Memory for Pictures: A Life-Span Study of the Role of Visual Detail Kathy Pezdek The Chremont Graduate School PEZDEK, KATHY. Memory for Pictures: A Life-Span Study of the Role of Visual Detail. CHILD DEVELOPMENT, 1987, 58, 807-815. This experiment assessed the effect of the amount of physical detail in pictures on picture recognition memory for 7-year-olds, 9-year-olds, young adults, and older adults over 68. Subjects were presented simple and complex line dxawings, tactorially combined in a "same-different" recognition test with simple or complex forms of each. For each age group, recognition accuracy was significantly higher for pictures presented in the simple dian in the complex form. This eflfect was due to diflferences between simple and complex pictures in the correct rejection rate but not die hit rate; subjects were less accurate detecting deletions fix>m changed complex pictures than addithns to changed simple picitures. The older adults were no better than chance at correctly rejecting changed complex pictures. Altfiou^ increasing the presentation duration from 5 sec to 15 sec increased overall accuracy, it did not increase subjects' ability to correctly reject changed complex pictures. Results are interpreted in terms of schematic encoding and storage of pictures. Accordingly, visual information that communicates the central schema of each picture is more likely to be encoded and retained in memory than information diat does not communicate this schema. Individuals have an impressive ability to remember pictures they have seen before. This has been demonstrated with recognition tests (Nickerson, 1965; Shepard, 1967; Standing, Conezio, & Haber, 1970) as well as with recall tests (Bousfield, Esterson, & Whitmarsh, 1957). In addition, a number of developmental studies have reported increases with age, from childhood to early adulthood, in recognition memory for large nxmibers of pictures (Hofi&nan & Dick, 1976), recall and recognition for visual objects (Dirks &c Neisser, 1977; Mandler, Seegmiller, 6E Day, 1977), and face-reex)gnition memory (Blaney fie Winograd, 1978). However, few of these studies have examined qualitative developmental differences in picture memory. In other words, are adults and older chilciren processing pictures differently than younger children, or are they just performing the same processes better? In typical picture recognition memory studies, subjects are presented a series of pictures to remember and are then presented a test that includes some of die "old" pictures and some "new," distractor pictures. In the large majority of these studies, the "new," distractor pictures are completely new pic- tures. This procedure thus tests how well subjects can distinguish pictures they have seen from picjtures they have not seen, and they can do this quite well. What we do not leam from these studies, however, is how much of the visual detail in a pic^ture has been retained in memory when a picture is recognized. The present study examines this particular aspect of picture memory and tests for qualitative differences in these processes with age. We initially addressed this issue in an earlier study from our laboratory (Pezdek & Chen, 1982). In this previous study, 7-yearolds, 9-year-olds, and young adults were presented simple and complex line elrawings of scenes. The simple and cK)mpIex forms of each picture contained the same c^entral information, but peripheral details, shading, and embellishment were added in the complex form of each pic^re. These pictures were selected from the set of pictures utilized by Nelson, Metzler, and Reed (1974) and originally constructed by Nickerson (1965). At test, pictures were presented one at a time in a "same-different" recognition test Half of the simple and complex line elrawings were tested in the same form as presentation; half This research was conducted while die author was supported by a grant from the National Institute of Education. I especially thank Sidney Fox for collecting the data for diis study and Tom Dougherty fbr analyzing the data, and I appreciate conceptual contributions and feedback on die manuscript provided by Ruth Maki. Requests for reprints should be sent to Kathy Pezdek, Department of Psychology, The Claremont Craduate School, Claremont, CA 91711. [Child Development, 1987,58, 807-815. © 1987 by tfie Society for Research in Child Etevelopment, Inc. All rights reserved. 