Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1986, Vol. 51, No. 3,578-585 Copyright 1986 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/86/$S0.75 Role Quality, Multiple Role Involvement, and Psychological Well-Being in Midlife Women Grace K. Baruch and Rosalind Barnett Brandeis University Women's occupancy of the social roles of paid worker, wife, and mother, and the quality of their experience in these three roles, were examined in relation to psychological well-being. Data were from a disproportionate random sample (N — 238) of Caucasian women ages 35 to 55. Well-being was measured by indices of self-esteem, depression, and pleasure; pleasure was assessed by a scale consisting of single-item measures of happiness, satisfaction, and optimism. Role quality was measured by scales developed for this study that assessed the balance between the positive and negative attributes women perceived in their roles. Hierarchical regression analyses controlling for age, education, and income indicated that role occupancy per se was unrelated to well-being with one exception: occupying the role of paid worker significantly predicted self-esteem. In contrast, the three role quality variables were significant predictors of the well-being indices, with one exception: quality of experience in the role of mother did not predict pleasure. Overall, the findings suggest the importance of qualitative rather than quantitative aspects of role involvement and the need to examine different dimensions of well-being in relation to social roles. involvement in several roles is seen as yielding a variety of In a review of the literature on women's social roles, Long sources of stimulation, gratification, and social validation. and Porter (1984) point to the existence of a kind of sex segregation in research and theory on multiple roles. For men, multiple Recent empirical evidence and theory are converging to sug- roles have been seen as beneficial; Gove and Tudor (1973), for gest that for women as well as men, involvement in a multiplic- example, attributed the better mental health of men compared ity of roles yields a net gain of benefits over costs with respect to women to their participation in both family and paid work to both physical and mental health. These studies tend to sup- roles. The topic of "dual roles," in contrast, has been seen as a port the enhancement hypothesis, because findings for both woman's issue; for women, the role of paid worker is assumed women and men typically show a positive relation between the to be added on to the normative condition of being a wife, number of roles a person occupies and various indices of psy- mother, and homemaker. Role overload, role conflict, guilt, anx- chological well-being (Crosby, 1983; Epstein, 1983; Pietromo- iety, and other hazards are expected to follow, resulting in im- noco, Manis, & Frohardt-Lane, 1984; Thoits, 1983; Verbrugge, paired well-being. 1982). For example, Pietromonoco et al. (1984) report that the Two major hypotheses have been put forward concerning the more roles a woman occupies, the more different areas of her relation of role involvement to well-being. The "scarcity" hy- life she reports to be sources of pleasure. pothesis (Marks, 1977), put forth by Goode (1960) and others, Both the enhancement and scarcity hypotheses are limited, assumes that the social structure normally creates overly de- however, by their focus on the number of roles occupied; both manding role obligations, the more so the more roles one occu- fail to examine how the nature of a particular role might con- pies. Because human energy is limited, well-being is impaired tribute to or impair well-being. The expansion hypothesis, for by the overload and conflict inherent in numerous, often incom- example, assumes a net gain of benefits over costs regardless patible roles. In contrast to this view, the "enhancement" hy- of which roles a person occupies. It may be, however, that the pothesis (Marks, 1977; Sieber, 1974) emphasizes the benefits particular roles occupied, and the quality of experience in each rather than the costs of multiple role involvement: status, privi- role, affect level of well-being more than the mere number of leges, increased self-esteem, the ability to trade off undesirable roles. The privileges and obligations, the rewards and concerns, components of roles. Like placing one's eggs in many baskets, and the cost/benefit balance for a woman who occupies the two roles of wife and mother may differ from those for a woman who occupies the two roles of wife and paid worker. Moreover, two women may occupy similar roles yet experience the quality of The research reported here was supported by National Science Foundation Grant BNS 77-26756. The authors extend their appreciation to Erin Phelps and Nancy Marshall for their assistance in data analysis and to Nathalie Dana Thompson for her assistance in manuscript preparation. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Grace K. Baruch, who is now at the Wellesley College Center for Research on Women, Wellesley, Massachusetts 02181. each role differently. Another issue in examining the relation of well-being to role involvement is that the way well-being is conceptualized and operationalized affects the findings. Psychological well-being is not a unitary construct (Andrews & Withey, 1974; Bryant & Veroff, 1982; Campbell, Converse, & Rodgers, 1976), and studies of social roles and well-being have focused on varying as- 578 ROLE QUALITY IN WOMEN pects, such as self-esteem, happiness, satisfaction, or symptomatology, both singly and in combination. As the following brief review indicates, a clearer understanding of the relation of role involvement to well-being requires examining specific roles and their quality, in relation to specific aspects of well-being. Paid Worker Evidence is growing that employment typically has positive psychological consequences for women, either as a primary source of well-being or as a buffer against stress experienced in other roles (Barnett& Baruch, 1985; Baruch, Barnett, & Rivers, 1985; Belle, 1982; Brown & Harris, 1978; Coleman & Antonucci, 1982; Crosby, 1983; Epstein, 1983; Kessler & McRae, 1981;Merikangas, 1985; Veroff, Douvan, &Kulka, 1981). Correlational studies such as these of course raise the question of the impact of selection: Are women selected into employment because of their high level of well-being? If so, well-being has "caused" them to be employed rather than vice versa. In a careful analysis of this question, Vebrugge (in press) has concluded that selection factors account for only a small proportion of the findings. Involvement in the role of paid worker, even if it requires that a woman must juggle many roles, appears to provide self-esteem and a sense of efficacy, alleviating effects of stressors. For example, a classic study of English women showed that for those in stressful life circumstances who did not have confidantes, being employed seemed to protect against the occurrence of psychiatric symptoms; symptoms developed in 79% of the nonemployed women but only 14% of the employed (Brown & Harris, 1978). More recently, Merikangas (1985) reported, based on data from a large-scale cachement area study of the incidence of psychiatric problems, that lack of employment is a risk factor for depression in women. Indeed, Long and Porter (1984) suggest that for women, the benefits of multiple role involvement are in fact due primarily to the paid worker role. These benefits, the studies just cited suggest, have a positive impact on well-being primarily with respect to self-esteem, and to a lesser extent, psychiatric symptomology. When happiness or satisfaction are used as indices of well-being, employed and nonemployed women typically do not differ significantly (Campbell et al., 1976; Serlin, 1980). These findings, however, refer to the effects of employment per se and do not take into account qualitative aspects of the paid worker role. Wife The heated debate about whether marriage is a "health hazard" for women reflects conflicting evidence about the relation of marital status to well-being (Aneshensel, Frerichs, & Clark, 1981; Bernard, 1972; Campbell etal., 1976; Glenn, 1975). One of the most consistent findings is that married women are happier and more satisfied than are those who are not married, whether the latter are never-married, divorced, or widowed (Baruch et al., 1984; Campbell et al., 1976; Depner, 1979; Glenn, 1975; Veroff etal., 1981; Ward, 1979). A recent review using meta-analytic techniques, however, showed that associations between subjective well-being, for example, happiness and morale, and marital status tend to be weak (Haring-Hidore, Stock, Okin, & Witter, 1985). In contrast, occupying the role of 579 wife typically is not associated with components of well-being other than happiness and satisfaction. Comparing never-married women over 30 with a matched sample of married women, Gigy (1980) found that never-married women were less happy, but their self-esteem was no lower nor did they report more psychiatric symptomatology. Indeed, Cleary and Mechanic (1983) have noted that gender differences in psychiatric symptomatology are greatest among married subjects. Although being married per se may typically enhance feelings of happiness and satisfaction, the effect is weak, and marital status apparently neither boosts women's self-esteem nor ensures against the development of psychiatric symptomatology. When the quality of the marriage is taken into account, however, the impact on well-being is stronger. For example, consistently negative interactions with the husband strongly predicts depression in wives (Brown, 1984.) Mother In contrast to the findings with respect to the roles of paid worker and wife, there is little evidence that being a mother per se enhances any aspect of a woman's well-being (Barnett & Baruch, 1985; Campbell et al., 1976; Depner, 1979; Sears & Barbee, 1977; Veroff etal., 1981). That such a seemingly profound difference in women's lives—that between having children and being childless— should be undetectable in its effects on well-being is counterintuitive and somewhat startling. Yet there is at least one obvious reason all too familiar to parents: children can make one's life miserable as well as wonderful. If one groups together all women who happen to be mothers, those for whom it has worked out well may be balanced by those who have had more suffering than pleasure from their role. In Occupation Housewife, Helena Lopata (1971) argues that being a mother is made especially difficult in our culture by the lack of agreement on standards for the role, combined with the unrealistic expectation that one can mold a child to be whatever one wants. Veroffet a). (1981) attribute the lesser well-being of women