blue flag is located….” The “Baseline” Group “Put the blue sticker on the map to show where you think the blue flag is located….” 2001-2002 and 2002-2003 classes (now 6th & 7th graders): The “Baseline” Group The “Baseline” Group 2001-2002 and 2002-2003 classes (now 6th & 7th graders): The “Baseline” Group Two scoring methods for sticker placements: (1) Quantitative: distance from center of student sticker to exact location, in sticker diameters. (2) Categorical Category 1 Error: Sticker is centered on a map object that resembles sticker in shape and size Correct Answer Type 1 Error Category 2 Error: Sticker is placed on symbol for grass, trees, or bushes; not on or adjacent to any built object Type 2 Error Correct Answer Category 3b Error: Sticker should have been adjacent to a road or path; placed instead on or adjacent to a structure Correct Answer Type 3b Error Category 4a Error: Sticker should have been on structure; placed on or adjacent to the wrong structure Correct Answer Type 4a Error Type 4a Error Category 5a Error: Sticker should be on structure; placed on correct structure but outside of “correct” ring Type 5a Errors Correct Answers Category 5b Error: Sticker should be adjacent to a road or path; placed on correct path or road segment, but outside the “correct” ring Correct Answer 5b Errors 2001-2002 and 2002-2003 classes (now 6th & 7th graders): The “Baseline” Group The “Baseline” Group Correct location is on a wall. Student stickers on the wrong building. Student sticker on the grass. Student sticker on trees or shrubs. Good student stickers. Percentage of stickers in each error category Error Group Error Number Baselin e 1 1.3 Representational 2 14.7 Correspondence 3a & 3b 6.3 4a & 4b 11.3 Configur ational Correspondence 5a & 5b 13.1 No Error No Error 53.4 All Representational Correspondence All Configur ational Correspondence All Errors 1, 2, 3a, & 3b 20.9 4a, 4b, 5a, & 5b 22.9 1, 2, 3a, 3b, 4a, 4b, 5a, & 5b 46.6 2001-2002 and 2002-2003 classes (now 6th & 7th graders): The “Baseline” Group The “Baseline” Group Result #1: Without instruction, many 4th graders (and some adults) find it difficult to accurately locate a realworld object onto a map. The “Explaining” Group Same as “Baseline”: SK A T PLACE ("reality") CHILD REPRESENTATION (map) Dependent Variable The “Explaining” Group Same as “Baseline”: Different: “After you put one of the stickers on the map, write down what clues you used to decide where to put the sticker…” SK A T PLACE ("reality") CHILD REPRESENTATION (map) Dependent Variable The “Explaining” Group The “Explaining” Group Result #2: Children who explained their reasoning did better than those who did not. Baseline Explaining Percentage of stickers in each error category Error Error Group Baselin e Reflecting Number No Error No Error 53.4 67.1 All 1, 2, 3a, & Representational 20.9 10.7 3b Correspondence All 4a, 4b, 5a, Configur ational 22.9 22.1 & 5b Correspondence …..even though: • the two groups of students were from the same school • had the same teachers • were the same age (end of 4th grade) • and were indistinguishable on standard paper and pencil tests of mental rotation ability. The “Self-Explaining Effect”: Eliciting “self-generated explanations” improves student performance on a wide range of tasks Chi et al (1989) Studying worked examples of physics problems Lin & Lehman (1999) Experimental design using controlled variables Chi et al (1994), Chi (2000) Studying biology textbook passage on blood circulation Aleven & Koedinger (2002) Solving geometry problems Didierjean & CauzinilleMarmeche (1997) Studying worked examples of factoring in algebra Kintsch (1994) Reading science text The “Explaining” Group Why? Self-explaining activates existing spatial competencies. Students find and self-correct incipient errors. Baseline Explaining The “Explaining” Group Insights from clueanswers should be taken as indicative of students performing closer to peak capacity. Coding of Clue-Answers • Did clue-answer include at least one landmark that exists in the real world? (landmark/no landmark) – If so, did the clue-answer accurately describe the real world? (veridical/ counterfactual) • If so, was the clue-answer sufficient to pinpoint the flag location within 15m? (sufficient/ accurate but insufficient) – If so, did the landmark exist on the map as well as in the real world? (Viable/ nonviable) • If so, did the