0009.3920/87/5803-000S$01.00] 808 d f l d Development were changed picUires. The changed test pic- 1969; Tverricy & Shrarman, 1975) as w ^ as for tures were arrived at by chan^s^ tiie pictures &ces (Lffi^«y, Alexander, & Lane, 1971) that had been presented in a simple form to increases w£th preseHiitation time and tfut ike tiie complex form of tibe same picture and beneSts of incireased presenl^fm tlo^ are changing pictures that had been presented in greater for yovrag c ^ & e n Aao for older c ^ a complex form to tiie simple form of tiie same dren and acblts ( H B ^ Mcairison, & Sheingold, 1970; Naus, Omstein, & Mvano, 1977; picture. Pezdefc & Mlceli, 19^). Seven-year-olds and The piincipal result of Pe2Kiek and Chen 9->^a'"0lds were included in the pre^nt (1982) was tbat for adults, recx^Eiitlon sen- study for cOTi^Mn^^ay wifli the P e z d ^ and sitivity, meastcred in terms of d', was grei^r Chen (19^) stucb' and, al«), bec»ise previd& ^ l for pictures in tiie simple than in tiuf C(^ preseirtation condition. However, for 7-i olds and 9-yeaiHklds, recxignltion sensUJ , . f^E»Uy scun pk^uw measured in temis of d', was similiB- for pic- cale atl^tlcm to central versus tures in the ^ns^effiEidccm^^fix p^es^rarttttion cJbbOls (see Goo^ban, 1S80; condition. 'Hus finding is in maslced cffliferast & Bruner, 1970; V « « | ^ t , 19 to Reese's (1970) hypothesis tiiat reteatitm of & present study ioduded a visual stimi^ should be posi^vdy rektedl to l k , haa^ on recent & the amount (^ dBtf^ in &e ^mali. It is ako also b e o ^ more Aan inconsistent witii studies that have repeated ger adiiAs &(»Q iacrettsed Tgs^Beao^0^im superior recall for compl«£ over simple pic- on meoHiry t a ^ (Cza& & BiS^nowUz, 1985; tures (Bevan & Steger, 1971; Evertson & Wingfield, Pooa, Unnbrnxli, & Lowe, 1965). Wicker, 1974) or no difierence between aduhs* recognition ctf uix^le and comidex^clife span were u ^ s e d in Ms s t e ^ to test if tures (Nelson et al., 1974). dS hiomn to ^dst in on the above s p ^ S ^ cliSf^Qces between the test items used by P e a ^ and Oran and those used in odi» studies. That is, Pe^ek and Chen utilized test |^c;tures v/iHk &e same centxal iafom^rtion as i^ctiues presented, but with added or deleted elabor^ve viswil det ^ s . This study thus speeifically tested memory for the visual di^ails in pfctaires thai retained the same thematic ctmtwat in their simple and comi^^ forms. Aj^paready, ^ten, faults' ability to cSiscrimuof^ same &om changed simple pictures is gr^^a: 6utQ their ability to discriminate swne feom r— r—--—. *• 1. J to discrinikiate same nom c^bai^ed plcte^es was similar for simile wid conqcHex jrfctures. The puipose of tiie present study was to farther p r c ^ the quiaiiidive deferences between Euhilts and c^dren in recc^p^tion memory for visual deteils in pU±iu%s. This study tested tiie hypt^esis ti^ tbe e^ differences reported by PezcJ^ and CSien (1982) could be accounted for by cHififei'raices in tiie time required to encode and (nocess infomiation at dififerent ages. That is, at a given presei^^lon dimrtion, it is hypoi^esisMMi tiiat c^iikben ce^^xae less of the aviidJtiMe vbual detEols in memory tiian do yoiuig adi^ts* and tiius tiieir memory perfiMmimce is simile with simple and complex pictures. It has been demonstrated that memory fbr pictures (Potter, 1976; Potter & Levy, pictures. In tiie present study. 7-year-old8,9-yearadMte (coAei^ stadente), toid jprocedkire c^ized hy Pezeiek md Chen ~), w ^ iHesectation time per ^ d u r e . .jen sd:^oete. Tbe |H»lHg#*UHi ut^xdhy P e z M and C&en ( I M ) was 8 sec per pctuie. In the iseBcait ^&^, each picture was pvescmUtd for 5 (»• 15 sec. If the 3es in i8co®^tion taemnixy for jf^^alev^ctiu:^ lepotted^ Pezand C3ien Jl^Z) are in paA chie to age v«««erences intbe speedof ei«x»lte®U!fiMana^^^^ ^ ^ ^ mwii^l^faaiE pn^ntttttbn ^Eoe in ^^ present ^udy ^ouM resiA in s u u i ^ pattems erf r e s i ^ among age ffotps ^ ^ slower pres^rtOtJon time but not at tiie faster S v i ^ e c t s . — V c a t y sufcg p ^ ^ each <kfourage gtoi^ps. Seven-yeotKiids (M = 6.8 years, SD = .23) and 9-year-olds (M = 8.9 years, SD = .35) partirafeaibed 6om public elimm^m schools in Otu^srant, Ci^for•ia, a mklc&Hdb^ subuib in L(» Angela Gounty. tlie yoimg a&Ut suli^iects were uMie^gmdiii^s vfho vokmteesedfrcnnclasses at tile CUrenHmt CdBeges (M = 21.5 yeais, SD - 5.44). Tlie dder a d t ^ were volunteers from a retirement community in Claremont. Kathy Pezdek 809 They ranged in age from 68 to 90 (M = 80.2 years, SD = 4.52), were generally well educated (M = 17.4 years of education, SD = 3.4), and were amply healthy to live selfsufiiciently. In each age group approximately equal numbers of males and females participated in each condition, but gender was not si)ecifically controlled for. sentation pictures. The 11 changed versions of simple presentation pictures were tiie complex versions of these pic:tures. The 11 changed versions of the complex presentation pic^res were the simple versions of these pictures. Thus, each of tiie 44 presentation pic*ires was included onc« in the test phase, in eitiiar the same or changed form. Design.