Early Development and Parenting, Vol. 4 (4), 191-207 (1995) Co-Morbidity of Crying and Feeding Problems with Sleeping Problems in Infancy: Concurrent and Predictive Associations Dieter Wolke Renate Meyer Barbara Ohrt Klaus Riegel University of Munich, Germany The co-morbidity of crying, feeding and sleeping problems at 5 months of age was investigated in a representative sample of 432 infants in South Germany. A crying, sleeping or feeding problem was reported in 32.7% of these infants by their parents and a further 14.6% had two or more of these problems. Little comorbidity between crying and feeding problems was found. There were moderate to strong associations between crying and sleeping behaviours. Feeding problems showed little relationship to sleeping behaviour, but feeding type and frequency of feeds were related to night waking. Breastfed infants woke much more often at night. Crying and feeding problems at 5 months were poor predictors of sleeping behaviour at 20 or 56 months of age. Later sleeping behaviour was best predicted by infant sleeping behaviour. At 56 months, maternal distress due to sleeping and co-sleeping practices was predicted by maternal distress due to crying and feeding practices at 5 months of age. The predictions were significant but generally weak to modest in strength. Future studies on the consequences of crying or feeding problems should take into account patterns of co-morbidity. So-called 'post-colicky' sleep problems are not due to increased crying per se but rather appear to be the consequence of associated infant sleeping problems and parental caretaking patterns for dealing with night waking in infancy. Keywords: co-morbidity; colic; sleep problems; feeding; stability; continuity. The belief that crying, feeding and sleeping behaviours are related in infancy is held by both Address for correspondence: Dieter Wolke, University of Hertfordshire, Psychology Division, College Lane, Hatfield, Herts, AL10 9AB. email: D.F.H.WOLICE@herts.ac.uk, CCC 1057-3593/95/040191-17 ©1995 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. parents and physicians (Oberlander et al., 1992). This belief is most prominent when the infant is seen as suffering from colic or night waking problems (Wolke, 1994a). Changes from breast- to bottle feeding, frequent changes of formula feeds or the introduction of cereals or solids as remedies for Received 12 January 1995 Accepted 14 August 1995 192 both night waking and colic are often applied by parents with or without the advice of their physician (Beal, 1969; Oberlander et al., 1992 Forsyth et al., 1985a; Keane et al., 1988; Macknin et al., 1989; Hide and Guyer, 1982; Loughlin et al., 1985). These practices testify to the strength of the belief that crying, sleeping and feeding are interrelated. This popular concept is also reflected in recent contentions that infants characterized by hyperarousability and difficulties in self-soothing (excessive crying), sleeping or feeding should be considered as regulatory disordered (Greenspan and Lourie, 1981; DeGangi et al., 1991). DeGangi et al. (1991) proposed that infants who manifested two of these four characteristics should be considered to have the diagnosis 'regulatory disordered infant'. This diagnosis thus presumes significant co-morbidity of crying, sleeping or feeding problems. However, it is not clear that these beliefs and assumptions are supported by empirical evidence. Is there empirical support that concurrent crying, sleeping and feeding behaviours are interrelated? How many infants have feeding or sleeping problems in addition to excessive crying? Is there evidence that multiple problems have stronger associations with later regulation problems such as sleep difficulties than single problems of crying, sleeping or feeding? The available evidence is more diverse and sparse than the folklore belief might suggest. There are numerous studies concerning normative developmental patterns of crying (e.g. Barr, 1990a; St James-Roberts and Halil, 1991), sleeping (e.g. Anders and Keener, 1985; Bamford et al., 1990; Parmalee et al., 1964) and feeding behaviours (e.g. Lindberg, 1994; Skuse and Wolke, 1992; Wolke, 1994a) considered by themselves. Some have reported patterns of development in two or all three of these behaviours (e.g. Forsyth et al., 1985b; Jenkins et al., 1980; Klackenberg, 1971; Michelsson et al., 1990; Lee, 1992, 1994) but have only cursorily considered interrelationships among them. There are also naturalistic or intervention studies of infants with excessive crying (e.g. Wolke, 1993a; Barr et a1., 1992), sleep problems (e.g. Johnson, 1991; Jenkins et al., 1984; Rickert and Johnson, 1988) or feeding problems (e.g. Wolke and Skuse, 1992; Lindberg, 1994). Again, however, co-morbidity of these problems has rarely been considered in these studies (Wolke et al., 1994a). Only the concurrent relationships between feeding and sleeping or crying behaviour have received D. Wolke, R. Meyer, B. Ohrt and K. Riegel much empirical scrutiny. Most of these studies show that breastfed infants wake or feed more frequently at night in the first year of life (e.g. Butte et al., 1992; Wright, 1993; Eaton-Evans and Dugdale, 1988; Elias et al., 1986; Wailoo et al., 1990; Van Tassel, 1985). Nutrition (e.g. sucrose) provided in a single feed influences crying and sleeping behaviour and soothes the infant immediately after the feed (Barr et al. 1994; Yogman and Zeisel, 1983; Blass and Smith, 1992; Blass and Ciaramitaro, 1994; Barr, 1994; Oberlander et al. 1992). There are a few retrospective reports (e.g. Golding, 1986; Pollock, 1992) on the long-term effects of early feeding type or nutrition on later sleeping or other behaviours, but these relationships have not been studied prospectively. Very little is known about the relationship of excessive crying and concurrent or later sleeping and feeding behaviours. Weissbluth (1981), Hurry et al. (1991) and Lee (1994) reported that concurrent total sleep duration of infants who are temperamentally difficult or who cry excessively is significantly shorter in comparison to 'easy infants' or normative criers. Weissbluth et al. (1984a) further reported that infants who had suffered colic in the past were more likely to have shorter sleep durations and more interrupted sleep (i.e. sleeping problems) between 4 and 8 months of age. Weissbluth (1987) interpreted these findings as suggesting that congenital biological factors led initially to colic and persisted for a period of time. Others suggest that state regulation problems may be maintained by the failure of the parents to establish regular sleep patterns when the colic dissipates, a problem of goodness of fit between infant characteristics and parental caretaking (Carey, 1989; Thomas and Chess, 1977). To better understand interrelationships between these early behaviours, a prospective community study of a representative sample of infants in South Germany is presented which addresses the following questions: (1) Is there significant comorbidity between increased crying and sleeping and feeding problems in infancy (at 5 months of age)? (2) Do multiple problems mean more distress for the parents? (3) Are increased levels of crying or early feeding problems predictive of more frequent sleeping problems in the preschool years (i.e. more than 1 and 4 years later)? and (4) Do multiple regulation problems (i.e. regulatory disorder) have a poorer prognosis for later sleeping behaviour than single problems such as increased crying or feeding problems per se? Co-Morbidity of Crying Problems METHOD Sample The cohort consisted of all infants born alive in the geographically defined region of South Bavaria (N=70 600). Infants were enrolled in the Bavarian Longitudinal Study (see Riegel et al., 1995; Wolke et al., 1994b,c, 1995a,b; Wolke, 1993b) during the 193 •- period from January 1, 1985 to March 31, 1986 and who required admission within the first 10 days of birth to one of the 19 children's hospitals or paediatric specialist centres in this region (7505 index children, 10.6% of all births). In addition, 916 children receiving normal postnatal care were enrolled as controls. From this cohort, a normative randomly chosen stratified sample was studied longitudinally which Table 1. Definitions of crying, sleeping and feeding variables at 5 months Crying Cry duration The continuous time variable was dichotomized as follows: <2 hours/day of infant crying (intensive and continuous distress vocalizations) >or=2 hours/day Cry amount Low to average according to maternal rating of crying per day Above average Soothability Easy: mother rated the infant to be usually easy to soothe once crying Difficult: mother rated the infant to be usually difficult to soothe Cry distress The mother was asked (a) about distress caused by crying currently; (b) distress caused previously (before 5 months). Answers were combined to classify mothers as: Never distressed: the mother never felt distressed by crying (neither previously nor currently). Distressed: (a) the mother felt distressed previously (b) the mother felt distressed previously and currently (c) the mother felt distressed only currently Sleeping Night waking The infant woke on 5 or more nights per week Long night-time awakenings No/short: The infant did not wake at night or when awoken the arousal