Reviews in Fisheries Science, 17(4):468–477, 2009 ISSN: 1064-1262 print DOI: 10.1080/10641260902985096 Feeding Preference of the Rio Grande Silvery Minnow ( Hybognathus amarus) HUGO A. MAGANA Downloaded By: [MAGANA, HUGO A.] At: 16:23 17 July 2009 USDA Forest Service, Rocky Mountain Research Station, Albuquerque, New Mexico, USA The Rio Grande silvery minnow (Hybognathus amarus) was historically the most abundant fish in the Rio Grande Basin. However, populations have been declining to the point of being listed under the Endangered Species Act. Potential causes for the decline have been studied, yet little attention has been paid to food resources. This study had three objectives: (1) Determine whether larval fish show a substrate preference when foraging. (2) Determine whether larval fish have a diatom preference when presented with 15 diatom species over six feeding trials. (3) I investigated the possibility of training/conditioning H. amarus to feed on natural food sources (diatoms) and observe conditioning response (reaction time to feeding). I found no difference between substrate preference (p = 0.26). Results for feeding trials 1, 2, and 3 revealed a preference for Nitzschia palea (p < 0.01). Trial 4 revealed a preference for N. paleaformis (p < 0.01). Navicula veneta was the preferred diatom species in feeding trial 5. Nitzschia cf. intermedia was preferred in trial 6 (p < 0.03). Results from these feeding trials proved that H. amarus larvae learn quickly and can be trained to feed on diatom cultures after only one 30-min exposure. Pre-conditioned H. amarus arrived at diatoms cultures in 49 sec ± 39 sec compared to non-conditioned H. amarus, which arrived at diatom cultures at 250 sec ± 550 sec. Keywords diatoms, feeding preference, Rio Grande silvery minnow, Hybognathus amarus INTRODUCTION The federally endangered Rio Grande silvery minnow (USFWS, 1994) was historically the most abundant fish in the Rio Grande Basin (Bestgen and Platania, 1991); however, H. amarus populations have been declining to the point of being listed under the Endangered Species Act (USFWS, 1994). From its headwaters in the San Juan Mountains of Colorado, the Rio Grande flows through a series of structural basins, where the alluvial valley is very wide, separated by intervening canyons where the valley is narrow (Schmidt et al., 2003). The occurrence of wide alluvial valleys and intervening narrow canyons is important in analyzing channel adjustment to the regulation of stream flow and sediment flux (Schmidt et al., 2003). Historically, the Rio Grande had a mobile bed and erodible banks, and the channel changed from year to year. Today’s channel is smaller, more stable, changes less from year to year, and infrequently inundates its former floodplain (Schmidt et al., 2003). The species’ This article is not subject to U.S. copyright law. Address correspondence to Dr. Hugo A. Magana, USDA Forest Service, Rocky Mountain Research Station, 333 Broadway Blvd #115, Albuquerque, NM 87102. E-mail: hmagana@fs.fed.us steady decline coincided with flood-control and river channelization projects that began in the 1940s and eventually converted much of the Rio Grande from a wide, shallow, meandering river to a narrow channel fragmented by dams (Ikenson, 2002). Extensive recovery efforts such as artificial propagation facilities, habitat restoration projects, and minnow refugium have been ongoing, yet little research has been performed on H. amarus food resources. This work investigates food awareness, diatom and substrate preference, and conditioning response of the Rio Grande silvery minnow (Hybognathus amarus). Diatoms are a desirable food source over other members of the primary production community through storage of photosynthetically produced sugars in the form of lipids rather than starch (Julius et al., 2007). Therefore, many members of higher trophic levels selectively feed on diatoms when present with other primary producers (Julius et al., 2007). Minnows are generalists and forage on diatoms found in benthic and planktonic communities (Sray, 1998). Other species of the genus Hybognathus feed on “diatoms, algae, larval insect exuvia, and plant material scraped from bottom sediments” (Whitaker, 1977). Adults of the genus Hybognathus are thought to be obligate herbivores because they lack a defined stomach and have a long, narrow, and coiled alimentary tract (Hlohowskyj et al., 1989; Etnier 468 Downloaded By: [MAGANA, HUGO A.] At: 16:23 17 July 2009 FEEDING PREFERENCE OF THE RIO GRANDE SILVERY MINNOW and Starnes, 1993; Ross, 2001). Shirey (2004) quantified gut contents of historical H. amarus specimens collected in 1874. Examination of the 1874 specimens indicates that H. amarus fed on 30 genera and 70 species of diatoms as well as cyanobacteria (Anabaena sp. and Merismopedia sp.), detritus, and pine pollen. The gut contents from H. amarus collected in 1874 revealed that Nitzschia palea and N. paleacaea were the 4th and 5th most dominant diatom taxa (Shirey, 2004). In a pilot study during 2004, I