Document 13399344

advertisement
24.964
Phonetic Realization
Articulatory Phonology
Readings for next time
• We will continue to talk about articulatory phonology,
moving on to the broader issue of the phonetics and
phonology of consonant releases.
• Review Gafos (2002), Steriade (1997), Jun (2002).
• I’ll also post Zsiga (2000) on overlap in consonant clusters
in Russian and English and Chitoran et al (2002) on
Georgian.
Duration compensation
• The ‘weighted constraints’ approach to modeling phonetic
realization is particularly well suited to situations in which
realization is analyzed as a compromise between
conflicting requirements.
– e.g. F2 transitions as a compromise between realizing targets and
minimzing movement.
• In principle allows for the influences of multiple factors on
the final outcome.
• Patterns of duration show both properties - many factors
affect segment duration, and resulting durations often seem
to involve compromise between these demands.
• Role of conflict and compromise is particularly clear in
duration compensation.
Duration compensation in Thai
• Data from Morén and Zsiga
(2006). Similar patterns in Zhang( 2004).
Compensation between V and coda:
• Codas are longer where V is short
• Long V is shorter in closed
syllable.
• Net effect: all rhyme types are
quite similar in duration in spite of
large differences in V durations.
Subject 1
CVV
.231
CVS
.106
CVO
.100 .118
.102
CVVS
.174
.079
CVVO
.189
.063
0
0.05
0.1
0.15 0.2
Seconds
0.25
0.3
0.25
0.3
Subject 2
CVV
.173
CVS .072
CVO .068
.080
.081
CVVS
.104
CVVO
.122
.063
0.05 0.1
0.15 0.2
Seconds
0
.068
Image by MIT OpenCourseWare. Adapted from Morén, B., and E. Zsiga. "The Lexical and Post-lexical Phonology of
Thai Tones." Natural Langauge and Linguistic Theory 24 (2006): 113-178.
Cantonese (Gordon 1998)
• Again: nasal coda is longer after short V, closed syllable V
shortening. Also pre-obstruent shortening. (cf. Zee 2002).
Contour supported
350
300
250
200
150
100
50
0
301
283
Contour not
supported
275
208
99
77
150
a
am
a:m
ap
a:p
Cantonese sonorous rhyme duration (ms)
Image by MIT OpenCourseWare. Adapted from Zhang, Jie. "The Role of Contrast-Specific and
Language-Specific Phonetics in Contour Tone Distribution." Phonetically-Based Phonology.
Edited by Robert Kirchner, Bruce Hayes, and Donca Steriade. New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press, 2004.
Duration compensation
• Longstanding idea: compensation between the duration of
segments within the same constituent (syllable, foot,
word).
• In the simplest case, the duration of the constituent is
constant, so adding or lengthening a segment must be
compensated by equal shortening of other segments.
• Total compensation is rare. More typical is the situation
observed in Thai - partial compensation:
– Coda C is longer after a short V, shorter after a long V,
but V:C is still longer than VC.
– Difference in coda durations does not equal difference
between V and V:
Duration compensation
Subject 1
• Further compensatory
relationships within the rhyme:
– V: is shorter in closed syllables
– V: is longer before shorter C (O
vs. S) (significant?) • Mutual compensation between V
and coda C has been observed in
English monosyllables (Munhall et
al 1992).
CVV
.231
CVS
.106
CVO
.100
.118
.102
CVVS
.174
.079
CVVO
.189
.063
0
0.05
0.1
0.15 0.2
Seconds
0.25
0.3
0.25
0.3
Subject 2
CVV
CVS
.173
.072
CVO .068
CVVS .104
CVVO
.122
0
0.05
.080
.081
.068
.063
0.1
0.15 0.2
Seconds
Image by MIT OpenCourseWare. Adapted from Morén, B., and E. Zsiga. "The Lexical and Post-lexical Phonology
of Thai Tones." Natural Langauge and Linguistic Theory 24 (2006): 113—178.
Modeling duration compensation
• Duration compensation requires complex rules if duration
is assigned to segments by context-dependent rules (e.g.
Klatt 1979) due to interdependence of segment durations.
• Some researchers have proposed top-down models to
account for duration compensation: duration is assigned to
syllables then divided up between segments (e.g. Kohler
1986, Campbell 1992). – Partial compensation is problematic. • In a constraint-based model it is possible to assign targets
to individual segments and to larger constituents.
– With weighted constraints, segment durations are a compromise
between segment and constituent requirements. – Partial compensation. Modeling duration compensation
• Simple example: Rhyme compensation
–
–
–
–
Targets for vowel, TV, coda, TC, Rhyme, TR.
Actual durations: DV, DT, DR.
