Chord Analysis in the Key of Math and Physics By Jenny Fromme Table of Contents 1. Introduction 2. Background 3. Results 4. Conclusions Appendices References Introduction Which is more musically pleasing, a Mozart piano sonata or a toddler pounding away at the keys of a piano? Which is more enjoyable to listen to, a symphony orchestra or an elementary school strings concert? Why is one more favorable to listen to than the other? When I started college, I began as a music education major. In the music theory classes, one question that I constantly revisited was why do certain notes sound good together? Through all of my music study, I searched for some sort of concrete answer to that question. Unfortunately, most answers gave either a strictly musical reason or a strictly physical reason, and as a result, most answers failed to adequately answer the question for me. When I became a math major, this question of why certain musical notes sound good together stayed with me. As I gained more knowledge and background in mathematics, my intuition told me that the answer lied somewhere in between music and physics and that likely, the answer could be explained mathematically. Thus, my research focuses on trying to find a mathematical explanation for musical consonance and dissonance. The rest of this paper is divided into 3 sections. The first section gives the background information necessary for understanding the problem from a musical, mathematical and physical standpoint and also offers some history on the study of this idea. The second section describes the procedures used to analyze this phenomenon and gives the results gained from the research. The last section gives possible interpretations of the data collected and offers some ideas for further study. 2 Background In order to study musical chords, several fundamental musical ideas must be understood. The first musical element is pitch. In musical terms, pitch refers to the highness or lowness of a sound. A sound wave is an oscillatory disturbance of some medium that travels as a wave through that medium, such as a pressure wave propagation through the air (Hecht, 2000). In the simplest case, such a wave can be modeled with a sinusoidal function, such as y=sin(x). With the sinusoidal wave, the pitch is represented by the period, or the amount of it takes for one cycle of the wave to occur, measured in seconds per cycle. Inversely, one could measure the number of oscillations which occur in one second. This is called frequency, measured in Hertz, which is cycles per second. Thus, frequency is the inverse of period. Lower pitches have a lower frequency and higher pitches have a higher frequency (Benade, 1960). Now that we have pitches, we can begin to build chords. Simply stated, a musical chord is just several pitches sounded simultaneously. More precisely, we define a musical chord as consisting of three or more unique pitches, none of which are the same note in a different octave (Kamien, 1996). Mathematically, this means no two pitches in the chord have frequencies that are related by a factor of two or a power of two. Measuring frequencies in Hertz (cycles per second) we require that 3 For all pairs of pitches f i and f j in a chord, there exists NO integer n such that fi 2 n f j The intervals, or frequency differences, between the pitches of a chord determine the chord’s quality, with the most common chord qualities in order of diminishing consonance being major, minor, diminished and augmented (Rossing, Moore & Wheeler, 2002). A major chord is a chord with a root note, a note a major third (or 4 half-steps) above it and a note a perfect fifth (or 7 half-steps) above the root note. A half-step is the smallest interval between notes in western music, such as D to D# or E to F. A minor chord is built using a root note, a note a minor third (or 3 halfsteps) above it and a note a perfect fifth (or 7 half-steps) above the root note. The diminished chord is made up of a root note