Document 13495994

advertisement
Highway road signs : a comparison of sign location, letter case, and word order with respect to driver
response time
by Kathleen Kuk Bissonnette
A thesis submitted to the Graduate Faculty in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of
MASTER OF SCIENCE in Psychology
Montana State University
© Copyright by Kathleen Kuk Bissonnette (1973)
Abstract:
The effects of sign location, letter case, and word order, on correct response time were studied using
simulated highway posted sign and pavement located messages. The general experimental procedure
employed in this research is a modified version of that described by Forbes (1964).
Subjects were selected from a pool of high school and university students. Selection was made on the
basis of age, binocular visual acuity, and driver licensing.
A Mobile Traffic Education Simulator was used to simulate the physical driving situation. A T-2K
Constant Illumination Tachisto-scope was used to present the stimulus materials.
Results indicate that sign location and letter case are mutually dependent (p<0.01). Closer investigation
of this interaction reveals that pavement messages in upper case lettering are significantly better with
respect to correct response time (p<0.01) than pavement messages in lower case lettering. No such
difference was found to exist with respect to posted sign messages. There is some evidence to indicate
that word order may have been confounded with reading strategy, both in the main effects and in
interaction with the other variables. In presenting this thesis in partial fulfillment of the require­
ments for an advanced degree at Montana State University, I agree that
the library shall make it freely available for inspection.
I further
agree that permission for extensive copying of this thesis for scholar­
ly purposes may be granted by my major professor, or, in his absence,
by the Director of Libraries.
It is understood that any copying or
publication of this thesis for financial gain shall not be allowed
without my written permission.
HIGHWAY ROAD SIGNS: A COMPARISON OF SIGN LOCATION, LETTER CASE,
AND WORD ORDER WITH RESPECT TO DRIVER RESPONSE TIME
by
KATHLEEN KUK BISSONNETTE
A thesis submitted to the Graduate Faculty in partial
fulfillment of the requirements for the degree
of
MASTER OF SCIENCE
in
Psychology
Approved:
Head, Major Department
MONTANA STATE, UNIVERSITY
Bozeman, Montana
December, 1973
Y
iii
Acknowledgments .
■ The investigator would like to thank Drs„ William D. Shontz,
M. Paul Willis, and Ralph W. Zimmer for their guidance and assistance
throughout the course of this study. Sincere thanks are also due Dr.
Glen L. Martin for his interest and cooperation.
The cooperation of Mr. Curtis R. Hahn and the Office of the Super­
intendent of Public Instruction for Montana, in the procurring and use
of the simulation equipment is gratefully acknowledged. Thanks are
also due the Montana Department of Highways for their assistance in
the road preparation. Special thanks are also due Mr. Sindey Thomas
for his time and assistance in the computer assisted analysis of the
experimental data. A final acknowledgment is due the Montana Depart­
ment of Intergovernmental Relations, Division of Highway Safety, for
their financial assistance in this research project.
iv
Table of Contents
List of T a b l e s .................... ............ ..............
List of figures
Abstract
v
.................... ..........................v i
......................................................... v ii
I n t r o d u c t i o n .................................... ..............i
9
M e t h o d .................................................. .
S u b j e c t s .......................... ............. .........
9
10
A p p a r a t u s .......................... ..................
Stimulus Preparation.................. ...................
Procedure
12
.................................................16
Results . . ........
. ............
. . . . . . . . . . . . . .
21
D i s c u s s i o n .............. ......................................29
References
........
. . . . .
........
. . . . . .
..........
A p p e n d i x ............ ..........................................40
38
V
List of Tables
Table I.
Height of Projected full-height Letters
Table 2.
Cell Means .....................
Table 3.
. ................15
Mean Correct Response Times for the Three Independent .
Variables . . . . . .................... . . . . . . . .
22
23
Table 4.
Analysis of V a r i a n c e .....................................24
Table 5.
Message Correlations.........................
27
Vi
List of figures
Figure I.
Physical sign location format ..................... .
14
Figure 2. Experimental d e s i g n ................................. 20
Figure 3. Interaction of location and letter case . .
J . . .
. 25
Figure A. Experimental recording sheet .........................
41
Figure B. Primary and auxiliary task instructions............. 42
Figure C. Examples of the posted located sign messages
........
Figure D. Examples of pavement located sign messages. . . . . .
Figure E.
Controls, timing equipment, and dual tachistoscope
(Top) and the experimental simulator unit (Bottom). . .
43
44
45
vii
Abstract
The effects of sign location, letter case, and word order, on
correct response time were studied using simulated highway posted
sign and pavement located messages. The general experimental proce­
dure employed in this research is a modified version of that described
by Forbes (1964).
Subjects were selected from a pool of high school and university
students. Selection was made on the basis of age, binocular visual
acuity, and driver licensing.
A Mobile Traffic Education Simulator was used to simulate the
physical driving situation. A T-2K Constant Illumination Tachistoscope was used to present the stimulus materials.
Results indicate that sign location and letter case are mutually
dependent (p<0.01). Closer investigation of this interaction reveals
that pavement messages in upper case lettering are significantly
better with respect to correct response time (p<0.01) than pavement
messages in lower case lettering. No such difference was found to
exist with respect to posted sign messages. There is some evidence to
indicate that word order may have been confounded with reading strat­
egy, both in the main effects and in interaction with the other.
variables.
'
Introduction
Although efficient motor vehicle transportation, primarily pri­
vately OMied transportation, is of high priority value to the modern
American lifestyle, failures in the driver-vehicle-highway system re­
main a major social problem.
Any such man-machine system, in order
to function properly, must be designed to minimize the possibility of
error.
In the driver-vehicle-highway system, the driver is in control.
Thus, all elements of the system must be designed around his limita­
tions and capabilities.