compared to men to the difficulties and uncertainties of having primary responsibility for childrearing, and to feeling responsible for how children turn out, despite the inability to control the outcome. According to Spreitzer, Snyder, and Larson (1979) the hypothesis that a good experience in one role compensates for problems in other roles is generally supported, with one exception—for women, problems in the parent role are not compensated for by satisfaction at work. Overall, however, the relation between the quality of a woman's experience in the role of mother and specific dimensions of her psychosocial well-being remains unclear. As this brief review indicates, roles are not interchangeable in their impact. In analyzing the relation of multiple role involvement to well-being, we need to examine not only the quantity but the quality of a woman's experience in specific roles. The data reported here examine the relative contribution of women's occupancy of social roles and the quality of their experience within roles to three dimensions of psychological wellbeing. With respect to well-being, Bryant and Veroff( 1982) have recently presented a three-dimensional model based on factor analyses of data on 18 indices of well-being included in two 580 GRACE K. BARUCH AND ROSALIND BARNETT Table 1 Role Pattern Groups Work status Employed n Not employed n Never married Married without children Married with children Divorced with children 49 40 46 46 15" 42 Note. N = 238. " It was not possible to locate the desired number of married, childless, non-employed women. Economic pressures and ideological changes have apparently made this role pattern very rare. large-scale national surveys. In their model (negative poles are labeled), they distinguish personal inadequacy (poor self-concept), strain (symptomatology), and unhappiness (negative affect). The study reported here also included multiple measures of well-being. Three indices analogous to the Bryant and Veroff dimensions were selected for analysis, self-esteem (Ro- senberg, 1965), depression (Derogatis, Lipman, Rickels, Uhlenhuth, & Covi, 1974), and pleasure (a scale constructed of three standard single-item measures of happiness, satisfaction, and optimism). With respect to role quality, positive (rewarding) and negative (distressing) role attributes were measured so as to assess the difference or balance between the two. The balance between rewarding and distressing aspects of one's roles seemed a key to of women who occupy one (n = 64), two (n = 126), or three (n = 46) of the major roles of wife, mother, paid worker. The first stage of the study consisted of intensive, semistructured interviews lasting 3 to 6 hr with a "snowball" or convenience sample of 12 women from each of the six groups shown in Table 1 (JV = 72). A major goal of these interviews was to identify the rewarding and distressing aspects of roles so as to assess role quality. Based upon content analyses of transcriptions of these taped interviews, a survey instrument was designed for the second stage of the study, in which approximately 45 women in each of the six role pattern groups were selected through disproportionate random sampling, using the voting lists of a community in the greater Boston area. Details of sample recruitment are provided in Baruch et al. (1985). Briefly, census data were used to assess the characteristics of communities in the Boston Standard Metropolitan Sampling Area (SMSA) so as to locate the community most likely to have adequate numbers of employed women and a wide range of occupational prestige levels. Based on that community's voting list, every woman aged 35 to 55 was assigned a random number; subjects were contacted in order of number. Telephone screening was used to assess whether a woman met the sampling criteria. (Although 6,000 women in the community were screened, it was not possible to locate adequate numbers of married childless women who were nonemployed. The changing economic and ideological climate has apparently made this pattern very rare.) Women were interviewed individually in their homes in 1978-1979; the response rate was 76%. The mean age of the sample was 43.6 years. The mean level of education was 14 years, 2 years beyond high school; the mean total family i ncome for the whole sample was $21,600.2 Subgroups varied greatly in income; however, the mean for never-married women was $15,200; for employed married women it was $30,700. In the sample as a whole, 180 of 238 women were employed, 142 were married, and 136 were mothers. Of the mothers, 30 had children under 8, and 89 had children 8 to 18 living at home. the quality of that role in relation to mental health; several researchers have argued, for example, that the balance between positive and negative affect is the best predictor of one's psychological state (Bradburn, 1969;Lowenthal&Chiriboga, 1973).' Method Subjects The sample, described in Table 1, consisted of women who occupied one of four family role patterns: never-married, married without children, married with children, and divorced with children. Half of the married women and all of the never-married and divorced women were employed. Employed women within each family status group were drawn equally from those in high-, medium-, and low-prestige occupations (Siegel, 1971). Nonemployed women were stratified similarly by the prestige level of their husbands' occupation. Women