—All subjects viewed simple and complex line drawings in the presentation phase and were tested with same and changed forms of these pictures. Twenty subjects in each age group were presented the pictures at a duration of 5 sec each, and 20 were presented the pictures at a duration of 15 sec each. The study can thus be described a s a 4 x 2 x 2 x 2 mixed &ctorial design with age and presentation duration as between-subjects factors and presentation form and test form as within-subjects Victors. In the tBSt phase, subjects viewed pictures one at a time at a rate controlled by tiie experimenter. For each pic;ture the experimenter asked, "Is this picture the same as a picture you saw before, or are there some changes in this picture?" Several practice slides were shown first to ensure that subjects understood what types of changes c:onstituted changed test items. The assignment of each picture to conditions of presentation and test and the sequencing of presentation and test slides were arranged in two orders. Half of the subjects in each condition were randomly assigned to each order. A 3-min intervening delay task (circling all of the twos on a random number sheet) was included between presentation and test to ensure that the test that followed measured long-term memoiy. Materials.—The materials were the same as those used by Pezdek and Chen (1982) and were selected from the set of pictures used by Nelson et al. (1974) and, originally, by Nickerson (1965). These included 44 basic pictures, each drawn in both a simple, unembellished line drawing form, and a cx)mplex, embellished line chawing form, for a t o ^ of 88 pictures. All drawings were black and white. The complex form of each picture was an exac^ reproduction of the simple form, with the adciition of elaborative details to both the principal figure and the background. The central information was thus the same in the simple and complex form of each picture. Examples of stimulus pictures are shown in Figure 1. The use of the same materials as in previous studies strengtiiens this study by allowing a comparison of results ac^ross studies without possible confounding effects of different materials. Procedure.—Subjects participated individually. They were presented a sequence of slides including 44 presentation pictures, followed by a 3-min delay task, and then 44 test pictures. The presentation pictures included 22 simple line drawings and 22 complex line drawings. In the presentation phase, subjects were instruc!ted to study each picture carefully, as it would be important in a later part of die experiment The pic;tures were presented on slides by a Kodak Carousel slide projector. During the presentation phase, slides were presented for 5 sec each or for 15 sec each. The inter-slide interval was 1 sec. The test sequence consisted of 22 pictures from the presentation phase—11 simple and 11 complex pic^tures. The remaining 22 test pictures were c^hanged versions of pre- Results The data were scared and analyzed in terms of the mean percent correct as well as the signal detection measure of d'. However, the m^jor results of this study concem di£Ferences between the percent correct data for same test items (i.e., tiie hit rate) and changed test items (i.e., the csoirect rejection rate). These two conditions cannot be examined separately with the d' measure. Throu^out the study, results are considered significant at tile .05 level. OveraU results.—A 4 (age) x 2 (presentetion duration) x 2 (simple or complex presentation form) X 2 (same or changed test form) mixed factorial analysis of variance was performed on the percent correct data. All main effects were significant First, there were signific^ant difierencies among the four £^e groups; young adults perfonned best (83.2%), followed by ^year-olds (75.4%), 7year-olds (67.6%). and older adults (67.0%), F(3,152) = 29.94, MS^ = 309.10. Tukey pairwise compariscms indic^ated that only the differences between young adulte and 7-yearolds and between young adults and older adtilts were significant Recognition acxnuacy was higher at the l5-sec rate (76.4%) than at the 5-sec rate (70.2%). F(l,152) = 20.04, MSe = 309.10. Sulgects were more accurate recognizing pic^res in the simple (79.5%) than cx)mplex (67.2%) presentation form, F(l,152) = 100.71, MSe = 240.39, and they were more 810 Child Development SIMPLE COMPLEX FIC. 1.