captured 247 H. amarus larvae from a restored floodplain in Los Lunas, NM, and analyzed gut contents for a subset. The H. amarus larvae collected in 2004 revealed that diatoms were the main component of their diet. A total of 13 genera and 15 species of diatoms were identified from the 2004 H. amarus larvae. Typically, 95% of hatchery-raised fish die from predation or starvation in the first few weeks following stocking when they are released into their natural environment (Suboski and Templeton, 1989; Brown and Laland, 2001). Suboski and Templeton (1989) suggest that hatchery fish die of starvation because they do not recognize natural food sources. Wiley et al. (1993) evaluated the potential to train hatchery-raised trout to increase post-stocking survival in streams by simulating natural conditions, feeding them natural foods, and raising them at moderate densities. I propose that hatchery-reared H. amarus can be trained en masse to recognize and feed on natural food resources (diatoms) prior to release, which may help to increase their survival in the wild. This study is unique in that I visually recorded H. amarus feeding habits using natural food sources (unialgal diatom cultures) on agar amended substrates to establish diatom preferences among 15 diatom species. The objectives of this study include the following: (1) determine substrate preference of H. amarus when foraging, (2) ascertain diatom preference of H. amarus among 15 species presented over six feeding trials, (3) determine conditioning response (reaction time to feeding) of pre-conditioned (trained) H. amarus metalarvae. METHODS Diatom Culturing Multi-species periphyton samples were collected from five sites located adjacent to the Middle Rio Grande (MRG) north 469 and south of Albuquerque, NM. Samples were collected in triplicate using three connected, bottomless, five-gallon buckets placed at the river margin. The use of bottomless buckets allowed for isolation of benthic samples from the scouring effect of current flow. Episammic and epipelic algal samples were collected within each bucket using a 100 × 15-mm Petri dish and removed with a spatula (Moulton et al., 2002) and transported to the U.S. Department of Agriculture Forest Service, Rocky Mountain Research Station, Albuquerque, NM. Samples were washed into 1/2 -dram glass vials with Bozniak community growth media (Bozniak, 1969) and placed in environmental growth chambers (10◦ , 15◦ , and 22◦ C at 10:14, 12:12, and 14:10 light/dark photoperiods). A single 20-μL sample was placed onto a microscope slide and examined at 1,000× magnification. Standard Pasteur pipettes (133 mm) were flamed and pulled to a thickness of 0.3 mm. The desired diatom was drawn up into the pipette via capillary action and deposited into a separate sterile drop of water. A new pipette was used to re-isolate the diatom and deposited into a new drop of water. The serial wash process was continued for six drops or until only the chosen diatom remained (Hoshaw and Rosowski, 1969). Individual diatoms were transferred to 50-ml Erlenmeyer flasks containing approximately 3 mm of #30 silica sand as a substrate and 20 ml of Bozniak growth media. After visible algal growth was observed (40–60 days), culture samples with <5% (determined by cell counts) of non-target diatom species were saved as inoculum for feeding trials. Repeated attempts to culture all identified diatoms (38 genera and 120 species) collected from the MRG proved unsuccessful regardless of modifications to growth media. Seven genera and 15 diatom species were successfully cultured at RMRS. Nine species of the genera Nitzschia were cultured, while only one species each of the other six genera was cultured. It is unclear why the Nitzschia species grew well, but not other genera (Table 1). Diatom cultures were processed and permanently mounted (Julius et al., 1997) and microscopically identified using keys by Krammer and Lange-Bertalot (1999). Substrate characteristics may influence diatom growth and foraging preference of fish (Webster and Hart, 2004). To test for differences experimentally, fine-grained sediment and coarsegrained sand (0.44–1.24 mm ± 0.05 mm SE ) were obtained from the margins of the MRG and prepared for diatom culturing. Sediment and sand were autoclaved and prepared by heating 250 ml of Bozniak media in a 1,000-ml beaker and adding 3.75 g of noble agar (Patrick and Wallace, 1953). After Table 1 Fifteen diatom species used in six individual feeding trials (FT) with six replicates each FT #1 Nitzschia palea N. linearis N. paleacea FT #2 FT #3 FT #4 FT #5 N. palea N. linearis N. paleacea N. palea Fragilaria crotonensis Synedra ulna Surirella angusta Cyclotella meneghiniana Nitzschia paleaeformis Navicula veneta Achnanthes suchlandtii Nitzschia palea N. cf. palea Navicula veneta Navicula sp. Nitzschia palea N. capitellata reviews in fisheries science vol. 17 4 2009 FT #6 N. paleaeformis N. cf. intermedia N. palea N. cf. palea Navicula veneta Nitzschia molestiformis 470 H. A. MAGANA boiling, an equal volume