1 Running Head: INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY Even If It’s Only On Your Mind: The Cognitive Toll of Incivility Amir Erez Warrington College of Business University of Florida Christine Porath McDonough School of Business Georgetown University Trevor Foulk Warrington College of Business University of Florida INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY Abstract In five experimental studies we found that incivility --whether it was witnessed or primed-- disrupted working memory performance. Studies 1 and 2 showed that observing incivility interfered with the memory maintenance function of working memory, which in turn, negatively affected performance on complex and creative tasks. Study 3 showed that ‘just being around’ incivility, by priming it, had similar effects on working memory and task performance. The results of study 4 demonstrated that incivility effects extend to visuo-special tasks in which individuals affected by incivility miss critical information. This study’s findings also revealed that incivility affects the attention control function of working memory. Study 5 extended these findings by showing that incivility disrupts the memory maintenance and attention control functions of working memory. Specifically, incivility interferes with goal-management in working memory and even with the planning and execution of limb movement. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed. Keywords: Incivility, Working Memory, Cognitive Functioning. 2 INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 3 Even If It’s Only On Your Mind: The Cognitive Toll of Incivility Individuals often claim that they have no time to be nice (cf. Pearson & Porath, 2009). In a fast-paced modern society, deadlines call, responsibilities mount, and stresses accumulate. This often results in anxious, rash, insensitive interactions between individuals. In fact, in many institutions the culture may even dictate brashness, causing uncivil behavior to run rampant throughout the ranks. In such environments, managers bark orders at employees, team members respond brusquely to one another, and customers treat salespeople and administrators with disrespect. Although these types of uncivil behaviors seem to be very prevalent (Cortina, Magley, Williams, & Langhout, 2001; Pearson & Porath, 2009), they are for the most part low in intensity and ambiguous in their intent to harm the target (Andersson & Pearson, 1999). One should ask then: would small, defused, uncivil acts that are not directed specifically toward the individual have significant effects on individual functioning? Some evidence suggests that even these kinds of low-intensity acts with ambiguous intent to harm the target have serious consequences for individual functioning. Targets of incivility suffer psychological distress (Cortina, Magely, Williams, & Langhout, 2001), negative emotional effects (Pearson & Porath, 2001), job burnout and employee emotional exhaustion (e.g., Ben-Zur & Yagil, 2005; Deery, Iverson, & Walsh, 2002; Dormann & Zapf, 2004; Grandey, Dickter, & Sin, 2004; Harris & Reynolds, 2003; Ringstad, 2005). They may lose time and cognitive resources worrying about the uncivil interaction (Pearson, Andersson, & Porath, 2000). However, an underlying assumption of this research is that a single incident of mild aggression is unlikely to have major consequences (Kern & Grandey, 2009; Rafaeli, Erez, Ravid, DerflerRozin, Efrat, & Scheyer, 2012). Rather, it may be the accumulated effect of frequent “daily INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 4 hassles” that are harmful to individual health and well-being (Anderson & Pearson, 1999; Cortina et al., 2001). More recently, findings from several studies suggest that even a single, brief uncivil incident can cause an immediate reduction in individuals’ functioning. For example, Porath and Erez (2007) found that experiencing incivility hampered participants’ ability to perform complex tasks and reduced their creativity. Similarly, Rafaeli et al. (2012) found that mild verbal aggression from customers reduced customer service representatives’ ability to recall relevant information and perform analytical tasks. Moreover, Porath and Erez (2009) also found that the effects of incivility are not limited to those who experience incivility; witnesses of incivility showed a reduced capacity for solving complex problems and were much less creative. This is theoretically and practically significant because it implies that incivility extends beyond the offender-target dyad to secondary stakeholders. It raises questions about how far-reaching the consequences of incivility stretch. Does ‘just being around incivility’ (e.g., working in an uncivil culture) affect individual functioning? This also begs the question of how does incivility affect the functioning of targets, witnesses, or those around it? Incivility research highlights emotional and motivational mechanisms (Cortina et al., 2001; Lim, Cortina, & Magley, 2008; Miner-Rubino & Cortina, 2004; Porath, Overbeck, Pearson, 2008.). Yet, interestingly, Porath and Erez (2007) did not find evidence for emotional or motivational mediating processes; they found that cognitive processes related to memory mediated the relationship between experiencing incivility and task performance and creativity. At least when it comes to performance-related consequences, they suggest that incivility hampered cognitive processes which, in turn, may have the most potent impact on individual functioning. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 5 Building on these findings, we theorize and test the specific cognitive processes that incivility may disrupt. Identifying the specific processes is critical to ascertaining that it is cognitive and not alternative emotional or motivational processes that drive the effects of incivility. Isolating what particular cognitive functions incivility may disrupt is also crucial to identifying the extent to which people may be able to regulate the effects of incivility on performance. In considering how incivility may disrupt cognitive performance, it is judicious to start with exploring the effects of incivility on working memory. Working memory is the “workbench” of the memory system (Ashcraft & Radvansky, 2010). It is the place where information is rehearsed and integrated; where planning and control of attention occurs and decision processes are initiated (Eysenck & Keane, 2003). Thus, if exposure to incivility interferes with cognitive functioning, it is very likely that these effects occur through working memory. Accordingly, we investigate (1) the processes through which this occurs (controlled or automatic), (2) the working memory systems disrupted (central executive, phonological loop, and visuo-spatial sketchpad), and (3) the cognitive functions (memory maintenance and attention control system) affected in five studies (see Table 1). We begin by exploring the role of working memory, and how witnessing incivility may negatively influence working memory performance. Extending this, we then test whether working memory is the (mediating) mechanism through which witnessing incivility affects cognitive performance on complex and creative tasks. Study 1 and 2 examine how incivility affects a controlled process (i.e. in which participants may be conscious of the effects) using a task which engages the central executive and phonological loop in memory maintenance. Study 3 expands our focus to how incivility may affect people automatically via a priming study. Although research has focused on controlled processes, we theorize that incivility is likely to INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 6 affect working memory via automatic (i.e. subconscious) as well as controlled processes, albeit through different mechanisms. We examine the automatic effects of incivility on memory maintenance in Study 3. Taken together, the first three studies demonstrate the insidious and farreaching effects of incivility on various cognitive systems and functions. In the latter two studies we test incivility’s effects on a broader range of cognitive tasks in an attempt to learn more about which other working memory systems and functions incivility affects. To date, most of the tasks employed in incivility research have been verbal tasks, yet our findings in Studies 1, 2, and 3 suggest that incivility’s effects may extend beyond verbal tasks. Thus in Study 4 we investigate whether incivility affects performance on visual-perceptual tasks. Doing so allows us to isolate whether incivility affects the visuo-spatial sketchpad—a particular component of working memory; and has important implications for how incivility may disrupt attention and cause people to ‘miss’ critical information. In Study 5 we extend our understanding by investigating how incivility affects goal management within the central executive supervisory attention control system of working memory. Overall, the combined results of these studies show that witnessing incivility or just “having it on ones’ mind,” as in the case of priming, tend to elicit severe negative effects on cognitive functioning, including memory maintenance and attention control. Incivility robs people of cognitive resources, disrupts all three components of working memory, and ultimately hijacks performance. We propose implications for cognitive and social psychology theories and conclude with a discussion of the implications for how best to manage incivility. Working Memory Although the theory of working memory (Baddeley & Hitch, 1975) has been significantly modified over the years, the basic components of this theory remain widely accepted among INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 7 cognitive scientists. According to this theory, working memory consists of three major components – the central executive, the phonological loop, and the visuo-spatial sketchpad. Updates to this basic theory have included the addition of a fourth component, the episodic buffer (Baddeley, 2000)-- the breaking of the phonological loop into two sub-systems (Smith & Jonides, 1997), and the breaking of the visuo-spatial sketchpad into two sub-systems (Logie, 1995). Nonetheless, the three component system is still widely accepted and has been used as the framework for many working memory studies (Baddeley, 2002). The phonological loop sub-system manages speech and sound-related information (Ashcraft & Radvansky, 2010). It both stores sound-related information for perceptual purposes and articulates sound related information for speech production purposes (Baddeley, 1990). Because the phonological loop holds information in a speech-based form, it is needed in order to maintain and reproduce sequences of articulated information (i.e., letters, words) and recall speech-like information. In order to transfer information from working memory to long-term memory (i.e., encoding), rehearsal of the information in the phonological loop of working memory is essential. Rehearsal is a purposeful mental process in which information can be converted to long term memory. It entails repetitive information recycling (i.e., repeating numbers)--information is held in short term memory as long as it is repeated; it disappears gradually when rehearsal is halted. Baddeley and Hitch (1974) showed that performing other tasks that require cognitive resources may interfere with such rehearsal. Accordingly, if incivility interferes with working memory it should interfere with rehearsal and cause accelerated forgetting from short-term memory, not allowing information to be transferred to long-term memory. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 8 The visuo-spatial sketchpad is responsible for managing visual and spatial information (Ashcraft & Radvansky, 2010). It is used for temporary storage of visual information as well as mental manipulation of this information (Baddeley, 1975). The visuo-spatial sketchpad also captures the features of the physical world and predicts what dynamic objects will do next (Ashcraft & Radvansky, 2010). The visuo-spatial sketchpad consists of two components (Logie, 1995) the visual cache, which stores information about visual form and color, and the inner scribe, which deals with spatial and movement information (Eysenck & Keane, 2003). Similar to the role of the phonological loop, the inner scribe rehearses information in the visual cache and then transfers information from the visual cache to the central executive. Thus, if incivility affects working memory and disrupts the processing of information it should also interfere with the control and manipulation of visuo-perceptual information and the recall of this information. The key component of working memory is the central executive; its functions include planning future actions, initiating retrieval and decision processes, and integrating information coming into the system (Ashcraft & Radvansky, 2010). The central executive component of working memory can be seen in such daily activities as prioritizing and timesharing when working on two tasks simultaneously and ignoring irrelevant stimuli (Baddeley, 1996). The central executive resembles an attentional system and is considered the master of the two other systems. For example, the central executive is responsible for switching between visual and phonological retrieval plans, timesharing between the sub-systems, selective attention to certain stimuli and ignoring others, and temporary activation of long-term memory (Baddeley, 1996). Each of the three components of working memory has limited processing capacity. Expenditure of processing capacity results in decreased performance on tasks that require the taxed component (Ashcraft & Radvansky, 2010). Moreover, there is evidence to suggest that INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 9 overloading the capacity of the central executive results in decreased performance on tasks that require one of the other two components of working memory (Feng, Pratt, & Spence, 2012; de Fockert, Rees, Frith & Lavie, 2001). According to Baddeley and Hitch (1974; Baddeley, 1993) the main two functions of working memory are memory maintenance and attention control. These two functions continuously interact in a dynamic process (Kane & Engle, 2003). For example, encoding information into long-term memory – a memory maintenance function is often done with elaborative rehearsal (Craik & Lockhart, 1972). Elaborative rehearsal, which uses the comprehension or the meaning of the message to encode it to long-term memory requires active attention. In fact, elaborative rehearsal requires an active learner, who not only performs the rehearsal by giving it a meaning, but is also conscious of the necessity to store the gist of the information in long-term memory (Ashcraft & Radvansky, 2010). As such, the transfer of information from short-term to long-term memory (memory maintenance function) also requires the attentional control function of working memory. In turn, any interference with the attention control system is likely to interfere with the memory maintenance system. In other words, when concentration to pay attention to the meaning of the message is challenged, recall of the message should be poor. Similarly, factors that interfere with the memory maintenance system may prevent the appropriate management and control of information in the attention control system. We hypothesize that incivility will be such a factor that will likely interfere with the attentional control system and the memory maintenance functions of working memory. As the example above shows, most often, the memory maintenance function and attention control function of working memory work in concert to manage complex cognitive functions. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 10 However, in some situations one of these functions is more important than the other. For example, when it comes to maintaining rote information in short-term memory the memory maintenance system may be more important than the attention control system. In Studies 1-3 we specifically test how incivility affects the memory maintenance function using a task that requires memory maintenance of rote information in short term memory (i.e., letters) (see Table 1). In contrast, one’s ability to focus attention in the face of seemingly irrelevant distractors mainly requires the attention control system. In Study 4 we test the effects of incivility on focused attention that specifically addresses the attention control function in working memory. Yet, in most situations the two functions of memory maintenance and attention control work as a dynamic interaction to manage cognitive tasks. In Study 5 we specifically test how incivility affects this dynamic process. According to Shallice (1988; Norman & Shallice, 1986), one of the central functions of the attention control system is to engage during conflicts among task goals and manage goal priorities. Cognitive tasks that are too complex to perform directly usually have to be decomposed to sub-goals that are tractable enough to achieve directly (Newell & Simon, 1972). These multiple sub-goals have to be managed in terms of scheduling and activation. That is, the right sub-goals have to be activated at the right time in the sequence of the steps needed to achieve the task. In addition, the cognitive system has to monitor the progress towards achieving the task and manage any conflicts that surface among the sub-goals and between the super-goal (i.e., the general task goal) and the sub-goals. In order to manage sub-goals, their schedule, and monitor progress toward the super-goal, the cognitive system has to maintain in memory the super-goal and the non-activated sub-goals while performing the activated goals. The cognitive system also needs to “pop back” to the temporarily inactivated goals to check for what progress INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 11 has been made in achieving these goals (Altman & Trafton, 2002). Thus, both the attention control function and the memory maintenance function are needed in order to perform complex cognitive tasks. We believe that incivility can interfere with the cognitive system ability to manage goals and test this proposition in Study 5. Incivility and Working Memory Why would incivility disrupt working memory? Individuals have a spontaneous need to evaluate and understand events that occur in their immediate environment and have implications to their well-being. Lazarus (1991) proposed two evaluation processes: a “primary appraisal” which is an automatic, fast-track process that is unavailable to consciousness (Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977); and a “secondary appraisal” process which is a conscious, deliberate process under volitional control. We theorize that incivility is likely to affect working memory via both processes. Primary appraisal involves automatically and subconsciously shifting the focus of attention away from the task at hand because of a spontaneous need to protect oneself. The need to protect oneself from potential harm or danger is a fundamental human motive (Daly & Wilson, 1988; Kenrick, Li, & Butner, 2003) leading people to respond to aggression by fight or flight type reactions. As a result, people are adept at identifying signs of social dangers and make swift decisions about whether events are good or bad for their well-being. Indeed, primary appraisals have been shown to spark spontaneous decisions. For example, Dimberg and Ohman (1996) found that in conditional response studies participants easily and instantaneously learned to associate angry faces with painful experiences (i.e., an electric shock). Because incivility is subconsciously perceived as a sign of social danger (e.g., a status challenge) (Porath, Overbeck, & Pearson, 2008), we expect it to evoke a spontaneous re-focus of attention to avoiding this INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 12 danger. That is, people who perceive incivility likely (subconsciously) engage in this primary appraisal—focusing attention on protecting themselves—regardless of whether they are the direct target. Neurophysiological studies further support the automatic nature of individuals’ reactions to remotely dangerous and relatively minor threats. LeDoux (1998) noted that threat is evaluated through two neurophysiological pathways: Sensory information gathered in the thalamus (the relay station of the brain) is communicated to the amygdala (the brain emotional hub) both directly and indirectly through the cortex. The amygdala, in turn, has two projections: To the autonomic nervous system where bodily reactions are produced (i.e., blood pressure elevations), and to the prefrontal cortex where cognitive reactions are produced (Kandel, Schwartz, & Jessell, 2000). Thus, an encounter with a stimulus interpreted as threat (i.e., uncivil interaction) produces an immediate bodily reaction (i.e., elevated heart rate) through the fast track of the nervous system (the thalamus-amygdala-autonomic nervous system). This swift reaction is not conscious but unavoidable as it is evolutionarily designed to prepare the body for presumed defensive responses (i.e., preparation for fight or flight) (Kandel et al., 2000). Thus, threat encounters create a “fast track” automatic shift of attention to survival, subconsciously directing attention away from other tasks. Growing evidence suggests that this reaction may hold true for witnesses since people are concerned with the well-being of others (see Kollock, 1998; Porath, MacInnis, & Folkes, 2011). Witnessing harm to others may arouse strong negative emotions (Durkheim, 1964; Vidmar, 2000) which should trigger an automatic response. Following the automatic reactions, interpretations of a hostile event occur in the slower track of the nervous system (thalamus-cortex-amygdala-prefrontal cortex) (LeDoux, 1998). When information about a potential threat reaches the prefrontal cortex a secondary appraisal INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 13 process is activated, whereby people assess control of the event, develop expectations about what the event means (Smith & Ellsworth, 1985; Smith & Kirby, 2001) and craft their reactions. Accordingly, we hypothesize that incivility affects the secondary appraisal process. For example, Rafaeli et al. (2012) theorized that when employees encounter uncivil behavior from customers they are likely to reduce attention to the tasks they are performing. Instead, they think about the implications of the customer’s aggressive behavior, trying to understand the reasons for the behavior and how to deal with it. Indeed, these authors found that customer incivility reduced employees’ ability to memorize relevant information, perform analytical tasks, and handle customers’ requests. We expect witnessing incivility to have the same effects. Witnessing incivility invokes negative emotions resulting from the appraisal of the unjust uncivil behavior, summoning thoughts and feelings of concerns for others and self (Porath & Erez, 2009). Porath and Erez (2009) found that witnessing incivility affected performance on a variety of cognitive tasks. They suggest that because witnesses are upset by the incident, they are likely to spend cognitive resources evaluating the situation (i.e. Am I next in the instigator’s line of fire?), engaging in sense-making (i.e. Why did that happen?), making moral judgments (i.e. Was someone wrongly mistreated?), and processing appropriate responses (i.e. Should I say something?) (see Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996 for a review). These off-task cognitions about the uncivil event are likely to disrupt cognitive processing, which decrease task performance (Kanfer & Ackerman, 1989). Consistent with this, Porath, Macinnis, and Folkes (2010) found that witnessing incivility produced rumination about the event implying that witnesses of uncivil behaviors are inflicted by a persistent intrusion of negative thoughts that may affect their regular and routine information processing (Watkins, 2008). INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 14 Thus, there are good reasons to believe that incivility evokes secondary appraisal processes wherein people appraise their control over the uncivil event (Smith & Ellsworth, 1985; Smith & Kirby, 2001), and construct reactions to the event. Consequently, these appraisal processes occupy witnesses’ thought processes, consuming cognitive resources and limiting the cognitive resources available for other tasks (Clore, 1994). In sum, incivility may cause a shift in focus of attention (automatic process, primary appraisal), deplete people of cognitive resources by reappraising the situation searching for coping strategies (controlled process, secondary appraisal), and managing the intrusion of negative thoughts (rumination). Witnessing incivility may evoke some or all of these dynamics. The result is the same: The central executive’s attention system is occupied, which leaves fewer resources available for other central executive functions, including control of attention or management of goals, and the management of functions between the central executive in the subsystems of the phonological loop and the visuo spatial sketchpad. Thus, Hypothesis 1: Incivility will negatively influence working memory performance. Study 1 To assess whether witnessing incivility affects working memory in Study 1, we use the dual-task method, which was developed to test the limited capacity feature of memory maintenance in working memory. More specifically, the dual-task method tests the interdependence of tasks between the central executive and one of the two sub-components (Baddeley & Hitch, 1975) and the interdependence of tasks within the sub-components (Morey & Cowan, 2004). In the dual-task paradigm participants are asked to complete two tasks simultaneously, a primary task and a secondary (distractor) task. If performance on the primary task decreases when the participant is also doing the secondary task then the two tasks are INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 15 drawing on the same resources in working memory. Our study is designed to investigate if witnessing incivility interferes with a primary task in this dual-task situation. We expect the experience of witnessing incivility to function as a secondary task that requires attention from the central executive and decreases memory maintenance. We also expect decreased performance in tasks that require the central executive directly as well as tasks that require coordination between the central executive and the phonological loop (i.e. tasks that require multiple simultaneous actions). Method Participants. Students enrolled in a required course at Western university were asked to participate in a laboratory study aimed at investigating emotions, personality, decision-making and task performance. Participation was on a voluntary basis. 67 undergraduates participated with age ranging from 18 to 27 years, and a median of 20. 54% were male, 42% were white, and 45% were Asian, 5% were black, 3% were Hispanic, and 5% listed “other” as race. Procedure. This study used a procedure similar to that used by Porath et al. (2010). Participants were randomly assigned to an uncivil or control condition. They were told that they would be asked several different kinds of questions about themselves and their experiences as a customer at a local bookstore. They would be asked to imagine himself/herself as a customer who goes to a bookstore to purchase a book he/she need for a paper. After they viewed this scenario they would perform a memory functioning task, and would take a brief survey that asked them about how they felt and their experience. They wore headphones and viewed a pretimed video PowerPoint slide show depicting photographs of a local bookstore--Bretano’s-exterior and interior, as well as images of salespeople. Audio instructions told the participants that they were to imagine that they had gone to the store to search for books they wanted to buy. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 16 Accompanying the audio were photos of bookshelves and reading areas in a typical bookstore. Participants were then told to imagine that after locating their books, they went to the cashier’s desk. At that point they viewed photos of a female and a male employee. The former was gossiping on the phone with a friend. The latter offered to help participants ring up their books. The uncivil manipulation occurred in the exchange between the two salespersons (see below). After the manipulation was conducted the experimenter administrated the working memory task. Finally, participants answered several additional questions (which included the manipulation checks), were debriefed and released. Incivility Manipulation. Incivility was manipulated by the nature of the male cashier’s response to the gossiping saleswoman. In the uncivil reprimand condition, he said “Get off the phone you idiot! What do you think you’re doing talking on the phone when customers are waiting?” In the civil reprimand condition, the male employee said to the female employee “Please be more considerate of the customers who are waiting. You can talk to your friends on your break.” Materials. After viewing this PowerPoint slide show, participants were instructed to complete a working memory task-- the Automated Operation Span task (OSPAN) developed by Randy Engle and colleagues (see Unsworth, Heitz, Schrock, & Engle, 2005). Prior research has established the reliability and validity of the OSPAN as a marker of working memory capacity (Kane & Engle, 2003). Participants began with a practice routine consisting of three parts. First, in a recall task, participants were presented with a series of letters, each appearing alone on the screen for 800 milliseconds (ms). After viewing the series of letters (varying from 1 to 6 in each trial), participants were presented with a screen containing letters (including the letters presented embedded in a list of multiple other letters) and were instructed to click a box next to each letter INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 17 that appeared in the order in which it was presented. Participants then received performance feedback on this portion of the task. Second, participants were given math problems (e.g. (1*2)+1=?). When the participants solved the equation, they were instructed to click the mouse to advance to the next screen, at which point they were presented with a single number (i.e., “3”) and asked to respond to a true/false prompt indicating if this number represented the correct solution to the problem they viewed. After each operation the participants received feedback. Third, after these practice sessions, participants were presented with several practice iterations combining the recall and math portions in the order they would be presented in the actual experiment. Participants were presented with the sequential letters, followed by a math problem, followed by the choices of letters with the instructions to select the letters in the order in which they appeared prior to the math task. After completing all of the practice sessions participants proceeded to the experiment. The procedure was the same as this third practice set, repeated multiple times. The program recorded scores for accuracy on the recall tasks as well as accuracy on the math tasks. Measures. Working Memory. Similar to prior research (see Kane & Engle, 2003) we used the sum of all perfectly recalled sets of letters from the recall task provided by the Autospan program to measure working memory capacity. For example, if the participants were presented with an ‘A’, then a ‘D’, then an ‘E’, and they clicked ‘A’ then ‘D’ then ‘E’ this would result in a score of 3. If they clicked ‘A’ then ‘B’ then ‘E’ or ‘A’ then ‘E’ then ‘D’, this would result in a score of 0 since the letters were not recalled correctly or in the correct order. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 18 Math Errors. Two types of math error measures were used. The first measure was the number of math errors the participant committed, and the second measure was the number of times the participant ran out of time on a math task. Results and Discussion To determine whether our incivility manipulation created the intended experimental condition, we conducted a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with incivility as the independent variable. For the dependent variable, participants rated their agreement with the item “I thought that the salesman was respectful” (where 1=Strongly Disagree and 7=Strongly Agree)1. This manipulation check was successfully used to test for civil versus uncivil discourse by Porath et al. (2010). The results confirmed the expected manipulation effects (Mcontrol= 3.60, SDcontrol=1.17; Muncivil= 2.84, SDuncivil= 1.35; F(1, 67)= 6.06 , p = .01). An ANOVA with working memory as the dependent variable and the incivility manipulation as the independent variable showed a significant effect (F(1, 65) = 4.72, p<.05, 2=.07). Those in the uncivil condition exhibited a reduction in the working memory capacity (M=43.28, SD=10.67) in comparison to the control group (M=50.03, SD=14.29). That is, those who observed the uncivil interaction between the two employees did not recall as many letters in the dual-task assignment as those who observed the benign interaction. Thus, H1 was supported. In a post-hoc analysis, we tested the influence of incivility on performance on the distractor task (in the operation span). Hence, we tested the effects of witnessing incivility on both the number of math errors committed and speed errors—errors in which the participant ran out of time in attempting to solve a given operation. A MANOVA with these two measures of 1 The manipulation checks in all five studies were given to participants at the end of the experiment after all the cognitive tasks were completed to avoid potential confounds. Self-report checks of manipulations can significantly undermine the ecological validity of the study. Such reports may induce experimental influences, response effects, focus participants’ attention on emotions and therefore interfere with the task, and make people self-conscious and suspicious (see Isen & Erez, 2007). INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 19 performance on the distractor task as the dependent variables and incivility as the independent variable was insignificant F(2, 64) = 1.00, ns. Study 1 findings showed that observing incivility interferes with working memory processes. Working memory is the workbench where conscious mental effort is applied and its major functions are planning future actions, initiating retrieval and decision processes, integrating information coming into the memory system, and transferring information to long-term memory (Baddeley & Hitch, 1975). Numerous studies have shown that cognitive activities such as recall, learning, reasoning, and problem solving ability are highly dependent on a functional working memory (Baddeley, 1992). Therefore, if incivility is distracting and affects working memory it should also affect other cognitive functions that depend on working memory. Study 1 results, then, may help explain the negative effects of witnessing incivility on complex task performance and creativity found in Porath and Erez (2009). Our test using the dual-task method suggests that witnessing incivility affects the functioning of the central executive and its interaction with the phonological loop, ultimately decreasing memory maintenance. To test whether working memory indeed mediates the relationship between witnessing incivility and task performance and creativity we designed Study 2. Study 2 More than 40 years of research on problem-solving show that complex tasks are difficult because people have to memorize and search through a large problem space to find a solution. Thus, complex tasks require an extensive memory search and may cause a major working memory overload, whereby individuals have to "hold more in their heads" in order to find a solution (Eysenck & Keane, 2003). Study 1 showed that incivility is likely to hamper working memory functioning as attentional resources are devoted to off-task activities. Such distractions INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 20 reduce the likelihood of task focus and flow (cf., Csikszentmihalyi, 1990, 1997) and may render the cognitive strategies needed to employ solutions to complex problems impossible. Incivility studies indicate that this is indeed the case, as people that experience or even witness incivility are less likely to solve complex problems in comparison to controls (Porath & Erez, 2007, 2009; Rafaeli et al., 2012). These findings also suggest that because it is more difficult for people who experience or witness incivility to concentrate they will also tend to be less creative. However, in addition to attentional resource depletion, there is also another way by which incivility may reduce performance and creativity. Creative thinking often requires deep analogical thinking, in which a conceptual structure set of ideas is mapped onto another target set of ideas. For example, to understand the structure of the atom, Rutheford used the solar system analogy, where the atoms circulate around the nucleus in the same manner that the planets orbit around the sun (see Gentner, 1983). Two cognitive operations are engaged in this mapping. First, using the memory maintenance function, people must retrieve from memory a domain of concepts that corresponds to the problems they face, and second, employing the central executive system, they must match and compare the two sets of ideas in order to find similar or complementary relational structure (see Gentner, 1983; Keane, 1988). This type of thinking requires various working memory functions such as switching of retrieval plans, timesharing between tasks, selective attention, and goal management. It demands effective coordination between the central executive (controlling the process) and the memory retrieval function. Thus, if incivility disrupts working memory it should also affect creativity as Porath and Erez (2007, 2009) showed. Thus, Hypothesis 2: Working memory will mediate the relationships between witnessing incivility and performance of complex tasks and creativity. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 21 Method Participants. Students enrolled in a required course at a Western university were asked to participate in a laboratory study aimed at investigating the personality correlates of task performance. Participation was on a voluntary basis. 54 students participated with age ranging from 18 to 33 years, and a median age of 21. Of the sample, 54% were male, 35% were white, 30% were Asian, 15% were black, 9% were Hispanic, and 11% listed “other” as race. Procedure. This study used a procedure similar to that used by Porath and Erez (2009). Participants were told that the experiment was designed to study how individual differences in personality and moods affect people’s performance and creativity. They were told that they would begin with a personality questionnaire and then would be asked to complete three tasks: a memory task and two other cognitive tasks. Participants came to the lab in a group of five for sessions that took about 40 minutes to complete. Participants completed a personality survey that was used as a time filler. About seven minutes into the personality survey a confederate arrived late to the room and said ‘I am really sorry that I am late, my class across campus was not released on time’. Sessions were randomly assigned to be in either an uncivil or neutral condition. In both conditions the confederate was dismissed from the room, and in the uncivil condition the experimenter dismissed the confederate in an uncivil manner (see description below of incivility manipulation). When participants had completed the personality questionnaire, the experimenter administrated three performance tasks. The first was the working memory task, followed by the 10 minute anagram and 5 minute brain-storming task. Finally, participants answered several additional questions (which included the manipulation checks), were debriefed and released. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 22 Incivility Manipulation. When the confederate arrived late to the session, he or she apologized for being late, after which the experimenter did one of two things. In the control condition the experimenter dismissed the confederate in a neutral tone. In the incivility condition, the experimenter dismissed the confederate rudely, saying, “What is it with you? You arrive late…you’re irresponsible…look at you…how do you expect to hold a job in the real world?” In both cases the confederate was dismissed from the room and not allowed to participate in the study. This manipulation was given in plain sight of the participants so they could see and hear the interaction. The incivility manipulation was designed to be a specific and direct uncivil act to the confederate, as opposed to the control condition which was delivered in a neutral tone that seemed appropriate for the situation. The uncivil manipulation was specifically designed to seem uncalled for, but was designed not to seem too aggressive. Measures. Task performance. Task performance was tested using two tasks that were previously used by Porath and Erez (2007, 2009). The first task was the number of anagrams (purposely scrambled words) participants solved correctly in a 10 anagram performance task. In a previous study (Erez & Isen, 2002) these anagrams were tested and shown to be moderately difficult. Second, participants were asked to produce as many uses as they could for a brick in 5 minutes, with the number produced counted as measure of performance. Psychologists frequently use this type of brain-storming task to test performance and motivation (Guerin, 1999; Harkins, 1987). Creativity. The brick brain-storming task is often also used to test creativity (Frick, Guilford, Christensen & Merrifled, 1959; Guilford, 1975). Consistent with previous studies, three graduate students (who were blind to the experimental condition) rated the uses participants produced for the brick using a scale ranging from 1=”low” to 7=”high”. Anchors taken from a INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 23 pilot study were given to the graduate students as a way to frame the high (6-7) and low (1-2) ends of the scale. An example of an anchor used for the low end of the scale was ‘build a house’ and an example of an anchor used for the high end of the scale was ‘decorate it like a pet and the give it to a kid as a present’. The inter-rater reliabilities were ICC1 (.86) and ICC2 (.95) suggesting that aggregation across raters was appropriate. Working Memory. To measure working memory we used the same working memory score used in Study 1. That is, the number of letters in the sets that were recalled correctly. Math Errors. The same two types of math error measures (the number of math errors the participant committed, and the number of times the participant ran out of time on a math task) used in Study 1 were also used here. Results To determine whether our incivility manipulation created the intended experimental conditions, we conducted a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with incivility as the independent variable. For the dependent variable, participants rated their agreement (1=Strongly Disagree to 7=Strongly Agree) with a five-item construct indicating the experimenter behavior toward other participants (=0.81). The items included "The experimenter treated others with dignity,” “The experimenter treated others with respect,” “The experimenter refrained from improper remarks and comments”, “I felt bad as a result of the experimenter treatment of people (reversed),” and “People were insulted by the experimenter (reversed)” (Porath & Erez, 2009). The first three items were taken from the interpersonal justice subscale of Colquitt (2001). The results confirmed the expected manipulation effects (Mcontrol= 3.96, SDcontrol=.88; Mincivility= 3.45, SDincivility= 1.02; F(1, 52)= 4.03, p = .05). INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 24 Means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations among the study variables are provided in Table 2. We tested our hypotheses using MANOVA for the three dependent variables of performance on the anagram task, performance on the brick task, and creativity. The overall model representing the influence of incivility on the three dependent variables was significant, Multivariate F(3, 50) = 3.35, p<.05, 2=.17. The results of a series of ANOVAs are presented in Table 3. These results show that participants in the uncivil condition performed worse than participants in the neutral condition on all three dependent variables. Next, we tested the influence of witnessing incivility on working memory. An ANOVA with working memory as the dependent variable and incivility as the independent variable showed a significant effect (F(1,52) = 3.99, p<.05, 2=.07). Those in the uncivil condition exhibited a reduction in the working memory capacity compared to the control group. Next, we tested whether working memory mediated the relationship between witnessing incivility and the three dependent variables using a bootstrapping approach with 2000 replications in Mplus 7.11 (Muthen & Muthen, 2012). In bootstrapping a random sample is drawn from the data set multiple times. In each random sample drawn, direct and indirect effects and their standard errors are estimated. We specified three models with a direct effect from incivility to each dependent variable, as well as an indirect effect through working memory. Table 4 shows that the relationship between witnessing incivility and anagram (-.43, 95% CI: [1.10, -.03]) performance and creativity (-.21, 95% CI [-.62, -.01]) were significantly mediated by working memory. However, the relationship between witnessing incivility and brick performance was not mediated by working memory as the 95% CI included zero [-1.04, .121]. Thus, H2 was partially supported. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 25 Here again, in a post-hoc analysis we tested the influence of incivility on performance on the distractor task. A MANOVA with the two measures of performance on the distractor math task as the dependent variables and incivility as the independent variable showed insignificant results F(2, 51) = 02, ns. Discussion Previous studies suggest that people get pulled off track after experiencing or even just observing incivility and that incivility disrupts their cognitive functioning (Porath & Erez, 2007, 2009; Rafaeli et al., 2012). However, these studies did not specify the cognitive mechanisms that mediate these relationships. We uncover the specific internal mental processes that incivility disrupts. Study 2 contributes to our understanding of how incivility disrupts cognition by showing that the effect of observing incivility on various forms of cognitive performance is carried through working memory. Because working memory is at the cornerstone of how people think, perceive, remember, and learn, it is likely that the negative effects of incivility on working memory are extended to numerous cognitive functions. Study 2 extends the findings of Study 1 by using a different manipulation to triangulate our results. Results from a single study that uses one manipulation is subject to questions or potential skepticism that the finding could be attributed to the specific manipulation employed. Triangulation involves using different methods to represent a concept and observing that they produce the same effects (Isen & Erez, 2007). Because our manipulation in Study 2 converged with our previous results we can conclude with reasonable confidence that it is witnessing incivility and not just something simply associated with one particular operationalization that was responsible for the results. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 26 In the next study we use a third manipulation to triangulate our results – we prime it. There are several reasons to believe that incivility ‘even if you are just around it’ will directly and perhaps automatically affect cognitions. First, incivility represents a social threat and individuals are very adept at identifying social dangers and responding to them in swift and automatic ways (see Miller, 2001; Vidmar, 2000; Porath et al., 2008). Second, it is now largely accepted that the environment can directly and subconsciously affect individuals through perception. Perception is the process by which the environment directly causes an internal mental representation of the outside world; it is largely automatic and not under intentional control (Bargh & Chartrand, 1999). Social perception is a subset of general perception and it entails representation of social objects, events, and others behaviors and similar to other forms of perception it is, to a large extent, an automated phenomenon (Bargh & Chartrand, 1999). For example, numerous studies show that others’ behaviors are automatically encoded as relevant trait concepts (e.g., Bargh & Thein, 1985; Winter & Uleman, 1984; Carlston & Skowronski, 1994; Uleman, Newman, & Moskowitz, 1996) and that contextual priming of trait concepts automatically affect people’s interpretations of other behaviors (Bargh, 1989; Higgins, 1989, 1996; Wyer & Srull, 1989). Accordingly, we theorize that incivility in the person’s environment can directly and automatically affect cognition, and as such, priming incivility should also disrupt cognition. While we know of no studies to date that investigated the effects of priming aggression on cognitive functioning, there are several studies that show that priming aggression can affect individuals’ behaviors. Participants in Carver, Ganellen, Froming, and Chambers (1983) experiment were first exposed to hostility-related words in a presumed “language experiment” and then took the role of a “teacher” who had to give electric shock to “learners” in what they INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 27 believed to be a separate experiment. Those who were primed with the hostile words were more likely than those who were not primed with hostility to give longer shocks to the learner. Similarly, Bargh, Chen, and Burrows (1996) primed participants with uncivil words and found that those in the uncivil condition engaged in uncivil behavior towards the experimenter (i.e., interrupted his conversation with a confederate) more than those who were not primed with incivility. Priming incivility appears to have direct and subconscious effects on individuals. We theorize that these effects may extend beyond behavior to cognitive processes such as working memory. Thus, in the next study we specifically test the hypothesis that priming incivility will affect working memory functioning. Study 3 We theorized that witnessing incivility may affect cognitive processing automatically (via primary appraisals) and subconsciously—or in a controlled manner (i.e. secondary appraisals). To test whether witnessing incivility affects the automatic process, we designed Study 3 in which we prime incivility. In this study we test whether the results of Studies 1 and 2 hold for the priming of incivility. This is relevant because if priming incivility has negative consequences then it implies that simply working in an uncivil culture (i.e. just being around it) may be detrimental. It casts a wider stakeholder net for who incivility touches and how, with practical implications for how best to manage incivility. At the outset, at least two of the characteristics of witnessing incivility do not seem to correspond to the criteria of automaticity and thus the consequences of incivility seem isolated to the controlled process, in which people are conscious of its effects. Automatic processes were originally conceptualized to be rapid, unintentional and unavoidable, outside of awareness, and to consume few if any conscious resources (Posner & Snyder, 1975; Shiffrin & Schneider, INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 28 1977). Witnessing incivility is clearly not outside of awareness and there is a strong evidence to suggest that it consumes cognitive resources (see Porath & Erez, 2009). Is it possible though that witnessing incivility also operates automatically, attacking individuals’ functioning in ways that people are not even aware of? Incivility researchers have implied that incivility’s effects are often insidious and long-lasting. For some targets or witnesses of incivility the effects result in a brain burn (see Hallowell, 1997), in which the memory is tattooed on the brain and is repeatedly resurrected subconsciously by various triggers (i.e. seeing your bosses’ door, where you witnessed an uncivil event long ago) (Pearson & Porath, 2009). According to Bargh (1989) automatic processes do not have to start with sub-conscious processes and may be primed by recent conscious experience of a construct, that in turn, increases the accessibility of that construct for some time after one is no longer consciously aware of it. Indeed, both Higgins, Bargh, and Lombardi (1985) and Lombardi, Higgins, and Bargh (1987) found that priming a construct with a conscious experience can linger for some time and exert preconscious influence on the interpretation of relevant stimuli. In addition, Strauman and Higgins (1987) found that priming participants with traits that represented the participant emotional vulnerabilities (i.e., agitation) automatically activated these emotions. Taken together, these studies suggest that people’s conscious experience can linger for a long time. As in the case of a ‘brain burn’, it can be activated or primed in influential ways if the experience (i.e. incivility) was meaningful (Hallowell, 1997). Because witnessing incivility is likely to be meaningful to most individuals it should prime an alerted state of mind long after the stimulus (i.e. the uncivil interaction) (Pearson & Porath, 2009). While automatic processes may not consume cognitive resources they can certainly start a process that interferes with other tasks by shifting attention to the primed task. For example, while driving does not normally interfere INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 29 with other tasks (i.e., listening to the radio) a sense of danger (i.e., a threat of an accident) will shift cognitive resources to the driving task and away from other tasks such as listening to the radio. In the same way, because it represents social threat, incivility will likely automatically shift the focus of attention from other tasks and start a process that can consume cognitive resources. Thus, Hypothesis 3: Priming incivility will disrupt working memory functioning, which in turn, will negatively affect performance of complex tasks and creativity. Method Participants. Students enrolled in a required course at a Western university were asked to participate in a laboratory study aimed at investigating the personality correlates of task performance. Participation was on a voluntary basis. 60 undergraduates participated with age ranging from 18 to 28 years, and a median age of 21. Of the sample, 55% were male, 29% were white, 50% were Asian, 9% were Hispanic, 5% listed race as “other”, and 7% did not report their race. Procedure and Manipulation. Participants completed a short personality survey, which was included as part of the cover story that this study was looking at the influence of personality on task performance. They were told that they would perform four cognitive tasks: a grammatical construction of sentences, a memory task, an analytical task, and a creative task. After the personality survey, participants were given a scrambled sentence task similar to that used by Bargh et al. (1996). This priming task was the manipulation in this study (see below). After completing the scrambled sentence task, participants completed the working memory task, anagram task, and brainstorming task, as described in study 2. Finally, participants answered INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 30 several additional questions (which included the manipulation checks), were debriefed and released. In the incivility condition we used the version of the priming task that Bargh et al. (1996) used to prime incivility. Participants were presented with 30 combinations of 5 words and instructed to use the five words to create a grammatically correct four word sentence. Of the 30 scrambled sentences, 15 contained a stimulus that was intended to prime incivility within the list of 5 words presented. The 15 words that were used to prime incivility were aggressively, bold, bother, disturb, intrude, annoyingly, interrupt, audaciously, brazen, impolitely, infringe, obnoxious, aggravating, and bluntly. For example, one of the 5 word combinations would have been ‘they her bother see usually.’ A correct assembling for this combination would yield the sentence ‘they usually bother her.” In the neutral condition, we used the version of the task that Bargh et al. (1996) used in their neutral condition. In this version, the 15 stimuli were exercising, flawlessly, occasionally, rapidly, gleefully, practices, optimistically, successfully, normally, send, watches, encourages, gives, clears, and prepares. For example, one of the five word combinations would have been ‘they her send see usually’. A correct assembling of this combination would yield the sentence ‘they usually see her.” Measures Task performance. Task performance was tested using the same two measures of anagram and brick performance used in Study 2. Creativity. The same method used in Study 2 to test for creativity was used in this study. Here again three graduate students who were blind to the experimental condition rated the uses participants produced for the brick using the same anchored scale used in Study 2. The INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 31 inter-rater reliabilities were ICC1 (.86) and ICC2 (.95), suggesting that aggregation across raters was appropriate. Working Memory. To measure working memory we used the same absolute score measure used in Study 1 and 2. Math Errors. We measured math errors by using the same accuracy and speed errors measures used in Study 1 and 2. Results and Discussion To determine whether our incivility manipulation created the intended experimental conditions, we conducted a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with incivility as the independent variable. For the dependent variable, participants rated their agreement with a fouritem construct indicating the nature of the sentences they constructed (=0.82). The items included "The sentences you created were impolite,” “The sentences you created were pleasant, (reversed)”, “The sentences you created involved incivility”, and “The sentences you created involved unpleasantness” (where 1=Strongly Disagree and 7=Strongly Agree). The results confirmed the expected manipulation effects (Mcontrol= 2.83, SDcontrol=1.10; Mincivility= 4.42, SDincivility= 1.20; F(1, 59)= 29.07, p < .01). Means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations among the study variables are provided in Table 5. We tested our hypotheses using MANOVA for the three dependent variables of performance on the anagram task, performance on the brick task, and creativity. The overall model representing the influence of incivility on the three dependent variables was significant, Multivariate F(3, 56) = 9.32, p<.01, 2=.33. The results of a series of ANOVAs are presented in Table 6. These results show that participants in the uncivil condition performed worse than participants in the neutral condition on all three dependent variables. Next, we tested the INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 32 influence of incivility on working memory. An ANOVA with working memory as the dependent variable and incivility as the independent variable showed a significant effect (F(1,59) = 4.16, p<.05, 2=.07). Those in the uncivil condition exhibited a reduction in the working memory capacity in comparison to the control group. Next, we tested whether working memory mediated the relationship between incivility and the three dependent variables using a bootstrapping approach with 2000 replications in Mplus 7.11 (Muthen & Muthen, 2012). We specified three models with a direct effect from incivility to each dependent variable, as well as an indirect effect through working memory. Table 7 shows that the relationship between witnessing incivility and anagram (-.29, 95% CI: [.86, -.02]) and brick (-.52, 95% CI: [-1.58, -.02]) performance, and creativity (-.27, 95% CI [-.71, -.03]) were significantly mediated by working memory. Thus, H3 has been supported. In the same post-hoc analysis, we tested the influence of incivility on performance on the operation span’s distractor task. Hence, we tested the effects of incivility on the number of math errors committed and speed errors in which the participant ran out of time in attempting to solve a given operation. A MANOVA with these two measures of performance on the distractor task as the dependent variables and incivility as the independent variable showed significant results F(2,58) = 3.33, p<.05, 2=.10. Those in the uncivil condition made more math errors (Mcontrol= 4.45, SDcontrol=2.98; Muncivil= 6.37, SDuncivil= 3.06; F(1, 59)= 6.15 , p = .05) and more speed errors (not solving the math problem in the time allotted) (Mcontrol= .81, SDcontrol=.95; Muncivil= 1.83, SDuncivil= 2.44; F(1, 59)= 4.76 , p = .05). This study shows that incivility affects performance because incivility is on the minds of the participants. In this study incivility was primed in the participants’ associative network (Collins & Loftus, 1975). Incivility was directly placed on the participants’ minds and effects INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 33 similar to those in Study 2 were observed. These priming results suggest that it is not simply witnessing an uncivil event that consumes cognitive resources, disrupts working memory, and harms performance. Rather, there seems to be something qualitatively disruptive about incivility in particular that elicits a response in people. Even subconscious triggers (i.e. our priming manipulation) of incivility weigh on people’s mind, taking a cognitive toll on working memory and subsequent performance. These results indicate that even if people are unaware of the effects of being around incivility, there are detrimental effects. In institutional contexts (i.e., the workplace, schools), the implications are significant. An uncivil or toxic culture may disrupt the functioning and