Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 26, No. 6, 1996 Hand Preference and Hand Skill in Children with Autism K. M. Cornish ~ University of Leicester I. C. M c M a n u s University College, London Hand preference and hand skill was assessed in two broad age groups of children with autism, children with learning disabilities, and control schoolchildren. The first group comprised children ages 3-5 years and the second group of children ages 11-13 years. Degree of handedness remained relatively stable across age groups, particularly within the autistic and learning-disabled populations. The main difference was between the subject groups, with the normal controls more lateralized than either the children with autism or children with learning disabilities. As with degree of handedness, consistency of handedness also differed significantly between subject groups with the normal controls more consistent in their hand preference than the other two groups. In addition, younger children were less consistent in their hand preference than older children. However, the present study found no evidence of a dissociation of hand skill and hand preference in children with autism compared to children with learning disabilities and normal developing children. INTRODUCTION An increased incidence of left handedness (LH) in children with autism compared to children in the normal population is well documented in the literature (e.g., Gillberg, 1983; Leboyer, Osherham, Nostem, & Robertou,x, 1988; Tsai, 1983; Soper et al., 1986), and as such is consistent 1Address all correspondence to Kim Cornish, Room B556, Faculty of Medicine, B Floor, Medical School, Queens Medical Centre, Nottingham NG7 2UH, United Kingdom. 597 0162-3257/96/1200-0597509.50/09 1996PlenumPublishing Corporation 598 Cornish and MeManus with the high prevalence frequently reported in children with moderate to severe learning difficulties (e.g., Pipe, 1988). A theory explaining this difference which continues to receive considerable attention is that of Satz and colleagues (e.g., Satz, Orsini, Saslow, & Henry, 1985; Satz, Soper, & Orsini, 1988; Soper & Satz, 1984). They argue that brain insult sustained early in development results in manual dominance switching from the right to the left hand, and that despite brain insults occurring randomly to right or left hemisphere, the outcome is nevertheless asymmetric since most individuals are left hemisphere dominant, and therefore "natural" right-handers are more likely to switch hand preference to become "pathological left-handers" (PLH) than vice versa (Satz, 1972). Additionally other aspects of lateralization involving speech and language may be affected (e.g., Satz et aL, 1985). The linking of left-hemisphere insult, learning disability, and raised LH has become attractive in the autistic literature since the primary deficits associated with autism seem to be those requiring left-hemisphere function (i.e., language comprehension, vocabulary, and phonology), whereas righthemisphere functions (i.e., visuospatial skills) remain relatively unaffected (e.g., see Fein, Humes Keplin, Lucci, & Waterhouse, 1984, for a review). However clinical studies in children with autism have consistently failed to confirm this hypothesis, reporting either bilateral damage (e.g., Tsai & Stewart, 1982) or right hemisphere insult (e.g., Gillberg, 1983). As well as having an increased rate of LH, children with autism have also been reported to have a higher incidence of ambiguous or inconsistent handedness (AH), an effect originally claimed to be specific to the condition (e.g., Soper et al., 1986). AH refers to reduced consistency of hand preference within tasks (e.g., preferring to carry out task A with the right hand on one occasion and with the left hand on a second occasion), and should not be confused with reduced degree of hand preference between tasks, in which one hand is preferred for task A and the other hand preferred for task B. The two are however partially confounded statistically (McManus, 1996; McManus, Murray, Doyle, & Baron-Cohen, 1992). According to Soper et al. (1986); AH is distinct from PLH and probably resuits from early bilateral hemisphere deficit rendering neither hemisphere sufficient for manual dominance. A review by Satz, Green, and Lyon (1989) estimated that AH accounts for approximately 40% of the handedness observed in individuals with autism and individuals with learning difficulties but is relatively rare in normal populations above the age of 4 years. However, in a study by McManus et al. (1992), AH was significantly increased in children with autism compared to a mentally retarded control group and normal control group, even when degree of handedness, age, and mental age were taken into account. It should also be noted that differences in Hand Preference and Hand Skill 599 hand preference (be they direction, degree, or consistency) and assessed typically by reaching, should not be confused with differences in hand skill, assessed typically by a task such as the Annett Pegboard, and