of fine-grained sediment or sand was added, mixed thoroughly, and poured into 100 × 15-mm Petri dishes until half full, covered, and sealed with Parafilm (American Can Co., Greenwich, CT, USA). Substrates were allowed to acclimate in environmental growth chambers for three days prior to inoculation. Substrates were inoculated with 1 ml of the diatom inoculum, resealed, and allowed to grow until visible diatom growth was evident (40–60 days). Downloaded By: [MAGANA, HUGO A.] At: 16:23 17 July 2009 Feeding Trials Eight 37.5-L aquaria were maintained at room temperature (23.3–26.5◦ C). Six aquaria were used for feeding trials and two for holding fish. A total of six feeding trials (FT) were performed with six replicates each. All H. amarus larvae used in FT were obtained from the Albuquerque BioPark, Albuquerque, NM. Hybognathus amarus larvae were randomly selected from a single aquarium and placed into one of six aquaria until each aquarium contained 10 fish each for FT 1–3 and six fish each for FT 4–6. Three to six algal cultures were randomly selected in various combinations for each FT. One short cylinder (in the shape of a hockey puck and herein referred to as a “puck”), 21 mm in diameter × 10 mm in height, 346 mm2 ) was removed from each diatom culture Petri dish with a cork borer. Diatom pucks were evenly spaced approximately 30 mm apart on a PlexiglasTM table in a 2 × 3 configuration and placed into each replicate aquarium for presentation and video recording. The three objectives for this study were: (1) determine substrate preference by summing total sampling time per diatom puck over 20 min, (2) determine diatom preference in the same fashion as substrate preference, (3) determine conditioning response. Conditioning response was reported as the time between the introductions of food stimuli to the time of first sampling. I used recently hatched H. amarus protolarvae (4.7–6.7 mm standard length) for feeding trials 1 and 2 to evaluate food awareness. I define food awareness as the time required to locate and sample a diatom puck from the time of introduction. To facilitate documentation of H. amarus food awareness, the front panel of each aquarium was divided into equal quadrants to identify x and y position of each fish in relationship to food stimuli that was placed in the same lower left quadrant for each trial. Food awareness was determined from the quadrant location of each fish in 5-min intervals over the 30-min feeding trial. I used pre-conditioned H. amarus from prior feeding trials (FT 1, 2, and 5) to assess conditioning response to food stimuli. In this study, “conditioned” means that H. amarus larvae were exposed to food stimuli for 30 min in a previous FT and used in the subsequent FT. The time between one FT and a subsequent FT ranged between 5 and 17 days. Larval fish were maintained R on aquarium flake food (TetraMin tropical flakes) between trials. Feeding trials were digitally videotaped (Sony DCRVX2100, 33 frames/sec) for 30 min each, but analysis was limited to the first 20 min since feeding ceased after 20 min. Video was transferred to a computer where substrate and diatom preference and conditioning response were examined in 5-min intervals using video editing software (Cyberlink PowerDirector 4, Cyberlink Corp., Fremont, CA, USA). Statistical Analyses Data that was not normally distributed were log10 transformed to meet the assumptions of ANOVA. Diatom and substrate preference was determined by recording the number of contacts and sampling time of each diatom puck and summing total sampling time recorded per diatom puck over 20 min, and then analyzed using randomized block rank test and Friedman’s statistic testing. Friedman’s test is a nonparametric analysis that may be performed on data from a randomized block experiment and compares the medians of three or more dependent groups. It tests the null hypothesis that the different samples were drawn from distributions with the same median. Alternative hypothesis states that at least one median is different from the rest. Friedman’s test uses ranks instead of original data (1 is assigned to lowest value, etc.). Prior to testing for conditioning response, I assessed food awareness of naive (non-conditioned) larvae. I examined whether H. amarus larvae were randomly distributed among quadrants or preferred one quadrant over another. A multi-response permutation procedure (MRPP) was conducted to test whether there were significant differences between two or more groups of sampling units. Following the completion of the MRPP, principal components (PCA) were estimated from the data, and the first two components were plotted by group to provide a visual description of separation among the groups. This analysis was conducted because total summed frequencies differed among tanks. Component one of the PCA analysis was quadrant 3 compared with quadrants 1, 2, and 4. Component two of the PCA was quadrant 1, and quadrant 2 compared with quadrant 4. The variables analyzed were the summed frequency of fish