Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2009, 109, 119-120. © Perceptual and Motor Skills 2009 SOME COMMENTS ON LOHMAN (2008): PSEUDOSTUTTERING AS A TRAINING METHOD1 MANISH K. RAMI University of North Dakota Summary.—This letter addresses some issues in Lohman’s (2008) article on students’ perceptions of face-to-face pseudostuttering experiences. Concerns include the valuation of the pseudostuttering experiences on the telephone as compared to face-to-face experiences in training graduate students in speech-language pathology. Lohman (2008) reported data on 36 graduate students’ perception of face-to-face pseudostuttering experiences in public. The data included responses from the participants on a pre- and postexperience questionnaire and a postexperience roundtable discussion. The data were interpreted as supporting the use of face-to-face pseudostuttering experiences by graduate students as an evidence-based teaching practice. This comment expresses concern for using face-to-face pseudostuttering experiences as compared to similar pseudostuttering experiences over the telephone. Rami, Kalinowski, Stuart, and Rastatter (2003) demonstrated that pseudostuttering experiences on the telephone had a significant effect on the self-perceptions of 29 graduate speech-language pathology students in training. The authors suggested that pseudostuttering experiences on the telephone could be used as a valuable teaching exercise for the graduate students in speech-language pathology. Lohman (2008) suggests “caution in applying the outcome” of Rami, et al.’s paper “because it involved simulating stuttering over the telephone” (p. 952), while also stating in the same paragraph that “people who stutter often assert that speaking on the telephone is one of their most feared speaking situations” (Leith & Timmons, 1983). The two statements seem contradictory. If speaking on the telephone is a feared situation in people who stutter, then the value of such an experience is clear. Lohman (2008) suggests that a pseudostuttering experience on the telephone may be easier or have less value than a face-to-face situation. On the contrary, the Rami, et al. (2003) data support the opposite claim, that the pseudostuttering experiences on the telephone are not easy and that such experiences cause the participants to feel anxious, withdrawn, quiet, guarded, and insecure. Lohman (2008) also suggests that “listeners Address correspondence to Manish K. Rami, Ph.D., Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of North Dakota STOP 8040, 290 Centennial Drive, MH 108, Grand Forks, ND 58202-8040 or e-mail (manish.rami@und.edu). 1 DOI 10.2466/PMS.109.1.119-120 120 M. K. RAMI may hang up, which may increase frustration, anxiety, and insecurity” (p. 952) in the participants. Hanging up by listeners should be considered as part of the participants’ pseudostuttering experience. Such rejection and failure of communication will contribute to feelings of anxiety and insecurity, which is the purpose of the pseudostuttering exercise. The long-term consequences of a face-to-face pseudostuttering experience so powerful that it brought at least one of the participants to tears (Lohman, 2008, p. 959) must be considered. While there are no data on the long-term effects of either the face-to-face pseudostuttering exercises or those on the telephone, the contact hypothesis cautions that the type of experience could influence an individual’s prejudice (Allport, 1954). If the experience is positive, the prejudice may decrease; if the experience is negative, it may consolidate the prejudice. An intensely emotional experience such as the one described above could strengthen negative characterization of persons who stutter (Stangor, 2000). Thus, one must make an appropriate choice of an experiential exercise; an exercise that is valuable and adequate should not prove so powerful that it could potentially confirm students’ biases. The pseudostuttering exercise on the telephone seems to fit these criteria, while the face-to-face experience may be too intense. Caution is in order. As a side note, references to Ham (1990) and Hanson, Gronhovd, and Rice (1981) are corrected below. REFERENCES Allport, G. W. (1954) The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley. Ham, R. E. (1990) Clinician preparation: experiences with pseudostuttering, “It was the longest day of my life!” Journal of Fluency Disorders, 15, 305-315. Hanson, B. R., Gronhovd, K. D., & Rice, P. L. (1981) A shortened version of Southern Illinois University speech situation checklist for the identification of speech-related anxiety. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 6, 351-360. Leith, W. R., & Timmons, T. L. (1983) The stutterer’s reaction to the telephone as a speaking situation. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 8, 233-243. Lohman, P. (2008) Students’ perceptions of face-to-face pseudostuttering experience. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 107, 951-962. Rami, M. K., Kalinowski, J., Stuart, A., & Rastatter, M. P. (2003) Self-perceptions of speech language pathologists-in-training before and after pseudostuttering experiences on the telephone. Disability and Rehabilitation, 25, 491-496. Stangor, C. (2000) Stereotypes and prejudice. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. Accepted June 9, 2009. COMMENTS ON LOHMAN (2008)