who had been working at least 17 hr a week for at least 3 months prior to being interviewed were classified as employed. Divorced women had been divorced or legally separated for at least a year prior to being interviewed. All subjects were Caucasian. The sample was selected to include (a) groups of theoretical relevance to the major concerns of the study—for example, groups vary in the number and nature of roles—and (b) relatively rare groups whose life patterns are of increasing social importance, such as married childless women and women in high-prestige occupations. The study was designed to sample specific groups rather than to be a probability sample of American women. Employed women in high-prestige occupations, for example, are overrepresented; one purpose of the design was to avoid confounding the effects of being employed with the effects of being employed in a low-prestige occupation. The design yields six subgroups Measures Psychological well-being. Three indices of well-being included in the survey were selected for final analyses, based upon the conceptual and empirical considerations discussed earlier. These were the Rosenberg (1965) Self-Esteem Scale; the depression subscale of the Hopkins Symptom Checklist (Derogatis et al., 1974), and a pleasure scale consisting of three standard single-item measures of happiness, satisfaction, and optimism. The alpha coefficient for the pleasure scale was .76. Role quality. As noted earlier, 72 women (12 per role pattern group) were interviewed in the pilot stage of the study to identify the rewarding and distressing aspects of each role they occupied. On the basis of re- 1 Our approach to assessing the quality of experience in a role also builds upon recent research suggesting that levels of psychological wellbeing and stress are affected more by "chronic" difficulties and gratifications inherent in social roles than by major, discrete life events (Pearlin, 1982). Daily concerns have been related to psychiatric disfunction, including anxiety and depression (Kanner, Coyne, Schaefer, & Lazarus, 1981; Lewinsohn & Talkington, 1979). Festively toned experiences, namely, rewards, are viewed as important both in preventing and mitigating the effects of stress (DeLongis, Coyne, Dakof, Folkman, & Lazarus, 1982; Kanner etal., 1981). 2 Family income was classified as follows: I = $1.00-$3,999; 2 = $4,000-$5,999; 3 = $6,000-$9,999; 4 = $10,000-$15,999; 5 = $16,000-$20,999; 6 - $21,000-$28,999; 7 = $29,000-$39,999; 8 = $40,000-$64,999; 9 = $65,000 and above. Education was categorized as follows: 1 = some high school or less; 2 = high school diploma; 3 = some college or trade school (without degree); 4 = trade school with degree; 5 = college degree; 6 = M.A.; 7 = higher than M. A. ROLE QUALITY IN WOMEN 581 Table 2 Well-Being Indices: Means and Standard Deviations for the Six Role Pattern Groups Self-esteem Depression Pleasure Roles occupied: Group" M SD M SD M SD Wife(M,NC,NE) a Paid worker (NM) Wife, mother (M,C,NE) Paid worker, wife (M, E, NC) Paid worker, mother (D) Paid worker, wife, mother (M, C, E) 3.35 3.37 3.30 3.57 3.56 3.59 .49 .57 .59 .44 .36 .41 .48 .48 .35 .38 .38 .33 .27 .45 .34 .49 .36 .29 -.69 -.94 -.45 2.48 2.58 2.62 2.61 1.94 2.46 .28 -.05 .72 Note. The means and standard deviations for the total sample were as follows: for self-esteem, M = 3.47, SD = 0.49; for depression, M = 1.39, SD = 0.39; for pleasure, M = .03, SD = 2.46. ' Initials refer to the sampling groups in Table I as follows: C = children; D = divorced; E = employed; NC = no children; NE = not employed; NM = never married. sponse frequency, equal number of rewards and concerns were used to construct scales for subjects in the survey stage of the study. Subjects used a 4-point scale to indicate to what extent, if at all, each of the items was rewarding or distressing. For example, for the role of paid worker, each employed subject was asked how rewarding she found the pay she received and how much of a concern a lack of challenge was. Each subject received three scores per role: a mean reward score (her mean response to the reward items), a mean concern score, and a balance score, for example, the difference between the mean reward score and the mean concern score. The balance scores constituted our index of the quality of experience in each role. reliability, alpha coefficients were calculated for the six scales and ranged from .71 to .94. Women who occupied the role of paid worker reported as the most rewarding aspects of their jobs being able to work on one's own, the sense ojaccomplishment/competence, and having a job that fits one's interests and skills. The most distressing aspects were having too much to do, having to juggle conflicting tasks, and lacking career growth opportunity. For the role of wife, the most rewarding aspects were having a husband who backs you up in what you want to do, the companionship, and having a husband who sees you as special. The most distressing aspects were husband's physical health problems, conflicts over chil- Results dren, and husband's job problems. For the role of mother, women found the most rewarding aspects to be the love children Intercorrelations Between the Well-Being Variables show, liking the kind of people they are, and pleasure in their accomplishments. The most distressing aspects were worry Intercorrelations between the well-being variables were highly significant. Self-esteem