Small-Scale RCTs in Clinical Practice: Word Finding, Grammar and Inferencing Susan Ebbels Moor House School, Hurst Green, Surrey, UK and Division of Psychology and Language, University College London Four small-scale RCTs 1. Verb semantic therapy and ‘Shape Coding’ therapy for verb argument structure (Ebbels, van der Lely & Dockrell, 2007) 2. ‘Shape Coding’ therapy for grammatical comprehension (Ebbels, Marić, Murphy & Turner, submitted) 3. Therapy for word finding difficulties (Ebbels, Nicoll, Clark, Eachus, Gallagher, Horniman, Jennings, McEvoy, Nimmo & Turner, 2012) 4. Inferencing Four small-scale RCTs • Design ‘Blind’ assessment (student SLTs / other researchers / SALTA) therapy group(s) vs. control group Control group receive same amount of intervention, but on a different topic (usually unspecified) • Participants From one special school for children with primary language impairments severe language difficulties >9 years old (mostly >11 years) Four small-scale RCTs • Therapy 1:1 With SLT • Statistical analyses First checked groups similar pre-therapy Compared progress (or normalised gain) between groups - reduces variability For each group, compared progress to zero Verb argument structure 27 participants (11;0-16;1) 9 9 Verb semantic therapy Shape Coding therapy 9 Control therapy (inferencing) Ebbels et al., 2007, JSLHR, 50, 1330-1349 Therapy • 1 x 30 minutes per week for 9 weeks (4.5 hours total) • 1:1 with SLT (me!) • Normal therapy and classroom teaching continued in addition on different topics Ebbels et al., 2007, JSLHR, 50, 1330-1349 Verb semantic therapy • SLT and child jointly devise a definition of verb with as much detail as possible “pour” • e.g., “fill” make something full to the top put something in it liquid or lots of things go down together to a new place container is tipping • SLT / child act out event and the other decides if event fits all the criteria (i.e., can be described by verb or not) Ebbels et al., 2007, JSLHR, 50, 1330-1349 Shape Coding therapy • Teach verbs in groups according to their broad meaning • Teach matching constructions/argument structures using Shape Coding templates • E.g: You are pouring the water in the glass MOVES CHANGES You are filling the glass with water Ebbels et al., 2007, JSLHR, 50, 1330-1349 Normalised gain Normalised gain score 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 -0.2 -0.4 -0.6 -0.8 -1.0 • Difference in gain between therapy groups and controls sig (p<0.04, d>1.2) • Two therapy groups equal gain • Gains significantly greater than zero at both timepoints for both therapy groups (p<0.02, d>0.7) to post-therapy to follow-up Semantic Shape Coding Therapy Group Control • Therefore progress maintained Ebbels et al., 2007, JSLHR, 50, 1330-1349 Conclusions • 4.5 hours of therapy can improve verb argument structure • Both therapy methods equally effective • (Progress generalised to other verbs, but not to morphology) • Progress maintained for 3 months Comprehension of conjunctions 14 participants (11;3-16;1) 7 Phase 1 Phase 2 Shape Coding Therapy Other therapy 7 Other therapy Shape Coding Therapy Ebbels et al., 2012, submitted Therapy • 1x 30 mins per week for 8 weeks (4 hours total) • 1:1 with own SLT • Using Shape Coding as a visual support Start by comparing ‘and’ and ‘but not’ Then add in ‘neither nor’ Then add in ‘not only but also’ • SLTs given sequence of steps to work through at child’s own pace, stopping after 8 sessions, regardless of step reached Ebbels et al., 2012, submitted Change in score 0.50 Phase 1 0.40 • Therapy group more progress than controls after Phase 1 (p=0.01, d=1.64) Phase 2 • After Phase 2, both groups equal progress 0.30 0.20 • Both groups made sig progress with therapy (p<.04, d>0.8) 0.10 0.00 -0.10 -0.20 -0.30 Original therapy group Waiting controls • Progress maintained by original group Ebbels et al., 2012, submitted Conclusions • 4 hours of therapy improved comprehension of conjunctions • (Progress led to significant change in standard score on TROG) • (no significant predictors of progress, including Pre-therapy language scores NV IQ (Matrices) Visual-perceptual skills Age) • Progress maintained for 4 months Word finding 21 participants (aged 9;9-15;11) Phase 1 Phase 2 8 Semantic therapy 6 Phonological therapy 7 Other therapy Semantic therapy Ebbels et al., 2012, IJLCD, 47: 35-51 Unfortunately…… • Pupils allocated to groups before testing completed • When analysed, phonological group different from semantic and control groups. 