usually lasted for less than 15 minutes Moderate/long:The infant woke at night usually for 15 minutes or more Parental distress The parent reported that s/he felt distressed by the infant's sleeping behaviour Feeding Feeding type Breastfeeding: the infant was still (a) fully or (b) partially breastfed at 5 months Bottle feeding: the infant had been (a) bottle fed since birth or (b) had been changed from breast-to full bottle feeding before 5 months of age Number of meals per day Total time spend feeding (min) Problems with drinking The infant had regular problems with drinking fluids (most days per week) such as excessive drooling or problems swallowing (either (a) previously or (b) still at 5 months) Vomiting The infant had brought up food regularly (most days per week) either (a) only previously or (b) still at 5 months of age Flatulence The infant suffered regularly (most days per week) from flatulence either (a) only previously or (b) still at 5 months Bowel (gastrointestinal) problems The infant had recurrent diarrhoea or constipation either (a) previously or (b) still at 5 months I.94 was representative of infants born in 1985 in Bavaria (Bayerisches Landesamt fiir Statistik und Datenverarbeitung, 1986; Bayerische Landesdrztekammer und Kassendrztliche Vereinigung Bayern, 1985) according to sex of child, size of community, educational level of mother, gestation at birth and hospital admission in the first 10 days of life (see for details Riegel et al., 1995; Wolke et al., 1994b,c). The final sample consisted of 432 infants and their parents. Procedures Parents were approached within 48 hours of the infant's hospital admission, the study aims were explained and the parents were asked to give written informed consent to participate. The assessments of biological and sociodemographic information have been described in detail elsewhere (Riegel et al., 1995; Wolke et al., 1994b,c, 1995a). Follow-up visits were scheduled at 5 and 20 months of age (corrected for prematurity) and at 56 months chronological age. As part of a neurodevelopmental assessment, parents received a standardized interview from paediatricians of the Bavarian Longitudinal Study Research Team regarding crying, sleeping and feeding behaviours and problems with these behaviours at 5 months. The information obtained and the definitions of crying, feeding and sleeping behaviours or problems are given in Table 1. In addition, parents were asked whether these problems had been present before 5 months of age. For example (see Table 1), it was asked whether the mother was distressed about crying at 5 months, and whether she had been distressed about crying prior to 5 months. Thus, mothers could be considered to never have been distressed about crying, distressed only at 5 months or only before 5 months or continuously distressed (before and at 5 months) by infant crying. Information on current and prior behaviour was also obtained for feeding type and for problems with drinking, vomiting, flatulence and gastrointestinal difficulties. Sleep problems were also assessed during a structured interview at 20 and 56 months of age (for details Wolke, 1995; Wolke et al., 19941,, 1995a). The information obtained included presence of night waking (5 or more nights per week) at 20 months and 56 months. At 56 months, co-sleeping practices of the parents (sleeping part of or all night in the same bed with the infant), problems falling asleep ((a) time until asleep >30 minutes or (b) parents staying with child until asleep) and 13. Wolke, R. Meyer, B. Ohrt and K. Riegel parental distress caused by the child's sleep behaviour were elicited. A distinction was drawn between caretaking behaviours of the parents, infant behaviours which are problems for parents and the impact of these problems. Behaviours were feeding type, number of meals, total time spent feeding and co-sleeping (at 56 months). These differences in behaviour or caretaking style are not usually considered as problems leading to help-seeking by parents (Barr and Elias, 1988; Wolke and Skuse, 1992; Lozoff et al., 1984). Problems were those behaviours which often led to complaints and help-seeking by parents. These included long crying duration (>2 hours/day), above-average crying amount and difficult soothability for crying (St JamesRoberts and Halil, 1991; Wolke et al., 1994a); night waking, long awakenings and difficulty falling asleep (at 56 months) for sleeping (Messer, 1993; Wolke, 1994b); and problems with drinking, vomiting, flatulence and gastrointestinal disturbances for feeding (Lindberg, 1994). Distress caused by crying or sleeping problems was considered as measures of impact on parents (St James-Roberts, 1992). Statistical Analysis Group comparisons of categorical data were carried out using chi-square statistics with continuity correction or Fisher's exact test depending on cell frequencies. The phi-coefficient was applied as a measure of association (correlation) between categorical variables. T-tests for independent samples were used for continuous variables and multiple predictions of categorical and continuous variables were computed using logistic regression analysis. The p-value was set at p<0.05 (two-tailed test). The predictions as to future sleep behaviour were always computed using the presence of behaviours or problems prior to or at 5 months. If the prediction of future sleep behaviour was different when the behaviour or problem was present only Table 2. Convergence of the four different cry behaviour measures (phi-coefficients) 1 Cry amount Soothability Distress Cry duration Cry amount Soothability 0.37 0.38 0.40 'All phi-coefficients are very significant at p<0.001. 0.25 0.34 0.31 Co-Morbidity of Crying Problems prior to 5 months, then this different prediction is also reported. 195 Table 3. The relationship of parent distress due to crying and sleeping or flatulence at 5 months of age Distress due to crying RESULTS Prevalence of Crying, Feeding and Sleeping Problems Different prevalences were found for the two variables defining crying problems. More mothers reported that crying occurred in above-average amounts (17.1%) than for more than 2 hours per day (4.8%). Difficult soothability was reported by 6.3% of mothers. More mothers were distressed by their infant's crying (14.9%) than might have been expected, given the rates of crying greater than 2 hours (4.8%) or difficulties with soothability (6.3%). A further 17.2% of mothers had been distressed by crying at some time since the birth of their infant, but this distress had subsided by 5 months. Thus about one in three mothers (32.1%) was distressed by crying at some time during the first 5 months of the infant's life. Among other implications, these findings suggest that the different measures of crying and distress caused by crying were tapping different dimensions of early crying behaviour. To explore this, the degree of convergence of cry measures is examined by intercorrelations (phi-coefficients; Table 2). Statistically very significant but only moderately strong convergence between the different measures of crying and distress caused by crying was found. Night waking was reported for 21.5% of infants. Nearly half of those with night waking problems were awake for long periods at night (9.7% of all infants). Distress concerning their infant's sleep behaviour was reported by 13.8% of parents at 5 months. Most mothers were bottle feeding at 5 months, because either they had bottle fed since birth (25.8%) or had changed from breast- to bottle feeding sometime in the first 5 months (51.4%). Only a few mothers were still fully (12.9%) or partly (9.8%) breastfeeding at 5 months. Notably, the most common feeding or gastrointestinal problems reported were flatulence (9.3% at 5 months; 30.6% only prior to 5 months) and vomiting (11.4% at 5 months; 5.1% only prior to 5 months), followed by problems with drinking (2.6% at 5 months; 3.2% only prior to 5 months). The average number of feeds/day was 4.6 + 0.9 and the total time spent feeding was 56.8 ± 26.7 minutes/day at 5 months. Night waking (%)* Distress due to sleeping (%)** Flatulence (%)*** Previously Still at 5 months Still at 5 months (N=64) Only previously (N=74) Never (N=292) 32.8 28.6 25.7 10.8 18.2 11.4 12.5 35.0 29.5 13.6 11.6 12.4 ***p<0.001; "p<0.01; .p<0.05. The prevalence of night waking was 21.8% at 20 months and 13.3% at 56 months. Parents reported that 12.1% of children had problems falling asleep, 27.1% co-slept with their children and 7.2% were distressed about their child's sleep at 56 months (see Wolke et al., 1994a). Co-Morbidity of Crying with Sleeping and Feeding at 5 Months No significant co-morbidity of cry duration with either sleeping or feeding problems was found. The only significant association was that infants whose crying was considered to be above average amounts were more likely to have mothers who were also distressed about infant sleep behaviour (22.1% vs 12.0%, p<0.05). Soothability was not related to any feeding variables, but it was related to two of the three sleep measures. Infants that were difficult to soothe more frequently had night waking (44.4% vs 20.0%, p<0.01) and the parents more often felt distressed by their infant's sleep (37.0% vs 12.3%, p<0.01). Crying impact measures were often related to other behaviours. Mothers who were distressed by crying at some time were also more likely to have infants who woke regularly at night (29.0% vs 18.2%, p<0.05). Separate analyses showed that night waking was most often found with infants whose mothers were still distressed by crying at 5 months and least often for those never distressed by crying (Table 3). Infants of mothers who had felt distressed about crying only prior to 5 months had night waking rates between these two extremes. Mothers distressed by crying also felt more distressed by their infant's sleep (19.0% vs 11.4, p<0.05). Mothers who were still distressed by crying at 5 months were also most frequently D. Wolke, R. Meyer, B. Ohrt and K. Riegel 196 Table 4. Type of feeding and sleeping behaviour at 5 months Feeding type Changed to Fully breastfed Partly breastfed bottle feeding (N=215) (N=54) (N=41) From birth bottle fed 43.9 24.4 55.6 22.0 44.4 8.3 2.8 33.3 5.7 55.6 Night waking (%)** Long arousals (total) (%)** Of night wakers only (proneness; N=91)(%) 1 Distress due to sleeping (total)(%)* Of night wakers only (proneness; N=91)(%) 2 46.3 16.7 36.0 26.4 56.0 18.1 9.3 51.3 14.0 69.2 (N=108) *p<0.01; **p<0.001. 