Interest in the human factors approach to highway safety began
as early as 1927 (Forbes, 1972).
The first research programs orien­
tated toward driver behavior came as a result of recommendations by
the National Research Council.
Much of this early work was directed
toward accident proneness - the psychology of the accident repeater.
Other more universal problems were approached as well, during these
early years.
The effects of fatigue, vision, perception, and general
attitude were among the more general topics considered.
Such:human
factors research was and is used to guide traffic engineers and high­
way design.
One specific area of traffic safety which has attracted research
interest is the task effectiveness of highway signs.
A highway sign’s
task is to transmit necessary information to the highway driver.
ure to transmit information can be the result of several factors,
Fail­
2
singly or in combination.
tected by the driver.
First, the sign may be completely unde­
Sign location, sign to background color and
brightness contrast, or stimulus overload may contribute to this kind
of failure.
Secondly, a message may be unreadable at the necessary
distance in order for a correct response to be accomplished.
Bright­
ness contrast, letter size and width, spacing, color-any or all of
these factors may be involved.
Finally, the message information may
be misinterpreted by the driver, thus yielding an incorrect response.
The problem in this case is primarily message clarity - the degree to
which the message is correctly understood.
This research investigation was designed to determine the effects
of sign location, letter case, and word order on the correct response
time of the automobile driver.
In this study, the investigator
attempted to examine the effects of these variables, individually and
in combination, on the correct response time of the automobile driver
with respect to simulated highway warning signs.
Correct response
time is defined as a composite of the time to detect, comprehend, and
react to simulated highway.warning signs.
Understanding the tasks in any given man-machine system is impor­
tant to understanding the system.
Task analyses of the automobile
driving task are available in the literature (Forbes, 1965).
tem is divided into three major components:
stimuli,
2)
The sys­
I) input or environmental
driver information processing including sensing and per-
3
ceiving of information plus vehicle-directed response, and
cle response.
3) vehi­
The information processing component is primary to the
specific problem of highway signing.
First, one must consider the
driving activity as the sign is approached.
Studies on eye movements
in the driving situation have shown that drivers’ eyes are not simply
fixating on the road surface ahead of his vehicle (Mourant, Rockwell,
and Rackoff, 1970;
Thomas, 1968).
Instead, in situations where no
car following is involved, the driver’s eye fixations seem to occur
primarily in the area above and above-right with respect to the road
surface ahead of the vehicle.
Occasional fixations on the road sur­
face just ahead of the vehicle and in the opposing lane, as well as
the area above the road surface in the opposing lane were also noted.
Thus, sign location should influence the initial detection time, and
consequently the total correct response time to the sign and sign
message.
Signs located where a driver is most likely to look should
have the lowest detection time.
Signs must also be detected as relevant stimuli in the presence
of other environmental stimuli.
Considering the natural environment,
,
•
the road, and the presence of other vehicles as a constantly changing,
yet constantly present visual input, the amount of information bombard­
ing the driver’s visual sensory system is extensive.
Assuming an un­
familiar highway situation, the problem seems to be on of selective
attention.
4
Treisman (1969) made an important differentiation between focused
and divided attention.
A subject can either focus his attention on
a single stimulus, or he can divide his attention between two or more
stimuli.
The sets of experimental questions regarding these two divi­
sions of selective attention are independent.
For instance, with
divided attention the investigator should be testing the necessity of
a selective mechanism.
In this case, one might measure the extent to
which the human being is capable of handling two or more sensory out­
puts efficiently - the extent to which attention is capable of being
divided.
With focused attention,.however, the question changes to how
efficiently can a human being select or focus on a single.stimulus
while ignoring all other information.
The driving, task is primarily an example of divided attention.
The driver is required to distribute his attention among several environmental factors in close temporal and spatial proximity.
>
He must,
for example, maintain vehicular position on the highway, be aware of
oncoming vehicles in both the same and opposing directions, detect and
comprehend all relevant signs, and be aware of any potential "danger
points" in his environment.
The complexity of the driving task is
obvious.
As is implied in the example above, the driving task consists of
several subtasks relying primarily on the visual sense.
Although audi­
tion and kinesthesis are also important in the actual, driving task
(horns, bumps, speed, etc.) their roles seem to be basically supportive
5.
to the visual tracking task.
Treisman (1969) indicated that the main limitation on efficiency
is set by the double use of a single analyzer in the selective process.
That is, then the same sense modality and essentially the same problem­
solving process is required for the inputs among which attention must
be divided, efficiency is low.
Studies cited by Treisman (1969), in­
dicated that efficiency in a visual-visual dual task problem is great­
er than that for an auditory-auditory dual task problem, but both are
significantly inferior to a mixed (visual-auditory) dual task.
Since
the driving task is primarily visual, and that a considerable amount
of the relevant information transmitted to the driver is done so
through the visual modality,, the auxiliary task using in this present
study was designed primarily as a visual.task..
Each subtask of the driving task is cognitively loaded.
That is,
each subtask requires a certain amount of cognitive processing time
and effort by the driver in order to be successfully accomplished.
This point was discussed by Forbes (1964) and Mourant et al (1970) and
incorporated into their research.
A driver has a cognitive load limit.
Beyond this limit, a driver becomes systematically or nonsystematicalIy
selective of the. information processed, .Thus, the auxiliary task used
in this present study was designed to contribute some degree of cogni­
tive load, in order to simulate a true driving subtask.
6
The readability of a sign message is essential to sign task
success. A sign appropriately positioned for easy detection and sol­
itary in attention value is of no use to the driver if the message can­
not be read and understood easily.
Tinker (1963) discussed the effect of letter case on the legibil­
ity of reading materials.
His findings indicated that the lower case
letters increase reading speed and accuracy of printed material.
Lar­
ger upper case letters require more eye fixations per line than do
lower case letters.