4/6 were within normal limits on the TAWF (0/8 in semantic and control groups) • Therefore, unwise to compare all three groups • Therefore, all analyses only on the semantic and control groups. Can tell us whether the semantic therapy effective, but can’t tell us anything about the phonological therapy Semantic therapy • 2 x 15 mins per week for 8 weeks (4 hours total) • 1:1 with own SLT • Sort photo cards of one category (animals, clothes, food) into semantic sub-categories (broad, then narrower) • Discuss attributes of pictures • Compare attributes of different pictures • 20 questions giving cues • Dice game – throw dice, pick up picture, give the cue on dice for that picture Ebbels et al., 2012, IJLCD, 47: 35-51 Change in Standard Score 25 Phase 1 20 • Therapy group more progress than controls after Phase 1 (p=0.04, d=1.0) Phase 2 • After Phase 2, both groups equal progress 15 10 • Both groups made sig progress with therapy (p<.02, d>0.8) 5 0 -5 -10 Original therapy group • Progress maintained by Waiting controls original group Ebbels et al., 2012, IJLCD, 47: 35-51 Conclusions • 4 hours of semantic therapy improved word finding on a standardised test • (no significant predictors of progress, including Pre-therapy word finding ability Pre-therapy language scores “Fast” vs. “slow namers”) • (effects of therapy did not generalise to discourse) • Progress maintained for 5 months Inferencing 54 participants (aged 10;7-16;0) Phase 1 Phase 2 18 Lexical inference therapy 18 Mental imagery therapy 18 Other therapy CANCELLED Therapy • 1 x 30 mins per week for 9 weeks (4.5 hours total) • 1:1 with own SLT • Lexical inference, based on Yuill & Oakhill (1988) • Mental Imagery - based on Visualising and Verbalising (Bell, 1991) • Waiting controls – other therapy, but class work supported by SLT in English continued, which often included work on inferencing Change in score 0.16 • No sig dif in progress between groups (p=0.9) 0.14 0.12 0.10 0.08 • All groups (including controls) made sig progress (p<.02, d>0.6) 0.06 0.04 0.02 0.00 -0.02 -0.04 Lexical inferencing Mental Imagery Waiting controls Conclusions • On average, all groups improved (although lots of variability). • Progress could have been due to: 1. Practice effect (doing the test twice) AND/OR 2. The work which continued elsewhere in the school curriculum which often included work on inferencing • Cannot distinguish between these as no control group who were not in the school. • However, adding 4.5 hours of 1:1 therapy on inferencing to the rest of the school curriculum did not lead to significantly greater progress Summary of four RCTs Verb argument structure Cn of Word conjunctions finding Inferencing Participants 27 14 21 → 15 54 Number of groups 3 2 3→2 3 Group size 9 7 7 or 8 18 Therapy > control? yes yes yes no Effect size of therapy vs. control >1.2 1.64 1.0 <0.2 Progress >0 yes yes yes yes Effect size of progress vs. 0 >0.7 >0.8 >0.8 >0.6 What I’ve learned! • Check groups are equivalent pre-therapy (see WFD study) • Have a proper control group who do not have any access to any other teaching / therapy on the topic (see inferencing study) • The waiting controls get the best deal – only receive therapy if proven effective (no ethical issue!) • The effect size is more important than the group size Both for finding a significant effect And for clinical value Effect sizes and significance • Effect size (d) is: difference between groups, or between progress and zero (bigger differences increase effect sizes) DIVIDED BY standard deviation (greater variability decreases effect sizes) • Significance depends on Effect size (larger effect sizes increase significance) Number of participants in a group (more participants increases chance of finding a significant effect, if there is one to find – increases “power”). So…. • If your effect size is big enough, you don’t need large groups to find a significant effect • I found significant effects in three studies for Group sizes of 7 or more With effect sizes of d>0.8 To conclude… • Small-scale RCTs are not too hard to carry out as part of clinical practice • lack of significance in small-scale RCTs is hard to interpret (can’t be used to disprove effectiveness due to insufficient power) • BUT, a significant effect is likely to be clinically important • Therefore, the effort of doing small-scale RCTs in clinical practice is worthwhile to find clinically important effects. Questions?