performance of those even seemingly unaffected. Over time, these cognitive performance losses may take a greater toll on individual and organizational performance. Taken together, studies 1, 2, and 3 indicate that working memory mediates the effect of incivility on performance and that this effect occurs because incivility is on the minds of participants—whether or not they are conscious of it. In Studies 4 and 5 we attempt to isolate what part of working memory incivility affects. In order to really understand why incivility affects performance on complex and creative tasks, these questions need to be addressed. It may also help to offer more detailed, accurate recommendations of what individuals should be particularly mindful of when they observe or are simply around incivility. Thus, the next set of studies test the influence of incivility on the attention control function of working memory. Study 4 Studies 1, 2, and 3 showed that incivility affected individuals’ working memory, performance on complex tasks, and creativity. However, all the tasks employed in these studies were verbal tasks tied (at least primarily) to the central executive and its coordination with the phonological loop. Thus, it is possible that incivility only interferes with articulatory processing INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 34 of information and not necessarily with other processes such as manipulation of visual information. In this study we investigate whether incivility also interferes with the attentional processes of visual information--the visuo-spatial sketchpad of working memory. Over the past decade, researchers started demonstrating that perception of visual information requires attention and that when attention is diverted to other objects or tasks individuals often fail to perceive unexpected objects, even if they are at fixation (Simons & Chabris, 1999). This phenomenon is termed ‘inattentional blindness’ (Mack & Rock, 1998). In a typical study an unexpected, taskirrelevant object appears for some duration during a continuous task (e.g., an actor dressed in a gorilla suit passes through people playing a ball game while participants count the number of ball-passes made, Simons & Chabris, 1999). Participants are then asked to report whether they were aware of any extra task-irrelevant stimulus, or anything unusual on the screen. Results show that participants often fail to notice the unexpected task-irrelevant stimulus. The studies on the inattentional blindness phenomenon mainly examined perception under conditions of heightened perceptual load but did not investigate other factors that may capture attention (Cartwright-Finch & Lavie, 2007; Koivisto & Revonsuo, 2009). That is, in a typical study participants are asked to perform a specific perceptual task that captures their attention (i.e., counting the number of passes the players make) and this load interferes with the attention required to perceive unexpected objects or events. However, there are reasons to believe that when people’s minds are occupied with other activities they also miss important perceptual information. For example, it is now well known that when people talk on the phone while driving they may miss important perceptual information that they would normally notice (i.e., a pedestrian step unexpectedly into the road) (Chabris & Simons, 2010). Similarly, we argue that incivility may occupy ones’ mind, shift the focus of attention to the uncivil event, and INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 35 contribute to inattentional blindness. Thus, H4: Incivility will lead to greater inattentional blindness. Method. Participants. Students enrolled in a course at a Mid-Atlantic university were asked to participate in a laboratory study. Participation was on a voluntary basis. They were simply told that they would be asked several different kinds of questions about themselves. 99 undergraduates participated with age ranging from 17 to 27 years, and a median of 20. 42% were male, 66% were white, 18% were Asian, 8% were Hispanic, 2% were Black, and 6%reported “other”. Procedure, Manipulation, and Materials. The procedure for this study was similar to that reported in Study 3, with a few notable changes. Incivility was manipulated using the same Bargh et al. (1996) priming task. After completing the priming task participants completed the Invisible Gorilla task to test the influence of incivility on selective attention and perception. The Invisible Gorilla task used in this study was similar to that used by Simons & Chabris (1999). All participants individually viewed a video of two groups of players passing a basketball. Members of one team were wearing black shirts and members of the other team were wearing white shirts. Participants were instructed to keep a silent mental count of the number of passes made by one of the teams and were instructed to count the passes of either the black team (the Black condition) or the white team (the White condition). During the video while participants were counting the passes a person in a black gorilla suit walked through the screen. After viewing the complete video participants were instructed to write down the tally for the number of passes made by the team they had been assigned to watch. They were then asked a series of several questions about what they have witnessed. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 36 Measures. Inattentional Blindness. Selective attention was measured by the participants' response to the question 'While you were doing the counting, did you notice anything unusual on the video?'. From a validity perspective our motive was to query the participants regarding their awareness of the gorilla without asking them specifically if they had seen a gorilla and thus cluing them into the purpose of the question. Prior research using a similar paradigm (Simons & Chabris, 1999) suggests that the response to this question and responses to questions specifically about the gorilla are highly equivalent. Therefore this question gauges the participants' awareness of the gorilla without notifying them of the presence of the gorilla. Results and Discussion Twenty-four percent of the participants in the uncivil condition reported not noticing anything unusual while only six percent in the neutral condition did not notice anything unusual. Comparing the results of the white and black groups we found that in the white color condition 38% of the participants in the incivility manipulation condition did not report seeing anything unusual while only, 9% in the neutral condition did not notice anything unusual. In the black color condition these percentages were 10% (uncivil condition) and 4% (neutral condition). To test our hypothesis that incivility may cause inattentional blindness, we ran a logistic regression analysis with bootstrapping of 1000 iterations where the dependent variable was inattentional blindness and the independent variables were the incivility manipulation, the color condition, and the interaction between these variables. The model showed that the interaction term was not significant. Thus, we ran a second model in which we did not include the interaction term. The overall model was significant (χ2=11.03, p <.01) and explained 12% of the variance in inattentional blindness. Similar to what was found in previous studies (Simons & INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 37 Chabris, 1999) our results showed that the color condition was significant (b=1.52, p < .05) and those in the white condition noticed the gorilla almost five times (odds-ratio=4.57) more than those in the black condition. The incivility effect on inattentional blindness was also significant (b=1.57, p <.05) with an odds-ratio of 4.82, suggesting that those in the neutral condition were about 5 times more likely to report noticing anything unusual than those in the uncivil condition. Thus, H4 was supported. Our results indicate that observing incivility leads to inattentional blindness. These results suggest that incivility not only harms working memory and subsequent performance based on verbal tasks, but also decreases attention to perceptual visual cues. Thus, the effects of incivility not only affect the phonological loop and central executive of working memory as shown in Studies 1, 2 and 3, but also the visuo-spatial sketchpad, and the coordination between it and the central executive. Those that were primed with incivility were nearly five times less likely to be attentive to seemingly obvious cues. These results suggest that people simply around incivility may miss important information. People working in an uncivil environment may be less attuned or attentive to key information, which may harm their performance in a variety of ways. If incivility leads individuals to miss important information in emergency situations (i.e., a surgery) than the results may be even fatal. These findings add to understanding of the insidious costs of incivility. In order to better understand these costs we examine more specifically the processes in the attention control system that incivility hampers in the following study. Study 5 An important process in the central executive of working memory is the cognitive management of goals. Goal management is a crucial part of problem solving, decision-making, and the planning of future actions and therefore is important to individual functioning. Thus, in INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 38 this study we test the effects of incivility on one specific key aspect of working memory tied to the attention control system of the central executive – goal management. Working memory capacity may be less than ideal for solving complex problems, but the goal management function of working memory can compensate for inadequate memory capacity by planning and structuring problems to subroutines (Altman & Trafton, 2002). If a task is too complex to perform directly it can be decomposed into sub-goals and if these sub-goals are also too complex they can be further decomposed to sub-goals (Ernst & Newell, 1969; Newell & Simon, 1972). Ultimately, the system reaches a sub-goal that can be managed directly. However, these subgoals need to be held in working memory, prioritized, and managed. If working memory is occupied by interfering tasks it is likely that the goal management function of working memory will be hampered. Thus, in Study 5 we test whether incivility negatively affects goal management in (the central executive of) working memory. In order to study goal management in working memory, we had participants complete the Tower of Hanoi task. This task is designed to test the central executive function of working memory, specifically. In the Tower of Hanoi task participants are asked to move a series of disks (in this case 4) from one peg to another among a set of three pegs. The correct solution takes 15 moves (Carpenter, Just, & Shell, 1990) and requires the participant to manage a recursive set of primary goals and subgoals (Welsh, Cicerello, Cuneo, & Brennan, 1995). Participants must regularly suspend and then resume progress on goals (Altman & Trafton, 2002). The task can be particularly taxing on working memory because it creates goal-subgoal conflict (Goel & Grafman, 1995). There are several moves where making progress on a higher order goal requires taking a step backwards on a lower order goal. Thus, some moves require managing more conflicting goals than others (Carpenter et al., 1990). If incivility interferes with goal INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 39 management it should particularly interfere with moves in the Tower of Hanoi task that require subgoal-conflict management. Thus, H5: Incivility will interfere with working memory’s ability to manage goals. Method Participants. Students enrolled in a course at a Mid-Atlantic university were asked to participate in a laboratory study aimed at investigating the personality correlates of task performance. Participation was on a voluntary basis. 154 undergraduates participated with age ranging from 17 to 32 years, and a median of 20. 52% were male, 70% were white, 14% were Asian, 6% were Hispanic, 4% were Black, and 5% reported “other”. Procedure, Manipulation, and Materials.The procedure for this study was similar to that reported in Studies 3 and 4, with a few notable changes. Similar to the previous studies, participants began with a short personality survey and then completed the incivility priming task. The priming task was identical to the task employed in Studies 3 and 4. Rather than completing the working memory, anagram, and brick tasks, participants completed the Tower of Hanoi task to test for goal management in working memory. The Tower of Hanoi task was played on a computer. At the beginning of the task all four of the disks were on the first peg (of the three pegs), with the largest disk at the bottom and the smallest disk at the top (see Figure 1). The participants were instructed to reconstruct this order of disks on the third peg, moving the disks across the pegs one move a time. Participants were also instructed that at no time could a larger disk be on top of a smaller disk. Moving the four disks from peg one to peg three requires a sequence of 15 specific correct moves. Because in the Tower of Hanoi there is only one correct sequence if participants made an error in a move the computer program rejected this move and moved the disk to the previous position. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 40 Measures. Task performance. Task performance was measured with three variables from the Tower of Hanoi task. First, we measured the amount of time in milliseconds participants spent thinking about a move. This decision making (DM) time was measured as the elapsed time from the completion of the previous move to the point of clicking the mouse on the screen for the next move. Physical move time (PM) time was measured as the amount of time spent physically moving a disk from one peg to another. Finally, errors were measured as the number of incorrect attempts made per move before the correct move was made. For each move, the decision-making time and physical move time were measured as the aggregate of the incorrect moves and the eventual correct move. For example, if on the 5th move the participant made 2 incorrect attempts and then made the correct move, the time spent thinking for this move was measured as the aggregate of the time spent thinking on the two incorrect moves and the correct move. Physical move time was measured in a similar way. Goal Management. Moves in the Tower of Hanoi task can be classified as requiring a high or low level of goal management. Some moves require the participant to evaluate multiple potential scenarios before making the move. In particular moves 1, 5, 9, and 13 require the participant to generate one or more sub-goals before executing the move (Carpenter et al., 1990). The other moves do not require specific goal management. For this study we coded moves 1, 5, 9, and 13 as moves that require a high level of goal management. Results To test the influence of incivility on goal management we used hierarchical linear modeling (HLM 6.08, Raudenbush, Bryk, & Congdon, 2009). Using the HLM model described below we regressed time spent thinking about a move (DM), time spent physically making a INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 41 move (PM), and the number of errors made (errors) on the difficulty of the move. At the first level of analysis (i.e., the move level), the specified model for each move is: ij = 0j + 1j X1ij+ rij Where, ij is each move i in individual j’s score of DM, PM or errors; 0j (the intercept) represents the average dependent variable scores for each individual; 1j represents the relationship between easy moves and hard moves and the dependent variable; X1ij represents the difficulty of move i within the person j (dummy variable – hard moves coded as ‘1’, easy moves coded as ‘0’); and rij represents the move error term. HLM incorporates a second-level (i.e., individual-level) in which the move-level intercepts (0j) and slopes (2j) are simultaneously regressed on the incivility condition variable: 0j = 00 +01W1j + U0j 1j = 10 + 11W1j + U1j Where 00 represents the grand mean, W1 represents the incivility condition (dummy coded – incivility condition coded as ‘1’ and control condition coded as ‘0’), 01 represents the relationship between the incivility condition and 0j, 10 represents the influence of move difficulty on the dependent variables, controlling for incivility. 