Also, there is a component of familiarity favor­
ing lower case letters over upper case letters.
cultures normally read lower case lettering.
People in Western
Upper case letters have
an advantage over lower case letters in that stimulus detection dis-.
tance is greater for upper case letters than for lower case letters.
Forbes, Moscowitz, and Morgan (1950) tackled the problem of com­
paring upper case letters specifically for highway signing.
They
found that the sign familiarity increases recognition distance for
both letter cases, but this increase is greater for lower case letters.
Words printed in lower case have specific form or patterns due to the
differing letter shapes.
For this reason, the word form could easily
be learned, and thus function as a symbol for the word message.
Whereas Forbes et 'al (1950) were concerned only with overhead
destination signs, the present study tested the effects of letter case
with respect to both pavement and posted sign message locations.
Also,
7
Forbes et al (1950) used city name messages.
Consequently, the lower
case messages contained a mix of initial upper case letters.
The
present study used warning messages with uniform lettering over the
entire message to eliminate any such mixing of letter case.
Tinker (1963) investigated the effects of word order on reading
speed and accuracy.
They verified the assumption that people of the
Western cultures do best when reading in a top to bottom direction.
Two word pavement messages, however, are uniformly printed in the re­
verse manner.
One justification for such positioning is that the
driver sees the word closest to him first, and consequently reads this
word before the one to follow.
however.
This justification can be questioned,
A critical minimum distance between the two words on the
pavement has been set in the Manual on Uniform Traffic Control Devices
(MJTCD, 1971).
If both words are legible simultaneously at the criti-
cle distance, perhaps this distance is not sufficient to ensure the
nearer word will be read before the further one.
Considering the
Western reading techniques, a strong tendency may exist to attempt a
top to bottom reading strategy when possible.
The present study was
designed to study the effect of word order on the response time of
drivers with respect to posted and, more importantly,, to pavement lo­
cated messages.
Thus, three variables were considered in this present research.
These were sign location, letter case, and word order.
In the MUTCD, '
8
pavement messages are cited as being best under favorable conditions
because these signs do not divert the driver’s attention from the road­
way.
The research on drivers’ eye movements cited previously indicate
that a driver’s eye fixations are not primarily located on the roadway.
A substantial amount of time is spent observing the shoulder area.
Thus, a difference in correct response time between posted sign mes­
sages and pavement location messages, given favorable conditions of
weather, traffic, and roadway, may exist.
The direction of this dif­
ference, however, may not be that indicated in the MUTCD (1971).
Letter case has been studied varying loop height,, spacing, and
width by Forbes et al (1950).
In this present research, letter case
is studied with respect to the standard alphabets (Standard Alphabets
for Highway'Signs, 1966).
Following MUTCD specifications, upper case
lettering is used in practice on all warning sign messages for both
the posted and pavement message locations.
The appropriateness of.
this procedure is tested in this present research with respect to
correct response time.
Finally, Tinker (1963) indicated that in the Western cultures,
normal reading order is top to bottom as opposed to bottom to top.
Two word messages in the posted location are designed on a top to
bottom basis, but two word pavement word messages are designed bottom
to top.
If both lines of a pavement message can indeed be seen simul­
taneously, then word order may be a factor affecting correct response
Method
The general experimental procedure employed in this research is
a modified version of that described by Forbes (1964).
Stimulus
slides taken of an actual driving scene with simulated highway signs,
and identical scene slides without signs were presented tachistoscopically.
An auxiliary task was used to load the subject and to assure
a varied eye fixation pattern approximating that of actual driving
conditions.
The auxiliary task consisted of a combination visual
search-consecutive counting problem.
The primary task consisted of
physical driving responses - braking, signaling, steering - made to
specific instructions given by the sign message.
The purpose of this
research was to measure whether or not differences in correct response
time occur under specific conditions.
The use of still pictures lim­
its the direct applicability of study results to those driving situa­
tions where a sign is suddenly exposed to the drivers' view.
Some
such traffic situations are rounding a curve, coming over a hill, or
passing another vehicle.
Subjects
A number of local high school and university students were avail­
able for this study.
In order to control for within population differ­
ences in age, binocular visual acuity, and general driving qualifica­
tions, a set of criteria were imposed on subject selection.
Each
subject was required to be between the ages of 16 and 25, have 20-20
vision as measured by the Snellen eye chart test, and be a holder of
10
a valid (U.S.) driver’s license.
Due to the limits imposed by these
criteria, and the relatively small university enrollment of summer
quarter, recruitment of the required 80 experimental subjects had to
be extended over several weeks.
The total subject sample consisted of 40 men and 40 women student
volunteers.
Of this sample, five men and five women were randomly
assigned to each of the eight experimental conditions tested.
Each
subject received monetary payment ($1.50) for his participation in the
study.
Each subject received 20 trials during the course of the exper­
iment.
The mean for these 20 trials constituted the dependent variable
value for each subject in the analysis.
The total experimental time
for each subject was about 15 minutes. ,
Apparatus
A Mobile Traffic Education Simulator (Allstate) owned, by the
Montana Department of Public Instruction, was available for this re­
search.
Each of the 16 units in the simulator is a replica of the
driver’s side of a m o d e m American-made sedan, complete with driving
controls and dashboard.
For purposes of this study, only one unit of
the simulator was used in order to control for subjects' distance from
the viewing screen.
The subject to projection screen distance was 23
feet.
A T^2K Constant Illumination Tachistoscope (Lafayette Instrument
Co.) was used to present the stimulus material. Each of the two
11
projectors was equipped with a 7 inch Kodak lens in order to provide
a clear, full-screen image at the projector-screen distance of
One of the projectors
54 feet.
was set in a normally open position (scene-slide
projector), while the second projector was set in a normally closed
position (stimulus-slide projector).