—Examples of pictures in both simple and complex form accurate recognizing pictures tested in the same form (hit rate = 77.9%) than in the changed forni (correct rejection rate = 68.7%), F(l,152) = 32.62, MS^ = 240.39. Interpretations of these main effects are qualified by three significant interactions. First, as can be seen in the bottom row of Table 1, the interaction of presentation form X test form was significant, F(l,152) = 32.31, MSe = 161.49. Post hex: comparisons revealed that the hit rate did not differ between pictures presented in the simple form (81.3%) and complex form (74.7%). However, the correct rejection rate was significantly less for pictures presented in the complex form (59.7%) than for pictures presented in the simple form (77.7%), i(152) = 1.81. In other words, subjects were significantly less accurate at detecting deletions from changed complex pictures than they were at detecting additions to changed simple pictures. This pattem of results was also the basis for the; significant main effect of presentation form on d'data,F(l,152) = 70.29, MS^ = 1.02, with d' greater for simple (d' = 2.12) than for complex pictures (d' = 1.17). The interaction of presentation form x test form on percent correct also entered into significant second-order interactions of age x presentation form x test form, F(3,152) = 10.22, MSe = 161.49, and presentation duration X presentation form X test fonn. Kathy Pezdek 811 TABLE 1 MEAN PERCENTAGE CORRECT IN EACH EXPERIMENTAL CONDITION PRESENTATION CONDITION Complex Simple TEST CONDITION 7-year-olds: 5 sec 15 sec Mean 9-year-olds: 5 sec 15 sec Mean Young adults: 5 sec 15 sec Mean Older adults: 5 sec 15 sec Mean Overall mean Same Changed Same Changed 71.4 82.7 77.0 61.4 74.5 67.9 60.5 69.1 64.8 64.5 56.8 60.7 80.4 90.9 85.7 76.8 80.9 78.9 72.7 80.9 76.8 62.3 58.2 60.2 83.6 87.7 85.7 87.3 91.8 89.5 80.0 88.6 84.3 71.8 74.5 73.2 73.6 79.5 76.6 81.3 68.6 80.0 74.3 77.7 68.2 77.3 72.7 74.7 40.0 49.1 44.5 59.7 F(l,152) = 4.99, MSe = 161.49. However, tiie overall age x presentation duration x presentation form X test form interacrtion did not approach significance (F< 1.00). The signific^ant interaction of age x presentation form X test form is of particnilar interest because this interaction allows an assessment of qualitative differences in picture recognition memory with age. These results are presented in Table 1. In order to assess the nature of this interaction, separate 2 (presentation form) X 2 (test form) analyses of variance were performed on the data for each age group. The presentation form X test form interaction was significant for each age group except 7-year-olds. Further, for each age group except 7-year-oIds the difference between the hit rates for simple and complex pictures was not significiant; however, the correct rejection rate was significantly less for changed c^omplex pictures than for changed simple pic^res. On the other hand, for 7-year-olds, although pic^tures presented in their simple form were recognized significantly more accurately than pictures presented in their complex form, F(l,38) = 12.08, the test form x presentation form interaction was not significant. In order to assess the nature of the significant interaction of presentation duration x presentation form x test form, separate 2 (presentation form) x 2 (test form) analyses of variance were periformed on the data at each presentation duration. The presentation form X test form interaction was significant at htiAi presentation rates. Further, the percent correct difference between the 5-sec and 15-sec conditions was significant in all cx)nditions except for changed complex pictures. The correct rejection rats fbr complex pic:tures was exactly the same (59.7%) at both presentation durations. Thus, increasing the presentation duration by threefold did not increase subjects' ability to detect the deleted details in pictures that had been presented in the complex form. Comparisons of young and older adults only.