in which one can also assess the direction of the more skillful hand, and the degree of difference ~between the hands. A third anomaly of handedness recently reported in children with autism is a dissociation between hand skill and hand preference (McManus et aL, 1992). Children with autism demonstrated a high level of discordance between hand preference and hand skill, 50% of them preferring to use the hand which was actually less skilled on the Annett Pegboard task (Annett, 1970). In contrast, almost all subjects in the control groups showed greater hand skill with the preferred hand. A problem of past research highlighted by Satz et al. (1985) is the tendency to use inadequate methodology in determining hand preference in autistic samples. In particular, the failure to assess AH using repeated testing within preference items. The present study employed a standard performance battery (McManus et al., 1988) that measured consistency and degree of handedness. The McManus et al. study matched children with autism and controls solely on the basis of mental age. In consequence, the children with autism were about 11 years old, considerably older than the normal controls (mean age 4.9 years). The present study was designed principally to measure the handedness of young children with autism and to assess their patterns of hand usage (and indeed to assess the feasibility of measuring handedness in that group) (Fein et aL, 1984). As such it can be seen as an extension of the McManus et al. study, so that when combined with it, along with group of older control subjects, a complete factorial design would arise by age and diagnostic group. It should therefore be possible to tell whether hand usage in younger autistic children is also atypical, or whether the pattern is only present in the older children. METHOD The present study consists of a development and an extension of the work of McManus et al. (1992); in particular additional groups of subjects were tested and then combined with the original subjects of McManus et al. (1992) to allow a more comprehensive analysis. 600 Cornish and McManus Participants Participants were in two broad age groups: Younger children, in the range 3-5 years (children with autism, age 4-5 years, n = 15; children with learning disabilities, age 4-5 years, n = 14; and control schoolchildren at ordinary schools, age 3-5 years, n = 65); and Older children, in the age 11-13 years (children with autism, age 12-13 years, n = 20; children with learning disabilities, age 11-12 years, n = 12; and control schoolchildren at ordinary schools, age 12-13 years, n = 24). Some of the younger control children, along with all of the older children with learning disabilities and children with autism are the same as those reported previously in McManus et al. (1992). All the autistic children were receiving special education in units for children with autism and were diagnosed according to established criteria (e.g., Rutter, 1978). None of the participants had any obvious physical handicap that would influence their hand preference. The new participants in the present study were resident in Northamptonshire, and in all cases were tested individually in their schools. Intellectual level in the younger children in all groups who had not been assessed in the previous study was measured by the Merrill-Palmer Scale of Mental Tests. Tests of intellectual ability were not administered to the older group of controls since the Merrill-Palmer is not suitable for that age group, and instead they were assumed to be typical of children in general of their age. Tasks Hand preference was assessed by a battery of 10 symmetrically presented unimanual preference tasks (e.g., coloring a square, eating with a spoon, picking up a sweet) (based on the items of McManus et al., 1988). The battery was repeated three times during the course of the testing session. Responses were scored as Right (R), Left (L) or Bimanual/Mixed (B). For each participant a Laterality Index (LI) was calculated across all items as: 100 (n(R)-n(L)/n(R)+n(L)+n(B)). The LI was partitioned into measures of direction of hand preference (LI < 0 for left handed; LI > 0 for right handed) and degree of hand preference (ILII, the absolute laterality index). Consistency of hand preference was calculated across all pairs of replications of each item (lst with 2nd, 2nd with 3rd, 1st with 3rd), two tasks were considered consistent only if the right hand was preferred on both occasions or the left hand was preferred on both occasions. Since in the present paper only 10 items were scored, whereas Hand Preference and Hand Skill 601 in McManus et al. (1992), 13 items were scored, there are minor differences in the results shown in Table I from those reported previously. It should be noted here that since, particularly among the younger subjects, a moderate number of bilateral or mixed preferences were given, the result is that laterality scores can take any value in the range 0-100%; this is in distinction to the slightly different definition used