The settings were such that the
two projectors were never both in an open position or a closed position
simultaneously.
The final effect was a constantly projected scene with
signs suddenly appearing and disappearing.
The scene slide projector was used to maintain constant illumin­
ation throughout the course of the experiment, and to serve as a
source of viewing material for the auxiliary task.
open for durations of six to ten seconds.
This projector was
The number of seconds be­
tween stimulus slide presentations was determined according to a pre­
arranged randomly selected sequence.
Probability and Statistics, 1966).
(CRC Handbook of Tables,for
This randomization was used to
prevent the learning of a set time duration between stimulus presenta­
tions.
The randomization schedule remained the same for all subjects
in all conditions, and was manually controlled with the timing appara­
tus .
Correct response time (CRT), the dependent variable, was defined
as the amount of time from stimulus presentation to the completion of
a correct response.
Correct response time was measured ,by an electron­
ic Hunter 0.01 second digital timer and recorded manually by the ex­
perimenter.
The timer was electrically set to start with the presenta-
12
tion of a stimulus slide and end with a correct response or at the end
of three seconds, whichever occurred first.
The stimulus slide projector was electrically regulated to be open
for three seconds.
The slides, five messages with four replications
of each message, were randomly arranged in the slide tray sequence.
This random order remained the same over all conditions studied.
Stimulus Preparation
The experimental scene consisted of a one-half mile segment of
rarely used state highway, a frontage road.
Permission to use this
highway segment was obtained from the Montana Department of Highways.
For experimental reasons, brightness contrast and direction, and color
were controlled factors in this study.
These factors with respect to
posted warning signs were changed so as to agree with the pavement
messages in these specific dimensions.
Posted sign stimuli were made using 36 inch X 36 inch fiberboard
painted, with #658 Spar Enamel (dearborn gray - Gilt Edge).
Stapled to
the fiberboard were letters, seven.inches in height, cut from white
posterboard.
Upper case letters followed the Series C design, and
lower case lettering conformed to the same illustrated in Standard
Alphabets for Highway Signs, 1966.
See Figure C.
A metal sign stub
and sign blank,were furnished by the Montana Department of Highways,
and installed along the road in standard position, as defined in MUTCD
(1971).
The posted sign stimulus was mounted over the sign blank with
13
heavy wire in '’picture-hanging’’ fashion.
Pavement letters were made from 24 inch wide white freezer wrap.
Upper case letters were made in accordance with the standard pavement
alphabet (Federal Highway Administration, Personal Communication, 1973).
Lower case lettering was adapted from the lower case alphabet in
Standard Alphabets for Highway Signs (1966), such that the full height
letters [l,b,h, etc.) were eight feet in height.
The elongation of
the letters was such that for every one inch in width the letter was
two inches high.
Letters were individually attached to the pavement
with gray sheet metal tape.
See Figure D.
Some problems arose in
preparing the pavement messages for photographing.
The exposures
could only be taken when the sun was behind the camera, allowing only
the morning hours to, be utilized for this purpose.
Also, the weather
conditions had to be ideal for this purpose; dry roads, and.clear skies
were necessary to provide favorable conditions.
The wind posed special
problems for pavement message preparation since the letters could not
be secured well due to the crumbling pavement surface.
Relative sign message position was determined so as to control
for visual angle differences between the pavement, message and the post­
ed message.
The posted message was located in standard position at
the right pavement edge, 63 feet from the camera lens.
In Figure I,
line C marks the center line between the pavement message line I and
the pavement message line 2.
Thus, the final projected image of each
14
message
line I
camera
Figure I.
Physical sign location format
15
Table I.
Height of projected full-height letters
' SPECIFICATIONS
POSTED
' PAVEMENT
UPPER
Total height line I
2"
I"
CASE
Total height line 2
2"
3.5"
LOWER
Total height line I
2"
I"
CASE
Total height line 2
2"
3"
16
posted message, line was equal in height to the average of the projected
pavement message line height, the average of pavement message line I
and pavement message line 2.
See Table I.
Stimulus slides (35mm) were photographed from the driver’s seat
of a 1963 Buick Electra automobile.
The camera (Nikon - 58mm lens)
was secured to a tripod such that the lens be 46 inches from the pave­
ment, the estimated eye position of a driver.
All exposures were tak­
en with the automobile located in the same position on the road (cen­
ter of the driving lane) in a parked, ”engine off" manner.
The wind­
shield remained in place while the photographs were taken. .Slides of
the experimental scene only (no signs) were taken within the same con­
straints of location and camera position.
All photographs were taken
using Ektachrome ASA64 slide film.
Procedure
Each subject was first tested for binocular visual acuity by
means of the Snellen eye chart test procedure.
He or she was then
seated in the experimental unit of the simulator and given the instruc­
tions necessary for the completion of the primary and auxiliary tasks.
Primary task.
The subject was instructed to engage in the aux­
iliary task until such time that a sign appeared in the projected
scene.
At this time, the subject was to stop the auxiliary task, and
respond to the sign by doing what the sign message instructed.
The
sign message instructions all required a simple driving response; steer
17
left, steer right, signal left, signal right, and pedal right (brake
with the right foot). These particular messages and responses were
chosen with the capacities of the simulator in mind, as well as the
applicability as warning messages and consequent responses.
"Pedal
right" was chosen to provide a foot response in opposition to the hand
responses of the other messages.
The phrase "pedal right" was coined
in order to maintain the two word, similar length message pattern, and
to include a direction similar to the other messages.
Auxiliary task.
The auxiliary task consisted of searching the
stimulus scene (no signs present) for objects which could be counted.
The prescribed manner of counting was a modified consecutive, numerical
order.
The subject was instructed to count in consecutive numerical
order and attach to each number in the sequence an object in the pro­
jected scene which appeared at least that many times.