and a minor third (3 half-steps) above it and a note a diminished fifth (or 6 half-steps) above the root note. The augmented chord is made up of a root note, a note a major third (or 4 half-steps) above it and a note an augmented fifth (or 8 half-steps) above the root note. The spellings of a C chord under each of these qualities is listed in the table below. Major G Spellings of a C Chord Minor Diminished Augmented G# G Gb E C E Eb Eb C C C Analyzing chords constructed from three unique pitches is more complicated than it might first appear. The complications arise as pairs of pitches in the chord interact with each other and with the listener’s perception of those pitches. When two pitches are sounded simultaneously, the pitches created by the sum and differences of the sinusoidal waves are also perceived (Benade, 1990). The sum and difference components can be seen in the equation of the sum of two sinusoidal functions: 4 cos2f 2 t cos2f1t 2sin 2 f 2 f1 t2 sin 2 f 2 f1 2t The presence of these tones were first discovered around 1714 by violinist Giuseppe Tartini, who used the difference tone to create pitches lower than the range of his violin, thus the difference tone is often called the Tartini tone (Grout & Palisca, 1996). As we shall see below, these perceived sum and difference frequencies will play an important role in the relative consonance or dissonance of a given chord. Now that we have an understanding of chords, we can talk about chord consonance and dissonance. Before we discuss our ideas on consonance and dissonance, we should discuss how people have previously attempted to these terms. Consonance is the pleasing quality of musical tone or chord. Hermann Ludwig Ferdinand von Helmholtz in 1877 described consonance by referring to Ohm’s acoustical law, which stated that the ear performs a spectral analysis of sound, separating a complex sound into its various partials (Rossing, Moore & Wheeler, 2002). A complex sound is a tone that includes a vibration at the frequency sounded-the fundamental, and at multiples of that frequency-the partials, corresponding to higher order modes of vibration of the system. The frequency’s partials are the integer multiples of the fundamental frequency formed by taking the original wavelength of a pitch, the fundamental, and dividing it into parts equal to the ratios given by the harmonic series: ½, 1/3, ¼, ….1/n. The analysis of these complex tones is also known as Fourier Analysis, after the mathematician Joseph Fourier (1768-1830). Fourier formulated the mathematical theorem that any periodic vibration, however complicated, can be built up from a series of simple, sinusoidal components, by choosing the proper amplitudes and phases of these components (Brown & Churchill, 2001). The Fourier Transform takes any periodic function, f(t), and transforms it into the distribution of frequencies, F(f), that make up the function F f f t e i 2 f t dt Conversely, dissonance is the unpleasing quality of musical tone. Helmholtz also concluded that dissonance occurs when partials of the two tones produce 30-40 beats per second. A beat is a pulsating sound that is produced when two pitches are 5 close together. The closer the two pitches are, the slower the beats are. These beats are simply the perceived difference frequencies, or Tartini tones described above. There is yet one more complication, namely, how to divide the octave into discrete steps, i.e. the mapping of note names onto specific frequencies. While this may seem trivial, the question of how to best do this has been debated for over 2000 years and is still disputed today. As we shall see, it also plays a role in mathematically assessing the consonance and dissonance of musical tones. The study of how music and mathematics relate has been traced by to the Ancient Greeks. Pythagorus was one of the first people to study the connection between mathematics and music. Boethius, the author of the first music theory text, written in the early