by McManus et al. (1992), where of necessity scores are constrained to the range 33-100%. It should also be noted that Table I of McManus et al. (1992) contains three minor typographic errors (4.3 should be 7.5 for the SD of average time on pegboard in the autistic group; and 9.9 and 9.6 should be reversed in the penultimate line), and the table in the present paper corrects those errors. H a n d skill asymmetry was assessed using the Annett Pegboard (Annett, 1970). This measured the difference in time for the left and the right hands to move a row of 10 dowelling pegs from one set of holes to another. The pegboard task was carried out twice with each hand, either in the order RLLR or LRRL. Several measures of performance were calculated. The average time to carry out the four tasks, the average time with the right and with the left hand, the difference between the average time with the right and the left hand, and the average time for the better hand and the worse hand. Additionally a standardized Skill A s y m m e t r y Index (SAI) was calculated using the formula (100(LR ) / ( L + R ) for the difference between the right and left hands, and additionally an analogous score (see Table I ) w a s calculated for the difference between the better and the worse hand. Finally, the direction of skill asymmetry was calculated as right if SAI was greater than zero and left if SAI was less than or equal to zero. Preference and skill were defined as concordant if the direction of skill lateralization was the same as the direction of preference lateralization. ~S~TS Intellectual Level. The young controls had a mean (SD; range; n) score on the Merrill-Palmer of 76.2 (13.6; 31-94; n = 43, 2 subjects would not cooperate with testing), the young children with autism had a mean of 47.2 (21.4; 26-81; n = 13, 2 subjects could not be tested), and the children with learning disability had a mean of 23.4 (8.4; 4-42; n = 14). A comparison between the groups using one-way ANOVA showed highly significant differences overall, F(2, 67) = 75.9, p < .001, with all three groups differing from all other groups using a Scheff6 post hoe comparison. 6.15 74.7 (26,5) 69,8 (27.5) 23.3 (4.7) 21.9 (4.9) 24,7 (5.0) 21.6 (4.6) 25,1 (5.1) 6.2 (6.2) Direction of handedness (% left-handed) Consistency of handedness Degree of handedness a Average time on pegboard (seconds) Right hand (time on pegboard) Left hand (time on pegboard) Better hand (time on pegboard) Worse hand (time on pegboard) Pegdiff (L - R) (time on pegboard) e II II 6.1 (11.6) 34.8 (12,7) 28.3' (9.5) 33.5 (12.9) 29.6 (9.9) 31.6 (10.8) 45,4 (31.7) 53.0 (3.1) 33,3 "= 0.0 r 2.7 (9.2) 51.6 (10.2) 44.7 (11.6) 50.4 (11.5) 47.0 (11.7) 48.2 (10.7) 48,9 (32.2) 52.4 (32.7) r Younger children II 0.0 II II 63.1 (15.4) 15.0 ~ 3,0 (5.5) 11.8 (1.2) 10.5 (1.0) 11.5 (1,3) 10.8 (1.2) 11.2 (1.1) 3.6 (11.9) 24.3 (8.9) 19.6 (6.3) 21.3 (8.9) 22.6 (7.2) 21.9 (7.5) 79.1 ~ 48.0 (30.9) (23.5) 82.8 (31.1) ~ 1,4 (12.4) 30.1 (7.1) 24.5 (3.4) 27.4 (4.0) 272 (Z9) 27.3 (4.8) 49.9 (25.1) 75.5 (9.6) 25.0 ~- Older children Table I, Comparison of Participants by Group ~ ns < .001 < .001 < .001 < .001 < .001 < .001 < .001 < .01 < .003 < .001 < .001 < ,001 < ,001 < .001 ns < .01 ns = ns < .001 < .001 < .02 < .001 < .003 ns ns < .01 x Significance tests (nominal significance-level) e'L m- ! O 2.8 (3.0) 17.2 Skill asymmetry index (SAI)g Discordant hand preference and skill (%) 20.6 3.9 (7.7) 10.1 (8.0) 0.6 (1.2) 20.0 30.8 5.4 (2.9) 2.3 (8.0) 7.8 (5.2) a Numbers in parentheses are standard deviations. bTwenty controls from McManus et ai. (1992). c Data from McManus et al. (1992). d Mean time for all four trials, two using the left hand and two the right hand. e Expressed as LPeg - RPeg. fExpressed as 100" (Worse - Better)/(Worse + Better). gExpressed as 100" ( L P e g - RPeg)/(LPeg + RPeg). 7.5 (4.4) Better hand vs. Worse hand (time on pegboard) f 50.0 1.3 (6.1) 10.0 (7.1) 16.7 0.1 (8.1) 9.6 (7.4) < as ns .01 ns < .001 ns us ns ns gl, 604 Cornish and McManus Table I provides a summary of results across age groups and between subject groups. Statistical analysis was by analysis of variance in all cases except the two binary dependent variables, direction of handedness and discordant hand preference and skill, where logistic regression was used instead; the two methods are however equivalent in their interpretation. Table I contains 33 statistical tests, not all of which are totally independent, and we therefore assume there are about 20 degrees of freedom overall. We have used a conventional (adjusted) alpha level of .05. However a strict interpretation of the table using a Bonferroni correction would accept as significant only those results meeting an unadjusted significance level of .05/20 = .001. In practice this produces few substantive changes in the interpretation of the data. Hand Preference. Eight of 35 (23%) children with autism, 3 of 26 (11.5) children with learning disability, and 4 of 90 (4.4%) of control children showed a left-handed preference. Logistic regression of handedness, showed no main