For example,
one telephone pole, two pavement stripes, three fence posts, etc.
The
purpose of this task was to insure that the subject, was looking over
the whole scene and not staring at the eventual sign message site.
General procedure.
The experimental task consisted of responding
to twenty sign stimulus slides, four replications of each of the five
messages.
The scene slide and the stimulus slides were alternated in
tachistoscopic fashion.
A practice period consisting of five sign
stimuli and the scene slide preceded the actual test run.
This practice
was necessary to familiarize the subject with the signs, the messages.
18
the appropriate responses, and the auxiliary task.
At the end of this
practice session, the subjects were able to ask questions about the
tasks.
During this break between practice and test periods, the scene
slide was projected to avoid any sudden changes in light intensity.
A small pilot run of the experiment suggested the problem of in­
complete trials - trials in which no correct response was made.
The
problem was resolved by manually reversing the stimulus slide sequence
and simply repeating the missed trial.
The subject was unaware of
this alteration and the times indicated no consistent effects.
This
technique was followed in order to insure a complete factorial arrange­
ment and to maintain a constant number of subjects in all cells of the
design.
The subjects who required the use of the repeat technique
appear in all cells of the design.
No subject had more than two such
repeated trials, and no trial was repeated more than once.
A post test interview was given a small informal sample of sub­
jects in all cells of the design.
Questions regarding the subject's
interpretation of the,message word order were posed at this time.
Randomization ,of subjects to the experimental cells was accomplish­
ed just prior to each subject arrival.
Each of the 80 subjects were
represented numerically according to cell on a small card.
These
cards were balanced according to sex, shuffled, and distributed into
two large envelopes.
The experimenter shuffled and drew a card from
the appropriate envelope to determine cell assignment.
The experimental
19
design was a completely randomized design in a 2 X 2 I 2 factorial
■
arrangement.
This design is illustrated in Figure 2.
I
20
Posted
Top to Bottom
LOCATION
WORD ORDER
Pavement
Bottom to top
Lower
LETTER CASE
Figure 2.
Experimental design
Completely randomized design in a 2 X 2 X 2 Factorial Arrangement: N = 70
Subjects:
ten per cell; five male and five female.
Results
Correct response time data were transferred to computer data
cards and, subsequently, the computer was used to aid in analysis.
Each subject
was represented by a single dependent variable value,
the mean CRT of his or her twenty trials.
Data were combined for
each cell over male and female subjects.
Statistical tests used in this analysis of the data were the
analysis of variance, Duncan’s Multiple Range Test, and correlations.
The null hypotheses were tested at the 0.05 level.
Mean response times for the various combinations of experimental
conditions are presented in Table 2.
The combination yielding the
shortest CRT was pavement-upper case-bottom to top, while that combin­
ation yielding the longest CRT was pavement-lower case-top to bottom.
Means for each level of the three independent variables studied
were also calculated.
See Table 3.
The largest between level differ­
ence occurred with letter case; the upper case mean being 0.076 seconds
shorter than the lower case mean.
Sign location has the next largest
between, level difference; the posted mean being 0.048 seconds longer
than the pavement mean, while word order had the smallest between lev­
el difference.
The analysis of variance is shown in Table 4.
The interaction of
location and letter case was significant at the 0.01 level. A graph
of this interaction (Figure 3) is useful to illustrate just what this
significance indicates.
Mean CRT,for the posted location appears to
22
Table 2.
CONDITION
Cells means
'TEAN
(sec.)
■ CONDITION "
'TEAN
Posted
Upper
Top to Bottom
1.369
Pavement
Upper
Top to Bottom
1.221
Posted
Lower
Top to Bottom
1.272
Pavement
Lower
Top to Bottom
1.462
Posted
Upper
Bottom to Top
1.411
Pavement
Upper
Bottom to Top
1.211
Posted
Lower
Bottom to Top
1.408
Pavement
Lower
Bottom to Top
1.373
23
Table 3.
I .V.*,
M e a n correct response times for the three independent variables
SIGN LOCATION
LETTER CASE
WORD ORDER
Posted
Upper case
Top-bottom
1.365
1.303
1.331
Pavement
Lower case
Bottom-top
1.319
1.379
1.351
24
Table 4.
SOURCE
Analysis of variance
SS
df
Location (A)
Letter case (B)
AB
Word Order (C)
AC
BC
ABC
0.0465129
0.115292
0.315507
0.007900
0.094806
0.0002664
0.0374543
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
0.0465129
0.115292
0.315507
0.007900
0.094806
0.0002664
0.374543
Between
Within
Total
0.617730
2.50893
3.12667
7
72
79
0.0348463
** p = 0.01
MS
F
1.33
3.31
9.05**
0.23
2.72
0.01
1.07
25
Mean CRT (Seconds)
PAVEMENT
POSTED
POSTED
1.34
PAVEMENT
LOWER
CASE
Figure 3.
Interaction of location and letter case
UPPER
CASE
26
vary only slightly with letter case; the upper case posted mean is 1.39
and the lower case posted mean is 1.34 seconds.
A test of this differ­
ence using Duncan’s Multiple Range Test indicated that this difference
was not significant.
The situation is different with the pavement location, however.
Here the difference between upper case pavement and lower case pavement
is quite large [difference = 0 . 2 0 seconds).
This difference is signif­
icant at the 0.01 level using Duncan’s Multiple Range Test.
Figure 3 also illustrates the effect of letter case on the differ­
ences between the two sign message locations.
For the lower case con­
ditions, the difference in CRT between sign locations is smaller than
for the upper case conditions. The difference between sign locations
for lower case conditions is not significant at the 0.05 level.
The
difference between sign location for the upper case conditions is sig­
nificant at the 0.01 level.