was correlated with depression at -.63 (p < .001) and with pleasure at .51 (p < .001). Depres- sion was correlated with pleasure at —.61 (p < .001). Thus the three indices measure related dimensions of well-being; each Table 3 Reward Items for Paid Worker Role: Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations With Well-Being explains from 26% to 40% of the variance of the others. Role Occupancy and Weil-Being: Correlational Analyses The number of roles a woman occupied was significantly correlated with the well-being indices; for self-esteem, the correlation was .16 (p < .01); for depression it was -.14 (p < .05); and for pleasure it was .23 (p < .001). Thus there was no evidence that involvement in multiple roles per se was detrimental to well-being. Table 2 shows the mean scores on the three well-being indices for the sample as a whole and for the six role-pattern groups. Role Quality and Well-Being Rewards and concerns. The reward and concern items (with Item M SD 1. Hours fit your needs 2. Job security 3. Appreciation and recognition 4. People you work with 5. Helping others/being needed 6. Liking your boss 7. Sense of accomplishment/competence 8. Variety of tasks 9. Opportunity for learning 10. Physical conditions 1 1 . Getting out of the house 12. Being able to work on your own 1 3. Helping others develop 14. Job fits interests and skills 15. Good income 16. Good support facilities 17. Opportunity for advancement 18. Challenging, stimulating work 19. Getting to make decisions 2.98 2.86 3.06 3.15 3.17 2.82 3.47 3.13 3.02 2.50 2.98 3.57 3.04 3.23 2.66 2.79 2.08 3.03 3.13 0.96 1.06 0.90 0.90 0.96 1.05 0.83 0.93 1.06 1.05 1.10 0.75 1.13 0.92 1.02 0.99 1.07 1.02 0.96 means and standard deviations) for the roles of paid worker, wife, and mother are presented in Tables 3 through 8. To assess Note.N= 180. 582 GRACE K. BARUCH AND ROSALIND BARNETT Table 4 Table 6 Concern Items for Paid Worker Role: Means. Standard Concern Items for Wife Role: Means, Standard Deviations, Deviations, and Correlations With Well-Being and Correlations With Well-Being Item M SD Item M SD 1. Having loo much to do 2. Job insecurity 3. Job conflicts with other responsibilities 4. Not liking boss 5. Having to juggle conflicting tasks 6. Not getting advancement you want/deserve 7. Job's not fitting skills/interests 8. Job is too regimented 9. Bad physical conditions 10. Lack of recognition/appreciation 1 1 . Job's dullness/monotony 12. Dissatisfaction with income 13. Problems re being a woman 14. Having to do things not part of job 15. Lack of opportunity for career growth 16. Unnecessary busy work 17. Lack of challenge 18. People you work with 19. Job too draining 2.16 1.59 1.73 1.38 2.04 1.82 1.45 1.24 1.53 1.50 1.40 1.95 1.40 1.56 1.95 1.67 1.57 1.53 1.78 1.04 0.88 0.85 0.77 0.99 1.02 0.84 0.64 0.81 0.77 0.78 1.01 0.78 0.82 1.10 0.88 0.92 0.79 0.83 1 . Husband being unavailable 2. Poor communication 3. Husband's physical health 4. Not getting enough appreciation 5. Conflicts about children" 6. Husband's job/career problems 7. Problems in sexual relationship 8. Lack of companionship 9. Husband's job instability 10. Problems re demands of husband's job 11. Husband has emotional problems 12. Not getting along 13. Conflict over housework 14. Not getting enough emotional support 15. Conflicts re children 1.57 1.63 2.08 1.51 1.89 1.70 1.51 1.26 1.43 1.51 1.46 1.44 1.23 1.41 1.39 0.89 0.83 1.18 0.66 0.99 0.98 0.85 0.57 0.82 0.85 0.77 0.66 0.53 0.68 0.68 Note.N= 142. a Correlations not available. Note.N= 180. striking was that between the marriage balance score and pleasure (r = .70). about their physical well-being, worry about the teenage years, and the financial strain. Regression Analyses Balance scores and well-being. In Table 9, we present the In the next analyses, we examined whether the quality of a mean balance (role quality) scores for the roles of paid worker, woman's experience in her roles, assessed by role balance wife, and mother for the sample as a whole and for the six role scores, added significantly to the predictions of her well-being pattern groups. (Reward scores were consistently higher than beyond the effects of role occupancy per se. Because relations concern scores for all the roles assessed, resulting in positive between well-being and the role variables could be an artifact mean balance scores.) of a woman's income or her educational level, the hierarchical The relation between the balance scores and the well-being multiple regression equations controlled for the effects of these indices are shown in the zero-order correlations presented in variables. (In our sample, employment status was significantly Table 10. All the correlations were highly significant; the most correlated with education, r= .25, p< .001, and marital status was significantly correlated with income, r = .46, p < .001.) Age was also entered as a control in these analyses. Role occupancy Table 5 Reward Items for Wife Role: Means, Standard Deviations, Table 7 and Correlations With Well-Being Item 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 1 2. 1 3. 14. 