presence in each quadrant over time interval for feeding trials 1–3. Conditioning response was evaluated using multiple comparison analysis (Dunnet, 1980) based on control trials (non-conditioned). Significance was determined using an alpha = 0.05. Statistical analyses for analysis of variance (ANOVA) R and Tukey’s multiple comparisons were performed with SAS 9.1 statistical programs (SAS Institute Inc. Cary, NC). RESULTS Substrate Preference Results of randomized block design ANOVA indicate no apparent variation associated with diatoms and fine-grained sediment or coarse-grained sand substrate (p = 0.26). When looked at individually, each of the three Nitzschia species (palea, reviews in fisheries science vol. 17 4 2009 FEEDING PREFERENCE OF THE RIO GRANDE SILVERY MINNOW 471 Feeding Duration(Log10 Seconds) 3 2 1 0 -1 linearis palea paleaceae Downloaded By: [MAGANA, HUGO A.] At: 16:23 17 July 2009 substrate linearis Sand palea paleaceae Sedi Figure 1 Notched box plots depicting diatom preference on two different substrates amended with agar (coarse-grained sand vs fine-grained sediment) and growth media for feeding trials 1 and 2. No significant difference was observed (p = 0.26). Notched box-whisker plots were used to present median species responses to the various treatment combinations at each location. “Boxes” are bounded by the first (25%) and third (75%) quartiles. The second quartile, which is the median, is represented as a line within the box. The notched-in (indented) portion of each box represents the 95% confidence interval about the median. Overlapping confidence intervals indicate similar responses between treatments. The “whiskers” show the extent of the rest of the data beyond the box. Whiskers extend to data minimum or maximum. Outliers are defined as data that occur beyond the whiskers. paleacae, and linearis) used in feeding trials 1 and 2 were fed upon equally, regardless of substrate. Results are depicted using notched box plots (Figure 1). Diatom Preference Results from all feeding trials suggest that H. amarus do not feed equally on diatom species from the MRG. The use of video playback in the present study elucidated diatom feeding selectivity of H. amarus in a laboratory setting. Feeding trial videos revealed that H. amarus moved quickly from diatom puck to diatom puck, touching and tasting until selecting a preferred diatom species and feeding. For FT 1–3, there were 3,000 seconds possible (5 min × 60 sec × 10 fish) of feeding time per time interval if all fish feed immediately and continuously. For FT 4–6, there were 1,800 seconds possible (5 min × 60 sec × 6 fish). No significance was indicated for FT one; however, H. amarus protolarvae (4.7–6.7 mm SL) fed more on Nitzschia linearis during the first 10 min, but fed most on N. palea during the last 10 min. Contact time of H. amarus was greater for N. palea (2,665 sec) compared to N. linearis (1,091 sec) and N. paleacae (577 sec) (Figure 2a and Table 2). Results for FT 2 show no significance between diatom species. H. amarus fed equally on the three diatom species during the first 10 min, but increased feeding on N. palea and N. paleacae during the next 5 min. Hybognathus amarus increased feeding on N. linearis eight-fold during the last 5 min, while feeding on N. palea increased slightly and feeding on N. palea- cae decreased by half. Contact time of H. amarus was greater for N. palea (770 sec) compared to N. linearis (657 sec) and N. paleacae (452 sec) (Figure 2b and Table 2). Hybognathus amarus protolarvae from FT 1 and 2 were held over and used as conditioned mesolarvae (6.8–9.2 mm SL) in FT 3. Results for FT 3 indicate a significant difference in diatom preference. Nitzschia palea was the preferred diatom species among the 5 species presented (p < 0.001). Hybognathus amarus fed on N. palea for 1,475 sec during the first 5 min then declined gradually for the remainder of the feeding trial. Contact time of H. amarus was greatest for N. palea (4,399 sec) compared to Synedra ulna (1,070 sec), Fragilaria crotonensis (921 sec), Surirella angusta (766 sec), or Cyclotella meneghiniana (172 sec) (Figure 2c and Table 2). Results from FT 4 using non-conditioned metalarvae (10.2– 12.3 mm SL) indicate a significant difference in diatom species. Nitzschia paleaformis was preferred over other diatoms (p < 0.001) during the entire feeding trial. Contact time was greater for N. paleaformis (2,255 sec) than N. palea (453 sec), Achnanthes suchlandtii (383 sec), Navicula veneta (307 sec), or Nitzschia cf. palea (167 sec) (Figure 2d and Table 2). Conditioned H. amarus mesolarvae (6.8–9.2 mm SL) from FT 4 were employed in FT 5 and preferred Navicula veneta over other diatoms (p < 0.001). Contact time of H. amarus was greatest for Navicula veneta (1,192 sec) than Navicula sp. (601 sec), Nitzschia palea (507 sec), or N. capitellata (311 sec) (Figure 2e and Table 2). Results for FT 6 indicate a significant difference in diatom preference. Nitzschia cf. intermedia was preferred over other diatoms (p < 0.03). Hybognathus