Correlations among response times for the different sign messages
were also determined and appear in Table 5.
appears in the correlational analysis.
One interesting phenomenon
’’Signal” message commands
correlate most highly with the command (i.e. signal right with signal
left and vice versa) while the ’’steer” message commands correlate most
highly with the direction [i.e. steer right with signal right and steer
left with signal left). Although these correlations are not signifi­
cantly higher than the others, the pattern is consistent.
27
Table 5.
Message correlations
Pedal R.
Signal L.
Steer L.
Steer R.
Signal
Pedal Ri
1.0000
0.5018
0.4553
0.5206
0.4753
Signal L.
0.5018
1.0000
0.6072
0.5817
0.6883
Steer L.
0.4553
0.6072
1.0000
0.5992
0.5637
Steer R.
0.5206
0.5817
0.5992
1.0000
0.6275
Signal R.
0.4753
0.6883
0.5637
0.6275
1.0000
28
The third independent variable studied was word order.
Analysis
indicated that the two levels of word order examined - vertical top to
bottom and vertical bottom to top - were not significantly different.
See Table 5.
Apparently, the sign messages studied were comprehended
with equal ease under both conditions of word order.
supplementary data which temper
But there are.
this conclusion.
Sex differences in CRT were also analyzed.
There was no signifi­
cant differences in CRT between males and females overall in this study.
Discussion
In the Methods section, mention was made that the methodology
used in this study was a modified version of that described by Forbes
(1964).
The most important procedural modification lies in the design
of the auxiliary task.
Forbes (1964) used a visual, cognitively load­
ed task essentially involving the relighting of a set of lights located
at road level on the projection screen.
In the present research pro­
cedure, the subject is required to search the entire projected screen
for objects which could be counted in the prescribed manner.
too, is a visual, cognitively loaded task.
This task,
In this latter task, a
large majority of the countable items or objects appear in the area
just above the road surface and to the right of the driving lane.
Re­
ferring to the eye movement studies cited earlier, Mourant et al (1970)
and Thomas (1968), this area is that in which most eye. fixations occur
in the driving task,, assuming no car following.
Thus, this modified
task is designed to yield a closer approximation to actual driver eye
movements. . Wfth his auxiliary task, Forbes was. able to insure that each
subject's eyes would travel the same "route", distance and direction,
from the auxiliary task stimulus to the primary task stimulus, for any
given stimulus presentation.
In the present study, this control is not
present, and thus the task yields a higher individual difference in
this category than does Forbes'.
The trade-off was made between exper­
imental control and the realism of actual driver eye movements.
30
A second very important methodological modification made in this
present study was in the subject’s response to the primary task.
In
Forbes’ technique, the subjects simply pressed an appropriate button
to indicate his response to the primary stimulus.
In the present study,
on the other hand, the emphasis was placed on closeness to the real
driving situation.
Subject responses are designed to be actual driving
responses made to the sign message information.
Response times most
important to the driving situation are driving type responses.
In reviewing the main effects of the independent variables consid­
ered here - sign location, letter case, and word order - it must be con­
cluded that these variables do not influence CRT- independently.
case and sign location effects were mutually dependent.
Letter
Word order may
have been confounded with reading strategy in this study; a point which
will be discussed further in later paragraphs.
The possible implications of the significant interaction between
letter case and sign location are important on both an applied and pure
experimental planes.
First, within the posted conditions, there was
no significant difference in CRT between upper and lower case letter­
ing.
The sign messages were designed to be relatively unfamiliar in
terms of wording.
Assuming these messages to be, in fact, unfamiliar
to the subjects, then perhaps any advantage the lower case lettering
should have had as a result of learning (Tinker and Paterson, 1950)
did not have sufficient time to be detectedi, Forbes (1950) found that
31
legibility distance increased more with lower case letters than with
upper case letters as familiarity increased.
If CRT and legibility
distance are highly correlated, then one could conclude that, in time,
performance on lower case lettering on posted sign messages would sur­
pass the performance on upper case lettering.
With the pavement conditions, however, there was a significant
difference between upper and lower case lettering - upper case being
superior to lower case in CRT.
Again this effect could well be mod­
ified by learning; lower case messages may not have had sufficient time
to demonstrate, greater effectiveness.
iliarity.
Another explanation may be fam- .
Familiarity has been established as a factor in recognition
distance (Forbes, 1950).
Keeping reaction time constant, an increase
in recognition distance would logically yield a decrease in CRT - CRT
being measured from a constant initial distance beyond the furthest
recognition distance.
written in upper case.
All pavement word messages are presently
Thus, upper case pavement messages may simply
be more familiar to the highway driver than pavement messages in lower
case.
Lower case lettering, then, had no effect on CRT for the posted
message location while significantly increasing CRT for the pavement
message. •Perhaps this phenomenon can be explained in terms of shape
recognition.
Hake (1957) stated that there is evidence to indicate
that the shape of an object (in this case, a word) is not invariant
32
with, orientation.
In fact, an object can be partially defined in terms
of orientation characteristics. Words written in lower case and posi­
tioned in the posted message location are in a familiar reading orienta­
tion.
Words written in lower case and positioned in the pavement mes­
sage location, however, are not in a familiar reading orientation.
Some positive transfer of shape recognition could logically take place
from common reading materials to posted sign messages due to the sim­
ilarity in orientation.
Thus, less time would be needed for the
shapes to be recognized in the posted message location than if no pos­
itive transfer had taken place.
If in fact lower case word shapes
are partially defined in terms of orientation characteristics,. then a
negative transfer of shape recognition would be expected to occur from
common reading materials to pavement sign messages due to the differ­
ences in orientation.
Thus, more time would be needed for the shapes
to be recognized in the pavement message location with negative trans­
fer occurring.
Words in upper letter case are uniform in shape; all words are
rectangular.