1 5. Companionship Having someone to take care of you Husband easy to get along with Physical affection Husband being a good father" Able to go to husband with problems The sexual relationship Husband backing you up Enjoyment of doing things for husband Husband sees you as special Husband is a good provider Husband's personality fits yours Husband's willingness to share housework Good communication Husband's willingness to have children Note.N= 142. " Correlations not available. Reward Items for Mother Role: Means, Standard Deviations, M 3.48 2.91 3.20 3.27 3.43 3.20 3.05 3.45 3.27 3.44 3.22 3.00 2.70 3.15 3.27 SD 0.77 0.99 0.95 0.89 0.89 0.77 0.99 1.02 0.81 0.77 0.77 0.92 0.93 1.22 0.92 and Correlations With Weil-Being and Pleasure Item 1 . Being needed 2. Pleasure from their accomplishments 3. Helping them develop 4. The love they show 5. Feeling proud of how they are turning out 6. Liking the kind of people they are 7. Being able to go to them with problems 8. Enjoying doing things with them 9. The help they give you 10. The meaning they give your life 1 1 . Being the best caretaker for them 12. The way they get along together 13. Seeing them mature and change 1 4. The way they change you for the better Nate.N= 136. M 2.98 3.63 3.61 3.72 3.61 3.67 2.58 3.43 2.59 3.38 2.90 2.94 3.60 3.10 SD 0.81 0.58 0.66 0.53 0.65 0.59 1.03 0.73 0.99 0.76 1.08 0.88 0.58 0.99 ROLE QUALITY IN WOMEN Table 8 Concern Items for Mother Role: Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations With Well-Being Item 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. The financial strain Feeling trapped/bored Worry: physical well-being Not getting along with each other Heavy demands/responsibilities Worry: teenage years Not sure if you're doing the right thing Their not showing appreciation/love Problem with their education/school Disappointment in what they are like Not having enough control over them Needing you less as they get older Too many arguments/conflicts with them Interference in relationship with husband M SD 2.20 1.44 2.37 1.96 2.17 2.36 2.05 1.54 1.99 1.32 1.52 1.31 1.55 1.34 0.97 0.90 1.07 1.03 .95 1.05 0.92 0.76 0.97 0.58 0.74 0.59 0.78 0.57 Note.N= 136. variables were entered in the second step of the equations; the balance scores, converted to z scores, were entered in the third and last step.' As is shown in Table 11, the total models were highly significant for all three well-being measures. For self-esteem, F(9, 193) = 6.18, p < .001; for depression, F(9, 193) = 6.54, p < .001; and for pleasure, F(9,176) = 16.99, p < .001. The regression analyses showed that family income was a significant predictor of all three well-being indices. The only role occupancy variable that was a significant predictor of well-being when age, income, and education were controlled was paid worker, which predicted self-esteem (B = .20, p < .01). For all three well-being measures, the role quality variables entered in the final step yielded a highly significant increment in the percent of variance explained. For self-esteem, the most powerful predictors after family income were occupancy of the paid worker role (B = .20, p < .01) and the balance score for the paid worker role (B = .20, p < .01). The two other balance scores were also significant predictors of self-esteem. For depression, Table 9 Mean Balance Scores of Role Pattern Groups Role Group Paid worker Never-married Married, no children, not employed Married, children, not employed Married, employed, no children Divorced Married, children, employed 1.16 Wife Mother 1.69 1.58 1.53 1.29 1.46 1.57 1.61 1.80 1.33 1.52 Note. Mean balance scores for the total sample were as follows: paid worker role = 1.35, SD = 0.81; wife role = 1.67, SD = 0.92; and mother role= 1.47,S» = 0.71. 583 Table 10 Zero-Order Correlations of Mean Balance Scores and Well-Being Indices Well-being index Role balance Work Marriage Children Self-esteem r n .36* .35* .34* 178 140 132 Depression r n -.35* -.45* -.42* 178 141 133 Pleasure r n .49* .70* .34* 164 132 123 *p<.001. the most powerful (negative) predictors after family income were the balance scores for paid worker (B = -.23) and wife (B = -.23), both significant at the p < .001 level. For pleasure, in contrast, the most powerful predictor was the quality of experience in the role of wife (B = .47, p < .001). Family income (B= .34, p < .001) and the balance scores for the paid worker role (B = .31, p < .001) were also significant predictors of pleasure. In contrast to findings for the other two well-being variables, the balance score for the role of mother was not a significant predictor of pleasure. Discussion The overall findings of this study support the view that neither the scarcity hypothesis nor the enhancement hypothesis is an adequate explanation of women's experiences in their social roles in relation to well-being, in part because both hypotheses focus on the quantity of roles. The findings reported here suggest two important points about role involvement and well-being. First, it is the qualitative rather than the quantitative aspects of a woman's experiences in her social roles that are the best key to understanding her psychological well-being, or lack thereof. In the regression analyses, only one role occupancy variable was related to a wellbeing index; being a paid worker was associated with higher selfesteem, consistent with the literature on women's employment and well-being just cited. Occupancy of the role of wife or mother is not by itself predictive of well-being. In contrast, the only role quality variable not related to all three well-being measures was the balance score for the role of mother, which did not predict pleasure. These findings on well-being are