1Phi=0.17,NS. 2Phi=0.19,NS. distressed by infant sleep (Table 3). In contrast, mothers whose distress about crying had ceased by 5 months reported no more distress with their infant's sleep than those who were never distressed by crying (Table 3). Distress about crying was also significantly related to reports of infant flatulence (55.1% vs 32.9%, p <0.001). Separate analysis showed that previous distress was related mostly to previous occurrence of flatulence while continuous distress was related to continuous flatulence problems (Table 3). Despite the large number of comparisons, significant associations tended to cluster at above chance levels only around relationships between crying and sleeping. Five of the 12 possible associations among the four crying and three sleeping variables were significant. In particular, soothability and distress due to crying showed significant associations with sleeping. By contrast, only one of 28 comparisons among the four crying and seven feeding variables was statistically significant. Co-Morbidity of Feeding with Sleeping at 5 Months Mothers who were breastfeeding at 5 months were more likely to have infants who were waking regularly at night than those who were bottle feeding (45.3% vs 14.9%, p<0.001). Night waking was found to be similar for fully and partially breastfed infants (Table 4). The lowest frequency of night waking was found for infants bottle fed since birth (Table 4). More breast- than bottle fed infants woke for long periods at night (20.0% vs 7.1%, p<0.001). Infants bottle fed from birth rarely woke for long periods while one of four partially breastfed infants did so (Table 4). To assess the frequency of long night waking independent of the base rate of night waking, the relative frequency of long periods of waking among night wakers only (i.e. proneness to long awakenings) as a function of feeding type was computed. As shown in Table 4, infants of mothers who had made clear decisions to fully breast- or bottle feed were least prone to long arousals once awoken, while partially breastfed infants, and bottle feeders who had changed from breastfeeding were prone to long arousals once they had awoken. This difference was, however, not significant due to the smaller sample of night wakers only. Breastfeeding mothers were also more often distressed by their infant's sleeping than bottle feeders (24.5% vs 11.3%, p<0.01). Separate analysis (Table 4) showed that mothers who had bottle fed from birth were the least distressed by sleep behaviour while the mothers who were fully breastfeeding were the most distressed by their infant's sleep. Interestingly, fewer mothers were distressed by their infant's sleep (13.8%) than there were infants who were night wakers (21.5%), but nearly all distressed mothers had infants who woke regularly at night. However, one breast- and four bottle feeding mothers reported distress due to sleep even though their infants did not wake regularly at night. Proneness to distress about infant sleep was computed as the relative frequency of distress reports among night wakers as a function of feeding type. As shown in Table 4, those mothers who had changed from breast- to bottle feeding were the most prone to report distress about infant sleep, although this was not statistically significant. Flatulence was also related to mothers' reports of night waking (27.9% vs 17.4%, p<0.05), long Co-Morbidity of Crying Problems Table 5. 197 Flatulence problems and sleeping behaviour at 5 months Flatulence problems Still at 5 months (N=40) Only previously (N=132) Never (N=259) 35.0 22.5 64.3 25.0 71.4 25.8 11.4 44.1 18.3 64.7 17.4 6.9 40.0 9.8 48.9 Night waking (%)* Long night time arousal (total)(%)* Of night wakers only (proneness; N=93)(%) 1 Distress due to sleep (total)(%)** Of night wakers only (proneness; N=93)(%)2 *p<0.05; **p<0.01. 'Phi=0.17,NS. Thi=0.19,NS. arousals at night (57.1% vs 38.0%, p<0.05) and distress caused by sleep behaviour (19.9% vs 9.8%, p<0.01). Infants who were reported to have continuous problems with flatulence were most frequently night wakers, woke for long periods and were more likely to have distressed mothers (Table 5). Previous problems with flatulence which had since subsided were still related to more sleep problems and increased proneness to distress due to sleep at 5 months, although the latter was not significant (Table 5). The frequency of feeds and the total time spent feeding were significantly related to night waking. Infants who awoke regularly at 5 months averaged one more feed/day than those who did not (5.3+1.0 vs 4.4+0.7, p<0.001) and were fed longer (62.1+27.6 vs 55.3 ± 26.3 min, p<0.05). Similarly, infants who awoke for long periods at night were 10 o farLel/ 0° CC ‘ .,s.':' ,b 4' • : e,cl‘ Single Problem , 61( 0- ,,c,•'4' • cs0° cc‘' ,,,, A,co •0° e.,41%. ie, ' c,c' c,..04'y Iry Regulatory Disordered Figure 1. Pattern of co-morbidities of crying, feeding and sleeping problems at 5 months of age. 198 D. Wolke, R. Meyer, B. Ohrt and K. Riegel Table 6. Distress due to crying as predictor of sleeping behaviour at 20 and 56 months of age Distress due to crying (5 months) Only previously Still at 5 months Never (N=74) (N=64) (N=292) Night waking at 20 months (%)** Co-sleeping at 56 months (%)* Distress due to sleeping at 56 months (°/0) • 32.8 35.9 10.9 29.2 35.1 12.9 17.3 23.3 5.0 **p<0.01; •p<0.05. fed more often (5.1 + 0.8 vs 4.5 + 0.9, p<0.001) and for longer (65.1 + 29.3 vs 55.9 + 26.3 min, p<0.05). Finally, mothers who were distressed by their infants' sleep fed their infants more frequently than those not distressed (5.0 + 1.0 vs 4.5 + 0.9, p<0.001), but there was no difference in total duration of feeding. Gastrointestinal problems, drinking problems and vomiting were not related to sleep behaviour at 5 months. Thus, feeding type, the number and duration of feeds, and flatulence showed associations with concurrent sleeping behaviour. Eleven of the 12 associations with the three sleeping variables were significant. As the associations with sleeping were similar for the above three variables, we investigated the convergence of feeding type with number and duration of feeds. Breastfed infants were fed more often than bottle feeders (5.3 +1.3 vs 4.4 +0.6, p<0.001) and spent more time feeding (68.9 +34.2 min vs 53.2 + 21.7, p<0.001). Fully breastfed infants were fed most often (5.5 ± 1.5) and for the longest durations (73.4 + 37.2 min). Infants who had been switched to bottle feeding or were bottle fed from birth were very similar, and had the fewest meals (switch: 4.3 + 2.6; from birth: 4.4 + 0.7) and shortest feeds (switch: 52.7 + 21.9 min; from birth: 54.2 + 21.4 min). Partially breastfed infants were between fully breastfed and fully bottle fed infants (5.1 +1.0 feeds/day; 63.4 + 30.0 min/feed; p<0.001). were reported in 14.6% of infants; a further 32.7% had a single problem and 52.7% had no problems. The relative frequencies of infants with only one, two or all three problems are shown by problem category in Figure 1. The frequency of single sleep, feeding and crying problems as well as combinations of these was similar. About 48% of the crying, 52% of the feeding and 54% of the sleep problems occurred without any comorbidity. We determined whether mothers of 'regulatory disordered' infants were more distressed by their infants' crying or sleeping at 5 months than those with just a single problem. More mothers were distressed about crying if the infant was regulatory disordered (47.6%) than if there was an isolated problem of any type (16.3%) or no problems (4.9%, p<0.001). Furthermore, an isolated cry problem was less likely to be distressing (38.1%) than a cry problem combined with a sleeping or feeding problem (60.0%, p<0.001). Similarly, more mothers were distressed about infant sleeping if the infant was regulatory disordered (42.9%) than if there was an isolated problem of any type (20.6%) or no problems (1.3%, p<0.001). Little difference in the reports of distress about sleeping was found, however, if the infant had an isolated sleeping problem (54.0%) or had a sleeping problem combined with a crying or feeding problem (62.8%). Multiple Problems and Parental