position of the sticker agree with the position described in the clue-answer? (agree/ disagree) • Did the clue-answer include topological, projective, or Euclidean spatial elements? • Did the clue-answer include at least two elements of spatial information? • Did the clue-answer describe process as well as observations? (kinesthetic, visual) Coding of Clue-Answers • Did clue-answer include at least one landmark that exists in the real world? (landmark/no landmark) – If so, did the clue-answer accurately describe the real world? (veridical/ counterfactual) • If so, was the clue-answer sufficient to pinpoint the flag location within 15m? (sufficient/ accurate but insufficient) – If so, did the landmark exist on the map as well as in the real world? (Viable/ nonviable) • If so, did the position of the sticker agree with the position described in the clue-answer? (agree/ disagree) • Did the clue-answer include topological, projective, or Euclidean spatial elements? • Did the clue-answer include at least two elements of spatial information? • Did the clue-answer describe process as well as observations? (kinesthetic, visual) Did the clue-answer include at least one landmark that exists in the real world? And if so, did the clue-answer accurately describe the flag’s position in the real world? (true flag locations) Veridical Counterfactual Did the clue-answer include at least one landmark that exists in the real world? And if so, did the clue-answer accurately describe the flag’s position in the real world? (light blue K09) “Well I was walking along a road that goes in a circle, I saw this flag by cars so I knew it was near the parking area.” (white K18) “It was across the tree on a light post.” (true flag locations) Veridical Counterfactual Did the clue-answer include at least one landmark that exists in the real world? And if so, did the clue-answer accurately describe the flag’s position in the real world? (light blue K09) “Well I was walking along a road that goes in a circle, I saw this flag by cars so I knew it was near the parking area.” (white K18) “It was across the tree on a light post.” (true flag locations) Veridical Counterfactual Counterfactual clue-answers are very rare in our data: only 7 out of the 223 Landmark clue-answers. If the clue-answer is a veridical description of reality, does the clue-answer contain enough information to pinpoint the flag location? Sufficient (true flag locations) Accurate but Insufficient If the clue-answer is a veridical description of reality, does the clue-answer contain enough information to pinpoint the flag location? (white K03) “I put my (white H14) “I found the white flag by the big tree close to the mansion.” white sticker where I put it because I saw it on a tree in the circling road.” (black H06) “I saw the (orange H13) “It’s on the mansion.” flag on the statue while I was walking.” Sufficient (true flag locations) Accurate but Insufficient If the clue-answer is a veridical description of reality, does the clue-answer contain enough information to pinpoint the flag location? (white K03) “I put my (white H14) “I found the white flag by the big tree close to the mansion.” white sticker where I put it because I saw it on a tree in the circling road.” (black H06) “I saw the (orange H13) “It’s on the mansion.” flag on the statue while I was walking.” Sufficient (true flag locations) Accurate but Insufficient “Accurate but Insufficient” clue-answers are quite common in our data: 136 out of the 223 Landmark clue-answers. Does the clue-answer agree with the sticker placement? Agree Disagree Does the clue-answer agree with the sticker placement? (yellow H19) “End of the parking lot.” (yellow K10) “I put it by the side of the road.” Agree Disagree Does the clue-answer agree with the sticker placement? (green H19) “The green flag is next to an old house.” (yellow H19) “End of the parking lot.” (yellow K10) “I put it by the side of the road.” Agree Disagree Does the clue-answer agree with the sticker placement? (green H19) “The green flag is next to an old house.” (yellow H19) “End of the parking lot.” (yellow K10) “I put it by the side of the road.” Agree Disagree “Agree” answer-pairs far outnumber “Disagree” answer-pairs: 139 to 13 (among “Viable” clue-answers) Result #3: Among the “explaining” students, we see: • the explanation are accurate descriptions of reality, • there are many explanations that