11 represents the influence of the incivility condition on the relationship between move difficulty and score on DM, PM and error scores of individual j, and U represented the individual-level error terms. To test whether performance in the Tower of Hanoi task was affected by the incivility priming we ran a reduced model in which the dependent variables were regressed only on the incivility condition. The results are presented in Table 8. Step 1 shows that those who were primed with incivility took more time to make a decision about moves, took more time to physically move the mouse, and made significantly more errors than those in the neutral INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 42 condition. Next, we included move difficulty in the model to test more specifically whether the moves that involved goal management were particularly taxing under the incivility condition. Step 2 shows that priming incivility affected the relationships between move difficulty and decision-making time and physical move time but not the relationship between move difficulty and number of errors. As shown in Figure 2, incivility did not affect decision-making time in the moves that did not require excessive goal management. However, incivility slowed decisionmaking time significantly in the moves that required goal management. The same pattern of results with regard to the effects of incivility on the relationship between goal management moves and physical time of moves is shown in Figure 3. Here again, incivility did not affect the physical move time of the moves that did not require goal management but it significantly slowed the physical moves that required goal management. Thus, H5 was supported. Discussion Study 5 results demonstrate how incivility influences goal management, a key function in the central executive component of working memory. Incivility slowed both decision-making and physical move time for goal management activities, but not those that didn’t require goal management. These results help us to better pinpoint how incivility taxes working memory, and in doing so, decreases performance on complex tasks. The term “goal” refers to a mental representation of an intention to achieve a specific state of the world such as accomplishing a task (Altmann & Trafton, 2002). However, in order to accomplish goals, they often must be suspended or set aside temporarily, as in hierarchical problem solving, where one has to suspend a goal, in favor of a sub-goal that must be achieved first (Miller, Galanter & Pribram, 1960). Goal management also requires resuming a suspended goal, which in turn, involves toggling back to the stage where the goal was suspended. The better the working memory system function INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 43 and one can recall how far it has progressed toward achieving the expected goal the more efficiently it can restart the goal (Altmann & Trafton, 2002). In fact, recommencing the task at the right stage, without wasting resources on redundant actions, but also without skipping steps, is essential for goal management (Latorella, 1996). Thus, any interference with the goal management system, such as incivility, entails disruption to the suspension and reactivation of the goals and sub-goals. Indeed, Study 5 findings showed that incivility disrupts this cognitive process. In addition, the results of Study 5 showed that incivility affected physical moves and these findings correspond to Study 4 findings that incivility affects the visuo-spatial functioning of working memory. Specifically, it is likely that incivility affects the functioning of the inner scribe part of the visuo-spatial sub-system. Accoring to Logie (1995) the visuo-spatial sketchpad consists of two inter-dependent visual and spatial components. One component is a passive visual storage system (the ‘visual cache’) and the other is an active spatial rehearsal apparatus (the ‘inner scribe’). The contents of the storage system of the visual cache are refreshed via the mechanism of the active inner scribe (Pearson, 2001). However, the inner scribe is also important during the planning and execution of body and limb movement (Pearson, 2001; Eysenck & Keane, 2003). A direct consequence of this is that factors such as incivility that interfere with the functions of the visuo-spatial sketchpad, as has been shown in the previous study, could potentially also interfere with the functioning of the inner scribe component of this system. As such, production of physical movement, or even just the planned production of movement, could potentially be harmed by incivility. Our results show that this may indeed be the case and that incivility effects on working memory can slow down physical movements. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 44 General Discussion In a series of five studies we improved our understanding of why incivility reduces performance, and showed how incivility disrupts cognitive functioning. We theorized and confirmed that this effect occurs through working memory. Digging deeper, we tested where and how this occurs, isolating incivility’s effects on the various components of working memory. We found that incivility interferes with the attentional system of the central executive, and in turn, with many tasks that require coordination between the central executive and the other two sub-systems. Across our studies we found that incivility disrupts the functioning of all three components of working memory—the phonological loop system, the visuo-spatial sketchpad, and the central executive system. None of the components are immune; incivility appears to rob resources from each of them. Perhaps this explains the rather potent effects we found across studies. Our results in Studies 1 and 2 (in which we used the dual-task method) suggest that witnessing incivility affects the functioning of the central executive and its interaction with the phonological loop and the memory maintenance function of working memory. Study 2 shows how this tends to reduce performance. Study 3 elaborates on our understanding of the impact of incivility by showing that our results hold for those simply primed with incivility. Incivility tends to affect people automatically and seems to prompt a shift in focus of attention, which occurs during primary appraisal. Witnessing incivility depletes additional cognitive resources as people reappraise the situation, weigh coping strategies (secondary appraisal), and may battle the intrusion of negative thoughts (rumination) through controlled processes (i.e. secondary appraisal). This occupation of the central executive’s attention system leaves fewer resources available for other central executive functions. Studies 4 and 5 provide information about which INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 45 particular working memory systems and functions incivility affects. Our findings from Study 4 demonstrate that witnessing incivility produces selective attention. People are more apt to miss visual information that is right in front of them. Our findings in Study 5 show how incivility seems to hijack the central executive’s ability to manage goals. The ability to remain focused on a task is essential to any coherent cognitive function (Lavie, 2005). It is not surprising then that many theories of human cognition attempt to explain how cognitive systems function in the face of interference from distractors. Indeed, the notion of interference dominated theories of forgetting in cognitive psychology for decades (Ashcraft & Radvansky, 2010). For example, theories of working memory hypothesize that “cluttering” one cognitive system (i.e., central executive) with information can interfere with the functioning of other systems (i.e., phonological loop, visuo-special sketchpad). To test the effects of interfering information on cognitive functions such as memory maintenance, cognitive scientists usually employ distractors that are cognitive in nature (i.e., letters, words, math problems). Our study adds to this body of knowledge by showing that other forms of information that are more emotional in nature (i.e., social threat) may have the same “cluttering” or interfering effects on working memory. Similarly, theories of selective attention show that “cluttering” the cognitive system with perceptual load interferes with perceptual awareness. In a typical study participants that are occupied with one perceptual task (i.e., counting basketball passes) fail to notice task-irrelevant objects (i.e., a gorilla) even if this object is in the center of their visual field. Here again our Study 4 results show that incivility, even if it is just primed, may have similar effects as perceptual load. That is, incivility not only affects memory maintenance but also interferes with perception by blocking information from entering into the cognitive system. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 46 Researchers of cognitive complexity also utilize the notion of interference and argue that one of the reasons that complexity is especially taxing is because in human memory old items do not decay instantaneously; they clutter the cognitive system making it difficult for the cognitive system to find critical items (Altman & Trafton, 2002). In order to reduce the clutter or the interference effect, goals and sub-goals that are not essential to directly performing the task should be kept at a lower level of activation. Thus, as long as the goal is above a certain level of activation it directs behavior; below this level of activation it is treated as a distractor and ignored (Altman & Trafton, 2002). However, in order to complete tasks the cognitive system has to pop back to these dormant goals and reactivate them (Altman & Trafton, 2002). As such, both the attention control system which regulates the schedule and order of the goals and sub-goals and the memory maintenance system which has to reactivate goals have to be engaged in complex tasks. When the working memory system is disrupted these two functions are hampered and so is goal management. Our Study 5 results show that incivility interferes with the functioning of this goal management system. This suggests that emotional events may have a major “cluttering” effect in the management of complex tasks. The extent psychological literature assumes that the main mechanism by which emotional events such as incivility tax the cognitive system is through deliberate regulation (Beal, Weiss, Barros, & MacDermid, 2005; Goldberg & Grandey, 2007; Grandey, 2003; Gross, 2002, 2013; Richards, 2004). However, our results suggest that the effects of incivility may be more immediate and direct. At least initially, individuals may not even have the ability to regulate their experience. In particular, Study 3 showed that simply being primed with incivility took a toll on cognitive processing. These results are disconcerting because they imply that people may be unaware of the toll of just being around incivility and may have limited ability to regulate their INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 47 reaction. These results raise questions and concern about how much control people have over the negative cognitive effects of incivility. At the same time our findings may also have some important implications for the literature on self-regulation. The idea that regulatory efforts deplete resources is now well accepted in the psychological literature (Muraven & Baumeister, 2000). For example, trying hard to focus on work, suppressing the urge to laugh, or staying on a diet plan all consumed regulatory resources and interfered with later attiontional focus on tasks (Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Muraven, & Tice, 1998). In fact, Baumeister and his colleagues provided strong evidence to support the idea that various forms of self-regulation all draw on the same resource (e.g., Baumeister et al., 1998). Interestingly, this vast literature also found that while selfregulatory efforts seem to effect task effort and persistence, they do not influence the cognitive aspects of tasks (Baumeister, Twenge, & Nuss, 2002; Schmeichel, Vohs, & Baumeister, 2003). However, unlike other forms of regulation, emotional regulation does seem to deplete pure cognitive resources. For example, Richards and Gross (2000) found that emotional regulation has negative effects on memory. Our results may help explain why emotional regulation may affect pure cognitions such as cue-recognition recall. While our studies did not test emotional regulation per se, in our studies we found that when individuals deal with negative stimuli it affected their working memory and specifically the central executive function in working memory. These findings lend indirect support to Richards and Gross (2000) findings that when people are trying to interpret and control negative stimuli and their own reactions to these stimuli it may affect the very core of cognition. Indeed, there is some evidence to suggest that emotional regulation requires the brain executive function and, as such, it should deplete cognitive resources for other tasks. For example, in an fMRI study, Ochsner, Bunge, Gross, and INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 48 Gabrieli (2002) found that emotional regulation involved activation of the lateral and medial prefrontal cortex that are essential for working memory (Miller & Cohen, 2001; Smith & Jonides, 1999). If people who encounter incivility are engaged in emotional regulation, than our results add to this body of research by showing that emotional regulation directly affects working memory -- the very workbench of the cognitive system. Future research would benefit from testing more directly the effects of emotional regulation on working memory functioning. Limitations Perhaps the most notable limitation of our study is that we did not investigate why incivility created cognitive disruption in working memory. In any given study researchers have to balance comprehensiveness with parsimony, therefore, given that first we had to establish that incivility created disruption in multiple cognitive functions of working memory we could not address this question. Nonetheless, we believe there are several possible explanations for why incivility creates cognitive disruption. Perhaps the most obvious explanation is that incivility presents a social threat that activates very basic emotional processes that interfere with higher level processes. There is now strong evidence from neuroscience research that suggests that the amygdala, located deep in the limbic system of the brain, is activated in the presence of even minor threats (Damasio, 1994). When activated, the amygdala shifts attentional resources from higher processes to a more primitive flight or fight response (Purves et al., 