Distress Later Sleep Problems: Single Predictors Following DeGangi et al. (1991), infants were defined as 'regulatory disordered' if they had at least two of the following persistent problems at 5 months: a crying problem (cry duration > 2 hours/day, above average amount of crying, or difficult to soothe: 20.1%), a feeding problem (vomiting, problem with drinking, or flatulence: 21.6%) or a sleeping problem (night waking; 21.8%). Multiple problems (regulatory disorder) Long crying duration and having felt distressed about crying during the first 5 months were significant predictors of night waking problems at 20 months. Infants with cry durations > 2 hours/ day at 5 months were more likely to wake regularly at 20 months (42.1%) versus those with shorter crying durations (21.0%, p<0.05). Similarly, mothers who felt distressed by crying during the first 5 months were more likely to have infants with Co-Morbidity of Crying Problems 199 night waking problems at 20 months (30.9% vs 17.3%, p<0.01). Separate analyses showed that those who continued to be distressed and those who were only distressed by crying prior to 5 months had infants who woke more frequently than those who never were distressed by crying (Table 6). None of the four cry measures were related to night waking or problems falling asleep at 56 months, although there were some non-significant tendencies in the expected directions. Co-sleeping, however, was predicted by maternal distress about crying in the first 5 months. Distressed mothers were more likely to co-sleep with their infants at 56 months (35.5% vs 23.3%, p<0.05). Separate analyses showed that co-sleeping was similar for mothers still distressed at 5 months and for mothers who had ceased to be distressed by crying by 5 months (Table 6). Furthermore, mothers distressed by crying in the first 5 months were more likely to be distressed by their child's sleep at 56 months (11.9% vs 5.0%, p<0.05). Again, there was no difference whether the distress had continued until, or only prior to, 5 months (Table 6). There were a number of relationships between feeding type or feeding problems and later sleep behaviour. Infants who were breastfed at 5 months more often had night waking problems at 20 months than bottle feeders (30.9% vs 18.8%, p<0.05). Infants fully breastfed at 5 months were most likely to wake at night at 20 months (35.8%), followed by those partially breastfed (24.4%) and those who had switched to bottle feeding (18.9%) or were bottle fed from birth (18.7%, p<0.05). This difference disappeared by 56 months. Night wakers at 20 months were fed more frequently at 5 months (4.8 + 1.1 vs 4.5 + 0.8, p<0.05). This tendency was still apparent at 56 months but not statistically significant. Mothers who co-slept at 56 months fed more frequently at 5 months (4.8+1.1 vs 4.5 + 0.8, p<0.01). Total duration of feeds at 5 months was not related to later sleep behaviour. Vomiting at or before 5 months was not related to night waking at 20 months but was related at 56 months (21.1% vs 11.8%, p<0.05). In particular, infants who had problems with vomiting at 5 months (26.5%) but not those with only previous vomiting problems (9.1%) had night waking problerns (p<0.05). Mothers who had had an infant with vomiting problems were also more likely to practise co-sleeping at 56 months (37.1% vs 25.2%, p<0.05). Again, those with vomiting problems at 5 months were more frequently co-sleeping (41.7%) than those with only previous problems (27.3%, p<0.06). A significant association of gastrointestinal problems at 5 months with problems in falling asleep at 56 months was found (29.2% vs 11.2%, p<0.05). This was due to those infants who had had gastrointestinal problems before 5 months (42.9%) and not to those whose problems were still present at 5 months (10.0%, p<0.05). In summary, crying amount, soothability, duration of meals, flatulence and drinking problems at 5 months were not predictive of later sleep behaviour. As shown only distress due to crying, frequency of feeds and vomiting at 5 months showed long-term associations with sleep in the pre-school years. Later Sleep Problems: Multiple-Problem Infants Significant predictions as to night waking at 20 months and co-sleeping at 56 months were found for both regulatory disordered and single-problem infants (Table 7). There was a tendency for more frequent night waking at 56 months in infants with a single problem compared to infants with multiple or no problems (p<0.07; Table 7). Furthermore, there was a tendency for infants both with a single Table 7. Later sleeping behaviour of infants with no, single or multiple problems (at 5 months of age) Number of problems at 5 months 20 months Night waking (%)*** 56 months Night waking (%)t Co-sleeping (%)" Distress (%)t *"p<0.001; "p<0.01; tp<0.10. No problem (N=227) Single problem (N=141) Multiple problems (N=63) 13.8 31.7 27.4 10.2 20.9 4.5 18.6 34.5 10.4 12.9 33.3 9.7 200 problem and with multiple problems to have mothers distressed about sleeping at 56 months (p<0.09; Table 7). No associations of regulatory disorder with problems falling asleep at 56 months were detected. In general, the findings show that, for predicting later sleep behaviour, it did not matter whether the infant had only one or many problems at 5 months (Table 7). To further investigate whether increased crying per se, independent of sleep or feeding problems, predicted later sleep behaviour, infants with only a crying problem (without co-morbidity), infants with a crying and other problems (crying and feeding, or crying and sleeping, or crying, feeding and sleeping problems) and infants without any crying problem (i.e. no problems, single feeding or sleeping problem, feeding and sleeping problems) were compared. If increased crying is important for prediction of later sleep behaviour, then only infants with a crying or crying plus additional problems should have later sleep problems more frequently. No differences were found between infants with no crying problem, only a crying problem, or a crying plus other problems and sleep behaviour at 20 and 56 months. The same type of analyses were repeated for sleeping (no sleeping problem, only a sleeping problem, sleeping plus other problems) and feeding (no feeding problem, only a feeding problem, feeding plus other problems). No differences among infants with no feeding problems, only a feeding problem or a feeding problem plus other problems were found in relation to later sleep. By contrast, infants with only sleep problems at 5 months (42.9%) or sleep plus other problems (38.1%) more often had night waking problems at 20 months compared to infants without sleep problems (16.4%, p<0.001). Sleep behaviour at 5 months tended to be related to night waking at 56 months (sleep problem only: 24.0%; sleep problem plus other problem: 9.5%; no problem: 12.2%) but was not as statistically reliable (p<0.06). Parents of infants who only had sleep problems at 5 months (16.7%) were more distressed by sleeping at 56 months than those who had sleep plus other problems (7.1%) or no sleep problems (5.8%, p<0.10) at 5 months. No significant relationships with co-sleeping and problems falling asleep were found. These analyses indicate that later sleep problems are mainly related to the co-morbidity of either crying or feeding problems with sleep problems at 5 months rather than to crying or feeding problems per se. Stepwise logistic regressions were computed to determine the independent contributions of D. Wolke, R. Meyer, B. Ohrt and K. Riegel crying, sleeping and feeding problems at 5 months to 20 and 56 months' sleep behaviour. The criterion for variables to enter the logistic function was set at alpha <0.05. Night waking at 20 months was predicted only by sleep problems at 5 months (model chi-square: 22.4; df-1, p<0.001). Crying and feeding problems made no additional contribution to the prediction. The odds ratio of having night waking problems at 20 months if sleep problems were present versus not present at 5 months was 3.5. Maternal distress concerning sleep at 56 months was also predicted only by night waking at 5 months (model chi-square: 3.9, df=1, p<0.05). The odds ratio of distress at 56 months if sleep problems were present at 5 months (versus not present) was 2.3. Night waking, co-sleeping and falling asleep at 56 months were not predicted by sleeping, feeding or crying problems at 5 months. Finally, logistic stepwise regressions were computed including measures of the impact of infant problems (distress due to crying, distress due to sleeping) and feeding behaviour (type, frequency and duration of feeding) as predictor variables, in addition to the three composite measures of crying, sleeping and feeding problems at 5 months. Sleep problems at 5 months remained the best predictor of sleep problems at 20 months (p<0.001, odds ratio 3.3), with maternal distress about crying entering the equation as an additional predictor (p<0.06; odds ratio 1.8; model chi-square: 25.4 df=2, p<0.001). Maternal distress about sleep at 56 months was best predicted by maternal distress about crying at 5 months (p<0.05, odds ratio 2.4), followed by sleep problems at 5 months (p<0.05, odds ratio 2.3; model chi-square: 8.0, df=2, p<0.05). Finally, co-sleeping practices at 56 months were predicted by a higher number of feeds (p<0.01) and