are insufficient to pinpoint the sticker, • there are many sticker placements that are wrong, but wrong in a way that is consistent with the corresponding explanation. Working hypothesis: the hard part about using a map in the real world lies in gathering the relevant information from the environment. Difficult Not so difficult The “Verbal Description” Group Same as “Baseline” and “Explaining”: The “Verbal Description” Group Same as Baseline and Explaining: Different: Verbal description read aloud in the classroom. “ The orange sticker is on the mansion. It’s on a corner of the mansion. It’s on the corner closest to the path that leads to the pond.” The “Verbal Description” Group Verbal description: • The orange sticker is on the mansion. • It’s on a corner of the mansion. • It’s on the corner closest to the path that leads to the pond. Exactly sufficient information, No more, no less. Result #4: The children who acquired information about the sticker position from a verbal description placed the stickers very accurately onto the map. Baseline Explaining Verbal Description Percentage of stickers in each error category Error Error Group Baselin e Reflecting Number No Error No Error 53.4 67.1 All 1, 2, 3a, & Representational 20.9 10.7 3b Correspondence All 4a, 4b, 5a, Configur ational 22.9 22.1 & 5b Correspondence Verbal Description 86.4 4.3 9.3 This supports our working hypothesis: the map itself is not that hard for children to work with; the hard part is identifying, gathering, and combining the relevant information from the environment. Difficult Not so difficult Representational Correspondence method Being able to translate from words to map and vice versa doesn’t mean a child can use a map competently within the represented space. Production Comprehension Representational Correspondence Meta-Representational Instant Map Skills (2004) Instant map skills: US (2004) Number of student products in each category: Marvelous map activities (2002) Neighborhoods and C ommunities (2002) Reading Topographic Maps (2001) Reading maps (2001) C omic-Strip Map Skills (2001 Amazing Hands-on Map Activities (2001) Maps,Globes,Graphs (2000) The majority of activities in elementary school map skills materials are of one type: Representational Correspondence…. Great Map Games (2000) Mega-fun map skills (1998) Mapping Skills (1998) Mapmaking with children (1998) Great Map Mysteries (1997) Orienteering and map games (1996) Map Adventures (1995) Teach with Topographic Maps (1994) Maps and Navigation (1994) Maps and Globes (1994) Using maps in your community (1993) Around and about (1993) Mapworks (1992) Map C oloring Book (1992) Finding your way (1992) Exploring maps (1992) Beginning Map Skills (1990) Geography Skills (1988) Exploring our world (1988) Map Skills for Today (1986) Learning About Maps (1985) 0 10 20 30 40 Number of student products in category 50 60 70 Representational correspondence exercises are necessary but not sufficient to learn to use a map… ….. because they lack the crucial connection with reality, the represented space. Recap • Many children have difficulty locating themselves or a real world object on a realistically-complex map. • Mistakes range from complete failure to grasp the stand-for relationship, to subtle nuances of configurational correspondence. • Metacognition improves children’s performance on a real world mapskills task. • The difficulty in map use lies in gathering task-relevant information from the represented space, not in positioning information from one’s mental model onto the map. • It may help to explicitly model the strategies of (a) seeking landmarks that exist both on the map and in the real world, and (b) uses two or more elements of spatial information. • Ability to translate from a verbal representation to a map does not mean the child can use a map in the real world. • Most map-skills curriculum materials under-emphasize making connections between maps and real world. Thanks to: • Co-PI Lynn Liben, Penn State Psychology Dept. • Data gathering and analysis by Margie Turrin, Linda Pistolesi, and Jennifer Griffith • GIS and graphics by Linda Pistolesi • 4th grade students and teachers of Cottage Lane School and Valley Cottage Elementary School • Many LDEO student and staff helpers during 4th graders’ visits • NSF Research on Learning & Education (ROLE) program • LDEO directors and admin for use of the grounds.