amarus sampled N. cf. reviews in fisheries science vol. 17 4 2009 H. A. MAGANA Downloaded By: [MAGANA, HUGO A.] At: 16:23 17 July 2009 472 Figure 2 Graph a corresponds to diatom preference of two-week-old H. amarus protolarvae. Graph b corresponds to diatom preference of four-week-old H. amarus metalarvae. Graph c corresponds to conditioning response of pre-conditioned H. amarus from FT one and two. Graph d corresponds to diatom preference of two-month-old H. amarus mesolarvae. Graphs e and f correspond to >3-month-old H. amarus mesolarvae. intermedia (1,007 sec) more than Navicula veneta (938 sec), Nitzschia palea (616 sec), N. molestiformis (421 sec), N. cf. palea (357 sec), or N. paleaformis (237 sec) (Figure 2f and Table 2). To normalize the data for unequal number of fish per trial, I calculated the number of seconds feeding per fish. Ranking for all FT was in accordance to Friedman’s rank test (Table 3). Conditioning Response Results of PCA indicate that the distribution of summed and proportion of summed frequencies among quadrants did not differ between trials 1 and 2 (non-conditioned fish), but differed for trial 3 (conditioned fish) compared to either trial 1 reviews in fisheries science vol. 17 4 2009 FEEDING PREFERENCE OF THE RIO GRANDE SILVERY MINNOW Table 2 Significance of diatom feeding preference for Rio Grande silvery minnow Table 3 Friedman’s rank test for preferred diatoms in feeding trials FT 1 2 3 4 5 6 Diatom preferred Diatom most eaten Diatom significance Feeding trial Diatom species Nitzschia linearis N. linearis N. palea N. paleaformis Navicula veneta Nitzschia cf. intermedia N. palea N. palea N. palea N. paleaformis N. veneta Nitzschia cf. intermedia None None (p = 0.001) (p = 0.001) (p < 0.001–0.045). (p = 0.001) 1 2 3 4 5 6 Nitzschia palea Nitzschia palea Nitzschia palea Nitzschia paleaformis Navicula veneta Nitzschia cf. intermedia or 2. Activity in trial 3 was concentrated in quadrant III (food location), while activity was more dispersed in trials 1 and 2 (p < 0.001) (Figure 3). Results from feeding trials 1, 2, and 4 indicate that non-conditioned fish were often not immediately attentive to the food stimuli presented. Mean time to first feeding in non-conditioned trials was 250 sec ± 550 sec. Hybognathus amarus used in feeding trials 3 and 5 were pre-conditioned from their use in feeding trials 1, 2, and/or 4, and arrived at diatom pucks in 49 sec ± 39 sec (p = 0.0014) (Figure 4), with some fish commencing feeding within 4 sec of food presentation. DISCUSSION Video playback has been an important tool to ethologists for the past decade, since it can provide important insights into feeding behavior, mate courtship, and visual receptor sensitivity (Kodric-Brown, 1999; Nicoletto and Kodric-Brown, 1999; Rowland, 1999). The use of video playback in the present study allowed for the determination of substrate preference, diatom feeding selectivity, and conditioning response of H. amarus in a laboratory setting. 0.2 Component 2 (0.1514) Downloaded By: [MAGANA, HUGO A.] At: 16:23 17 July 2009 473 Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 0.15 0.1 0.05 -0.4 -0.2 0 -0.05 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 -0.1 -0.15 -0.2 Component 1 (0.7806) Figure 3 Results of the principal components analysis (PCA) for fish location among quadrants for feeding trials 1, 2, and 3. Feeding trials 1 and 2 (nonconditioned fish) compared with feeding trial 3 (conditioned fish). Food was placed in quadrant 3 for all feeding trials. Seventy-eight percent of variation in fish location is explained by component 1, which is quadrant 3 compared with quadrants 1, 2, and 4. Fifteen percent of variation in fish location can be explained by component 2, which is quadrant 1 and quadrant 2 vs quadrant 4. Activity in trial 3 was concentrated in quadrant III, while more dispersed in trials 1 and 2. The results from this study provide unequivocal evidence that H. amarus eat diatoms and prefer certain diatom species over others. Given a suite of diatom species presented, N. palea, N. paleaformis, Navicula veneta, and Nitzschia cf. intermedia were preferred by H. amarus in the feeding trials. Interestingly, the results from the present study concur with results of Shirey (2004), who reported that N. palea and N. paleacae were the 3rd and 5th most abundant diatoms found in digestive tracts of H. amarus specimens collected in 1874. Nitzschia palea and N. paleacae comprised 8% and 7%, respectively, out of 70 diatom species found in 1874 H. amarus specimens. One possible explanation why H. amarus prefer certain diatom species over others is growth form. Scanning electron microscopy (SEM) images of the preferred diatoms reveal that these diatom species have an erect growth form (Figure 5a,b) compared to least preferred diatoms, which have prostrate growth forms (Figure 5c,d). In a concurrent study, I used high-speed photography (500 frames/sec) to elucidate the feeding biomechanics of H. amarus. Many feeding behaviors, especially in larval and juvenile animals, occur