years of the sixth century AD, wrote that Pythagorus observed the relation of the weights of hammers used by blacksmiths and the tones that the strikes of the hammers against their anvils produced. When a hammer was half the weight of another hammer, it produced a pitch one octave higher (or double the frequency) than the pitch produced by the hammer twice its weight. Pythagorus then went on to find the ratios between the weights of other hammers and the intervals produced by those pitches, focusing on creating true fourths and fifths, intervals common to Greek music (Appendix 1). Boethius’s music theory text, De institutione musica (The Fundamentals of Music), consisted of three parts. The first part consists of three books, which was based off of the writings of the Pythagoreans, the second part contains the writings of Euclid and Aristoxenus, and the last part is based on the writings of Ptolemy. Most readers came away with the message that music was a science of numbers and that numerical ratios determined the melodic intervals, consonances, composition of scales and the tuning of instruments and voices. The set of ratios used for tunings as described by Boethius was the most commonly used tuning until around 1025-28 AD when Guido of Arezzo simplified the ratios determined by Pythagorus by dividing a string into sections. In additional to being numerically simpler, these simplified tuning ratios also made intervals of thirds and sixths more pleasingly in tune at the expense of the consonance of fourths and fifths. Since the Church though intervals of thirds and sixths represented the Holy 6 Trinity and were, therefore, more divine, and since the Church was the major influence of musical development at the time, the new simplified tuning quickly gained prominence. These tuning were rigorously developed in 1482 by Spanish mathematician and music theorist Bartolome Ramos de Pareja. This system was then further developed by Franchino Gaffurio (1451-1522) into the system of tuning that is now known as the just intonation system. Though the just intonation system simplifies the ratios and improves the sound of the thirds and sixths, the sound of the fifths and fourths were compromised. The just intonation system was predominant until 1722 when Johann Sebastian Bach wrote a set of 24 preludes and fugues in all 24 keys, Das wohltemperirte Clavier (The Well-Tempered Piano), thus needing equal intervals between all pitches on the piano. In addition, most of the notes which created semitones between the musica ficta (white keys on the piano plus Bb), were dropped thus giving us our current 12 note chromatic scale. This new system created 12 equal intervals per octave with a constant frequency ration of 2^(n/12) between sequential notes. This system is referred to as the equal temperament system and is the system universally accepted today. There is still much controversy today in the music world as to which scale system is more pleasing and useful, the just temperament system or the equal temperament system (Grout & Palisca, 1996). Results Now that we have defined some of the basic ideas, we can begin to look at our research question, is there a mathematical trend in musical chords that correspond with consonance and dissonance. The first part of the research focused on finding the sum and difference frequencies for major, minor, diminished, and augmented chords using the equal temperament scale. The sum and difference frequency for each combination of two notes from the three that made up each chord was taken, thus giving three sum and three difference frequencies. These frequencies were then compared to the frequencies of actual pitches and a percentage of error was determined between the sum and difference frequencies and the frequencies of the actual pitch. If the error 7 percentage was less than one percent, the sum or difference frequency was considered significant. This was repeated twelve times, each time building the chord off