Constraint: Di = Ti
cost:
wi(Di - Ti)2
Total cost for VC rhyme:
wV(DV - TV)2 + wC(DC - TC)2 + wR(DR - TR)2
• Conflict arises if Tv+TC ≠ TR.
• Implementation using Excel solver.
• Additional constraints are required. E.g. pre-obstruent
shortening in Cantonese.
• Compensation can be observed across syllable boundaries.
Additional applications: VOT
120
110
initial
stops
/p/
/t/
100
/k/
final consonants
/pt/
/n/
tion
90
ura
/kIn/
lD
T=
Vow
e
80
/kIpt/
/tIn/
VO
70
/pIn/
io
/tIpt/
n
60
D
ur
at
n
tio
ura
Vo
w
2
=1
/
O
T
40
0
lD
we
el
50
V
VOT in ms
• Port and Rotunno (1979)
found that in English VOT
increases with duration of the
following vowel,
– but VOT is not a fixed
proportion of the vowel.
• Could be targets for VOT,
voiced vowel duration and
total vowel duration.
– but intercept is different
for tense vowels.
0
Vo
/3
/pIpt/
1
T=
VO
70 80 90 100 110 120 130 140 150 160 170 180 190 200 210
Vowel Duration in ms for /I/
Image by MIT OpenCourseWare. Adapted from Port, Robert F., and Rosemarie
Rotunno."Relation Between Voice-Onset Time and Vowel Duration." The
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 66, no. 3 (September 1979): 654-662.
Additional applications: VOT
110
100
90
VOT in ms for initial /t/
• Port and Rotunno (1979) found that in English VOT increases with duration of the
following vowel,
– but VOT is not a fixed proportion of the vowel.
• Could be targets for VOT,
voiced vowel duration and
total vowel duration.
– but intercept is different for tense vowels.
T=
VO
80
2
1/
/tIpt/
70
50
1/3
T=
VO
on
ati
/tIn/
/tIn/
/tIpt/
60
we
Vo
ur
lD
l
we
Vo
ion
rat
Du
vowels
/I/
final consonants
/pt/
/n/
/i/
40
0
0
100
120
140
160
180
200
220
240
260
280
300
Vowel Duration in ms
Image by MIT OpenCourseWare. Adapted from Port, Robert F., and Rosemarie
Rotunno."Relation Between Voice-Onset Time and Vowel Duration." The
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 66, no. 3 (September 1979): 654-662.
��
Articulatory Phonology
• Theory developed by Browman and Goldstein (1986,
1987, 1989 etc).
• Not a theory of phonology.
• The basic unit of articulatory control is the gesture.
• A gesture specifies the formation of a linguistically
significant constriction.
• Defined within the framework of Task Dynamics
(Saltzmann and Munhall 1989).
Articulatory Phonology
• A gesture specifies the
formation of a linguistically
significant constriction.
• The goals of gestures are
defined in terms of tract
variables (e.g. lip aperture).
• Movement towards a
particular value of a tract
variable is typically achieved
by a set of articulators.
• A gesture takes a tract
variable from its current value
towards the target value.
Tract variable
Articulators involved
LP
lip protrusion
upper and lower lips, jaw
LA
lip aperture
upper and lower lips, jaw
TTCL
TTCD
TBCL
TBCD
tongue-tip constriction location
tongue-tip constriction degree
tongue-body constriction location
tongue-body constriction degree
tongue-tip, tongue-body, jaw
tongue-tip, tongue-body, jaw
tongue-body, jaw
tongue-body, jaw
VEL
GLO
velic aperture
glottal aperture
velum
glottis
TBCL
VEL
velum
TTCL
TBCD
TTCD
LA
LP
upper lip
tongue tip
tongue-body
centre
jaw
lower lip
GLO
glottis
Image by MIT OpenCourseWare. Adapted from Haskins Laboratory's Introduction to
Articulatory Phonology and the Gestural Computational Model. Originally in Browman, C. P.,
and Goldstein, L. "Articulatory Gestures as Phonological Units." Journal of Phonetics 18 (1990): 299-320.
Articulatory Phonology
• Since a gesture involves the formation of a constriction it
is usually specified by:
– constriction degree
– (constriction location)
– (constriction shape) – stiffness • In the Task Dynamic model, movement along a tract
variable is modeled as a spring-mass system.
• In Browman and Goldstein’s model critical damping is
assumed, so articulators move towards the target position
on the tract variable in a non-linear, assymptoting motion.
Damped mass-spring model
x
friction
Ff=-bv
x0
m
spring
Fs=-k(x-x0)
• Hooke’s Law (linear spring): Fs = k(x x0 )
• Friction:
F f = bv = bẋ
• Newton’s 2nd Law:
F = ma = mẋ˙
m˙ẋ = bẋ k(x x0 )
• Equate:
m˙ẋ + bẋ + k(x x0 ) = 0
Damped mass-spring model
x
friction
F=-bv
x0
m
spring
F=-k(x-x0)
• If there’s no damping (b = 0), then the solution is
sinusoidal oscillation.