consistent with others focusing on stress, as assessed by indices of role overload, role conflict, and anxiety. Analyses carried out by the authors using the same data set indicated that role quality variables were much stronger predictors of stress outcomes than were role occupancy variables (Barnett & Baruch, 1985). In their roles as workers, spouses, and parents, women (and men) experience both suffering and gratification. Our findings suggest that to the degree that a particular role yields a net gain of benefits over costs, involvement in that role will have a positive impact on well-being, even if such involvement also in- 3 For each role, balance scores were set at zero for subjects not occupying that role. 584 GRACE K. BARUCH AND ROSALIND BARNETT Table 11 strongly on their lives, the quality of work may be an even Hierarchical Multiple Regression A nalyses: stronger predictor of well-being. Their greater difficulties in es- Well-Beine Indices tablishing a stable marriage because of the economic and employment problems of Black men may make marriage and mar- Predictor Self-esteem Depression B" B" ital quality less or more salient to their well-being. Further, the Pleasure role of mother may be more central to the self-esteem of Black women than Caucasian women (Malson, 1982). In short, re- Control variables: Step 1 Education Age Family income R2 for Stepl Role occupancy variables: Step 2 Paid worker Wife Mother R2 for Step 2 Quality of experience variables: Step 3" Paid worker Wife Mother R2 for Step 3 lations between well-being and involvement in social roles may .04 -.04 .25*** .07 .07 -.08 -.24*** .06 .11 .02 .34*** .11 vary not only for different roles and different dimensions of well-being, but for different groups of women. References .20*** -.05 .06 .11 .20** .16* .18** .22** -.03 -.07 -.10 .07 -.23*** -.23*** -.19** .23** .05 -.01 .13 .13 .31*** .47*** .09 .46*** a Standardized regression coefficient for variable entered into regression at that step. "Delta R1 calculations indicated that this step produced a significant increment in variance explained. * p < .05. **/> < .01. ***p< .001. creases the number of roles a woman occupies. Although merely occupying a role in no way guarantees that the role will be rewarding, not occupying a role clearly preludes deriving any benefits from it. The second point is that differential patterns exist with respect to the relation of role quality to specific dimensions of well-being. For example, a satisfying marriage was strongly tied to pleasure (happiness, satisfaction, and optimism) but was not as strongly associated with self-esteem or depression. Putting these two points together, if well-being is conceptualized as having more than one dimension, and if the sources of well-being differ for each dimension, involvement in multiple roles may be a prerequisite for well-being. A limitation of the findings just noted is that in a cross-sectional study, the direction of relations found cannot be known. It is possible, for example, that women who are high in selfesteem shape their roles so as to make them satisfying. Furthermore, pre-existing emotional problems, that is, impaired wellbeing, may cause a woman to perceive her roles as low in quality. Additional limitations of the study that restrict the generalizability of the findings include the restricted age range of the sample and the focus on Caucasian women. With respect to age, for example, women younger than 35 who are mothers are more likely to have small children, and the quality of the mother role may be a more powerful influence on well-being than for older women. The issues addressed in this study need to be investigated in different age groups. Perhaps most important, major racial and/or ethnic differences may exist among women. For example, because Black women have more consistently assumed a lifelong need to be employed, the paid worker role may relate differently to their well-being; to the extent that the work role impacts more Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1974). Developing measures of perceived life quality: Results from several national surveys. Social Indicators Research, I, 1-26. Aneshensel, C. A., Frerichs, R. R., & Clark, V. A. (1981). Family roles and sex differences in depression. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 22, 379-393. Barnett, G. C., & Baruch, O. K. (1985). Women's involvement in multiple roles, role strain, and psychological distress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 135-145. Baruch, G., Barnett, R., & Rivers, C. (1985). Lifeprints: New patterns of love and work for today's women. New York: Signet Books. Belle, D. (Ed.). (1982). Lives in stress: Women and depression. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Bernard, J. (1972). The future of marriage. New York: World-Times. Bradburn, N. (1969). The structure of well-being. Chicago: Aldine. Brown, G. W, & Harris, T. (1978). Social origins of depression: A study of psychiatric disorder in women. New York: Free Press. Brown, G. (1984). Stress, social supports and depression- Paper presented at Harvard University School of Public Health, Boston, MA. Bryant, F. B., & Veroff, J. (1982). The structure of well-being: A sociohistorical analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 653-673. Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rodgers, W. L. (1976). The situation of women. In A. Campbell, P. E. Converse, & W. L. Rodgers (Eds.), The finality of American life (pp. 395-442). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Cleary, P. D., & Mechanic, D. (1983). Sex differences in psychological distress among married people. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 111-121. Coleman, L. M., & Antonucci, T. C. (1982, Winter). Women's wellbeing at midlife. ISR Newsletter. (Available from Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor) Crosby, F. (1983, November). Work and domestic life. Paper presented at the conference "Effective Management of Work and Personal Life," McGill University, Montreal, Canada. DeLongis, A., Coyne, J. C., Dakof, G., Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1982). Relationship of daily hassles, uplifts, and major life events to health status. Health Psychology, 1, 119-136. Depner, C. (1979, August). The parental role and psychological wellbeing. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, New York. Derogatis, L. R., Lipman, R. S., Rickels, K., Uhlenhuth, E. H., & Covi, L. (1974). The Hopkins symptom checklist. In P. Pichot (Ed.), Psychological measurements in psychopharmacology. Paris: Karger. Epstein, C. (1983, April). The new total woman. Working Woman, pp. 100-103. Gigy, L. L. (1980). Self concept of single women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 5,321-340. Glenn, N. D. (1975). The contribution of marriage to the psychological well-being of males and females. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 37, 594-601. ROLE QUALITY IN WOMEN Goode, W. J. (1960). A theory of strain. American Sociological Review, 25, 483-496. Gove, W. R., & Tudor, J. (1973). Adult sex roles and mental illness. American Journal of Sociology, 78, 812-835. Haring-Hidore, M., Stock, W. A., Okin, M. A., & Witter, R. A. (1985). Marital status and well-being: A subjective synthesis. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 47, 947-954. Kanner, A. D., Coyne, J. C., Schaefer, C., & Lazarus, R. S. (1981). Comparisons of two models of stress measurement: Daily hassles and uplifts versus major life events. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 139. Kessler, R. C., & McRae, J. A. (1981). Trends in the relationship between sex and psychological distresses: 1957-1976. American Sociological Review, 46, 443-452. Lewinsohn, P. M., & Talkington, J. (1979). Studies of the measurement of unpleasant events and relationship with depression. Applied Psychological Measurement, 3, 83-102. Long, J., & Porter, K. L. (1984). Multiple roles of midlife women: A case for new directions in theory, research, and policy. In G. Baruch & J. Brooks-Gunn (Eds.), Women in midlife. New York: Plenum. Lopata, H. (1971). Occupation housewife. New York: Oxford University Press. Lowenthal, M. E, & Chiriboga, D. (1973). Social stress and adaptation: Toward a life-course perspective. In C. Eisdorfer & M. P. Lawton (Eds.), The psychology of adult development and aging. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Malson, M. (1982). Black women's sex role integration and behavior (Working Paper #87). Wellesley, MA: Wellesley College, Center for Research on Women. Marks, S. R. (1977). Multiple roles and role strain: Some notes on human energy, time and commitment. American Sociological Review, 41,921-936. Merikangas, K. (1985, May). Sex differences in depression. Paper presented at Murray Center (Radcliffe College) Conference: Mental Health in Social Context, Cambridge, MA. Pearlin, L. (1982, October). Social roles and personal stress. Paper pre- 585 sented at the National Conference on Social Stress Research, Durham, NH. Pietromonaco, R., Manis, J., & Frohardt-Lane, K. (1984, August). Psychological consequences of multiple social roles. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Sears, P. S., & Barbee, A. H. (1977). Careers and life satisfaction among Terman's gifted women. In J. Stanley, W. George, & C. Solano (Eds.), The gifted and creative Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. Serlin, E. (1980). Emptying the nest: Women in the launching stage. In D. McGuigan (Ed.), Women's lives: New theory, research, and policy. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Sieber, S. D. (1974). Toward a theory of role accumulation. American Sociological Review. 39, 567-578. Siegel, P. M. (1971). Prestige in the American occupational structure. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago. Spreitzer, E., Snyder, E. E., & Larson, D. L. (1979). Multiple roles and psychological well-being. Sociological Focus, 12, 141-148. Thoits, P. A. (198 3). Multiple identities and psychological well-being: A reformulation and test of the social isolation hypothesis. American Sociological Review, 48, 174-187. Verbrugge, L. M. (1982). Women's social roles and health. In P. Berman & E. Ramey (Eds.), Women: A developmental perspective (NIH Publication No. 82-2298). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Verbrugge, L. M. (in press). Role burdens and physical health of men and women. In F. Crosby (Ed.), Modern women: Managing the dual role. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Veroff, J., Douvan, E., &Kulka, R. (1981). The inner American: A selfportrait from 1957101976. New York: Basic Books. Ward, R. (1979). The never-married in later life. Journal of Gerontology, 34, 861-869. Received March 5, 1985 Revision received February 6, 1986 •