Thus, the learning of word shape and word shape recogni­
tion should not play an important role in defining words in upper case
lettering.
The shape orientation problem discussed relative to lower
case lettering would not apply to as great an extent to upper case
letters.
negative.
Little or no transfer would be expected to occur, positive or
Therefore, CRT should not be affected one way or the other
33
by shape orientation or shape recognition.
The major implication of the significant finding for signing de­
sign is a warning about warning signs.
New innovations found to be
effective for one sign location may not be effective for another sign
location.
For instance, the new international signs, once learned,
are expected to reduce CRT over posted word messages (MJTCD, 1971).
The effect on CRT of these symbols used in the pavement location is con
jecture at this point, and should not be assumed.
If the hypothesis
is correct that the shape orientation is in part defining the shape,
then there is a strong possibility that using these symbols in both
orientations would yield confusion and increases in CRT.
One might suggest from studying Figure 3 that the combination of
pavement location upper case lettering has the lower mean correct re­
sponse time, and is therefore the "best" of the combinations studied.
To be sure, applying Duncan’s Iiultiple Range Test, this combination
has significantly lower CRT than all the other combinations.
This argument has one severe limitation, however.
The results
apply only to the experimental conditions given, a characteristic of
any Fixed Constant design.
While the pavement located messages were
designed in apparent closeness to the standards expressed in MUTCD
[1971), those in the posted location were not.
For experimental
reasons, color, brightness contrast and direction were controlled;
the posted sign being changed to agree with the pavement messages in
34
these specific dimensions.
These factors have been shown to contri­
bute quite highly to the task success of the sign messages.
Pain, Joyce, and Fry, 1968; Pain, 1969).
(Forbes,
In these respects, then, the
posted message suffered the disadvantage over pavement messages.
There­
fore, the data processed here for the posted location is negatively
biased with respect to standard highway warning signs, and should not
be considered equivalent to warning data.
In light of the data, the
inclusion of standard posted warning signs in the framework of this
present study would have provided a test of these allegations.
Word order as a main effect and in interaction with the other
variables had no apparent effect of CRT.
There was some evidence,
however, which indicated that the variable could have been influenced
by another, uncontrolled factor, reading strategy.
A small informal sample (N = 13) of post test interviews, taken
from those subjects receiving the pavement location condition, indi­
cate that there were at least two different reading strategies employ­
ed by those subjects.
The first strategy - the traditional one in
Western cultures - is the straightforward top to bottom order.
The
second strategy is the "read the larger word first" technique - thus
bottom to top on the pavement.
Both strategies seemed to have been
employed in spite of any given message word order.
Certainly, the
traditional strategy, would logically yield shorter CRT's in the top to
conditions.
Of course the reverse would be true in .the case of the
other strategy.
Referring to Table 2, one will note that the mean CRT's
35
within the pavement conditions as a whole were consistently shorter for
the bottom to top conditions„
Perhaps this fact indicates that the
"read the larger word first” strategy was used more frequently than the
traditional one.
Considering that the two reading strategies are
necessarily opposing, then one might consider the possibility that the
strategies were working against each other within the word order con­
ditions, thus cancelling out at least partially what may have been a
word order effect.
Further evidence that this confounding may have taken place is
available from the data for the posted location conditions.
Those
subjects sampled under these conditions (N = 10) all indicated the use
of the standard strategy - the traditional top to bottom order. Re­
ferring again.to Table 2, the cell means for the posted conditions in­
dicate that the top to bottom word order yields consistently shorter
CRT's.
Also, one might note that the differences between top to
bottom and bottom to top word order conditions for the posted location
conditions are much greater than those for the pavement location con­
ditions.
Perhaps the smaller difference occurring with the pavement
location conditions is due to some cancellation resulting from the
use of the opposing strategies.
Some important information may be gleaned from this apparent con­
founding of word order with reading strategy with specific reference
36
to pavement word messages.
Two distinct and opposing reading strategies
were employed in the pavement conditions.
One must consider the effect
of this finding on the argument for the present pavement word order
system.
The basic argument is that a driver sees and reads the first
word he approaches before he sees and reads the ones remaining.
If in
fact this reading order is desirable, then the present critical distance
between any two pavement message lines is not sufficient.
The present
critical distance apparently does allow for the use of another reading
strategy.
A major requirement of a successful man-machine system is
the minimization of human error.
Reading a message backwards logically
yields temporary confusion, and even temporary confusion can lead to
error.
Several procedural variations and independent variables for. con­
sideration were left for further research.
variation lies in the stimulus presentation.
The most obvious procedural
For strict laboratory or
simulation research, moving picture presentation of the stimulus ma­
terial
should be approached.
Moving pictures would allow for more
generally realistic presentation, as well as rather naturally approx­
imating the normal driving behavior including eye movements.
The prob­
lem of the dependent variable arises with moving picture presentation.
A technique used in the educational films accompanying the Mobile
Traffic Education Simulator and developed for its use by Allstate, may
be a feasible solution.
In these films, correct driving responses are
37
programmed into the sound track of the film.
For instance, if a stu­
dent should be applying his brakes at a particular moment of the film,
the programmed directions will check for the correct response.
Sim­
ilarly, a point from which a response measurement should begin could
be so programmed.
This technique would give a constant point from
which time could be measured.
Although expensive and technical in
preparation, this technique could be useful in incorporating realism
into the laboratory.
Ideally, of course, an actual highway surrounded by a relatively
constant environment would be desirable for complete realism.
natural environment is by nature uncontrolled.
But the
Thus, in order for the
natural situation to yield usable results, we need data on these var-'
iables uncontrollable in the natural environment. Therefore, the need
for further laboratory and simulation research is essential.
The amount of information gained from human factor research with
specific relevance for highway signing is extensive.