• B&G assume critical damping (no oscillation):
x(t) = (A + Bt)e
−
k
t
m
Damped mass-spring model
1.2
1
0.8
x(t) = (A + Bt)e
0.6
0.4
−
0.2
0
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
• Gesture moves towards its target along an exponential trajectory, never
quite reaching the target.
• If stiffness, k, is higher, tract variable changes faster.
• So a gesture specifies a movement from current tract variable values
towards target values, following an exponential trajectory.
• Speech movements do show characteristics of being generated by a
second order dynamical system (a damped ‘mass-spring’ system)
k
t
m
• In the movements of a damped massspring system, peak velocity is
proportional to displacement (distance
moved).
slope depends on stiffness k.
POS CM.
• This relationship has often been
observed in arm movements and speech
articulator movements.
• E.g. Ostry & Munhall (1985) studied
tongue body movements during [ku, ko,
ka, gu, go, ga] at two speech rates.
5.47
0.650
o
0.780
0.998
1.070
1.210
1.358
1.210
1.358
Time (S)
VEL.
12
T
0
Vmax
-11
0.650
0.780
1.070
20
0.998
1.070
Time (S)
Subject SG
18
16
14
12
10
8
6
4
2
0
20
Subject AD
0
2
4
6
8
10
12
14
16
18
20
Total movement amplitude of tongue dorsum in mm
VOICE
0.780
0
0
0.998
Time (S)
0.650
8
6
4
2
18
16
14
12
10
8
6
4
2
8.81
8.04
Subject CB
18
16
14
12
10
Maximum velocity of tongue dorsum in cm/s
–
20
1.210
1.358
Image by MIT OpenCourseWare. Adapted from Ostry, D. J., and Munhall K. G.
"Control of Rate and Duration of Speech Movements." Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America 77 (1985): 640-8.��
Image by MIT OpenCourseWare. Adapted from Ostry, D. J., and Munhall
K. G. "Control of Rate and Duration of Speech Movements." Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America 77 (1985): 640-8.��
Articulatory Phonology
• Gestures are coordinated together to produce utterances
(represented in the ‘gestural score’ format).
Input String: /1paam/;
velum
Tongue-body
constriction
degree
phar narrow
lab
clo
lab
clo
glot
100
Velic aperture
Lip aperture
Glottal aperture
200
300
400
Time (msec.)
Image by MIT OpenCourseWare. Adpated from Browman, C. P., and Goldstein, L. "Articulatory Gestures as Phonological Units."
Journal of Phonetics 18 (1990): 299-320.
Gestural overlap
• Overlap is the basic mechanism for modeling coarticulation ­
coarticulation as coproduction (Fowler 1980).
– E.g. vowel gestures will typically overlap with consonant
gestures.
• When two gestures involve the same tract variables (e.g. vowels and
velars, two vowels), blending results (a compromise between the
demands of the two simultaneously active gestures).
– In CV blending, consonant constriction prevails. – Constriction location is averaged. • Coarticulatory effects will also result from the fact that gestures
specify movement from the current location to form a particular
constriction, so the articulator movements resulting from a given
gesture will depend on the initial state of the articulators.
Timing and coordination
• In Articulatory Phonology, coordination is specified in terms of the cycle of an
abstract undamped spring-mass system with the same stiffness as the actual
critically damped gesture.
• The onset of a gesture is 0°, the target is taken to be achieved at 240°, and the
release at 290°.
• In Browman and Goldstein (1990, 1995), coordination is assumed to be
achieved by rules specifying simultaneity of particular points in the cycles of
two gestures.
– e.g. in -C1C2- cluster 0° in C2 is aligned to 240° in C1.
• So timing is specified in terms of coordination of landmarks internal to
gestures, not via specified durations and an external clock.
Phasing rules
• Provisional rules for coordinating gestures in English: (1) A vocalic gesture and the leftmost consonantal gesture of an associated consonant
sequence are phased with respect to each other. An associated consonant sequence is defined
as a sequence of gestures on the C tier, all of which are associated with the same vocalic
gesture, and all of which are contiguous when projected onto the one-dimensional oral tier.
(2a) A vocalic gesture and the leftmost consonantal gesture of a preceding associated
sequence are phased so that the target of the consonantal gesture (240 degrees) coincides with
a point after the target of the vowel (about 330 degrees). This is abbreviated as follows:
C(240) = = V (330)
peep op
Excerpted from Browman, Catherine P., and Louis
Goldstein. “Tiers in articulatory phonology, with
some implications for casual speech.” In Papers in
Laboratory Phonology I: Between the Grammar and
Physics of Speech. Edited by John Beckman and
Mary Kingston. New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press, 1990. ISBN: 978-0521368087 .