each of the twelve musical notes. Note that there is no difference between major chords built off different pitches in the equal temperament system since the difference in frequency between the pitches are equal. Equal Temperament Since the frequency ratios of all sequential notes are the same in the equal temperament system, we can take one example, e.g. the C chord, and use it as a generalization for the rest. The significant pitches produced from the C major chord were from the sum of C and E, which resulted in a D (22/12), the sum of C and G, which resulted in an E (24/12), the difference between C and G, which resulted in a C (2) and the difference between E and G, which resulted in a B (211/12). The C and G that were perceived are pitches which are part of the original C major chord and as a result, reinforced those notes, and the D and B notes are other notes in the scale, but are not present in the chord. SUMS DIFFERENCES G F# E F B E D# D C D C# C For each of the chord qualities under the equal temperament system, different notes were reinforced and perceived. As stated above, for the major chord, the base 8 note and the 24/12 were reinforced and the 22/12 and 211/12 were perceived. For the minor chord, the base note and the 27/12 were reinforced and the 24/12 and the 25/12 were also perceived. For the diminished chord, no notes were reinforced, but the 27/12 and the 210/12 notes were perceived and for the augmented chord, the 28/12 note was reinforced and the 26/12 note was perceived. Unfortunately, though, since the intervals between notes in the equal temperament system are irrational frequencies, none of the sum and difference frequencies matched up exactly to actual frequencies that represented pitches. Regardless, there seems to be a correlation between consonance and occurrences of sum and difference frequencies. Just Intonation Then the entire process was repeated using notes from a scale created using the just intonation system. Since there are more than twelve musical notes in a diatonic scale created using the just intonation system, where there were two ratios that could represent a single note in the equal temperament system, the simpler of the two ratios were chosen. This was done because overtime, a common trend of new tuning systems was toward simpler frequency ratios. Unlike in the equal temperament system, frequency ratios between sequential notes in the just intonation system do not stay constant. Therefore, one example of a chord could not be used to represent all chords. Thus, sum and differences were generated for chords built from all possible base notes. The results of these sums and differences (along with the sums and differences from the equal temperament chords and the complex tones to follow) are listed in Appendix 2. Among all the major chords formed using each of the twelve tones as a root note, three principle patterns of sum and difference frequencies were observed. In one group of major chords the sum and differences reinforced the root note and the 5/4 note. In another group, the sum and differences reinforced the root note and the 5/4 note. In addition, the 9/8 note was also perceived. In the last group of major chords the root note and the 5/4 note were reinforced with the 15/8 note being perceived. 9 The minor chords had only one pattern run through some of the chords, which was the root note and the 6/5 note being reinforced with the 4/3 and the 8/5 notes being perceived. The rest of the minor chords which didn’t fit this single pattern also didn’t seem to form any other clear pattern amongst themselves. Similarly, the augmented and diminished chords both didn’t seem to form any patterns of reinforced or perceived notes. Overall, with chords built using a just intonation tuning system, as consonance decreased, the number of notes reinforced and perceived decreased. Also, as consonance decreased, the number of patterns between different chords of the same quality lessened and those