presence of a feeding problem (p<0.05) at 5 months (model chi-square: 12.0, df=2, p<0.01). DISCUSSION As part of a longitudinal community study on the cognitive, motor and socioemotional development of infants, the prevalence of crying, feeding and sleeping problems, the co-morbidity of crying with feeding and sleeping problems and the prediction from crying and feeding behaviour in infancy to later sleeping behaviour were analysed. The prevalences of crying problems (20.1%) and their impact (i.e. maternal distress about crying: 14.9%), sleeping problems (21.5%) and their impact Co-Morbidity of Crying Problems (13.8%) and feeding problems (21.6%) in this study were generally comparable to those of previous community studies of infants in western societies in the first year of life using parent report measures (see introduction). Nearly half of all infants (47.3%) were reported to have a crying, sleeping or feeding problem according to our definitions at 5 months. The prevalences of sleep problems and sleeping practices at 20 and 56 months of age were also highly comparable to previous studies with preschool children (e.g. Jenkins et al., 1984; Klackenberg, 1968, 1971; Wolke et al., 1995a). Co-Morbidity Patterns The more 'objective' parent report measures of duration and amounts of crying showed no or only weak associations with sleep behaviour. In contrast, measures which assessed the impact of crying on the mother (St James-Roberts, 1992, 1993), such as distress due to crying, showed stronger associations, particularly with sleep behaviour. These findings are in agreement with Weissbluth et al. (1984a), who found associations between retrospective reports of colic and more frequent night waking in infants at 4-8 months. Aggregation of various crying and feeding problem measures permitted comparisons of single versus multiple problems of crying, feeding and sleeping behaviours. Between 48 and 54% of the crying, sleeping and feeding problems occurred without any co-morbidity, while the remainder showed overlap with problems in one or two other areas of functioning (regulatory disordered infants, Figure 1). The overall levels of co-morbidity of crying problems with sleeping and feeding problems (50%) in this community sample are slightly lower than those reported in two recent clinical studies. In a sample of 3-4-month-old infant referred for excessive crying, Wolke et al. (1994a) reported that 70-80% of these infants had concurrent feeding problems and 50-80% had concurrent sleep problems by maternal report. Fifty to 80% of the mothers had previously seen health professionals for these additional problems. Papougek et al. (1994) reported moderate to high levels of co-morbidity in their sample of infants aged 1-24 months treated at their clinic for crying babies. They reported that 53% had multiple persistent problems in behavioural regulation, while a further 26% had multiple problems associated with psychological disturbances in the parents at the time of referral. Not all parents of excessively crying infants ask for help. Parental 201 impact of crying and help-seeking is dependent on crying features and family factors such as the number of older siblings (more siblings is associated with lower impact), multiple births (higher impact), social class (higher SES, higher impact), early mother—infant relationship (early problems, higher impact), caretaking support (lower support, higher impact) and psychosocial problems (presence of problems, higher impact) (Golding and Butler, 1986; Hide and Guyer, 1982; St James-Roberts, 1992, 1993; Wolke et al., 1994d). The higher rates of co-morbidity reported in clinical studies in comparison to this and a British community study (Golding, 1986) are likely to be due to selective referral, such that infants are referred when their parents feel more distressed by infant crying and when infants have multiple problems (Wolke et al., 1994a; Papougek et al., 1994). However, having an infant who wakes at night is enough to distress parents independent of other behaviours (e.g. crying or feeding) or daytime sleep behaviour. Notable and somewhat surprising was the relative lack of association between feeding type and crying (Barr et al., 1989). Clinically, changes in feeding have frequently been recommended for crying problems (Woolridge and Fisher, 1988; Breslow, 1957) and are adopted by parents with and without paediatric advice (Barr et al., 1991; Forsyth et al., 1985b). Changes from breast- to bottle feeding are more often initiated by parents when infants cry excessively (Golding and Butler, 1986; Hide and Guyer, 1982; Loughlin et al., 1985). This may change the pattern of crying by small amounts in non-problem criers (Barr et al., 1989), but there is no proof of its effectiveness in infants who cry excessively (Forsyth et al., 1985a; Stahlberg and Savilahti, 1986; Evans et al., 1981; Wolke and Meyer, 1995). No differences in the prevalence of colic in breast- versus bottle fed infants have been found in previous community studies (Hide and Guyer, 1982; Golding and Butler, 1986). Changing feeding type may, however, affect parental perceptions and attributions: it may strengthen parents' belief that their infants are vulnerable or have longterm health problems even when this is unfounded (Forsyth and Canny, 1991; Warner and Hathaway, 1984; Wolke and Meyer, 1995). Furthermore, the number or durations of feeds also did not differ with duration or amount of crying, soothability or distress caused by crying. Barr and Elias (1988) also reported that shorter feeding intervals (and thus more frequent feeding) had no influence on crying at 4 months, although crying at 2 months was less when feeding intervals were shorter. 202 Of the feeding problems, only the presence of flatulence was related to distress caused by crying, but it was not related to crying itself as reported by the parents. Under- or overfeeding and gastrointestinal problems are the causes most commonly implicated by pediatricians for excessive crying (Keller et al., 1990; Woolridge and Fisher, 1988; Breslow, 1957) despite a lack of empirical evidence (Liebman, 1981; Miller and Barr, 1991d; St JamesRoberts, 1993; Danielsson and Hwang, 1985). The most common interventions concentrate on feeding changes or medication for flatulence (Keller et al., 1990; Danielsson and Hwang, 1985). We did not have information about frequency of consultations or medications prescribed in this study, but distressed parents may have adopted the explanation of flatulence often used by paediatricians to explain crying in their infant. Individual feeding problems such as vomiting, drinking problems or gastrointestinal difficulties were seldom related to sleep. In contrast, sleep at 5 months showed strong associations with feeding type and with feeding frequency and duration. Breastfed infants awoke more frequently at night (e.g. Wright et al., 1983; Eaton-Evans and Dugdale, 1988; Kleitman and Engelmann, 1953; Wailoo et al., 1990) and were also awake for longer at night. This is likely to be due to breastfeeding the infant at night if awoken (Van Tassel, 1985), which may account in part for the shorter infant sleep durations reported previously (e.g. Beal, 1969; Butte et al., 1992; Elias et al., 1986; Keane et al., 1988). Thus the relationship of frequency and duration of feeds to night waking, night time arousal and distress is mostly accounted for by feeding type. These frequent and longer feeds may also explain the greater distress reported by breastfeeding mothers who experience sleep deprivation and poorer mood (Alder and Bancroft, 1988; Alder and Cox, 1983). Consequences for Later Sleeping Our findings clearly indicate that night waking and parental distress about sleep at pre-school age were not related to increased crying or feeding problems per se (St James-Roberts and Plewis, 1992), but rather that early night waking persisted into the pre-school years (Wolke et al., 1994b, 1995a; Jenkins et al., 1984; Zuckerman et al., 1987). Distress about crying, breastfeeding and number of feeds was predictive of later sleep, but this is partly accounted for by their high D. Wolke, R. Meyer, B. Ohrt and K. Riegel association with sleeping difficulties at 5 months. Learning to sleep through the night is highly dependent on environmental cues and caretaking practices (van Tassel, 1985), such as the choice to breastfeed on demand. In early infancy, frequent breastfeedings are necessary to satisfy the physiological demands for growth and brain development (Skuse et al., 1994a,b,c.). In the second half of the first year, breastfeeding at night is neither necessary nor sufficient to maintain adequate growth (Habbick and Gerrard, 1984; Skuse et al., 1994a; Wolke and Skuse, 1992). Most infants will have been introduced to cereals and solid food (Heinig et al., 1993; Wolke, 1994b) and growth hormones play an increasing role in regulating growth (Karlberg et al., 1987; Skuse et al., 1994a,b). Furthermore, by 6 months, neurophysiological sleep structure is comparable to that of adults (Coons and Guilleminault, 1982; Ferber, 1990; Wolke, 1994a,b), although infants need more sleep and have several naps during the day. Nervous system immaturity in very pre-term infants is not related to impaired development of day—night rhythms (Anders and Keener, 1985; Shimada