Audio waveform
i
Tongue rear
(horizontal)
a
Tongue blade
(vertical)
�
0
20
�
�
40
60
80
100
Lower lip
(vertical)
120
Time (frames)
Image by MIT OpenCourseWare. Adapted from Browman, Catherine P., and Louis Goldstein. “Tiers in
Articulatory Phonology, with Some Implications for Casual Speech.” In Papers in Laboratory
Phonology I: Between the Grammar and Physics of Speech.. Edited by John Beckman and
Mary Kingston. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, 1990, pp. 341-376
Gafos (2002)
• Analyzes gestural coordination in terms of OT constraints.
• Assumes coordination operates in terms of a few
landmarks in gestures: Onset, Target, C-Center, Release,
Release offset.
ALIGN (G1, landmark1, G2, landmark2): Align
landmark1 of G1 to landmark2 of G2
Landmarki takes values from the set {ONSET,
TARGET, C-CENTER, RELEASE}
Excerpted from Gafos, A. “A Grammar of Gestural Coordination.”Natural Language and Linguistic
Theory 20 (2002): 269-337.
Target
C-Centre Release
Onset
Image by MIT OpenCourseWare. Excerpted from Gafos, A. “A Grammar of Gestural Coordination.”
Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 20 (2002): 269-337.
Overlap and stop releases
• In consonant clusters, the presence or absence of stop releases can
depend on the patterns of coordination between consonants. – Close vs. Open transition CC-COORD = ALIGN(C1, C-CENTER, C2, ONSET)
C-Center
Release
Target
Open Vocal
Tract
Onset
Image by MIT OpenCourseWare. Adapted from Gafos, A. “A Grammar of Gestural Coordination.”
Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 20 (2002): 269-337.
A.
cc1
r1t2
O2
B.
roff1
r1
t2
O2
Image by MIT OpenCourseWare. Adapted from Gafos, A. “A Grammar of Gestural Coordination.” Natural Language
and Linguistic Theory 20 (2002): 269-337.
References
• Browman, C. and L. Goldstein (1986). Toward an articulatory phonology.
Phonology yearbook 3, 219-252.
• Browman, C. and L. Goldstein (1989). Articulatory gestures as phonological
units. Phonology 6, 201-252.
• Browman, C. P.,& Goldstein, L. (1990). Tiers in articulatory phonology, with
some implications for casual speech. In J. Kingston and M. E. Beckman (eds),
Papers in Laboratory Phonology I: Between the Grammar and the Physics of
Speech. Cambridge, U. K.: Cambridge University Press. (pp.341-376).
• Campbell, Nick (1992). ‘Multi-level timing in speech’. ATR Technical Report
• Chitoran, I. (1998) Georgian harmonic clusters: Phonetic cues to phonological
representation. Phonology 15:2. 121-141
• Chitoran, I., L. Goldstein, and D. Byrd (2002) Gestural Overlap and
Recoverability: Articulatory Evidence from Georgian. In C. Gussenhoven and
N. Warner (eds.) Laboratory Phonology 7. Berlin, New York: Mouton de
Gruyter. 419-447
• Gafos, A. (2002). A grammar of gestural coordination. Natural Language &
Linguistic Theory 20, 269-337.
• Klatt, Dennis H. (1979). ‘Synthesis by rule of segmental durations in English
sentences’. Björn Lindblom and Sven Öhman (eds) Frontiers in Speech
Communication Research, Academic Press, New York, 287-300.
References
• Kohler, Klaus J. (1986). ‘Invariance and variability in speech timing: From
utterance to segment in German’. J.S. Perkell and D.H. Klatt (eds) Invariance
and Variability in Speech Processes, LEA, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 268-289.
• Morén, B. and E. Zsiga (2006) The Lexical and Post-lexical Phonology of
Thai Tones. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory.
• Munhall, Fowler, Hawkins & Saltzman (1992). Compensatory shortening.
Journal of Phonetics 20, 225-239.
• Ostry D. J. and Munhall K. G. (1985). Control of rate and duration of speech
movements. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 77: 640-8.
• Port, R. F. and R. Rotunno (1979) Relation between voice-onset time and
vowel duration. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 66, 654-662
• Zhang, Jie (2004). The role of contrast-specific and language-specific
phonetics in contour tone distribution. Robert Kirchner, Bruce Hayes & Donca
Steriade (eds). Phonetically-Based Phonology. CUP, Cambridge.
MIT OpenCourseWare
http://ocw.mit.edu
24.964 Topics in Phonology: Phonetic Realization
Fall 2006
For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms.
Download