patterns that were observed became less apparent. Complex Tones So far, the research has focused on looking at pitches made up of a single frequency. This was necessary to see what effect sum and difference tones had on the chords, but real musical tones are more complicated than this. Musical tones are made up of the frequency at the pitch being played, and the pitches that result from multiples of the given frequency. These notes are called overtones. This presented several problems. One was that there is essentially no upper limit to the overtone frequenciesthe overtones go on forever, so the decision was made to cut the overtones off after the seventh overtone. Also, the amplitudes of the overtones differ between instruments. Thus, the Fourier transform of the sound of several instruments was taken. Several instruments including a cello, trombone, clarinet, trumpet, voice, flute were recorded playing an “A” at 220 Hz as indicated below in their resulting Fourier transforms. Flute at 220 Hz 10 Voice at 220 Hz Cello at 220 Hz Trumpet at 220 Hz Clarinet at 220 Hz Trombone at 220 Hz There was not only a difference in the amplitude of the overtones between the different instruments, but there was also a difference in the range of frequencies sounded at each overtone (seen as the sharpness of the peaks). Thus, in addition to, the sum and difference frequencies between pairs of two pitches from the original three chord pitches, but also between the notes in the different overtones and between the range of the frequencies sounded at each overtone frequency. When the sum and difference frequencies of the overtones were looked at, results occurred which were similar to the results of the chords without overtones in that as consonance decreased, so did the number of pitches that reinforced the notes of the chord. Over two-thirds of the significant pitches formed by the sums and differences of the overtone frequencies reinforced the notes of the chord. Also, the lower sum and difference frequencies tended to reinforce the root pitch and the note a third above it 11 while the higher sum and difference frequencies tended to reinforce the note of the chord that was a fifth above the root in all four common chord qualities. Conclusion This research shows that there is a correlation between the perceived consonance of a musical chord and the types of sum and difference frequencies that are produced by those tones. In each of the cases, as consonance decreased, the number of overall sum and difference frequencies created decreased and the number of sum and difference frequencies that reinforced the pitches in the original chord also decreased. Since the differences between chords formed using the equal temperament system and chords formed using the just intonation system were looked at, we can also conclude from our results that if as the number of frequencies of sum and difference pitches that match up to frequencies of actual pitches increases, consonance increases, then the just intonation system of tuning provides more consonant tunings of chords than the equal temperament system of tuning does. One inference that can be derived from our data is that since the frequency that relates to the pitch a major seventh (11 half-steps) above the root note often appeared in the sum and difference frequencies, this could explain why seventh chords were developed and occur frequently in music. Another inference that could be made is that the switch from the just intonation system to the equal temperament system facilitated the development of atonal music, or music that does not conform to our standard notions of a scale or key. This inference is supported by the fact that the widespread switch in tuning systems occurred just prior to the development of atonal music and that none of the sum and difference frequencies of a chord created using the equal temperament system match up to actual pitches, thus desensitizing listeners of the time to