at speeds that exceed the resolution of conventional videography, but their visualization is crucial for an extensive understanding of a species’ biology. For example, cycles of feeding behaviors in fishes often occur within a few milliseconds, which would only be captured in 1 or 2 frames of a conventional 30-Hz video camera. However, when filmed at higher speeds (250 or 500 frames/sec), it is possible to distinguish discreet feeding behaviors. Three principal feeding behaviors have been described for fishes, including suction feeding, in which small food particles are moved toward the fish through buccal suction; ram feeding, in which the fish overtakes the food item; and food manipulation, in which the fish dislodges food from a substrate. Hybognathus amarus uses the latter feeding behavior. The observed feeding behavior of H. amarus larvae was to touch the substrate with the premaxilla, rise, and open and close its mouth as if tasting. This process was repeated several times until the larval fish opened its mouth and bit into the substrate, removing the diatom layer and substrate. A scanning electron microscope micrograph (SEM) image (230× magnification) of a preserved two-week-old (6.8–9.2 mm SL) H. amarus larvae revealed putative developing taste papillae on the mandible (Figure 6a). The SEM image (160× magnification) of a preserved six-month-old (60 mm SL) H. amarus show putative developed taste papillae inside the mouth and on the premaxilla (Figure 6b). These reviews in fisheries science vol. 17 4 2009 474 H. A. MAGANA Time to First Feeding (Seconds) 1000 750 500 250 0 -250 Downloaded By: [MAGANA, HUGO A.] At: 16:23 17 July 2009 1 2 3 4 5 6 Feeding Trial Figure 4 Notched box plots depicting results of conditioning response of H. amarus to diatom pucks. Mean time to first feeding is recorded in seconds. The letter “x” on each notched box plot represents the mean, and the line through the box plot represents the median. The small square at the top or bottom of feeding trials 1, 2, and 4 represent the maximum or minimum observations. Non-conditioned fish were used in feeding trials 1, 2, 4, and 6. Pre-conditioned fish from feeding trials 1 and/or 2 were randomly selected for use, as in feeding trial 3. Time to first feeding for feeding trials 3 and 5 were recorded as conditioning response. Non-conditioned fish from feeding trial 4 were randomly selected for use as pre-conditioned fish in feeding trial 5. Feeding trial 6 measured feeding response of older non-conditioned H. amarus. similar structures have been identified as taste papillae of other fish (Kortschal, 1992; Kortschal, 2000; Gomahr et al., 1992). Other research groups have performed feeding studies with H. amarus and improved survivability of post-hatch (4–20 day) larvae, decreasing the mortality rates to less than 1% by providing live food (Artemia naupulii) and improved growth rates by providing manufactured flake and pelleted feed (Caldwell, 2004). While these studies refined captive rearing methods, they did not address naturally occurring food resources in the MRG. With stress placed on our natural resources, many fisheries increasingly rely on restocking from hatchery-reared sources in an attempt to maintain commercially viable populations (Brown and Laland, 2001). While restocking is widely used as a fisheries management tool, it has also been used for the conservation and management of threatened species (Flagg et al., 1995). Many commercial fish species are not able to cope with the transition from life in captivity to life in the wild (Brown and Laland, 2001). On a global scale, 5 × 109 hatchery-reared salmon are released annually, but less than 5% survive to adulthood (McNeil, 1991). To survive to reproductive age, an animal that is released from captivity must have, or quickly acquire, appropriate migratory, feeding, and anti-predator behaviors (Heggberget et al., 1992). The rearing protocol at the Albuquerque BioPark is to transfer two-week-old post-hatch Hybognathus amarus mesolarvae to outdoor tanks filled with filtered city water and fed a mixture of phytoplankton and zooplankton. Hybognathus amarus larvae are then switched to live food (Artemia salina) and then sup- plemented with manufactured flake feed (Silver Cup fish feed, Nelson & Sons, Inc., Murray, UT, USA). While this protocol has proven successful in hatchery settings, it does not mimic natural conditions found in the MRG. Turbidity in the MRG is continually elevated (20–1,200 NTU) (personal communication, David Van Horn, UNM) and light attenuates to zero at 40–50 cm. Low light levels restrict primary productivity to the shallow margins of the river and sand bars, where light penetration is higher (Anderholm et al., 1995). The literature reports that H. amarus larvae eat diatoms in the wild, but the purpose of this study was to see if H. amarus could be trained to feed on natural foods (diatoms) quicker than they would without training, thus increasing probability of surviving in the wild to reproductive age. Using social