effect of age, X2(1),p = .04, ns); a significant main effect of group, X2(2) = 9.24, p = .010; and a significant Group x Age interaction, ~2(2) = 9.01, p = .011, none of which would be significant applying a Bonferroni correction. The incidence of left handedness in the groups is however consistent with those reported elsewhere (see Fein et aL, 1984). Degree of Handedness. This differed significantly between the three subject groups, F(2, 145) = 14.26, p < .001, with the normal controls more lateralized than either the children with autism or the learning-disabled controis. The latter two groups did not differ in their degree of lateralization, F(1, 57) = 0.134, ns). There was no main effect of age, F(1, 145) = 1.445, ns), nor an interaction with age, F(2, 145) = 0.278, ns). Consistency of Handedness. This differed between the groups, F(2, 145) = 9.255, p = .001, with controls being more consistent than the other two groups; there was no significant difference between children with autism and children with learning disability, F(1, 57) = 0.937, ns). Younger children were less consistent than older children, F(1, 145) = 6.334, p = .013, although there was no interaction between age and group, F(1, 145) = 0.783, ns (see Table I). Again a Bonferroni correction would nullify the age effect. Hand Skill Asymmetry. The average time taken to complete the Annett Pegboard differed significantly between the groups, F(1, 143) = 110.18, p < .001, with the normal controls significantly faster than the autistic group and the learning disability group at each age group, the interaction not being significant after taking the Bonferroni correction into account. The differences betweerf groups at the younger age remain significant after taking differences in Merrill-Palmer into account using analysis of covariance, F(2, 64) = 13.4, p < .001. The distribution of the L-R difference scores did not differ across Hand Preference and Hand Skill 605 the three groups, F(2, 143) = 2.09, ns), as neither did the SAI, although the difference between the better and worse hand was significantly greater in the autistic group compared to the control groups, F(2, 145) = 4.65, p = .011, before Bonferroni correction). Analysis of just the younger children using Schefft's test showed that autistic children were significantly (p < .05) faster than learning-disabled controls and slower than normal developing controls on total pegboard time, time with right and with left hand, and with better and worse hand; however there were no differences between groups in difference scores between hands. Skill-Preference Concordance. In each of the groups there were individuals who showed discordance between the direction of hand preference and the direction of hand skill (see Table I). However, logistic regression showed that there was no main effect of age, X2(1) = 2.03, ns, there was no significant effect of group, Z2(2) = 3.66, ns, and there was no significant Group • Age interaction, Z2(2) = 3.41, ns. DISCUSSION This study shows clearly that handedness, both measured as preference and as skill using the Annett Pegboard, can straightforwardly be measured in children with autism and children with learning disorders who are as young as 4 years of age, and it is clear that the younger children with autism are slower overall on the pegboard task, although not as slow as the children with learning disorders, even after taking Merrill-Palmer scores into account. Hand Preference. In accordance with previous findings (e.g., Soper et al., 1986), the present study found a nonsignificant increase in the incidence of left-hand preference in children with autism and children with learning disabilities, although the number of left-handed autistic children was twice that of learning-disabled children and five times the incidence of normal developing children; however it must be said that the incidence of LH in our control group (4/90; 4.44%) was somewhat lower than is typically found in other studies. One possible explanation of the raised incidence of LH is that children with autism, in addition to the learning disabled, demonstrate a high incidence of PLH to the extent suggested by Satz and colleagues (e.g., Satz et aL, 1989). However, as noted by Bishop (1990), observing an increased rate of LH may not be indicative of pathological handedness but may be attributable to other factors, including developmental motor delay. Indeed, in the present study when hand preference is assessed in the two age groups separately, left-handed preference decreases from 33% in the younger children with autism to 15% in the older children with autism; a level that falls within the normally accepted range (albeit an incidence which is three times that in our normal controls). 