—Several previous studies have reported tiiat differences in memory performance between young and older adults can in part be acxiounted for by age differences in pDDcessing time (Craik 6c Rabinowitz, 1985; Wingfield et al., 1985). The older adults were included in the present study to examine qualitative ciifferences in memory for details in pictures between tiiese two age groups and, specific^ally, to probe whetiier age differences in processing time underlie these memory differences. Furtiier. the significant age X presentation form x test fonn interaction reported above statistically justifies examining these two age groups separately. A 2 (age) x 2 (presentation duration) x 2 (presentation form) x 2 (test form) mixed factorial analysis of variance was performed on the percent correct data. All main effects 812 were significant in tiie same direction as in the overall axisAysis reported above. Of particular interest is tiie finding thiA recogmtion accuracy was higher for yoimg adults (83J2%) tiian for older aduhs (67.0%), F(l,76) = 74.89, MSe = 278.17. There were also signlficfuit interactions of age X presentE^on form, F(l,76) = 5.27, MSe = 240.13, age x test form, F(l,76) = 7.71, MSe = 348.J®, and presentation fctrm X test form, F(l,76) = 55.59, MS^ = 150.51. Interpretations of each of these firstKJrder interactions are qualified by the significant secx}nd-orc^r interaction of £^e x presentation form x test form, F(l,76) = 3.95, MSe = 103.40. As can be seen in the bottom half of Table 1, the hit rates for botii £^e groups did not significantly differ between simple and complex presentation pictures. However, for botii age groups, the correct rejection rate was hi^ier for pictures pres^ted in the simple form than in the complex fc»tn, but the size of this dil&ren<% was almost twice as laige for older adidts as for younger adults. The older adults were, in foct, no better than chance at detecting deletions in changed complex pictures at both the 5-sec and 15-sec presentfUion duratum. No iii*eracticms invcjving pxesenta/don rate ai^rcnac^ied significance. Toother, tiiese results su^^est tiuit there are iKjtfi quantitB^ve and q u i ^ t ^ v e deferences between young and c^der »lults in menwiy iox detfdls in pictures. However, tiie absence t^ a significant iotenKition wi£b presentfrfion duration incUcates tiiat tiiese xHlferences are not singly a result of procjessing rate differences between these two age groups. Disciuwion This study examined develqcHnental differences in memory for details in pictores. Across £dl four ^ e groups, psrtures presented in their simj^e form were reco^ized mwe accun^ly than pictim«s j^reseated in tiieir complex form, liiis fincite^ c^lfers &cun results repented elsewhere wi^ recsall measures (Bevm & Steger, 1971; Everlson & Wtdcer, 1974) as well as witii a recogniticm measure using completely new picbiinss as d^iactor items (Nelson et al., 1974). t h e results of tiie present study suggest tiiat sul^ects axe more accurate at Astinpii^U]^ srane hom duinged simple pic;hires ti^ tiiey are at c^c»im&nf^ing same hom didnged com^^ex pictures. Why is this the case? One interpretation of these results is based on previous findii^ t h ^ pictures, like prose materials, are processed schematically (see Friedman, 1979; Goodman, 1980; Nickerson & Adams, 197^). For example, consider the chawing represented at the bottom left of Figure 1. l l i e schema applied to tike |»cture wcmld be the prepositional representation of a sentence such as, "The clown is crying." Acc^txlin^y, infinmation that communict^s the central Schema of each picbire is mcare lticely to be ehcoded and retained in memory than information tiiat does not communicate this schema. It is important to note that tiie implication here is not that schemata are preserved in memcffy in a verbal form but, rotiier, that the pictorial ui&nrmatkat that is enc»>dbd schematkraily is stoaikz' to tiie type of in&sm^ion that can be represented in a sentence tiiat summarizes the picture. Relevant to the present study, it is suggested that the schema that is cilerived from the simple wid the cc»nplex form of eaeh picture is e s ^ ^ ^ y the same aad corresponds to the mfonn£^on in the simple fcaxn of efuh picHwre. Thus, if subjects' memory repre^ntation fc»- both simpilb and cora^ilex pictures is similar to the simple version of each picture, they would be less accurate disciimiimtiQg same from chwaged comt^ex pictures tiuin discaimii^ing same firom clmiged simple pictures. Tlie next question, however, is how this suie dtuatitHi in tiie present study. Ute results c^ the prese^ study, ^^etiser with those of otiiers (see Potter, 1976; P<»ter & Levy, 