learning protocols prior to release may help to increase their survival in the wild (Suboski and Templeton, 1989; Laland et al., 2003). Social learning or “local enhancement” is the behavior (or simply the presence) of one individual attracting the attention of another individual to a particular location or stimulus, about which the naive individual subsequently learns something (Brown and Laland, 2003). Learned behavior can result from simple exposure of fish to conspecifics engaged in particular activities. Fish that observe a conspecific consume a novel food later show an increased tendency to eat that food themselves (Suboski and Templeton, 1989). Large-scale training of foraging skills is feasible, relatively simple, and inexpensive to initiate, and could enhance the viability of hatchery fish prior to their release into the wild (Brown and Laland, 2001). reviews in fisheries science vol. 17 4 2009 Downloaded By: [MAGANA, HUGO A.] At: 16:23 17 July 2009 FEEDING PREFERENCE OF THE RIO GRANDE SILVERY MINNOW 475 Figure 5 Erect growth form of preferred diatoms Nitzschia palea (a) and N. paleaformis (b). Prostrate growth forms of least preferred diatoms Nitzschia molestiformis (c) and Achnanthes suchlandtii (d). Figure 6 SEM image (a) of a preserved two-week-old H. amarus (230×). Arrows point to developing putative taste papillae on premaxilla. SEM image (b) of preserved six-month-old H. amarus (160×). Arrows point to putative taste papillae inside mouth and on premaxilla. reviews in fisheries science vol. 17 4 2009 476 H. A. MAGANA Downloaded By: [MAGANA, HUGO A.] At: 16:23 17 July 2009 CONCLUSION When presented with 15 diatom species in a variety of combinations, H. amarus preferred Nitzschia palea in three of the six feeding trials (50%). There was no preference to substrate type regardless of diatom species. Throughout this feeding study, when one fish fed continuously on a diatom puck, cohorts were observed to congregate and commence feeding on the same diatom puck. In making decisions, such as how to find food and mates or avoid predators, many animals utilize information that is produced by others (Brown and Laland, 2003). I have proven that hatchery-reared H. amarus can be trained to recognize and feed on natural food resources (diatoms) after only one 30-min exposure. Using this information and applying it to the hatchery setting may improve survivability of H. amarus in the wild by teaching them to recognize food sources and prevent the extirpation of this fish species in the MRG. This study greatly increases the knowledge base concerning the feeding habits of H. amarus and elucidated some of the preferred diatoms available to the H. amarus in the MRG. Further studies are required to determine if H. amarus cues on taste or nutritive value of diatom species when selecting food sources. This information can be advantageous to H. amarus propagation managers, as well as propagation managers of other threatened or endangered fish species that eat the same foods as H. amarus. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I have many people to thank for their help during this research project. My research was partially funded by the USDA Forest Service, Rocky Mountain Research Station, and the US Bureau of Reclamation (Dr. Michael Porter) (Agreement 02-AI11221602-061). Thanks to Terina Perez and staff (Albuquerque BioPark) for Rio Grande silvery minnow used in feeding trials. Many thanks to Dr. Rudy King (USDAFS, RMRS, Fort Collins, CO) and Dr. Darin Law (USDAFS, RMRS, Albuquerque, NM) for assistance with experimental design and statistical analyses. Rio Grande silvery minnow used in feeding trials were used under USFWS permit TE097324-0 (H. A. Magana). REFERENCES Anderholm, S. K., M. J. Radell, and S. F. Richey. Water-quality assessment of the Rio Grande Valley study unit, Colorado, New Mexico, and Texas: analysis of selected nutrient, suspended-sediment, and pesticide data. Water-resources investigations report (USA) no. 944061 (1995). Bestgen, K. R., and S. P. Platania. Extirpation of Notropis simus simus (Cope) and Notropis orca Woolman (Pices: Cyprinidae) from the Rio Grande in New Mexico, with notes on their life history. Occasion. Papers Museum Southwest. Biol., 6: 1–8 (1991). Bozniak, E. Laboratory and Field Studies of Phytoplankton Communities. Ph.D. dissertation, Washington University, St. Louis, MO. 106 pp. (1969). Brown, C., and K. N. Laland. Social learning and life skills training for hatchery reared fish. J. Fish Biol., 59: 471–493 (2001). Brown, C., and K. N. Laland. Social learning in fishes: A review. Fish Fish., 4: 280–288 (2003). Caldwell, C. Propagation of the Rio Grande silvery minnow: Effects of diet on growth, survival, and performance throughout life history stages. AZ-NM Chapter, Am. Fish. Soc., 35: 6–7 (2004). Dunnett, C. W. Pairwise multiple comparisons in the homogeneous variance, unequal sample size case. J. Am. Statist. Assoc., 75: 789– 795 (1980). Etnier, D. A., and W. C. Starnes. The Fishes of Tennessee. The University of Tennessee