et al., 1993; McMillen et al. 1991) or later sleeping (Wolke et al., 1995a; Wolke, 1995). This is the best available evidence so far that environmental cues rather than neurological maturity are involved in the development of circadian and ultradian rhythms and uninterrupted night sleeping in infancy. Thus, there is no apparent physiological reason to continue frequent night time breast- or bottle feeding after 6 months. Rather, as shown here and elsewhere, feeding choice leads to different caretaking and interaction patterns, especially at night (Butte et al., 1992; Elias et al., 1986; Wright, 1993). Co-sleeping and frequent breastfeeding is the usual caretaking practice in most non-western societies (Jackson, 1990; Barr, 1990b) including highly industrialized countries such as Japan (Caudill and Plath, 1966; Nugent et al., 1994) and South Korea (Lee, 1992). However, night waking, frequent night time feeds and co-sleeping are often distressing to mothers in Germany (Wolke et al., 1994b, 1995a) and other western societies, where such practices are frequent but not the norm (Johnson, 1991; Lozoff et al., 1984; Barr, 1990b). Night waking of infants is often a problem for the parents because it disturbs their sleep, interferes with intimate partner relationships and affects daytime functioning (Minde et al., 1993; Minden and Durand, 1993; Wolke, 1994b). Co-Morbidity of Crying Problems To conclude, our findings support Weissbluth et al.'s (1984a; Weissbluth, 1986, 1987) assertions that so-called 'post-colicky' sleep problems are likely to be due to a failure of the parents to establish and maintain regular sleep schedules. This is further supported by the finding that treatments for colic which do not alter caretaking such as medication (Weissbluth et al., 1984b) or herbal teas (Weizman et al., 1993) do not improve sleep behaviour. Only if the older infant learns to self-control the process of falling asleep (i.e. is not 'tricked' into sleeping by feeding, filling up, long car rides or co-sleeping) will s/he develop coping strategies to fall asleep unassisted when waking at night (Ferber, 1987; Mindell and Durand, 1993; Minde et al., 1993; Pinilla and Burch, 1993; Schmitt, 1987; Wolke et al., 1994b). Consequently, it is not because infants are breastfed or co-sleep in infancy, but because breastfeeding and co-sleeping are used as a way of settling the infant to sleep that interrupted night time sleep is maintained into the pre-school years (Pinilla and Birch, 1993; Richman, 1981; Wolke, 1994a; Kataria et al., 1987). This conclusion does not blame parents for sleep difficulties; rather, it recognizes why many parents adopt strategies to deal with night waking in the least conflictual manner by night feeding or co-sleeping. This may be especially true of parents who are dealing with a temperamentally more difficult infant (Carey, 1989; Weissbluth, 1986; Scher, 1992; Minde et al., 1993) who is demanding in interactions during the day, distresses the parents, affects their well-being negatively and wakes them at night (Van den Boom and Hoeksma, 1994; Miller et al., 1993; Murray, 1994). Study Limitations and Implications We assessed prospectively the effects of crying or feeding behaviour at 5 months on later sleep, but did not assess the continuity of crying or feeding behaviour. These data were not collected for crying, and have not yet been entered for analysis for feeding. Our data are based on parent reports in interviews only. More detailed and reliable data can be obtained by the use of diaries (e.g. Barr et al., 1988; St James-Roberts et al., 1993; Wolke et al., 1994e), but this was not possible in the current study aimed at assessing a large range of family, cognitive, neurological and somatic variables. Nevertheless, parent perceptions of infant problem behaviour have ecological validity. They determine whether parents seek help and make use of health services (St James-Roberts, 1992). 203 It can be concluded that reports of increased crying and distress caused by crying often do not occur in isolation, but are accompanied by sleeping or feeding problems as well. Whether these infants are inherently physiologically regulatory disordered (DeGangi et al., 1991; Lewis, 1992) or caretaking practices mitigate against behavioural organization of these infants cannot be concluded with certainty. Studies of both behavioural phenotype and physiological organization are required. Our findings, however, indicate that if conclusions are to be drawn about the long-term effects of increased or excessive crying (e.g. Forsyth and Canny, 1991; St James-Roberts and Plewis, 1992), these co-occurring problems need to be considered for their contribution to that outcome. Further, clinicians should be aware of co-morbidity when treating colic if they aim to prevent long-term problems such as sleep disturbances. Our understanding of underlying developmental mechanisms will benefit from considering similarities and differences in developmental changes of crying, sleeping and feeding behaviours and in co-morbidity patterns in both normative and clinical samples (Wolke, 1992, 1994a, 1995). ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We would like to thank the numerous colleagues who participated in the data collection and administration of the study. Special thanks are due to the participating hospitals. The research has been supported by grants PKE 24 (FKZ 0706 224) and JUG 14 (FKZ 0706 564) of the Federal Government of Germany, Ministry of Science and Technology (BMFT). REFERENCES Alder, E. and Bancroft, J. (1988). The relationship between breast feeding persistence, sexuality and mood in postpartum women. Psychological Medicine, 18, 389396. Alder, E. and Cox, J. L. (1983). Breastfeeding and postnatal depression. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 27, 139-144. Anders, T. F. and Keener, M. A. (1985). Developmental course of nighttime sleep-wake patterns in fullterm and premature infants during the first year of life. Sleep, 8, 173-192. Bamford, F. N., Bannister, R. P., Benjamin, C. M., Hillier, V. F., Ward, B. S. and Moore, W. M. 0. (1990). Sleep in the first year of life. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 32, 718-724. 204 Barr, R. G. (1990a). 'The normal crying curve: what do we really know?' (Annotation). Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 32,356-362. Barr, R. G. (1990b). The early crying paradox: a modest proposal. Human Nature, 1, 355-389. Barr, R. G. (1994). A new look and new questions-A commentary. In E. M. Blass and V. Ciaramitaro (Eds), A new look at some old mechanisms in human newborns: taste and tactile determinants of state, effect and action. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, (Serial No. 239). Barr, R. G. and Elias, M. (1988). Nursing interval and maternal responsiveness: effect on early crying. Pediatrics, 81, 529-536. Barr, R. G., Kramer, M. S., Boisjoly, C., McVey-White, L. and Pless, I. B. (1988). Parental diary of infant cry and fuss behavior. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 63, 380387. Barr, R. G., Kramer, M. S., Pless, I. B., Boisjoly., C. and Leduc, D. (1989). Feeding and temperament as determinants of early infant crying/fussing behavior. Pediatrics, 84, 514-521. Barr, R. G., Quek, V. S. H., Cousineau, D., Oberlander, T. F., Brian, J. A. and Young, S. N. (1994). Effects of intra-oral sucrose on cyring, mouthing and handmouth contact in newborn and six-week-old infants. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 36, 608618. Barr, R. G., Rotman, A., Yaremko, J., Leduc, D. and Francoeur, T. E. (1992). The crying of infants with colic: a controlled empirical description. Pediatrics, 90, 14-21. Barr, R. G., Woolridge, J. and Hanley, J. (1983). Does incomplete lactose absorption predispose to crying in normal infants? Society for Research in Child Development Program Abstract, 4, 12. Barr, R. G., Woolridge, J. and Hanley, J. (1991). Effects of formula change on intestinal hydrogen production and crying and fussing behavior. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 12, 248-252. Bayerische Landesdrztekammer und Kassendrztliche Vereinigung Bayervs (1985). Bayerische Perinatal- und Neonatalerhebung 1985. BPE-Jahresbericht 1985. Mianchen: Bayerische Landesarztekammer und Kassendrztliche Vereinigung Bayern. Bayerisches Landesamt hit Statistik und Datenverarbeitung (1986). Statistisches Jahrbuch 1985 fur Bayern. Mianchen: Bayerisches Landesamt far Statistik und Datenverarbeitung. Beal, V. A. (1969). Termination of night feeding in infancy. Journal of Pediatrics, 75,690-692. Blass, E. M. and Ciaramitaro, V. (1994). A new look at some old mechanisms in human newborns: taste and tactile determinants of state, effect, and action. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1 (Serial No. 239). Blass, E. M. and Smith, B. A. (1992). Differential effects of sucrose, fructose, glucose, and lactose on crying in 1- to 3-day-old human infants: qualitative and quantitative considerations. Developmental Psychology, 28,804-810. Breslow, L. (1957). A clinical approach to infantile colic. A review of ninety cases. Journal of Pediatrics, 50, 196-206. D. Wolke, R. Meyer, B. Ohrt and K. Riegel Butte, N. F., Jensen, C. L., Moon, J. K., Glaze, G. and Frost, J. D. (1992). Sleep organization and energy expenditure of breastfed and formula-fed infants. Pediatric Research, 32, 514-519. Carey, W. B. (1989). Practical applications in pediatrics. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates and M. K. Rothbart (Eds), Temperament in Childhood Chichester: Wiley, pp. 405420. Caudill, W. and Plath, D. W. (1966). Who sleeps by whom? Parent-child involvement in urban Japanese families. Psychiatry, 29, 341 366. Coons, S. and Guilleminault, C. (1982). Development of sleep-wake patterns and non-rapid eye movement sleep stages during the first six months of life in normal infants. Pediatrics, 69, 793-798. Danielsson, B. and Hwang, P. (1985). Treatment of infantile colic with surface active substance (simethicone). Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica, 74, 446450. DeGarigi, G. A., DiPietro, J. A., Greenspan, S. I. and Porges, S. W. (1991). Psychophysiological characteristics of the regulatory disordered infant. Infant Behavior and Development, 14, 37-50. Eaton-Evans, J. and Dugdale, A. E. (1988). Sleep patterns of infants in the first year of life. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 63, 647-649. Elias, M. F., Nicolson, N. A., Bora, C. and Johnston, J. (1986). Sleep/wake patterns of breast-fed infants in the first 2 years of life. Pediatrics, 77, 322-329. Evans, R. W., Fergusson, D. M., Allardyce, R. A. and Taylor, B. (1981). Maternal diet and infantile colic in breastfed infants. Lancet, 1, 1340-1342. Ferber, R. (1987). Sleeplessness, night awakening, and night crying in the infant and toddler. Pediatrics in Review, 9, 69-82. Ferber, R. (1990). Childhood insomnia. In M. J. Thorpy (Ed.), Handbook of Sleep Disorders. New York: Dekker, pp. 435-456. Forsyth, B. W. C. and Canny, P. F. (1991). Perceptions of vulnerability 31 years after problems of feeding and crying behavior in early infancy. Pediatrics, 88, 757-763. Forsyth, B. W. C., McCarthy, P. L. and Leventhal, J. M. (1985a). Problems of early infancy, formula changes, and mothers' beliefs about their infants. Journal of Pediatrics, 106, 1012-1017. Forsyth, B. W. C., Leventhal, J. M. and McCarthy, P. L. (1985b). Mothers' perceptions of problems of feeding and crying behaviors. American Journal of Disease in Childhood, 139, 269-272. Golding, J. (1986). Feeding and sleeping problems. In N. R. Butler and J. Golding (Eds), From Birth to Five. Oxford: Pergamon, pp. 80-97. Golding, J., and Butler, R. (1986). The first months. In N. R. Butler and J. Golding (Eds), From Birth to Five. Oxford: Pergamon, pp. 46-63. Greenspan, S. I. and Lourie, R. S. (1981). Development structuralist approach to the classification of adaptive and pathologic personality organizations: application to infancy and early childhood. American Journal of Psychiatry, 138, 725-735. Co-Morbidity of Crying Problems Habbick, B. F. and Gerrard, J. W. (1984). Failure to thrive in the contented breast-fed baby. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 131, 765-768. Heinig, M. J., Nommsen, L. A., Peerson, J. M., Lonnerdal, B. and Dewey, K. G. (1993). Intake and growth of breast-fed and formula-fed infants in relation to the timing of introduction of complementary foods: the DARLING study. Acta Paediatrica, 82,999-1006. Hide, D. W. and Guyer, B. M. (1982). Prevalence of infant colic. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 57,559-560. Hurry, J., Bowyer, J. and St. James-Roberts, I. (1991). The development of infant crying and its relationship to sleep-waking organisation. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, Washington. Jackson, D. (1990). Three in a Bed-Why You Should Sleep With Your Baby. London: Bloomsbury. Jenkins, S., Bax, M. and Hart, H. (1980). Behaviour problems in pre-school children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 21, 5-17. Jenkins, S., Owen, C., Bax, M. and Hart, H. (1984). Continuities of common behaviour problems in preschool children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 25, 75-89. Johnson, C. M. (1991). Infant and toddler sleep: a telephone survey of parents in one community. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 12, 108-114. Karlberg, J., Engstrom, I., Karlberg, P. and Fryer, J. G. (1987). Analysis of linear growth using a mathematical model. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica, 76, 478-488. Kataria, S., Swanson, M. S. and Trevathan, G. E. (1987). Persistence of sleep disturbances in preschool children. Journal of Pediatrics, 110, 642-646. Keane, V., Chamey, E., Strauss, J. and Roberts, K. (1988). Do solids help baby sleep through the night? American Journal of Diseases of Children, 142, 404-405. Keller, H., Ubozak, c. and Risau, J. (1990). The concept of colic and infant crying in pediatrics: an exploratory study. Early Child Development and Care, 65, 71-76. Klackenberg, G. (1968). The development of children in a Swedish urban community. A prospective longitudinal study. I. The sleep behaviour of children up to three years of age. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica (Suppl. 187), 105-121. Klackenberg, G. (1971). A prospective longitudinal study of children: data on psychic health and development up to 8 years of age. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica (Suppl. 224), 74-82. Kleitman, N. and Engelmann, T. G. (1953). Sleep characteristics of infants. Journal of Applied Physiology, 6, 269-282. Lee, K. (1992). Pattern of night waking and crying of Korean infants from 3 months to 2 years old and its relation with various factors. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 13, 326-330. Lee, K. (1994). The crying pattern of Korean infants and related factors. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 36, 601-607. Lewis, M. (1992). Individual differences in response to stress. Pediatrics, 90, 487-490. 205 Liebman, W. B. (1981). Infantile colic association with lactose and milk intolerance. Journal of the American Medical Association, 245, 732-735. Lindberg, L. (1994). Early feeding problems. A developmental perspective. Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis. Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Social Sciences, Uppsala. Loughlin, H. H., Clapp-Charming, N. E., Gehlbach, S. H., Pollard, J. C. and McCutchen, T. (1985). Early termination of breast-feeding: identifying those at risk. Pediatrics, 75, 508-513. Lozoff, B., Wolf, A. W. and Davis, N. S. (1984). Cosleeping in urban families with young children in the United States. Pediatrics, 72, 171-182. Macknin, M. L., VanderBrug Medendorp, S. and Maier, M. C. (1989). Infant sleep and bedtime cereal. American Journal of Diseases of Children, 143, 1066-1068. McMillen, C., Kok, J. S. M., Adamson, T. M., Deayton, J. M. and Nowak, R. (1991). Development of circadian sleep-wake rhythms in preterm and full-term infants. Pediatric Research 29, 381-384. Messer, D. (1993). The treatment of sleeping difficulties. In I. St. James-Roberts, D. Messer and G. Harris (Eds), Infant Crying, Feeding and Sleeping: Development, Problems and Treatments. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf, pp. 194-210. Michelsson, K., Rinne, A. and Paajanen, S. (1990). Crying, feeding and sleeping patterns in 1 to 12 month-oldinfants. Child: Care, Health and Development, 16, 99-111. Miller, A. R. and Barr, R. G. (1991). Infantile colic: is it a gut issue? Pediatric Clinics of North America, 38, 14071423. Miller, A. R., Barr, R. G. and Eaton, W. 0. (1993). Crying and motor behavior of six-week-old infants and postpartum maternal mood. Pediatrics, 92,551-558. Minde, K., Popiel, K., Leos, N., Falkner, S., Parker, K. and Handley-Derry, M. (1993). The evaluation and treatment of sleep disturbances in young children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 521-533. Minden, J. A. and Durand, V. M. (1993). Treatment of childhood sleep disorders: generalization across disorders and effects on family members. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 18, 731-750. Murray, L. (1994). The infant's contribution to the mother-infant relationship in the context of postnatal depression. Presented at the 9th International Conference on Infant Studies (ICIS), Paris, France. Nugent, K., Kawasaki, C. and Brazelton, T. B. (1994). The sleep environment of Japanese and US infants. Presentation at the 14th Annual Conference of the Society for Reproductive and Infant Psychology, Trinity College, Dublin. Oberlander, T. F., Barr, R. G., Young, S. N. and Brian, J. A. (1992). Short-term effects of feed composition on sleeping and crying in newborns. Pediatrics, 90, 733740. PapouSek, M., von Hofacker, N., Malinowski, M., Jacubeit, T. and Cosmovici, B. (1994). Friiherkennung und Pravention von Storungen der Verhaltensregulation und der Eltern-Kind-Beziehungen: Erste Ergebnisse aus der 'Miinchner Sprechstunde ftir 206 Schreibabies'. Sozialpadiatrie und Kinderiirztliche Praxis, 16, 680-688. Parmalee, A. H., Wenner, A. H. and Schultz, H. R. (1964). Infant sleep patterns: from birth to 16 weeks of age. Journal of Pediatrics, 65, 576-582. Pinilla, T. and Birch, L. L. (1993). Help me make it through the night: behavioral entrainement of breastfed infants' sleep patterns. Pediatrics, 91, 436-444. Pollock, J. I. (1992). Predictors and longterm associations of reported sleeping difficulties in infancy. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 10, 151-168. Richman, N. (1981). A community survey of characteristics of one- to two-year-olds with sleep disruptions. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 20, 281-291. Rickert, V. I. and Johnson, C. M. (1988). Reducing nocturnal awakening and crying episodes in infants and young children: a comparison between scheduled awakenings and systematic ignoring. Pediatrics, 81, 203-212. Riegel, K., Ohrt, B., Wolke, D. and Osterlund, K. (1995). Die Entwicklung gefdhrdet geborener Kinder bis zum funften Lebensjahr. Die Arvo Ylppir-Neugeborenen-Nachfolgestudie in Sildbayern und Sildfinnland. Stuttgart: Enke-Verlag. Scher, A. (1992). Toddlers' sleep and temperament: reporting bias or a valid link? A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33,1249-1254. Schmitt, B. D. (1987). Seven deadly sins of childhood: advising parents about difficult developmental phases. Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 421-432. Shimada, M., Segawa, M., Higurashi, M. and Akamatsu, H. (1993). Development of the sleep and wakefulness rhythm in preterm infants discharged from a neonatal care unit. Pediatric Research, 33,159-163. Skuse, D., Reilly, S. and Wolke, d. (1994a). Psychosocial adversity and growth during infancy. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition (Suppl. 48), 113-130. Skuse, D., Pickles, A., Wolke, D. and Reilly, S. (1994b). Postnatal growth and mental development: evidence for a 'sensitive period'. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35,521-545. Skuse, D., Wolke, D. and Reilly, S. (1994c). Socioeconomic disadvantage and ethnic influences upon infant growth in inner London. In A. Prader and R. Rappaport (Eds), Clinical Issues in Growth Disorders: Evaluation, Diagnosis and Therapy. Tel Aviv: Freund Publishing House, pp. 57-70. Skuse, D. and Wolke, D. (1992). The nature and consequences of feeding problems in infants. In P. J. Cooper and A. Stein (Eds), The Nature and Management of Feeding Problems and Eating Disorders in Young People. New York: Harwood Academic Publishers, pp. 1-25. Stahlberg, M. R. and Savilahti, E. (1986). Infantile colic and feeding. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 61, 12321233. St James-Roberts, I. (1992). Measuring infant crying and its social perception and impact. Association of Child Psychology and Psychiatry Newsletter, 14, 128-131. St James-Roberts, I. (1993). Explanations of persistent crying. In I. St James-Roberts, G. Harris and D. Messer (Eds), Infant Crying, Feeding and Sleeping. Development, D. Wolke, R. Meyer, B. Ohrt and K. Riegel Problems and Treatment. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf, pp. 26-46. St James-Roberts, I. and Halil, T. (1991). Infant crying patterns in the first year: normal community and clinical findings. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32,951-968. St James-Roberts, I., Hurry, J. and Bowyer, J. (1993). Objective confirmation of crying durations in infants referred for excessive crying. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 68, 82-84. St James-Roberts, I. and Plewis, I. (1992). Stabilities and linkages among infant behaviour problems. Presented to the British Psychological Society, Developmental Section's Annual Conference, Edinburgh. Thomas, A. and Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and Development. New York: Brunner & Mazel. Van den Boom, D. C. and Hoeksma, J. B. (1994). The effect of infant irritability on mother-infant interaction: a growth-curve analysis. Developmental Psychology, 30, 581-590. Van Tassel, E. B. (1985). The relative influence of child and environmental characteristics on sleep disturbances in the first and second years of life. Journal of Development and Behavioral Pediatrics, 6, 81-87. Wailoo, M. P., Petersen, S. A. and Whitaker, H. (1990). Disturbed nights in 3-4 month old infants: the effects of feeding and thermal environment. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 65, 499-501. Warner, J. 0. and Hathaway, M. J. (1984). Allergic form of Meadow's syndrome (Munchhausen by proxy). Archives of Disease in Childhood, 59,151-156. Weissbluth, M. (1981). Sleep duration and infant temperament. Journal of Pediatrics, 99, 817-819. Weissbluth, M. (1986). Early sleep problems and temperament. In G. A. Kohnstamm (Ed.), Temperament Discussed-Temperament and Development in Infancy and Childhood. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger, pp. 147-158. Weissbluth, M. (1987). Sleep and the colicky infant. In C. Guilleminault (Ed.), Sleep and Its Disorders in Children. New York: Raven Press, pp. 129-140. Weissbluth, M., Davis, A. T. and Poncher, J. (1984a). Night waking in 4- to 8-month-old infants. Journal of Pediatrics, 104, 477-480. Weissbluth, M., Christoffel, K. K. and Davis, A. T. (1984b). Treatment of infantile colic with dicyclomine hydrochloride. Journal of Pediatrics, 104, 951-955. Weizman, Z., Alkrinawi, S., Goldfarb, D. and Bitran, C. (1993). Efficacy of herbal tea preparation in infantile colic. Journal of Pediatrics, 122,650-652. Wolke, D. (1992). Infant crying, sleep-wake organization and sleep problems. Presented at the 4th International Workshop on Infant Cry Research, Miinchen. Wolke, D. (1993a). Langzeitprognose von Friihgeborenen: was wir wissen und was wir wissen sollten. In A. Lischka and G. Bernert (Eds), Aktuelle Neuropiidiatrie 1992. Wehr/Baden: Verlag Ciba Geigy, pp. 99-121. Wolke, D. (1993b). The treatment of problem crying behaviour. In I. St James-Roberts, G. Harris and D. Messer (Eds), Infant Crying, Feeding and Sleeping. Development, Problems and Treatments. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf, pp. 47-79. Co-Morbidity of Crying Problems Wolke, D. (1994a). Feeding and sleeping across the lifespan. In M. Rutter and D. Hay (Eds), Development Through Life: A Handbook for Clinicians. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific, pp. 517-557. Wolke, D. (1994b). Die Entwicklung und Behandlung von Schlafproblemen und exzessivem Schreien im Vorschulalter. In F. Petermann (Ed.), Verhaltenstherapie mit Kindern, 2nd edn. MUnchen: Gerhard ROttger Verlag, pp. 154-208. Wolke, D. (1995). Einschlaf- und Durchschlafprobleme bei biologischen Risikokindem und gesunden Vorschulkindem. In C. Becker-Carus (Ed.), Fortschritte der Schlafrnedizin. MUnster: Lit-Verlag, pp. 67-80. Wolke, D. (1995). Wo die klassische Pàdiatrie an ihre Grenzen stOsst: Die Erkldrung und Behandlung von RegulationsstOrungen bei Kindern. In K. Pawlik (Ed.), Bericht des 39. Kongress der Deutschen Gesellschaft fib . Psychologie. Gottingen: Hogrefe, pp. 469-476. Wolke, D. and Skuse, D. (1992). The management of infant feeding problems. In P. J. Cooper and A. Stein (Eds), Feeding Problems and Eating Disorders in Children and Adolescents. Chur. Harwood Academic, pp. 27-59. Wolke, D., Gray, P. and Meyer, R. (1994a). Excessive infant crying: a controlled study of mothers helping mothers. Pediatrics, 94, 322-332. Wolke, D., Meyer, R., Ohrt, B. and Riegel, K. (1994b). Hdufigkeit und Persistenz von Ein- und Durchschlafproblemen im Vorschulalter: Ergebnisse einer prospektiven Untersuchung an einer reprasentativen Stichprobe in Bayern. Praxis der Kinderpsychologie und Kinderpsychiatrie, 43, 331-339. Wolke, D., Ratschinski, G., Ohrt, B. and Riegel, K. (1994c). The cognitive outcome of very preterm infants may be poorer than often reported: an empirical investigation of how methodological issues make a big difference. European Journal of Pediatrics, 153, 906-915. Wolke, D., Meyer, R., Ohrt, B. and Riegel, K. (1994d). Prevalence and risk factors for infant excessive crying 207 at 5 months of age. In W. Koops, B. Hopkins and P. Engelen (Eds), Abstract of the 13th Biennial Meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development (ISSBD), 28. Leiden: Logon, p. 152. Wolke, D., Gray, P. and Meyer, R. (1994e). Validity of the Crying Pattern Questionnaire in a sample of excessively crying babies. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 12, 105-114. Wolke, D., Meyer, R., Ohrt, B. and Riegel, K. (1995a). The incidence of sleeping problems in preterm and fullterm infants discharged from special care units: an epidemiological longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 203-223. Wolke, D., SOhne, B., Ohrt, B. and Riegel, K. (1995b). Follow-up of preterm infants: important to document dropouts. Lancet, 345, 447. Wolke, D. and Meyer, R. (1995). The colic debate: a reply. Pediatrics, 96, 165-166. Woolridge, M. W. and Fisher, C. (1988). Colic, 'overfeeding', and symptoms of lactose malabsorption in the breast-fed baby: a possible artifact of feed management? Lancet, 2, 382-384. Wright, P. (1993). Mothers' ideas about feeding in early infancy. In I. St James-Roberts, G. Harris and D. Messer (Eds), Infant Crying, Feeding and Sleeping. Development, Problems and Treatment. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf, pp. 99-117. Wright, P., MacLeod, H. A. and Cooper, M. J. (1983). Waking at night: the effect of early feeding experiences. Child: Care, Health and Development, 9, 309-319. Yogman, M. W. and Zeisel, S. H. (1983). Diet and sleep patterns in newborn infants. The New England Journal of Medicine, 309, 1147-1149. Zuckerman, B., Stevenson, J. and Bailey, V. (1987). Sleep problems in early childhood: continuities, predictive factors, and behavioral correlates. Pediatrics, 80, 664671.