1969; Tvetsky & Sherman, 1975), s u p ^ r t Ae c»iu}lusion ^ak memory for pictures inciieases as the exp<»ure dtmttton per ftfcture increases. The mterpretation of this effect offered in tire i^evious stucUes has been that increased stiic^ tune leads to better niemory singly because more in^cKmatkm about the detidSs of tiie pic^ores is encodbd smd retuoed at the loiiger inteiWls. However, only cme of tile studies repo^q|! increased picture rec?ognition m«mo^ with sti*dy time (T^^tsky & Sherman, WfSi) required subjects to cUstinguish staaefrconchss^ged test ^ctures, and in tiua stady the rect^i^iiion dala wexe not analyzed separately for hits mid correct rejections. Ala>, in the presoat study, i d # ^ u ^ increasii^ tiie eiqposius duration from 5 sec to 15 sec increasoi overall picture recogititiion accniracy, xeco^aitioD accuracy for changed complex pictures was low and ex»ctiy the sanw (59,7%) at b ( ^ pres^itii^n Orations, llius, althot^^ increi^ing study tm^ does lead to b^ter o\%rall pidhire recc^iUtion ni«mc»y, tiieje is no suppaxt for the hypothesis t h ^ tiiis is simply because more de~ Kathy Pezdek tails are encoded and retained at longer intervals. In line with the schematic prcwessing notion outlined above, one interpretation of the finding that pictures are better recognized at longer study intervals is that this is due to qualitative differences rather than quantitative differences in encx)ding and storage of detail information. If we view picture memory as a schema-driven process, then with longer on-time subjects would be able to bet' ter abstract the central schema of each picture and enrich the memory representation of the schema by incorporating into it more of the schema relevant infonnation in the picture. This would result in better schemata in memory, not merely the storage of more details from the picture. There are a few other studies in which recognition memory for additions to pictures and deletions from pictures have been compared. However, each of those studies differs in subtle but significant ways from the present study. In some studies, for example, subjects were instructed to respond "old" to original pictures and to changed original pictures, and "new" only to completely new, distractor pictures. Results of these studies are not relevant to the present interest in subject' ability to distinguish original from changed pictures. The study by Park, Puglisi, and Sovacool (1984) is one snch study. In a more relevant study, Mandler and Ritchey (1977) presented college subjects with eight line drawings, each containing six objects. The recognition test that followed included 64 "old" pictures composed of the eight target pictures plus seven transformations of each, and 64 completely "new" distractor pictures. The two transformations relevant to the present discussion were (a) additions and (b) deletions, in which a new object was added to or deleted from a target picture. They reported no significsmt loss over 4 months in the recognition of additions or deletions (in the organized picture condition), and recognition accuracy did not differ between these two types of pictures. These Tesults differ from results reported in the present study. However, there are two important differences between these two studies. First, in the Mandler and Ritchey (1977) study, subjects were "correct" if they classified either the target pic^res or the transformed pictures as "old." Thus, their results do not allow us to assess how well subjects distinguished target pictures from transformed versions of these pic^res. Second, 813 additions and deletions in the Mandler and Ritchey (1977) study involved adding or deleting a whole object in an array of objects. Additions and deletions in the present study involved adding or deleting more general elaborative details in the simple and complex version of each picture. Differences between the results of these two studies c^an be accounted for by these methodological differences. In another relevant study. Brown and Campione (1972) had preschool children study pictures from children's bcwiks. Two hours, 1 day, or 7 days later they were presented completely new distrac:tor pictures, identical original picrtures, and changed original pictures. The changed version of each original included the same character (same clothes, same colors, etc.) as in the original picture but in a different pose. Subjects responded "old" or "new" and tiien classified "old" items as either "identical" or "changed." Subjects weTe