606 Cornish and McManus Degree of Hand Preference. Degree of hand preference remained relatively stable across age groups, particularly within the autistic and learning-disabled children. Although nonsignifi~mt the increase in degree of hand preference from 3 years to 12 years in the normal controls is consistent with findings reported elsewhere (McManus et aL, 1988). As with consistency of hand preference, degree of hand preference did not differ significantly between children with autism and children with learning disabilities but was more lateralized in the normal controls. The findings of a reduced degree of hand preference in the learningdisabled population has been reported in previous studies (e.g., Batheja & McManus, 1985) and would be anticipated if degree of laterzlization is dependent upon experience of tasks lateralized to one hand (such as writing), or if there is less reinforcement of performance carried out by the better hand. This process is obviously not specific to autism per se but is more likely to be a nonspecific consequence of a generalized learning disability. Consistency of Hand Preference. Children with autism and children with learning disability were significantly less consistent in their handedness than were normal developing children. In addition, younger children showed a tendency to be less consistent in their hand preference than older children, although examination of Table I shows that while the normal controls remain at a comparably high level of consistency from the age of approximately 3 years to 12 years, the pattern exhibited in children with autism is one of reduced consistency at 3 years with a slight increase at a later age, although not approaching the level exhibited in the normal controls. In contrast, consistency of hand preference in young children with learning disabilities begins at an equivalent level to that of children with autism but appears to increase sharply, so by 11 years it is approaching a level comparable with the normal children. While these findings lend support to those reported by Satz and colleagues (e.g., Satz et aL, 1988) who reported reduced consistency of hand preference, referred to by them as ambiguous handedness (AH), in autistic populations and mentally retarded populations compared to the incidence in the normal population, the support is not unequivocal. According to Satz et al., AH may indicate the presence of bilateral brain damage which has rendered neither hemisphere capable of establishing motor dominance. They also postulate that the overall level of intellectual functioning should be lower in comparison to normal controls (although McManus et aL, 1992, found no evidence that AH correlated with intelligence). To what extent Satz's theory is in accordance with the present study is debatable. For instance, how can the theory account for the increase in hand consistency found in the older autistic and learningdisabled group; it may simply be that the reduced hand preference consistency of the younger autistic and learning-disabled children is the result of poor motor functioning or delay in which practice with unimanual items will improve consistency, although not to a level comparable to the normal population. Hand Preference and Hand Skill 607 Skill-Preference Concordance. McManus et al. (1992) reported a dissociation of hand skill and hand preference in children with autism compared to children with learning disabilities and normal developing children, who were matched for mental age. It must be remembered in interpreting the present study that 52 of the 151 children for whom data are reported are the same individuals as those of McManus et al. (1992), and in partitular they form all of the older autistic and older learning-disabled group. Within the older group it is still clear that the autistic children have a higher incidence of discordance than the learning-disabled and control groups, despite the control groups now being matched for chronological rather than mental age. However the major discrepancy is in the younger children, where there is no real evidence that any of the three groups matched for chronological age differ in the extent of their concordance. Finally, it must be noted that if the age groups are combined, although the differences between groups do not reach statistical significance, the autistic group still shows a higher incidence of discordance (37.1% of 35), compared with 18.0% of 89 normal developing controls, and 24.0% of 25 learning-disabled controls; of course this trend might, in part, reflect that the overall group still contains the same subjects as in the previous study of McManus et al. Interpretation of these results is difficult. We have not carded out a strict replication of the McManus et al. study, since the new subjects in the present study are much younger; and in that group there seems indeed to be little evidence that children with autism differ from other children. It may be that a proper replication of the McManus et al. study would indeed find a similar effect; if so, then it will be necessary to develop a theoretical model that can explain why discordance is present only in older children with autism, as would seem to be the case here. Alternatively, the lack of difference in the younger group may cast doubt on the phenomenon found in the older group, implying