dissonance. There are many extensions to the research presented here. One major extension could be to look at the amplitudes of the overtone frequencies produced by different instruments and establish a weight to the sum and difference frequencies produced by the different combinations of overtones. This could help to show which overtones are 12 better perceived by the listener and perhaps give a more reliable way to set an upper boundary for the study of overtones which are significant to the perception of consonance and dissonance. If this study of amplitudes is done, one could study a chord formed by producing different notes of a chord on different instruments and by looking at the sum and difference frequencies between the instruments. This could be extremely useful in the study of orchestration as a means of determining the best voicing for an arrangement of a piece of music. There are several other extensions. One extension would be to try to use the sum and difference frequencies to better simulate musical sounds created electronically. Another extension would be to try to find a set of ratios that would create a musical scale which would have the optimal amount of sum and difference frequencies that matched up to actual pitches. Also, pieces of music could be analyzed by not only looking at the pitches written, but also at the sum and difference pitches perceived and possibly derive a deeper understanding of certain works. 13 References: Benade, Arthur H. (1990). Fundamentals of Musical Acoustics. 2nd revised ed. New York: Dover Publications, Inc. Benade, Arthur H. (1960). Horns, Strings, and Harmony. Garden City, NY: Doubleday and Company, Inc. Brown, James Ward & Ruel V. Churchill. (2001). Fourier Series and Boundary Value Problems. 6th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill Higher Education. Grout, Donald J. & Claude V. Palisca. (1996). A History of Western Music. 5th ed. New York: W. W. Norton and Company. Hecht, Eugene. (2000). Physics: Calculus. 2nd ed. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. Kamien, Roger. (1996). Music: An Appreciation. 6th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill. Rigden, John S. (1977). Physics and the Sound of Music. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Rossing, Thomas D., F. Richard Moore & Paul A. Wheeler. (2002). The Science of Sound. 3rd ed. San Francisco: Addison Wesley. 14 APPENDIX 1 This table shows the relationships between the note names and their corresponding ratio in each tuning system. In the equal temperament system, some of the notes are combined and thus share the same ratio (Rigden, 1977). Note Name C C# Db D D# Eb E E# Fb F F# Gb G G# Ab A A# Bb B B# Cb C Pythagorean ratio Just intonation ratio 1 1 2187/2048 25/24 256/243 16/15 9/8 9/8 19683/16384 75/64 32/27 6/5 81/64 5/4 177147/131072 125/96 8192/6561 32/25 4/3 4/3 729/512 45/32 1024/729 36/25 3/2 3/2 6561/4096 25/16 128/81 8/5 27/16 5/3 59049/32768 225/128 16/9 9/5 243/128 15/8 531441/262144 125/64 4096/2187 48/25 2 2 15 Equal temperament ratio 1 2^(1/12) 2^(2/12) 2^(3/12) 2^(4/12) 2^(5/12) 2^(6/12) 2^(7/12) 2^(8/12) 2^(9/12) 2^(10/12) 2^(11/12) 2 APPENDIX 2 This appendix shows the sum and difference frequencies for each set of two notes within a major chord and its overtones formed using the equal temperament system of tuning as an exmaple of the work done to find the sum and difference frequencies. Due to the length and number of spreadsheets necessary to find all the data we collected, the other spreadsheets are not printed here. For more information on the other spreadsheets, please contact the author. Sums Frequency Actual Pitch Percent Error 1100 C#/Db-6 0.79 1320 E-6 0.11 1540 1.82 1760 A-6 0.00 497.18263 B-4 0.66 774.36526 1.24 1051.54789 C-6 0.48 1328.73052 E-6 0.77 1605.91315 2.36 1883.09578 A#/Bb-6 0.98 2160.27841 2.65 549.62756 C#/Db-5 0.86 879.25512 A-5 0.08 1208.88268 2.83 1538.51024 1.92 1868.1378 A#/Bb-6 0.19 2197.76536 C#/Db-7 0.90 2527.39292 1.52 1320 