similarly accurate classifying original and changed pictures as "old" at each of the three retention intervals. However, subjects were more accurate classifying changed pictures as "changed" than they were classifying identical pictures as "identical." Although these findings differ from those reported in the present study, they do suggest that subjects are generally able to "notice what is new" in changed old pictures. The differences in results between the prt;sent study and that by Rrown and Campione (1972) can be attributed to differences in the type of changes included in "changed" test pictures as well as to differences in the age of the subjects. Thus, several other studies have investigated memory for additions to and deletions from pictures. None of these studies concurs with the principal result of the present study, that extra detail added to changed simple pictures is detected more accurately than detail deleted from changed complex pictures. However, there are notable methodological differences between the present study and each of these other studies. There are two major developmental differences in the pattem of results in the present study. First, for 7-year-olds the (difference in the correct rejection rate for pictures presented in the simple (67.9%) and complex form (60.7%) was not statistically significant, although this difference was significant for each of the other three age groups. This finding is consistent with the results of Pez- 814 Child Development dek and Chen (1982) that differences among the four means defined by conditions of presentation form and tost form were less for the younger children than for the young adults. This result is also in line with the above interpretation of the overall memory advantage for simple over complex pic;tures. That is, if simple pictures are better retained than complex pictures because of schematic processing of the pictures, and if young children are less likely than older children and adults to encode pictures schematically, then it would be expected that the correct rejection rate difference between simple and complex pictures would be less for younger children as well. The second developmental difference in the obtained pattern of results involves a comparison of the young and older adults. Although both young and older adults were significantly more accurate rejecting changed simple pictures than changed complex pictures, older adults were far less able than young adults to correctly reject changed complex pictures. In feet, older adults were no better than chance at correctly rejecting changed complex pictures. According to the interpretation previcmsly outlined, these results suggest that when older adults encode complex pictures, they retain far less of the elaborative details than do young adults. Thus, at the time of test, their memory of complex pictures is similar to the simple form of each picture, and they respond, "Same." Further, the fact that differences between the young adults and the older adults did not result in significant interactions with presentation duration suggests that the processing differences between young and older adults are not simply a result of processing rate differences. This study leads to the conclusion that although adults and children are extremely accurate at discriminating old pictures from completely new pictures (Hof&nan & Dick, 1976; Nickerson, 1965; Shepard, 1967; Standing et al., 1970), they are far less accurate discriminating same from changed versions of old pictures, especially when the changes involve detecting what extra detail has been deleted from changed complex pictures. These results are important because they suggest that when we "remember" a picture or a real world scene, we do not necessarily retain all, or even most, of the elaborative detail presented. This is consistent with the notion that pictures, like prose materials, are processed schematically. As such, scheme-relevant information in pic:tures is likely to be retained well in memory, whereas less scheme- relevant visual elaboration, as manipulated in the present study, is not retained well. These results also hi^light the fact that various measures of recognition memory tap very different aspects of what is retained in pictures. References Bevan, W., & Steger, J. A. (1971). Free recall and abstractness of stimuli. Science, 172, 597-599. Blaney, R. L., & Winograd, E. (1978). Developmental differences in children's recognition memory for faces. Developmental Psychohgy, 14, 441-442. Bousfield, W. A., Esterson, J., & Whitmarsh, G. A. (1957). The effects of concomitant colored and uncolored pictorial representations on the leaming of stimulus words. Jourruil of Applied Psychohgy, 41, 165-168. Brown, A. L., & Campione, J. C. (1972). Recognition memory for perceptually similar pictures in preschool children. Joumai of Experimental Psychology, 95, 55-62. Craik, F. I. M., & Rabinowitz, J. C. (1985). Tlie efFect of presentation rate and encxxling task on age-related memory de&cits. Journal of Gerontology, 40, 309-315. Dirks, J., & Neisser, U. (1977). Memory for objects in real scenes: The development of recognition and recall. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 23, 315-328. Evertson, C. M., & Wicker, F. W. (1974). Pictorial concreteness and mode of elaboration in children's leaming. Journal of Experimental Child Psychohgy, 17, 264-270. Friedman, A. (1979). Framing pictures: The role of knowledge in automatized encoding and memory for gist Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 316-355. Coodman, C. S. (1980). Picture memoiy; How the action schema affects retention. Cognitive Psychohgy, 12, 473-495. Haith, M. M., Morrison, F. J., & Sheingold, K. (1970). Tachistoscopic recognition of geometric forms by children and adults. Psychonomic Science, 19, 345-347. Hofftnan, C. D., & Dick, S. A. (1976). A developmental investigation of recognition memory. Child Devehpment, 47, 794-799. Laughery, K. R., Alexander, J. V., & Lane, A. B. (1971). Recognition of human faces: Effects of target exposure time, target position, pose position and type of photograph. Journal of Applied Psychohgy, 55, 477-483. Mackworth, N. H., & Bmner, J. S. (1970). How adults and children search and recognize pictures. Human Devehpment, 13, 149-177. Mandler, J. M., & Ritchey, C. H. (1977). Long-tenn memory for pictures. Journal of Experimental Psychohgy: Human Leaming and Memory. 3, 386-396. Kathy Pezdek Mandler, J. M., Seegmiller, D., & Day, J. (1977). On the ccxling of spatial information. Memory and Cognition, 5,10-16. Naus, M. U Omstein, P. A., & Aivano, S. (1977). Developmental changes in memory: The ef' fecte of prcK:es8lng time and rehearsal instruc' tions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2a, 23^7-251. Nelson, T. O., Metzler, J., & Reed, D. A. (1974). Role of details in tiie long-tenn rec:ogmticm of pictures and verbal descripticms.J'cmma/ of Experimental Psychohgy, 102,184-186. Nickerson, R. S. (1965). Short-term memoiy for complex visual configurations: A {demonstration of capacity. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 19,155-160. Nickerson, R. S.,ficAdams, M. J. (1979). Long-tenn memory for a conunon object Cognitive Psychohgy, 11,287-307. Park, D. C., Puglisi, J. T , & Sovaoool, M. (1984). Picture memory in older adults: Effects of cx>ntextual detail at encoding and retrieval. Journal cf Gerontohgy, 39,213-215. Pezdek, K., & Chen, H.-C. (1982). Developmental di£ferences in the role of detail in picture recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychohgy, 33» 207-215. Pezdek, K., 6c Miceli, L. (1982). Ufe-span diSerences in memoiy integraticm as a &mction of prcKiessing time. Devehpmental Psychohgy, 18,485-490. 815 Potter, M. C. (1976). Short-term conceptual memory for pictures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Leaming and Memory, 2, 509522. Potter, M. C , & Levy, E. L (1969). Recognition memory for a rs^id sequence of pic:tures> Journal of Experimental Psychohgy, 81,10-15. Reese, H. W. (1970). Inuigery and contextual meaning. In H. W. Reese (Chair), Imagery in children's leaming: A symposium. Psychohgical .Bulletin, 73,40^-414. Shepaid, R. N. (1967). Recognition memory for words, sentences, and pictures. Joumo/ of Verbal Leaming and Verhal Behavior, 6,156-163. Standing, L., Conezio, J., & Haber, R. N. (1970). Perception and memory for pictures: Singletrial leaming of 2500 visual stimuli. Psychonomic Science, 19, 73-74. Tversky, B., & Shennan, T. (1975). Picture memoiy improves with longer on time and oS time. Journal of Experimental Psychohgy: Human Leaming and Memory, 104,114-118. Vurpillot, E. (1968). The development of scanning strategies and their relation to visual differentiation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychohgy, 6,632-650. Wii^field, A., Poon, L. W., Lombardi, L., & Lowe, D. (1985). Speed of processing in normal ^ n g : Effects of speech rate, linguistic stnio ture, and processing tixae. Journal of Gerontology, 40,579-585.