that a proper replication would fail to find differences similar to those found by McManus et al. (1992), and that their result would then have to be construed as a Type I statistical error. Only additional data can distinguish these two possibilities. Conclusions The present study has found a number of useful findings concerning the nature of handedness in children with autism. In particular it shows that it is possible to test children with autism in the age range 4-6 years and shows dearly in that age range that the broad pattern of handedness is similar to learning-disabled controls of similar chronological age, and that degree and consistency of hand preference are both reduced substantially below normal 608 Cornish and McManus developing controls of similar age. Of particular importance is that there are no significant differences between autistic and learning-disabled children on degree and consistency of hand preference, suggesting that these phenomena are not specific to autism, but are a more general characteristic of children with learning disabilities. However children with autism do differ from other groups on the pegboard task, in particular although children with autism are significantly impaired in comparison with age-matched normal controls at their overall speed on the pegboard, they are nonetheless significantly faster than learning-disabled controls; similar results apply for pegboard time with the fight hand, with the left hand, with the better hand and with the worse hand, but not for difference measures between the two hands. It is not clear why this pattern of results should differentiate young children with autism from other young children. Perhaps the most disappointing finding is the lack of a difference between groups in the younger children in skill-preference concordance. As discussed earlier there are several interpretations of that finding. Nevertheless it is clear that although in many ways children with autism are similar to other children with learning disabilities, there is stillsuggestive evidence that children with autism show a distinctive and individual pattern of handedness. REFERENCES Annett, M. (1970). A classification of hand preference by association analysis. British Journal of Psychology, 6L 303-321. Batheja, M., & McManus, I. C. (1985). Handedness in the mentally handicapped. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 27, 63-68. Bishop, D. V. M. (1990). Handedness and developmental disorder. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Fein, H., Humes, M., Kaplin, E., Lueei, D., & Waterhouse, L. (1984). The question of left hemisphere dysfunction in infantile autism. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 258-281. Gillberg, C. (1983). Autistic children's hand preference: results from an epidemiological study of infantile autism. Psychiatry Research, 10, 21-30. Leboyer, M., Osherham, D. N., Nostem, N., & Robertoux, P. (1988). Is autism associated with anomalous dominance? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18, 539-551. McManus, I. C. (1996). Handedness. In J. G. Beaumont & J. Sergent (Eds.), Blackwell dictionary of neuropsychology. Oxford: Blackwell. McManus, I. C., Murray, IC, Doyle, K., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1992). Handedness in childhood autism shows a dissociation of skill and preference. Cortex, 28, 373-381. MeManus, I. C., Silk, G., Cole, D. R., Mellon, A. F., Wong, J., & Kloss, J. (1988). The development of handedness in children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 257-273. Pipe, M. (1988). Atypical laterality and retardation. PsychologicalBulletin, 104, 343-347. Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definition. In M. Rutter & E. Schopler (Eds.), Autism: A reappraisal of concepts and treatments. New York: Plenum Press. Satz, P. (1972). Pathological left handedness: An explanatory model. Cortex, 8, 121-135. Satz, P., Orsini, D. L., Saslow, E., & Henry, R. (1985). The pathological left handedness syndrome. Brian and Cognition, 4, 27-46. Hand Preference and Hand Skill 609 Satz, P., Soper, H. V., & Orsini, D. L. (1988). Human hand preferen~: Three non-dextral subtypes. In D. L. Molfeses & S. J. Segalowitz (Eds.), Brain Lateralization in Children. New York: Guilford. Satz, P., Green, M. F., & Lyon, N. (1989). Some pathological substrates in manual laterality. Brain Dysfunction, 2, 25-33. Soper, H. V., & Satz, P. (1984). Pathological left handedness and ambiguous handedness: A new explanatory model. Neuropsychologia, 22, 511-515. Soper, H. V., Satz, P., Orsini, D. L., Henry, R. R., Zvi, R., & Sehulman, M. (1986). Handedness patterns in autism suggest subtypes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 155-167. Tsai, L. Y. (1983). The relationship of handeduess to the cognitive, language and visuo-spatial skills of autistic patients. British Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 156-162. Tsai, L. Y., & Stewart, M. A. (1982). Handedness and EEG correlation in autistic children. Biological Psychiatry, 17, 595-597.