This informa­
tion has been used to improve the task success for signing in many
different dimensions.
Due to the importance and relevance of human
factors research to highway safety problems in general, one final
suggestion needs to be made.
On page 5 of the MUTCD (1971), future
publications should justifiably read:
"The decision to use a particular device at a particular
location should be made on the basis of an engineering
study of the location with full consideration of the human
factors involved."
References
Beyer, William H.
CRC Handbook of Tables for Probability and Statistics.
The Chemical Rubber Co., Pub. 1966.
Federal Highway Administration, Office of Traffic Operation, Washington,
D.C. Lacy, J.D. Personal Communication.
Forbes, Theodore W.
Predicting attention-gaining characteristics of
highway traffic signs:
measurement technique.
Human Factors. J5,
1964, 371-374.
Forbes, T.W. (Ed.)
Human Factors in Highway Traffic Safety Research,
Wiley-Interscience, div. John Wiley and Sons, Inc.
Forbes, T.W., Pain, R.F., Fry, J.P. Jr., and Joyce, R.P.
1972.
Color and
brightness factors in simulated and full-scale traffic sign vis­
ibility, Highway Research Record. 216. 1968, 55-65.
Forbes, T.W., Pain, R.F., Fry, J.P.
Jr., and Joyce, R.P.
Effect of
sign position and brightness on seeing simulated highway signs.
Highway Research Record, 164, 1967, 22-37.
Forbes, T.W., Moscowitz, K., and Morgan, G.A.
A comparison of lower
case and capital letters for highway signs.
search
Board.
Hake, Harold W.
Proc. Highway Re­
1950, 30, 355-373.
Form Discrimination and the Invariance of Form,
in
Pattern Recognition, Uhr, Leonard (Ed.) John Wiley and Sons, Inc.
1966.
Mourant, R.R., Rockwell, T.H., and Rackoff, N.j.
ments and visual workload.
Drivers' eye move­
Highway Research Record, 292, 1970,
1-10.
Pain, Righard, Brightness ratio as factors in the attention value of
highway signs, Highway Research Record, 275, 1969,32-40.
Tinker, Miles A.
Press, 1963.
Legibility of Print, Ames, Iowa State University
39
Tinker, M.A. and Paterson, D.G.
Typography1 and legibility in Reading.
Handbook of Applied Psychology1, Fryer, D. and Henry, E. (Ed.).
Rinehart and Co. 1950, I, 59, 59.
Thomas, E 1L 4 Movements of the eye.
Scientific American, August 1968,
85-95.
Treisman, A.M.
Strategies and models of selective attention.
Psych.
. Review, 76, 1969, 282-299.
U.S. Department of Transportation, Manual on Uniform Traffic Control
Devices, Federal Highway Administration, 1971.
U.S. Department of Transportation, Standard Alphabets for Highway Sighs.
Federal Highway Administration, Office of Traffic Operations, 1966.
U.S. Department of Transportation, Standard Highway Signs, Federal
Highway Administration, 1972.
Appendix
41
SIGN
TDE .
SIGN
TDE.
-
6
pedal right
6
signal right
9
signal left
7
pedal right
steer left
8
steer right
7
steer right
9
signal left
10
pedal right
8
steer left
6
pedal right
6
steer left
9
signal right
10
.9
signal left
.
7
steer right
8
signal right
9
steer right
7
steer left
.6
signal left
10
signal right
-------------------------------------------------------------------------- - I
COMMENTS:
CELL #
DATE '
TIME '
Figure A.
Experimental Recording Sheet
42
This study is concerned with highway signing and driver response
time.
You will be required to accomplish two tasks in succession, one
right after the other. First, a scene will appear on the screen.
Like
this _ . While maintaining normal driving position i.e., hands on wheel
and your right.foot on the accelerator pedal, you are to count any items
you can identify in this picture in the following manner:
1
...
2 ........
^
(fence post)
(trees)
(clouds) etc.
Count these items OUT LOUD as accurately and as quickly as you can.
If
you are unable to find the next number of items, you must immediately
return to I and begin again.
You must continue to count in this manner
until a sign appears in the picture.
At this time, stop counting, read
the message on the sign, and respond by doing what the sign says:
RIGHT, STEER LEFT, PEDAL RIGHT, SIGNAL RIGHT, STEER LEFT.
STEER
(DEMONSTRATE)
Respond to the sign as quickly as you can and as accurately as you can.
This sign will remain on the screen for only a very few seconds.
Soon after you respond, the sign will disappear and you will begin
counting again.
This will begin your next trial.
You may repeat any
items used in previous trials plus any new ones you choose. You may re­
use any item anytime you return to I and begin again.
a practice set to get you acquainted with the task.
Now we will begin
Count and respond
to this practice set just as you would the real thing.
After the five
practice trials, a slide will appear upside-down - that will be the cue
that the practice is over and the experiment is about to begin.
REMEMBER:
COUNT OUT LOUD - IHE OBJECT IS TO GET YOU TO LOOK ALL OVER
THE PICTURE SO DON'T JUST STICK TO ONE PLACE.
RESPOND TO THE SIGN AS QUICKLY AS POSSIBLE AND AS ACCURATELY
AS POSSIBLE.
WRONG RESPONSES WASTE TIME AND ONLY CORRECT
RESPONSES CAN STOP THE CLOCK.
Figure B.
Primary and Auxiliary Task Instructions
43
Figure C.
Examples of the posted located sign messages
44
Figure D.
Examples of pavement located sign messages
45
Figure E.
Controls, timing equipment, and dual tachistoscope (Top), and
the experimental simulator unit (Bottom)
MONTANA STATE UNIVERSITY LIBRARIES
3
762 1001 2921 O
Eissonnette, Kathleen
Highway road signs
A/3
6 '/C>
Download