E-6 0.11 1540 1.82 1760 A-6 0.00 1980 B-6 0.23 717.18263 2.61 994.36526 B-5 0.66 1271.54789 2.13 1548.73052 1.24 1825.91315 2.12 2103.09578 C-7 0.48 2380.27841 1.30 769.62756 1.87 16 Differences Frequency Actual Pitch Percent Error 660 E-5 0.11 880 A-5 0.00 1100 C#/Db-6 0.79 1320 E-6 0.11 57.18263 1.90 334.36526 1.42 611.54789 1.75 888.73052 A-5 0.98 1165.91315 D-6 0.75 1443.09578 2.56 1720.27841 2.31 109.62756 A-2 0.34 439.25512 A-4 0.17 768.88268 1.96 1098.51024 C#/Db-6 0.93 1428.1378 2.19 1757.76536 A-6 0.13 2087.39292 C-7 0.27 440 A-4 0.00 660 E-5 0.11 880 A-5 0.00 1100 C#/Db-6 0.79 162.81737 1.23 114.36526 1.90 391.54789 G-4 0.11 668.73052 1.42 945.91315 1.44 1223.09578 1.75 1500.27841 1.35 110.37244 A-2 0.34 1099.25512 1428.88268 1758.51024 2088.1378 2417.76536 2747.39292 1540 1760 1980 2200 937.18263 1214.36526 1491.54789 1768.73052 2045.91315 2323.09578 2600.27841 989.62756 1319.25512 1648.88268 1978.51024 2308.1378 2637.76536 2967.39292 1980 2200 2420 1157.18263 1434.36526 1711.54789 1988.73052 2265.91315 2543.09578 2820.27841 1209.62756 1539.25512 1868.88268 2198.51024 2528.1378 2857.76536 3187.39292 2420 2640 1377.18263 1654.36526 C#/Db-6 A-6 C-7 A-6 B-6 C#/Db-7 A#/Bb-5 F#/Gb-6 A-6 B-5 E-6 G#/Ab-6 B-6 E-7 F#/Gb-7 B-6 C#/Db-7 B-6 F-7 A#/Bb-6 C#/Db-7 E-7 G#/Ab-6 0.86 2.24 0.08 0.23 2.83 1.69 1.82 0.00 0.23 0.79 0.52 2.48 0.78 0.49 2.30 1.13 1.41 0.19 0.06 0.75 0.15 1.78 0.03 0.25 0.23 0.79 2.85 1.51 2.61 2.83 0.66 2.14 2.13 0.94 2.88 1.87 0.23 0.86 1.55 2.24 1.61 2.85 0.11 1.43 0.41 219.25512 548.88268 878.51024 1208.1378 1537.76536 1867.39292 220 440 660 880 382.81737 105.63474 171.54789 448.73052 725.91315 1003.09578 1280.27841 330.37244 0.74488 328.88268 658.51024 988.1378 1317.76536 1647.39292 220 440 660 602.81737 325.63474 48.45211 228.73052 505.91315 783.09578 1060.27841 550.37244 220.74488 108.88268 438.51024 768.1378 1097.76536 1427.39292 220 440 822.81737 545.63474 17 A-3 C#/Db-5 A-5 A#/Bb-6 A-3 A-4 E-5 A-5 E-4 #N/A E-4 E-5 B-5 E-6 G#/Ab-6 A-3 A-4 E-5 G-5 C#/Db-5 A-3 A-4 C#/Db-6 A-3 A-4 G#/Ab-5 0.34 1.00 0.17 2.77 1.96 0.15 0.00 0.00 0.11 0.00 2.40 1.71 1.79 1.95 1.94 1.53 2.79 0.23 #N/A 0.23 0.11 0.04 0.06 0.84 0.00 0.00 0.11 2.57 1.23 1.13 1.90 2.38 0.11 1.30 0.73 0.34 1.03 0.34 2.06 1.00 2.14 0.00 0.00 0.95 1.60 1931.54789 2208.73052 2485.91315 2763.09578 3040.27841 1429.62756 1759.25512 2088.88268 2418.51024 2748.1378 3077.76536 3407.39292 2860 1597.18263 1874.36526 2151.54789 2428.73052 2705.91315 2983.09578 3260.27841 1649.62756 1979.25512 2308.88268 2638.51024 2968.1378 3297.76536 3627.39292 1817.18263 2094.36526 2371.54789 2648.73052 2925.91315 3203.09578 3480.27841 1869.62756 2199.25512 2528.88268 2858.51024 3188.1378 3517.76536 3847.39292 831.54789 1108.73052 1385.91315 1663.09578 C#/Db-7 D#/Eb-7 A-6 C-7 A#/Bb-6 F#/Gb-7 G#/Ab-6 B-6 E-7 F#/Gb-7 G#/Ab-7 C-7 D-7 E-7 A#/Bb-6 C#/Db-7 A-7 G#/Ab-5 C#/Db-6 F-6 G#/Ab-6 2.28 0.40 0.12 1.11 2.64 2.29 0.04 0.20 2.86 1.66 1.89 2.49 2.31 1.83 0.52 2.72 2.48 2.55 0.78 1.91 0.70 0.19 1.75 0.06 0.28 0.75 2.81 2.61 0.06 0.94 0.44 1.16 2.10 1.14 0.27 0.83 1.58 2.26 1.64 0.06 2.69 0.11 0.00 0.79 0.11 268.45211 8.73052 285.91315 563.09578 840.27841 770.37244 440.74488 111.11732 218.51024 548.1378 877.76536 1207.39292 220 1042.81737 765.63474 488.45211 211.26948 65.91315 343.09578 620.27841 990.37244 660.74488 331.11732 1.48976 328.1378 657.76536 987.39292 1262.81737 985.63474 708.45211 431.26948 154.08685 123.09578 400.27841 1210.37244 880.74488 551.11732 221.48976 108.1378 437.76536 767.39292 277.18263 554.36526 831.54789 1108.73052 18 #N/A A-4 A-2 A-3 A-5 A-3 C-6 C-2 D#/Eb-5 B-5 E-5 E-4 #N/A E-4 E-5 B-5 B-5 D#/Eb-3 B-2 A-5 C#/Db-5 A-3 A-4 C#/Db-4 C#/Db-5 G#/Ab-5 C#/Db-6 2.54 #N/A 2.71 1.55 1.15 1.77 0.17 1.01 0.68 1.14 0.25 2.71 0.00 0.35 2.40 1.11 1.71 0.77 1.79 0.32 0.26 0.23 0.45 #N/A 0.45 0.23 0.04 1.45 0.22 1.41 2.02 0.96 0.30 2.07 2.82 0.08 0.59 0.67 1.72 0.51 2.16 0.00 0.00 0.11 0.00 1940.27841 2217.46104 606.81019 936.43775 1266.06531 1595.69287 1925.32043 2254.94799 