Press, Knoxville, TN (1993). Flagg, T. A., C. V. W. Mahnken, and K. A. Johnson. Captive broodstocks for recovery of Snake River sockeye salmon. Am. Fish. Soc. Symp., 15: 81–90 (1995). Gomahr, A., M. Palzenberger, and K. Kotrschal. Density and distribution of external taste buds in cyprinids. Environ. Biol. Fishes, 33: 125–134 (1992). Heggberget, T. G., M. Staurnes, R. Strand, and J. Husby. Smoltification in salmonids. NINA-Norsk-Institutt-for-NaturforskningForskningsrapport, 31: 3–42 (1992). Hlohowskyj, C. P., M. M. Coburn, and T. M. Cavender. Comparison of filtering pharyngeal apparatus in seven species of herbivorous cyprinid genus Hybognathus (Pisces: Cyprinidae). Copeia, 1: 172– 183 (1989). Hoshaw, R. W., J. R. Rosowski. Methods for microscopic algae. In: Phycological Methods, Culture Methods and Growth Measurements (J. R. Stein, Ed.). Press Syndicate of the University of Cambridge, NY, pp. 53–68 (1969). Ikenson, B. Rio Grande silvery minnow. Endangered Species Bulletin. U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service, Albuquerque, NM. p. 3 (2002). Julius, M. L., E. F. Stoermer, S. M. Colman, and T. C. Moore. A preliminary investigation of siliceous microfossil succession in late quaternary sediments from Lake Baikal, Siberia. J. Paleolimnol., 18: 187–204 (1997). Julius, M. L., J. Stepanek, O. Tedrow, C. Gamble, and H. L. Schoenfuss. Estrogen-receptor independent effects of two ubiquitous environmental estrogens on Melosira varians Agardh, a common component of the aquatic primary production community. Aquat. Toxicol., 85: 19–27 (2007). Kodric-Brown, A. Preface to the video and animation techniques. Environ. Biol. Fishes, 56: 283–284 (1999). Kortschal, K. Quantitative scanning electron microscopy of solitary chemorecptor cells in cyprinids and other teleosts. Environmental Biology of Fishes, 35: 273–282 (1992). Kortschal, K. Taste(s) and olfaction(s) in fish: A review of specialized sub-systems and central integration. Pfluegers Archiv. Euro. J. Physiol., 439: 178–180 (2000). Krammer, K., and H. Lange-Bertalot. Bacillariophyceae (1–4). In: Süßwasserflora von Mitteleuropa (B. Büdel, G. Gärtner, L. Krienitz, and G. M. Lokhorst, Eds.). Spektrum Akademischer Verlag Heidelberg, Berlin, Germany (1999). Laland, K. N., C. Brown, and J. Krause. Learning in fishes: From second memory to culture. Fish Fish., 4: 199–202 (2003). McNeil, W. J. Expansion of cultured Pacific salmon into marine ecosystems. Aquaculture, 98: 173–183 (1991). Moulton, S. R., J. G. Kenne, R. M. Goldstein, and J. A. Hambrook. Revised protocol for sampling algal, invertebrate, and fish communities as part of the national water-quality assessment program (NAWQA). U.S. Geological Survey, Reston, VA. 83 pp. (2002). reviews in fisheries science vol. 17 4 2009 FEEDING PREFERENCE OF THE RIO GRANDE SILVERY MINNOW Sray, J. C. Reconstructing Historical Diatom Community Composition of Rivers Using the Cyprinid Fish Hybognathus nuchalis. Master’s Thesis, John Carroll University, Cleveland Heights, OH, 67 pp. (1998). Suboski, M. D., and J. J. Templeton. Life skills training for hatcher fish: Social learning and survival. Fish. Res., 7: 343–352 (1989). U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service. Endangered and threatened wildlife and plants; final rule to list the Rio Grande silvery minnow as an endangered species. Federal Register I, vol. 59, no. 138 (1994). Webster, M. M., and P. J. B. Hart. Substrate discrimination and preference in foraging fish. Animal Behav., 68: 1071–1077 (2004). Whitaker, O. J. Seasonal changes in food habits of some cyprinid fishes form the White River at Petersburg, Indiana. Am. Midland Natural., 97: 411–418 (1977). Wiley, R. W., R. A. Whaley, J. B. Satake, and M. Fowden. An evaluation of the potential for training trout in hatcheries to increase poststocking survival in streams. North American Journal of Fisheries Management, 13: 171–177 (1993). Downloaded By: [MAGANA, HUGO A.] At: 16:23 17 July 2009 Nicolette, P. F., and A. Kodric-Brown. The use of digitally-modified vidoes to study the function of ornementation and courtship in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Environmental Biology of Fishes, 56 (3): 333–341 (1999). Patrick, R., and N. M. Wallace. The effect of agar on the growth of Nitzschia linearis. Am. J. Botany, 40: 600–602 (1953). Ross, S. T. The Inland Fishes of Mississippi. University Press of Mississippi, Jackson, MS. 624 pp. (2001). Rowland, W. J. Studying visual cues in fish behavior: A review of ethological techniques. Environ. Biol. Fishes, 56: 285–305 (1999). Shirey, P. D. Foraging Habits and Habitat Utilization of the Rio Grande Silvery Minnow (Hybognathus amarus) as Inferred by Diatom Frustules. Master’s Thesis, New Mexico State University Las Cruces. 60 pp. (2004). Schmidt, J. C., B. L. Everitt, and G. A. Richard. Hydrology and geomorphology of the Rio Grande and implications for river rehabilitation. Special Publications, Museum of Texas Tech University, vol 46, p. 44 (2003). 477 reviews in fisheries science vol. 17 4 2009