2004). Incivility may disrupt cognition by automatically activating the primitive brain systems that communicates that rather than the cognitive task at hand, the priority is “self-protection”. Another possibility, however, is that incivility may present people with an informational challenge. It may not be clear to the victim or even a bystander what the uncivil person “really” wants, why he or she is being rude, and how to respond. Instead of concentrating on the task at INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 49 hand, the victim or the observer is likely to be focused by trying to understand what the source of the problem is and how to address it. As a result, incivility increases cognitive load and makes the task at hand more cognitively complex. In other words, it is possible that it is not only the emotional challenge that creates the cognitive distraction but also the informational challenge that is presented by the uncivil person. Future research should benefit from addressing the question of why incivility affects working memory, is this because of primary appraisal processes, secondary appraisal processes, or both? Implications and Future Research Our findings demonstrate that individuals pay a price for incivility—perhaps without knowing they are vulnerable. This is troubling, particularly in some industries where the stakes of attention and cognitive performance are critical. For example, in a recent study most of the 800 physician executives surveyed said that disruptive behavior happens in their hospitals at least once a month (Rosenstein & O’Daniel, 2008). Ten percent called it a daily occurrence and almost all believed that this bad behavior negatively affected their patients’ care. Nearly onefourth said that it led to actual harm to their patients. The news from doctors and nurses actually treating patients is even more frightening. Nearly three out of four identified bad behaviors within their teams that led to medical errors; and more than one-fourth were convinced that these behaviors contributed to the deaths of their own patients. Our findings may help explain why health care mistakes occur and patient care may suffer despite well-intentioned efforts. It’s clear that incivility impairs individual’s ability to think, even when individuals are not the target. Witnessing incivility—or even being around it-- triggers reactions in the brain, making it tougher to manage tasks and solve problems. Incivility increases the likelihood that people miss important information. In the case of medical teams, we’ve been told that people may miss INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 50 important information on the patient’s chart. Researchers and doctors have highlighted that a climate of trust and respect, or psychological safety, benefits learning (Edmondson, Bohmer, & Pisano, 2001; Gawande, 2002). Our study may offer some specifics about why this is the case. The implications of this study span far beyond health care though. Employees’ whose work demands focus, problem-solving, decision-making, creativity, or cognitive performance should be wary of incivility’s impact. Beyond the workplace, our findings hold important implications for school settings. Students who are simply around an uncivil setting (e.g., classroom behavior, playground antics) may suffer negative cognitive effects and decreased performance. We suspect that uncivil environments will reduce students’ ability to be attentive, learn, and perform well. We encourage research that tests the longer term effects; as such an environment may impact student learning, achievement, and career success. We also encourage research that studies how incivility encountered in one setting may carry over to negatively impact an individual or those they interact with in another setting. For example, people that encounter incivility in organizational settings may bring home the effects, with negative effects on their personal, work, or other roles. Recent research (Lim & Tai, in press) has shown how incivility encountered with one’s family negatively affects work performance. Perhaps these relationships flow other ways, too. This prompts another interesting avenue for future research. How long do these negative effects of incivility last? Our studies focused on short-terms effects. However, research suggests that the effects of excitation—even from small infractions-- can remain in the body for long periods of time (Zillman, 1988). Moreover, the effects of arousal from a previous small infraction (such as witnessing incivility) can be combined with arousal of another situation to intensify the emotional reaction (Fiske & Taylor, 1991). Research that uncovers more about how INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 51 the effects of incivility may endure and accumulate could potentially be theoretically and practically useful. Our results highlight the importance of being mindful of the cognitive toll of incivility. For some, simply being aware of how distracting incivility can be is helpful. Ideally people steer clear of incivility whenever possible, limiting their involvement with uncivil colleagues and organizations. When around incivility, one should attempt to re-focus attention mindfully as best as possible. Mindfulness, meditation, and other related practices may be helpful in improving individual functioning (Hunter, 2013; Tan, 2012)—particularly when people are dealing with emotions and stress stemming from incivility. Unfortunately though, our results suggest that people may not realize that working in this environment may limits their potential and harm their career. They may be unaware that incivility is silently chipping away at their well-being, productivity, and creativity. Research reveals that ordinary daily hassles considerably outstrip major life stressors and their impact on emotional, social, and work functioning (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Incivility may be one such hassle; yet may not be consciously acknowledged. We encourage people to be more mindful of uncivil interactions and uncivil environments. While many recommendations for managing incivility are aimed at the target (i.e. cope with incivility, taking care of self, addressing the offender), our results shift the focus and onus to managers and organizations. Specifically, our results highlight the far-reaching potent effects. Managers can limit incivility through recruiting and selection with an eye for civility, setting expectations, and training. When incivility occurs, they cannot tolerate it. Managers must deal with it swiftly in order to limit the negative consequences. We hope our results raise awareness about the costs of incivility—the cognitive toll it takes on people, sneakily robbing them of resources, disrupting working memory, and ultimately hijacking performance. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 52 References Altmann, E.M., & Trafton, J.G. (2002). Memory for goals: An activation-based model. Cognitive Science, 26(1): 39-83. Andersson, L.M., & Pearson, C.M. (1999). Tit for tat? The spiraling effect of incivility in the workplace. Academy of Management Review, 24(3), 452-471. Ashcraft, M.H., & Radvansky, G.A. (2010). Cognition (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Baddeley, A.D., & Hitch, G.J. (1975). Working memory. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 8, 47-89. Baddeley, A.D. (1990). Human memory: Theory and practice. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Baddeley, A.D. (1992) Working memory. Science, 255, 445-559. Baddeley, A.D. (1993). Working memory or working attention? In A. Baddeley & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.), Attention: Selection, awareness, and control (pp. 152-170). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Baddeley, A.D. (1996). Exploring the central executive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49(1), 5-28. Baddeley, A.D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417-423. Baddeley, A.D. (2002). Is working memory still working? European Psychologist, 7(2), 85-97. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 53 Bargh, J.A., & Chartrand, T.L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54(7), 462-479. Bargh, J.A., & Thein, R.D. (1985). Individual construct accessibility, person memory, and the recall-judgment link: The case of information overload. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(5), 1129-1146. Bargh, J.A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. Unintended Thought, 3, 51-69. Bargh, J.A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 230-244. Baumeister, R.F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D.M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1252-1265. Baumeister, R.F., Twenge, J.M., & Nuss, C.K. (2002). Effects of social exclusion on cognitive processes: Anticipated aloneness reduces intelligent thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(4), 817-827. Beal, D.J., Weiss, H.M., Barros, E., & MacDermid, S.M. (2005). An episodic process model of affective influence on performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(6), 1054-1068. Ben-Zur, H., & Yagil, D. (2005). The relationship between empowerment, aggressive behaviours of customers, coping, and burnout. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 14(1), 81-99. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 54 Ben-Zur, H., & Yagil, D. (2005). The relationship between empowerment, aggressive behaviours of customers, coping, and burnout. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 14(1), 81-99. Carlston, D. E., & Skowronski, J. J. (1994). Savings in the relearning of trait information as evidence for spontaneous inference generation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(5), 840-856. Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., & Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological Review, 97(3), 404-431. Cartwright-Finch, U., & Lavie, N. (2007). The role of perceptual load in inattentional blindness. Cognition, 102(3), 321-340. Carver, C. S., Ganellen, R. J., Froming, W. J., & Chambers, W. (1983). Modeling: An analysis in terms of category accessibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19(5), 403421. Chabris, C. F., & Simons, D. J. (2011). The invisible gorilla: And other ways our intuitions deceive us. New York: Crown. Clore, G. L. (1994). Why emotions are felt. In P. Ekman & R.J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 103-111). New York: Oxford University Press. Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82(6), 407-428. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 55 Colquitt, J. A. (2001). On the dimensionality of organizational justice: a construct validation of a measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(3), 386-400. Cortina, L. M., Magley, V. J., Williams, J. H., & Langhout, R. D. (2001). Incivility in the workplace: Incidence and impact. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 6(1), 6480. Craik, F. I., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11(6), 671-684. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Literacy and intrinsic motivation. Daedalus,119(2), 115-140. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding flow: The psychology of engagement with everyday life. New York: Basic Books. Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. New York: Aldine de Gruyter. Damasio, A.R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam. Deery, S., Iverson, R., & Walsh, J. (2002). Work relationships in telephone call centres: Understanding emotional exhaustion and employee withdrawal. Journal of Management Studies, 39(4), 471-496. de Fockert, J. W., Rees, G., Frith, C. D., & Lavie, N. (2001). The role of working memory in visual selective attention. Science, 291, 1803-1806. Dimberg, U., & Öhman, A. (1996). Behold the wrath: Psychophysiological responses to facial stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 20(2), 149-182. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 56 Dormann, C., & Zapf, D. (2004). Customer-related social stressors and burnout. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 9(1), 61-82. Durkheim, E. (1964). The division of labor in society. (G. Simpson, Trans.) Toronto: CollierMacMillan. (Original work published 1858) Edmondson, A. C., Bohmer, R. M., & Pisano, G. P. (2001). Disrupted routines: Team learning and new technology implementation in hospitals. Administrative Science Quarterly, 46(4), 685-716. Erez, A., & Isen, A. M. (2002). The influence of positive affect on the components of expectancy motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(6), 1055-1067. Ernst, G. W., & Newell, A. (1969). GPS: A case study in generality and problem solving. New York: Academic Press. Eysenck, M.W., & Keane, M.T. (2003). Cognitive psychology. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press. Feng, J., Pratt, J., & Spence, I. (2012). Attention and visuospatial working memory share the same processing resources. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 103. Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw Hill. Frick, J. W., Guilford, J. P., Christensen, P. R., & Merrifield, P. R. (1959). A factor-analytic study of flexibility in thinking. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 19, 469495. Gawande, A. (2002). The Learning Curve. The New Yorker, 77(45), 52-61. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 57 Gentner, D. (1983). Structure-mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science, 7(2), 155-170. Goel, V., & Grafman, J. (1995). Are the frontal lobes implicated in “planning” functions? Interpreting data from the Tower of Hanoi. Neuropsychologia, 33(5), 623-642. Goldberg, L S., & Grandey, A. A. (2007). Display rules versus display autonomy: Emotion regulation, emotional exhaustion, and task performance in a call center simulation. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12, 301-318. Grandey, A. A. (2003). When “the show must go on”: Surface acting and deep acting as determinants of emotional exhaustion and peer-rated service delivery. Academy of Management Journal, 46(1), 86-96. Grandey, A. A., Dickter, D. N., & Sin, H. P. (2004). The customer is not always right: Customer aggression and emotion regulation of service employees. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25(3), 397-418. Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology, 39(3), 281-291. Gross, J. J. (2013). Emotion regulation: Taking stock and moving forward. Emotion, 13(3), 359365. Guerin, B. (1999). Social behaviors as determined by different arrangements of social consequences: Social loafing, social facilitation, deindividuation, and a modified social loafing. The Psychological Record, 49(4), 565-578. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 58 Guilford, J. P. (1975). Varieties of creative giftedness, their measurement and development. Gifted Child Quarterly, 19(2), 107-121. Hallowell, E.M. (1997). Worry. New York: The Random House Publishing Group. Harkins, S. G. (1987). Social loafing and social facilitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 23(1), 1-18. Harris, L. C., & Reynolds, K. L. (2003). The consequences of dysfunctional customer behavior. Journal of Service Research, 6(2), 144-161. Higgins, E. T. (1989). Knowledge accessibility and activation: Subjectivity and suffering from unconscious sources. In J.S. Uleman & J.A. Bargh (Eds), Unintended thoughts (p. 75123). New York: Guilford Press. Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 133-168). New York: Guilford Press. Higgins, E. T., Bargh, J. A., & Lombardi, W. J. (1985). Nature of priming effects on categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11(1), 59-69. Hunter, J. (2013). Is mindfulness good for business? Mindful, April, 53-59. Isen, A. M., & Erez, A. (2007). Some measurement issues in the study of affect. In A.D. Ong & M.H.M. van Dulmen (Eds.), Oxford handbook of methods in positive psychology (pp. 250-265). New York: Oxford University Press. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 59 Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., & Jessell, T. M. (Eds.). (2000). Principles of neural science (4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(1), 47-70. Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (1989). Motivation and cognitive abilities: An integrative/aptitude-treatment interaction approach to skill acquisition. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74(4), 657-690. Keane, M .T. (1988). Analogical problem solving. Chichester, UK: Ellis Horwood. Kenrick, D. T., Li, N. P., & Butner, J. (2003). Dynamical evolutionary psychology: individual decision rules and emergent social norms. Psychological Review, 110(1), 3-28. Kern, J. H., & Grandey, A. A. (2009). Customer incivility as a social stressor: The role of race and racial identity for service employees. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 14(1), 46-57. Koivisto, M., & Revonsuo, A. (2009). The effects of perceptual load on semantic processing under inattention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(5), 864-868. Kollock, P. (1998). Social dilemmas: The anatomy of cooperation. Annual Review of Sociology, 24, 183-214. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 60 Latorella, K. A. (1996). Investigating interruptions: An example from the flightdeck. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 40(4), 249253. Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9(2), 75-82. Lazarus, R.S. & Folkman, S.( 1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Cognition and motivation in emotion. American Psychologist, 46(4), 352367. LeDoux, J. (1998). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon and Schuster. Lim, S., Cortina, L.M., & Magley, V.J. (2008). Personal and workgroup incivility: Impact on work and health outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(1): 95-107. Lim, S., & Tai, K. (In press). Family incivility and job performance: A moderated mediation model of psychological distress and core self-evaluation. Journal of Applied Psychology. Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuo-spatial working memory. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd. Lombardi, W. J., Higgins, E. T., & Bargh, J. A. (1987). The role of consciousness in priming effects on categorization assimilation versus contrast as a function of awareness of the priming task. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13(3), 411-429. Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 61 Miller, D. T. (2001). Disrespect and the experience of injustice. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 527–553. Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 67–202. Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & P ibram, . H. (1960).Plans and the structure of behavior. New York, NY: Henry Holt and Co, Inc. Miner-Rubino, K., & Cortina, L.M. (2004). Working in a context of hostility toward women: Implications for employees’ well-being. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 9(2): 107-122. Morey, C. C., & Cowan, N. (2004). When visual and verbal memories compete: Evidence of cross-domain limits in working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(2), 296301. Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 247–259. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998-2011). Mplus User's Guide (6th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving (Vol. 14). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 62 self-regulation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 4, pp. 1–18). New York: Plenum Press. Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Rethinking feelings: An fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 1215-1229. Pearson, D. G. (2001). Imagery and the visuo-spatial sketchpad. In J. Andrade (Ed.), Working memory in perspective (pp. 33-59). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Pearson, C. M., & Porath, C. L. (2001). Effects of incivility on the target: Fight, flee or take care of “me”. In annual meeting of the Academy of Management, Washington, DC. Pearson, C. M., & Porath, C. L. (2009). The cost of bad behavior: how incivility is damaging your business and what to do about it. New York: Portfolio. Pearson, C. M., Andersson, L. M., & Porath, C. L. (2000). Assessing and attacking workplace incivility. Organizational Dynamics, 29(2), 123-137. Porath, C. L., & Erez, A. (2007). Does rudeness really matter? The effects of rudeness on task performance and helpfulness. Academy of Management Journal, 50(5), 1181-1197. Porath, C. L., & Erez, A. (2009). Overlooked but not untouched: How rudeness reduces onlookers’ performance on routine and creative tasks. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109(1), 29-44. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 63 Porath, C.L., MacInnis, D., & Folkes, V. (2011). It’s Unfair: Why customers who merely observe an uncivil employee abandon the company. Journal of Services Research, 14(3): 302-371. Porath, C., Macinnis, D., & Folkes, V. (2010). Witnessing incivility among employees: Effects on consumer anger and negative inferences about companies. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(2), 292-303. Porath, C.L., Overbeck, J., Pearson, C.M. (2008). Picking up the gauntlet: How individuals respond to status challenges. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38(7): 1945-1980. Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Facilitation and inhibition in the processing of signals. In P.M.A. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (Eds.), Attention and performance V (pp. 669-682). New York: Academic Press. Purves, D., Augustine, G.J., Fitzpatrick, D., Hall, W.C., LaMantia, A.S, McNamara, J.O., &Williams, S.M. (2004). Neuroscience (3rd edition). Sutherland, MA: Sinauer. Rafaeli, A., Erez, A., Ravid, S., Derfler-Rozin, R., Treister, D. E., & Scheyer, R. (2012). When customers exhibit verbal aggression, employees pay cognitive costs. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97(5), 931-950. Raudenbush A., Bryk A., & Congdon R.(2009). HLM (Version 6.08) [Computer software]. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International. Richards, J. M. (2004). The cognitive consequences of concealing feelings. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(4), 131-134. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 64 Richards, J., M., & Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: The cognitive costs of keeping one’s cool. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(3), 410-424. Ringstad, R. (2005). Conflict in the workplace: Social workers as victims and perpetrators. Social Work, 50(4), 305-313. Rosenstein, A. H., & O'Daniel, M. (2008). A survey of the impact of disruptive behaviors and communication defects on patient safety. Joint Commission Journal on Quality and Patient Safety, 34(8), 464-471. Schmeichel, B. J., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Intellectual performance and ego depletion: Role of the self in logical reasoning and other information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(1), 33–46. Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84(2), 127-190. Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception-London, 28(9), 1059-1074. Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 813-838. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 65 Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1997). Working memory: A view from neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychology, 33(1), 5-42. Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283, 1657-1661. Smith, C.A., & Kirby, L.D. (2001). Toward delivering on the promise of appraisal theory. In K.R. Scherer, A. Schorr & T. Johnston (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, Methods, Research (pp.121-138). New York: Oxford University Press. Strauman, T. J., & Higgins, E. T. (1987). Automatic activation of self-discrepancies and emotional syndromes: When cognitive structures influence affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(6), 1004-1014. Tan, C. (2012). Search Inside Yourself: The unexpected path to achieving success, happiness (and world peace). New York: Harper Collins. Uleman, J. S., Newman, L. S., & Moskowitz, G. B. (1996). People as flexible interpreters: Evidence and issues from spontaneous trait inference. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 211-279. Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2005). An automated version of the operation span task. Behavior Research Methods,37(3), 498-505. Vidmar, N. (2000). Retribution and revenge. In J. Sanders & V. L. Hamilton (Eds.), Handbook of justice research in law (pp. 31–63). New York: Kluwer Academic. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 66 Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 163-206. Weiss, H.M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A theoretical discussion of the structure, causes and consequences of affective experiences at work. Research in Organizational Behavior, 18, 1-74. Welsh, M., Cicerello, A., Cuneo, K., & Brennan, M. (1995). Error and temporal patterns in Tower of Hanoi performance: Cognitive mechanisms and individual differences. The Journal of General Psychology, 122(1), 69-81. Winter, L., & Uleman, J. S. (1984). When are social judgments made? Evidence for the spontaneousness of trait inferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(2), 237. Wyer Jr, R. S., & Srull, T. K. (1989). Memory and cognition in its social context. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Zillmann, D. (1988). Cognition-excitation interdependencies in aggressive behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 14(1), 51-64. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 67 Table 1. Hypotheses Tested in the Five Studies Study Process Controlled 1 2 3 4 5 Incivility will negatively influence working memory performance Working memory will mediate the relationships between incivility and performance on complex tasks and creativity Priming incivility will negatively influence working memory, complex task performance, and creativity Systems Engaged Automatic Functions Affected Central Executive Phonological Loop VisuoSpatial Sketchpad Memory Maintenance X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X Attention control Priming Incivility will disrupt visual attention Priming incivility will interfere with goal-directed information management in working memory X X X INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 68 Table 2 Means (M), Standard Deviations (SD), and Intercorrelations Among Study 2 Variables. M SD 1 1. Number of anagrams solved 5.50 2.02 --- 2. Number of brick ideas 9.28 3.49 .40 --- 3. Rated creativity for the brick uses 4.02 1.57 .38 .45 --- 48.19 15.10 .45 .20 .38 --- 4. Working Memory 5. Math Errors 6. Incivility 2 3 4 7.19 6.07 -.16 -.05 -.20 -.24 .48 .50 -.30 -.34 -.32 -.27 5 6 --.03 --- Notes: N=54. Correlations above .28 are significant at the p < .05 level. Correlations greater than .37 are significant at the p < .01 level. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 69 Table 3 Influence of Witnessing Incivility on Task Performance, Creativity, and Working Memory in Study 2. Control 1. Number of anagrams solved 2. Number of brick ideas 3. Rated creativity for the brick uses 4. Working Memory Incivility Mean SD Mean SD F 6.07 2.02 4.88 1.89 4.97* 10.39 3.92 8.08 2.51 6.57* 4.42 1.30 3.60 1.11 6.10* 52.04 14.80 44.04 14.58 3.99* Notes: N= 54 (N=28 neutral condition, N=26 witnessing incivility condition); *p < .05. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 70 Table 4 Mediation Effects of Working Memory on the Relationship Between Witnessing Incivility and Performance, Study 2. Anagrams Task b Incivility to Working Memory Working Memory to DVs Direct effect of witnessing incivility to DVs controlling for working memory Indirect effect of witnessing incivility to DV Through working memory -8.00* .05** -.76 s.e Brick Task Creativity b s.e. b s.e. 4.03 -8.00* 4.03 -8.00* 4.03 .02 .03 .03 .03* .01 .51 -2.09* .94 -.43 (-1.10, -.03) -.23 (-1.04, .121) -.60 .34 -.21 (-.62, -.01) Notes: **p< .01, *p < .05. The coefficients are unstandardized and are based on bootstrapping 2000 re-sampling. 95% Confidence intervals for indirect effect are in parenthesis. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 71 Table 5 Means (M), Standard Deviations (SD), and Intercorrelations Among Study 3 Variables. M SD 1 1. Number of anagrams solved 4.21 2.31 --- 2. Number of brick ideas 7.25 3.63 .32 --- 3. Rated creativity for the brick uses 3.23 1.68 .20 .55 --- 48.25 14.48 .37 .35 .35 --- 4. Working Memory 5. Math Errors 6. Incivility 2 3 4 5.39 3.14 -.16 -.27 -.15 -.40 .49 .50 -.55 -.32 -.28 -.26 5 6 --.31 --- Notes: N=60. Correlations above .26 are significant at the p < .05 level. Correlations greater than .34 are significant at the p < .01 level. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 72 Table 6 Influence of priming Incivility on Task Performance, Creativity, and Working Memory in Study 3. Control Incivility Mean SD Mean SD F 1. Number of anagrams solved 5.45 2.78 2.93 1.53 25.51** 2. Number of brick ideas 8.39 3.58 6.07 3.34 6.82* 3. Rated creativity for the brick uses 3.68 1.64 2.76 1.62 4.74* 51.87 12.74 44.50 15.40 4.16* 4. Working Memory Notes: N= 60 (N=31 neutral condition, N=29 witnessing incivility condition); *p < .05, **p < .01. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 73 Table 7 Mediation Effects of Working Memory on the Relationship Between Priming Incivility and Performance, Study 3. Anagrams Task b Incivility to Working Memory Working Memory to DVs Direct effect of priming incivility to DVs controlling for working memory Indirect effect of priming incivility to DV Through working memory s.e -7.37* 3.46 .04* .02 -2.23** .48 -.29 (-.86, -.02) Brick Task b Creativity s.e. -7.37* 3.46 .07* -1.80 .03 .93 -.52 (-1.58, -.02) b s.e. -7.37* 3.46 .04* -.68 .01 .41 -.27 (-.71, -.03) Notes: **p< .01, *p < .05. The coefficients are based on bootstrapping 2000 re-sampling. Confidence intervals for indirect effect are in parenthesis. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 74 Table 8. Incivility Effects on Goal Management in the Tower of Hanoi Task, Study 5 Decision Making Time Physical Move Time Number of Errors Step 1 Effects on Intercept β0 1. Intercept 00 2. Priming Incivility 01 2.86** .34* 2.32** .26* 1.70** .22* 2.04** 0.09 1.93** .14* -2.60** .44* 3.05** .96* 1.47** .46* 2.13** -0.36 Step 2 Effects on Intercept β0 1. Intercept 00 2. Priming Incivility 01 Effects on Slope β1 1. Intercept 10 2. Priming incivility 11 Note: *p < .05 (one-tailed), **p < .01. β0 represents first-level intercept coefficient; β1 represents relationship between an indicator of difficult move and the DVs. ’s represent second level coefficients.. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY Figure 1. Tower of Hanoi Task 75 INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 76 Figure 2. Moderating effect of goal difficulty on the relationship between incivility and decision making time on the Tower of Hanoi task. INCIVILITY AND WORKING MEMORY 77 Figure 3. Moderating effect of goal difficulty on the relationship between incivility and physical move time in the Tower of Hanoi task.