2584.57555 1385.91315 1663.09578 1940.27841 2217.46104 2494.64367 883.99282 1213.62038 1543.24794 1872.8755 2202.50306 2532.13062 2861.75818 1940.27841 2217.46104 2494.64367 2771.8263 1161.17545 1490.80301 1820.43057 2150.05813 2479.68569 2809.31325 3138.94081 2494.64367 2771.8263 3049.00893 1438.35808 1767.98564 2097.6132 2427.24076 2756.86832 3086.49588 3416.12344 3049.00893 3326.19156 1715.54071 C#/Db-7 A#/Bb-5 F-6 G#/Ab-6 C#/Db-7 D#/Eb-7 A-5 A#/Bb-6 C#/Db-7 C#/Db-7 D#/Eb-7 F-7 F#/Gb-6 D#/Eb-7 F-7 G-7 D#/Eb-7 F-7 A-6 C-7 G#/Ab-7 1.82 0.00 2.55 0.44 1.70 1.74 2.61 1.66 2.03 0.79 0.11 1.82 0.00 0.23 0.45 2.55 1.60 0.44 0.68 1.70 2.37 1.82 0.00 0.23 0.79 1.16 0.73 2.43 2.65 0.38 0.55 0.09 0.23 0.79 2.85 2.88 0.45 0.22 2.55 1.34 1.60 2.74 2.85 0.11 2.59 1385.91315 1663.09578 52.44493 382.07249 711.70005 1041.32761 1370.95517 1700.58273 2030.21029 277.18263 554.36526 831.54789 1108.73052 1385.91315 224.7377 104.88986 434.51742 764.14498 1093.77254 1423.4001 1753.02766 277.18263 554.36526 831.54789 1108.73052 501.92033 172.29277 157.33479 486.96235 816.58991 1146.21747 1475.84503 277.18263 554.36526 831.54789 779.10296 449.4754 119.84784 209.77972 539.40728 869.03484 1198.6624 277.18263 554.36526 1056.28559 19 F-6 G#/Ab-6 G#/Ab-1 C-6 C#/Db-4 C#/Db-5 G#/Ab-5 C#/Db-6 F-6 G#/Ab-2 A-6 C#/Db-4 C#/Db-5 G#/Ab-5 C#/Db-6 F#/Gb-6 C#/Db-4 C#/Db-5 G#/Ab-5 G-5 G#/Ab-3 C#/Db-4 C#/Db-5 C-6 0.79 0.11 1.01 2.60 1.86 0.50 1.89 2.31 2.69 0.00 0.00 0.11 0.00 0.79 2.11 1.01 1.26 2.60 1.37 1.86 0.40 0.00 0.00 0.11 0.00 1.60 1.35 1.13 1.42 1.72 2.48 0.28 0.00 0.00 0.11 0.63 2.11 2.76 1.01 2.77 1.26 2.00 0.00 0.00 0.93 2045.16827 2374.79583 2704.42339 3034.05095 3363.67851 3693.30607 3603.37419 1992.72334 2322.3509 2651.97846 2981.60602 3311.23358 3640.86114 3970.4887 2269.90597 2599.53353 2929.16109 3258.78865 3588.41621 3918.04377 4247.67133 988.88268 1318.51024 1648.1378 1977.76536 2307.39292 2637.02048 1648.1378 1977.76536 2307.39292 2637.02048 2966.64804 2307.39292 2637.02048 2966.64804 3296.2756 2966.64804 3296.2756 3625.90316 3625.90316 3955.53072 4285.15828 497.18263 994.36526 1491.54789 A#/Bb-7 B-6 E-7 F#/Gb-7 G#/Ab-7 B-7 B-7 B-5 E-6 G#/Ab-6 B-6 E-7 G#/Ab-6 B-6 E-7 F#/Gb-7 E-7 F#/Gb-7 G#/Ab-7 F#/Gb-7 G#/Ab-7 B-7 B-4 B-5 F#/Gb-6 2.34 1.07 2.49 2.44 1.23 0.97 2.31 0.86 1.16 0.56 0.73 0.34 2.43 0.49 2.31 1.44 1.05 1.95 1.91 0.84 1.45 0.11 0.00 0.79 0.11 1.82 0.00 0.79 0.11 1.82 0.00 0.23 1.82 0.00 0.23 0.79 0.23 0.79 2.85 2.85 0.11 2.31 0.66 0.66 0.78 726.65803 397.03047 67.40291 262.22465 591.85221 921.47977 277.18263 1333.46822 1003.84066 674.2131 344.58554 14.95798 314.66958 644.29714 1610.65085 1281.02329 951.39573 621.76817 292.14061 37.48695 367.11451 329.62756 659.25512 988.88268 1318.51024 1648.1378 1977.76536 329.62756 659.25512 988.88268 1318.51024 1648.1378 329.62756 659.25512 988.88268 1318.51024 329.62756 659.25512 988.88268 329.62756 659.25512 329.62756 57.18263 114.36526 171.54789 20 C-4 D-5 C#/Db-4 #N/A D#/Eb-5 D-4 F#/Gb-4 E-4 E-5 B-5 E-6 G#/Ab-6 B-6 E-4 E-5 B-5 E-6 G#/Ab-6 E-4 E-5 B-5 E-6 E-4 E-5 B-5 E-4 E-5 E-4 1.83 1.27 2.81 0.23 0.76 1.18 0.00 1.12 1.60 2.22 1.35 #N/A 1.13 2.32 2.65 2.85 2.00 0.08 0.52 2.08 0.78 0.00 0.00 0.11 0.00 0.79 0.11 0.00 0.00 0.11 0.00 0.79 0.00 0.00 0.11 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.11 0.00 0.00 0.00 1.90 1.90 1.79 1988.73052 2485.91315 2983.09578 3480.27841 549.62756 1099.25512 1648.88268 2198.51024 2748.1378 3297.76536 3847.39292 606.81019 1213.62038 1820.43057 2427.24076 3034.05095 3640.86114 4247.67133 B-6 D#/Eb-7 F#/Gb-7 C#/Db-5 C#/Db-6 G#/Ab-6 C#/Db-7 G#/Ab-7 0.66 0.12 0.78 1.14 0.86 0.86 0.75 0.86 1.66 0.75 2.69 2.55 2.55 2.43 2.55 2.44 2.43 6.98 228.73052 285.91315 343.09578 400.27841 109.62756 219.25512 328.88268 438.51024 548.1378 657.76536 767.39292 52.44493 104.88986 157.33479 209.77972 262.22465 314.66958 367.11451 21 A-2 A-3 E-4 A-4 E-5 G#/Ab-1 G#/Ab-2 G#/Ab-3 C-4 F#/Gb-4 1.90 2.71 1.79 2.07 0.34 0.34 0.23 0.34 1.14 0.23 2.16 1.01 1.01 1.13 1.01 0.23 1.13 0.78